TERRORISM AND POLITICAL VIOLENCE 20i9, vol. 00, No. 00, 1-18
https://doi.orgl1 0.1 080/09546553.201 9.1 608951
1)
Routledoe
$\
raytorarran.i#rorpO Check tor updates
Does
Terrorism
Dominate
Citizens'
Hearts
or
Minds?
The
Relationship
between
Fear
of
Terrorism and Trust
in
Government
Ramon Van
Der
Doesu, Jaroslaw Kantorowiczb, sanneke Kuipersband
Marieke Liem."Universite catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium; blnstitute of Security and Global Affairs,
Leiden
5 University, The Hague, Netherlands; 'Universiteit Leiden Faculty of Governance and Global Affairs, lnstitute of Security and Global Affairs, The Hague, NetherlandsQ3
ABSTRACT
Terrorism only poses a small risk
to
people but tends to be a major source of public fear. Through fear, terrorism has far-reachingimpli-cations
for
public governance.ln
this paperwe
lookat trust
in government as a potential mitigating factor of fear of terrorism. We discern between calculative trust, based on analytical assessment ofprevious and expected future actions, and relational trust, based on emotions and perceived value similarity with government. We find
that
relational trust decreases fear of terrorism.A
similarbut
lessrobust negative relationship exists between calculative
trust
and fear. However, our regression analyses suggest that relational trust,in fact, may mediate the relationship between calculative trust and fear of terrorism. ln other words, the more citizens think government is able to prevent terrorist attack and feel that authorities are doing enough, the more they, in turn, feel that their government shares
their values, and the less fearful they are of future terrorist attacks.
KEYWORDS Trust; fear of tenorism; public administration; public
policy; survey research 10 Q4
15
20
lntroduction
Terrorism
only poses a smallrisk
ofvictimization
but tends to be a major sourceof
public fear.Through
fear,terrorism
has far-reachingimplications for public
governance. Fearof
attacks
not only
changescitizen
behaviorin public
spaces,but
also poses a "realphysical
25danger" as
citizens-paradoxically-engage in riskier
activities.lA
famous example of this mechanism includes the substantial increasein
the number of fatal highway crashesin
theUnited
Statesin
the
yearfollowing the
9/11 attacks.While driving is much riskier
thanflyttg,
many Americans decidedto
drive rather than tofly
after the attacks. The casualtiesof
the highway crashes can be viewed as an extreme exampleof
the"indirect
damagesof
30terrorism,
mediatedthrough
our minds."' Othe.
examplesinclude
increased stress levelsand
concomitant
health issues3 and the costsof additional
security measuresin
responseto public
fear.aDespite its
well-known
effects on public health, safety, and finances, westill know little
about
how
fearof terrorism
can be mitigated. Studiesof
security measures installedafter
35 CONTACT Jaroslaw KantorowiczI
jj.kantorowicz6fgga.leidenuniv.nl@
lnstitute of Security and Global Affairs, Leiden University, The Hague, NetherlandsColor versions of one or more of the figures in the article can be found online at wwwtandfonline.com/ftpv.
@ Supplementary data for this article can be accessed here.
@ 2019 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
o
R. VAN DER DOES ET AL,terrorist
attacksshow that
such measuresmay be
ineffectivein
reducing
fearsor
even increase citizens' feelingsof
insecurity.6We rely on
theliterature on risk
perceptions to arguethat
building
trust
in
government could
be
a
more promising
way
to
mitigatecitizens'
fear
of
terrorism. The political
scienceliterature has already dealt
with
the relationship betweentrust
in
governmentand
fearof
terrorism.t
However,both
theory
40 andempirics
have focusedon the
questionof how
fearof
terrorism
influencestrust in
government, rather than the other way around.s Moreover,
trust
has
generally beentreated
as
a
unidimensional concept, obfuscating
much
of
the
complexity
of
the construct.e The goalof this
article isto
address theselimitations.
In terms of
theory
we draw on the literature on risk perceptions to conceptualize trustand
45specify our hypotheses. This body ofliterature has already extensively reflected on the role trust in authorities plays in mitigating the perceived risks of various hazards.lo As risk perceptions and
fear are closely related empirically,ll we are able to use these insights to theorize how trust in government could affect fear. In terms of empirics, we make use of a unique survey administered among a representative sample
of
theDutch
populationin
November 2017(N
=
1,077).In
50contrast to many existing studies, this survey allows us to examine both of the two dimensions commonly attributed to trust: its calculative dimension, based on analytical thinking, or matters
of the mind, concerned with expected future actions, and its relational dimension, or matters
of
the heart, based on emotions and perceived value similarity with a trustee.Furthermore, our case selection provides analytical leverage to study how trust
in
govern-
55ment affects fear of terrorism. Preceding studies focus on countries in which a terrorist attack recently occurred.l2
Terrorist
attacks evoke unusually high levelsof
fear among the public which makesit
likely that citizens abruptlyput
their trustin
governmentin
order to reducefeelings of uncertainty.l3 This increases the likelihood of observing a reversed
relationship-that is, fear influencing trust rather than the other way around. By contrast, in situationswhere
60 no recentterrorist
attacks have occurred, such asin
the Netherlands, we may expectit
to be Ieastlkely
that citizens' fear of terrorism drives their trustin
government.Studies
in
various
countries suggestthat
increasedtrust
in
government afterterrorist
attacks isonly
short-lived.l4 The patternof
a steep increase followedby
aquick return
to levelsof trust
before aterrorist
attack has been documentednot only in
the UnitedStates
65after
9lll
but
alsoin
Spain after the 2004train
bombingsin Madrid
andin
Belgium afterthe
2014shooting
in
Brussels.ltThis
is
alsoin
line with
evolutionary theory:
"indivi-duals
...
turn
more
towards those seen asbeing
in
protective roles during
periodsof
heightened
threat."r6
In
other words,
only
in
the
exceptional casesin
which
people experience an immediate threat (such asafter
aterrorist
attack)do
they "seekprotection
70from
a
stronger, tougher
resource"such as
their
government.lTCombined
with
theaforementioned
empirical findings that show a quick return
to
standard levelsof
trustafter
terrorist
attacks,this
strengthensour
conviction
that
in
a context
of
"politics
asusual"l8 we are able
to
observe the influenceof trust on
fear insteadof
the reverse.Our results show that relational trust decreases fear of terrorism. We find a similar but
less
75robust negative relationship between calculative
trust
and fearof terrorism.
However, ouranalyses suggest
that
relational trust,in
fact, mediates the relationship between calculative trust and fear of terrorism.In
other words, the more citizensthink
the government is able to prevent terrorist attacks and feel the government is doing enough, the more they, in turn, feelTERRORTSM AND POL|TICAL VTOLENCE
@
3In
the
following,
we
first
review
the risk
perception
literature regarding
trust
in
government.
This
allows usto
specifrour
hlpotheses regarding fearof terrorism
in
the subsequent section. Afterwards, we present our methodology and the results. We concludewith
abrief
discussionon the implications for
governmentcounterterrorism policy
and
85goYernment
risk
communication.Theoretical expectations
Two dimensions
of
trust
We define trust as "a psychological state comprising the intention to accept vulnerability based
upon positive expectations of the intentions or behavior of another" [emphasis added].re
This
90definition
reflectsthe
academic consensusthat trust
hastwo
dimensions: calculative and relational.20 Calculativetrust
is basedon
an assessmentof
past performance and relies on consecutive inferences aboutthe
future.2lIt
refersto
"the
belief, basedon
experience or evidence (e.g., past performance),that
certainfuture
eventswill
occur as expected."22 By contrast, relationaltrust is
meant"to
reduce complexitythrough the
acceptanceof
risk,"
95 regardlessof
the consequences.'3It
doesnot
somuch rely on
expectedfuture
actions butrather "on
ajudgment of similarity of
intentionsor
values."2aThis
makes relational trusta function of social identification instead of instrumentulity.'u
These dimensions
of
trust
reflect thetwo
fundamental waysof thinking
referredto
in
cognitive psychology as the
rational
and the experiential system.26 Theformer
is slowand
100analytic, driven by evidence and logic; the latter is quick and unreflective, driven by
associa-tions and emotions. We therefore view calculative trust as mostly a matter of "the head" (i.e.,
rationality)
and relational trust as a matterof
"the heart" (i.e., emotions).27 The differences between the two types of trust may have far-reaching implications for how we understand the relationship between trustin
government and people's fear of terroristattacks.
105Risk
perceptions
and
trust
in
government
Trust in
government has already been extensively studiedwith
regardto risk
perceptions. Risk perceptions and fear are closely related empirically.2s Therefore, we drawon
insightsfrom
therisk
perceptionliterature to
specifyour
hlpotheseson
the relationship betweentrust
in
government and fear
of
terrorism.
We first
provide a
brief
overview
of the
110respective body
of
literature.In general, both types of trust are important to explain risk perceptions.'e In a recent review on natural hazards, Gisela Wachinger and colleagues conclude that
"[in]
addition to personal experience, the second mostimportant
factorsfor
risk perception of natural hazards...
aretrust
in
scientific experts and authorities [i.e. relationaltrust]
and confidencein protective
lt5
measures [i.e. calculative trust]."3O Both types oftrust
seem similarlyimportant
correlatesof
perceived riskwith
regard to technological hazards.3l Furthermore, both trust types appear tosuppress perceived risks: existing studies consistently show a significant negative relationship between trust and citizens' risk perceptions across a wide variety of hazards.32
In line with these overall findings, studies examining the two dimensions of trust separately 120
o @
*.vAN DER DoEs Er AL.flood defenses and risk management (i.e., calculative trust) significantly reduced risk percep-tions among Dutch citizens, both
with
regard to the perceived likelihood of a flood and thatof
becoming a victim. Similar results are reported in studies of technological hazards,3a
including
125nuclear waste and power35 and genetically-modified food.36
Relational trust
in
government and/or involved authorities appears to similarly decreasethe risks people
associatewith
various
hazards,including
hazardous waste disposal,3Tnuclear power,3t and
electromagnetic fields.3eNote
that
while
these studiesmay
usedifferent
targetsof
perceivedrisk
(e.g.,risk
to
oneself,to
others,or
in
general),in the
130end, they all tend to formulate some version
of
thefollowing
conclusion: the more peopletrust
authorities, the lessrisk they
associatewith
the relevant hazard.aoThe
risk
literature tendsto
explain the comparable patterns across thetwo
dimensionsof trust in
governmentby
arguingthat
the affective relationship between thetrustor
and the trustee (the basisof
relationaltrust)
influences the evaluationof
the trustee'sperfor-
135mance (the basis of calculative trust).ar However, empirical evidence tends to be restricted
to
observationaldata
collectedat
one
point
in
time.a2Finding a positive
relationship between the two types of trustfor
the case of genetically-modified food,Wouter
Poortinga andNick
Pidgeon therefore more cautiously conclude that"[w]hile
the results suggest that the proposed model is a plausible one, more systematic (experimental) research isneeded
140to
clarify the
direction
of
the
relationships."43Put
differently, while
risk
perceptionresearch often assumes
that relational trust
influences calculativetrust,
it
may equally bethe
casethat
these studies'correlational findings imply that
calculativetrust
influences relationaltrust
instead.In
sum, whereas the existingbody of literature
provides consistent evidencethat
trust
145in
government andother
trusteesis
associatedwith lower
perceived risks across a wide rangeof
hazards, the empirical evidence remainsthin with
regardto how
itstwo
dimen-sionsrelate
to
eachother.
Basedon
this
discussion,we now
turn to our
expectations regarding the relationship betweentrust
in
government and fearof
terrorism.Fear
of
terrorism and trust
in
government
150The question arises
how both forms of trust
in
governmentmatter for
explainin gfear
of
terrorism.
We
definefear
of
terrorism
asan individual's anxiety about future terrorist
attacks.a Most terrorism
researcherswill
agreethat
terror by definition is intended to
evoke
an
emotional
responseamong
the public
in
order
to
attain political
ends.asAlthough terrorist
attacksoccur
infrequently,
their
potentially
high
impact
and
the
155emotional
responsethey tend
to
evoke makes peoplelikely
to
overestimatetheir
risk,whilst
neglecting the actualimprobability of
an attack.a6As
terrorism
is associatedwith
strong emotions andhigh
uncertainty, we may expect most peopleto
respondto
it
on
the basisof
experiential ratherthan
analytical thinking.aT This makes relational trustin
government a potentially strong predictor of people's fearof
160terrorism.
Forit
suggests that peoplewill
not
basetheir
responseon
rational evaluations,but
ratheron
cognitive shortcuts such as valuesimilarity
in
orderto
reduce uncertainty.as Given the consistent evidencethat
relational trustin
government reduces risk perceptions, this may mean that the more people feel that government sharestheir
values, the lessrisk
they
associatewith
terrorism
and
the
less fearful
they
are.
This
leadsto our first
165TERRORTSM AND POLTTTCAL VIOLENCE
@
sHypothesis
1:The
more relational trust citizens have in government,
the
less fearfulthey
areof
terrorism.However, we
know from
the risk perception literature discussed above thatcalculative
170trust
matterstoo for how
citizens respondto risk.
Findings, however, arenot
conclusive:A
survey conductedby
MarcosMisis
and colleaguesae among undergraduate studentsin
the
United
States corroboratesthe
negative relationship
between calculativetrust
in
government and fear
of terrorism.
They show that better performance evaluationsof
thegovernment and domestic
intelligence agencieswith
regard
to
counterterrorism
were
175related
to
lessfear
of
future terrorist
attacksoccurring
in
the U.S. Similarly,
SamuelSinclair and Alice LoCicero
alsofind
a positive relationship
betweenfear
of
terrorism and calculativetrust
in
government.so By contrast, a study conductedby
ShellyMcArdle
and
colleaguessrshowed
no
significant
relationship between confidence
in
the U.S. governmentto
preventfuture terrorist
attacks and concerns aboutvictimization
or
180the occurrence of another major
terrorist
attackin
the United States. Given the indicationsfor
calculative responses mentioned here and the consistent evidencein
the risk
percep-tion
literature
that
calculativetrust
matters, we
still
speci$' the
second hypothesis asfollows:
Hypothesis 2:
The more
calculativetrust citizens have in government,
the
lessfearful
185they
areof
terrorism.As we
pointed out
in
the preceding section,how
thetwo
typesof trust
relateto
eachother
remains less clear
than
most
of
the
risk
perceptionsliterature
suggests. Riskperception studies suggest that the effect
ofrelational
trust on fearofterrorism
ismediated
190by calculative trust.s2
In
other words, relationaltrust
("the government shares my values")is expected
to
increase calculativetrust
("the government is doing a good jobin
preventingterrorist
attacks"), which,in turn, would
decrease people's fear ofterrorism.
However,not
only
do we lackempirical
evidence toverifr
this, we also have good theoretical reasons tosuggest that relational trust may be the mediating variable. That is to say, when citizens
are
195convinced that the government performs well, they
will, in turn,
be morelikely
tothink
it
also has the
right intentions
and sharestheir
values.s3 This isin
linewith
the literature onpolicy
feedback suggesting that policy performance impacts citizens' broader orientations towardspolitics.s
It
follows that how
citizens judgewhat the
governmentis doing
(the basisof
calculativetrust)
islikely to
"feed back"into how
they viewtheir
overallrelation-
200ship with
government(the
basisof relational trust). This is why we
deliberately remain agnosticwith
regardto
potential mediation
effects.We
therefore speciS'two
competingmediation
hypotheses:Hypothesis
3: Calculativetrust
mediatesthe
relationship between relational trust
andfear
of
terrorism.
ZOs6
@
R. VAN DER DOES ET AL.Data and
methods
210Dato
We
use datafrom an online
representative survey conductedin
the
Netherlands among 1,400 membersof
the Longitudinal Internet
Studiesfor
the
Social sciences (LISS) panel (November 6-28,2017).su The LISS panel is based on a true probability sample of the Dutch population and is commonly usedin
social science research.56 The present survey waspart
215of
a larger study on risk perceptions and communication related to terrorism threatfor
the Researchand
Documentation Centre
of
the Dutch Ministry
of
|ustice
and
Security (Referenceomitted
to
ensureblind
review).The
survey hada
non-response rateof
23o/o,resulting
in
afinal
sampleof
1,077 respondents. The sample is largely representativeof
theDutch
populationin
termsof
common background characteristics such as sex, age,educa-
220tion,
and parents' countryof origin
(Online Appendix A).At
thetime
the survey was administered,no terrorist
attacks had recently occurredin
the Netherlands. Even though various authors have suggested
that
fearof terrorism
maynot only
resultfrom
the(indirect)
experienceof
an attack,but
alsofrom
the exposure topolitical rhetoric
andmedia
stories,sTwe find no
clear signsfor
such tendenciesin
the
225 Netherlandsat
the time. The Online Appendix reflects that parliamentary and
mediaattention
for
terrorism
remainedat
stable levelsthroughout
the study period.
Further,survey data
gatheredby
the National Coordinator
for
Securityand
Counterterrorism(NCTV)
and the Eurobarometer suggest that thepublic's
levelof
fear remained constantas well (Online Appendix B). These observations make
it
unlikely that political speechesor
230 media stories caused a sudden increasein
fearof terrorism
amongDutch
citizens. Thisassures us
that
theDutch
contextin
November 2017 can be characterized as"politics
asusual"58 rather
than "an
atmosphereof
fear and alarm."5eOperationalizations
Dependent
Variable
235Table 1 displays the operationalization of the variables included
in
the analyses. The depen-dent variable (Fearofterrorism)
refersto
respondents' worries about terrorism because this tendsto
solicit responses abouttheir
"anxiety about future victimizations" rather than their"sense of an immediate threat."6o The focus on worries thereby fits our research objective
of
explaining anxiety about terrorism more generally.6l
It
is measured by the survey item:"To
240 what extent do you worry about the possibility that you or (someone from) your family will bea
victim of a terrorist
attack?"The initial four-point
scale(1 = Not
worried,2 = A
little
worried, 3
=
Very worried, 4
=
Extremelyworried) is
recodedinto the three-point
scalereported
in
Table
l,
mergingthe
highesttwo
categories giventhat option
4
(extremelyworried)
only contained 16responses.
245lndependent
VariablesFor the
operationalizationof
Relational trust we cannot rely on a common multi-item
scale as usedin
other
work,62but
insteadrely
on a
singleitem
as aproxy
of
relationaltrust:
generaltrust
in
government.
Respondentswere
askedhow much they
trust
theDutch
governmenton
a scalefrom
I
(do not trust at all)
to
tO(trust very much).
This
250TERRORTSM AND pOLTTTCAL VTOLENCE
@
7 Table 1. Operationalization and descriptive statistics.Variable name Mean
(5D)
Min Max Operationalization Dependent variable Fear of tenorism lndependent voriables Relational trust Calculative trust Control variables Knowledge 1.648 (0.609) 1 5.457(2.034)
1 3,067(0.880)
1 0.372 (0.320) 03
Worries about the possibility of oneself or a family member becoming a victim of a terrorist attack. 1 = Not wonied, 2 = A little wonied, 3 = Very worried.10
Trust in government. 1 = No trust at all, 10 = A lot of trust.5
Composite index of three items (described in telt).1
Average self-reported knowledge of counterterrorism measures and terrorism threat communications issued by the government. 0 = Not familiar, 1 = At least a little familiar.4
Average fear of 8 disasters occurring in The Netherlands. 1 = Not fearful, 2 = A little fearful, 3 = Fearful, 4 = Very fearful,1
Sex.0=Male, 1 =Female.91
Age in years.'10.5 Net monthly income in thousands of Euros.
1
Secondary education as highest level of completed education.0=No,1=Yes.
1
Higher vocational or university education as highest level ofcompleted education. 0 = No, 1 = Yes.
1
Both parents born in The Netherlands.0 = No, 1 = Yes.1
Marital status. 0 = Not married, 1 = Manied.9
Number of people in household (max. = 9).9
Number of children in household (max. = 9).1
Unemployed.0=No, 1 =Yes.1
Declared Christian.0 = No, 1 = Yes.1
DeclaredMuslim.0= No, I=Yes.5
Population density of respondent's place of residence (postal addresses/km2).1 =< 500,2 = 500-1,000,3 = 1,000-1,500, 4 = 1,500-2,500, 5 = > 2,500.1
lnternet as one of the most frequent news sources (social media, government websites, or other websites). 0 = No, 1 = Yes. General fear Female Age lncome 5econdary education Higher education Dutch Married Household Children Unemployed Christian Muslim Urbanization lnternet 1.38s(0.429)
1 0.s61 (0.4e7) 0 51.98s (18.394) 16 1.s92 (1.062r' 0 0.s84 (0.493) 0 0.3s2 (0.478) 0 0.808 (0.394) 0.493 (0.s00) 2.418 (1.320) 1 .69s (1.080) 0.329 (0.470) 0.3s9 (0.480) 0.029 (0.167) 3.121 (1.300) 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0.483(0.s00)
0calculative
trust
(discussed below), that is,"trust in institutions
is askedwithout
referenceto
the performanceof institutions or their
occupants."63Furthermore, relational trust is strongly correlated
with
how much people identifywith
the parties in government. People who intended to vote for a political party in government whenthe
255survey was administered tended
to
show higher relationaltrust
in
government(r =
0.40,p
< .001). When one views this expression of political support as an indicator of value similarity
between the respondent and the government, this correlation suggests that the general measure
of trust
in
government indeed seems to specifically tap respondents' relational trustin
govern-ment. This is further supported by the findings that this measure ofvalue similarity correlates
less
260 strongly with trustin
other institutions as well as with our measure of Calculativetrrcf
(Online Appendix C). These findings taken together underpin the discriminant validity of our measure&: related variables correlate less strongly with value similarity than Relational trust.Calculative trust is measured by asking respondents about the government's
ability
and performance regarding counterterrorism.6s Following Misis et a1.,66 theformer
iscaptured
265 by two questions: one on prevention ofterrorist
attacks (i.e.,"To
what extent do youthink
theDutch
government is ableto
prevent aterrorist
attack?") and one on themitigation
of
the potential
consequencesof
attacks(i.e.,
"To
what extent
do
you
think the
Dutchgovernment
is
able
to
constrain
the
consequencesof
a
terrorist
attack?"). Perceived performance is measured by asking respondents about the sufficiency of thegovernment's
270t @
*.vAN DER DoEs ET AL.aterroristattack?"(i=Not,2=Barely,3=Reasonably,4=Sufficiently,5=Well).All
three questions were asked
conjointly
and the order of the questions wasnot
randomized. As the three items correlate strongly6s and show highreliability
(Cronbach's alpha = 0.82),we combine the three measures
into
a composite indexof
Calculative trust. We adjustthe
27 5item
scoresfor
their
relative loadingson the underlying
dimensionof
calculative trust. The loadings are derivedfrom
an exploratoryfactor
analysis(Online Appendix
C).Control
VariablesCitizens' fear of terrorism as well as their assessment of government performance likely depends
on how much (they
think)
they know about counterterrorism measures implemented bythe
280 government.6e To control for this, we rely on the following question: "To what extent are you familiarwith
the following: (a) The factthat
the government distributesinformation
about counterterrorism; (b) The website www.crisis.nl with information about whatyou can do duringa disaster or crisis, such as a terrorist attack; (c) The activities of the National Coordinator for Security and Counterterrorism
(NCTV); (d)
The current threat levelof
the TerroristThreat
285 Assessment Netherlands(DTNX"
(l
= Yes, familiar, 2 = A little familiar, 3 = No, not familiar,4 = Not applicable). The responses are recoded
to
1 when the respondent indicated to be at least a little familiar with the respective topic (all other responses are set to 0). As the four items show an acceptable level of reliability (Cronbach's alpha = 0.73), we average the responses to create themeasnre
Knowledge.
290In
addition, how fearful
citizens arein
general tendsto
correlatepositively with
their fear of terrorism.T0 To the extent thatit
captures fear related to previous instances of crisis, general fear may also be relatedto trust.
If
one arguesthat
earlier crises affectboth
trustand fear
of
terrorism, by controlling
for
general fear,we explicitly
dealwith
this
con-founder. Wecontrol
for
general fearby relying on
eight survey itemsthat
askedrespon-
295 dents how fearful they are of the occurrence of various disasters:"How
fearful are you thatin
the
Netherlandsthe following
will
take
place?"(1 = not
fearful,
2 =
a
little
fearful,3 = fearful, 4
=
ye|y fearful).71 As the items show highreliability
(Cronbach's alpha = 0.86),we average the scores
to
construct anindex of
General fear.Lastly, we
control
for
socio-economic background characteristicscommonly included
300in
studiesof
fearof
terrorism.T2Most of
these characteristics are includedin
models that attemptto
explaintrust
in
government.T3We
therefore accountfor
the possible variancethat might
be relatedto
these background factors rather thanto
the relationship between the variablesof
interest (i.e.,trust
in
government and fear).Statistical
procedures
305We
test hlpothesesI
and2 by
meansof multiple
orderedlogit
models, as the dependent variable Fear is measured on anordinal three-point
scale. For hypotheses3
and 4, we relyon the most common
approachto
mediation
analysis:the
causal steps approach.Ta We estimate the modelsfor
the mediation analysisby
meansof
orderedlogit
modelsfor
theoutcomevariable
Fearof
terrorismandbymeansof ordinaryleastsquares(OtS)forthe
310two
models explaining the potential mediators (Calculativetrust
andRelational trust).In
the analyses presented here, we exclude caseswith
missing values, leadingto
a final sampleTERRORTSM AND pOLtTtCAL VTOLENCE
@
9In
addition to
themain
analyses, we perform a series of robustness checks. First, given recentcriticism of
the causal steps approachto
mediation analysis,Ts weverifr our results
315by
calculating the average causalmediation
effect(ACME)
as suggestedby
Kosuke Imai,Luke
Keele,Dustin
Tingley, and Teppei
YamamotoT6making use
of
the
mediation packagein
the
R
software environment.TTWe
show
that
this
produces substantivelysimilar
results(Online Appendix
D).Next, we estimate
our
modelswith
respondents' fear of an attack happeningin another
320 country as the dependent variable. As one would expect, both types of trustin
governmentdo not
relate significantlyto
respondents'concernsfor
attacks abroad when the various controls are takeninto
consideration. This enhancesour
confidencein
thevalidity of our
trust
measures.We
alsorun
models
with
different
specificationsfor
calculative trust.These
by
and largeconfirm
themain
results(Online Appendix
C).
325What
is
more,
a
key assumption
of
ordered
logit
models
is
that the
regressioncoefficients are equal across
the
levelsof the
dependent variable.We
therefore estimatemodels
for
which we
a)
partially and
b)
completely
relax
this
assumption
(OnlineAppendix
E).This
doesnot
leadto
substantiallydifferent
results.Furthermore, as excluding cases
from the
analysescould potentially
leadto
different
330results,
we
also
run
models
which
minimize
the
number
of
missing
cases (OnlineAppendix C)
aswell
as modelsthat
insteadrely on
the lowest numberof
cases observed acrossthe models reported here
(online
Appendix
F).
Finally, we
run
models with
additional controls (Online Appendix
F)
aswell
as modelswith
weightsincluded for
key demographic variables
to
adjustfor
over- and under-sampling(Online Appendix
A).
335All
in
all, the resultspoint
towardssimilar
substantive conclusions.Results
Table
2
summarizes the resultsfor
the orderedlogit
modelsthat
seekto
explain citizens' fear of terrorism. Thefit
of the models is comparableto
other statistical models relying on survey datato
explain fearof
terrorism.TsTable 2. Ordered loqit regression models explaininq fear of terrorism.
340
Model 1 Model
2
Model 3 Model 4 Model5
Model 6 Relational trust Calculative trust Knowledge General fear 1't Threshold 2nd Threshold Control variables Observations McFadden fi2 -0.0871 **x (-2.84]. _0.1 26*** (_3.26) -0.00421 (-0.0s) -0.0980 (_0.46) 1.955*** (9.2s) 1 .361 *** (3.47) 4.625*** (10.s8) No 886 0.075 -0.07174* (-2.12) -0.1 00** (_2.3s) -0.0645 (-0.67r' 0.172 (0.74',) 1.984**x (8.e4) 'l .7g3xx* (2.60) 5.1 90**x (7.1 8) Yes 810 0.103 -0.0183 (-o.oe) 1,706**x (10.26) ,l.495*** (s.12) 4.685*** (r 3.8s) No 1071 0.068 -0.1 36* (-1.76) -0.143 (-0.67) 1.976*x* (e.371 1,672*** (4.39) 4.905*x* (1 1.43) No 886 0.068 0.273 (1.30) 1.739*xx (e.33) 2..l 1 8*** (3.6s) 5.437**+ (8.8s) Yes 969 0.089 -0.1 73** (-2.03) 0.133 (0.s8) 2.005**x (e.04) 1.858*** (2.72\ 5.249*xx (7.2e) Yes 810 0.1 00 Note. Ordered logit regression coefficients with z statistics in parentheses.10
O
R. vAN DER DoEs Er AL.Turning
to
the
hypotheses,we
first
of all
find
strong support
for
Hypothesis
l:
relational
trust
in
governmentsignificantly
reduces citizens' fearof terrorism.
The effect is also substantialin
size. When we accountfor
all control variables (Model 6), a one-point increaseon the ten-point
scaleof
relationaltrust
leadsto
a
10% decreasein
the oddsof
someone being fearful of terrorism.
We
caninterpret
this changein
odds as a measureof
345the
reducedlikelihood
of
someonereporting
a
higher level
on
the fear
of
terrorism variable (i.e., alittle
fearful vs.not
fearful,or
very fearful vs. alittle
fearful).To
illustrate, the oddsof
someone being a littlefearful
ofterrorism
insteadof
notfearful
are about 45%higher
for
someonewith
absolutely
no
relational
trust
in
government (score
of
1)compared
to
someonewith
averagerelational trust in
government (scoreof
5.5).
350We
find
weaker evidencefor
Hypothesis
2.
While
calculativetrust
in
governmentappears
to
significantly
reducethe odds
of
someonebeing
fearful
of
terrorism,
evenwhen
all controls
are added(Model 5),
it
loses significancewhen
also relationaltrust
isadded
to
themodel (Model6).
Turning
to
Hypotheses3 and 4, the
steps approachto
mediation
analysisprovides
355support
only for
Hypothesis 4. That is, relational trust appearsto
mediate the relationship between calculativetrust
and fear
of
terrorism.
This
is
substantiatedby the following
findings. First, calculative trust significantly reduces fear
ofterrorism
when relational trust isnot
takeninto
consideration(Model
5).Next,
it
has asignificant
positive relationshipwith relational trust (B
=
I.I2,
p
<
.001)(full
modelnot reported
here).Finally,
it
loses
360 significance once relationaltrust is
addedto
the model (Model 6).
In
fact,the
insignif-icanceof
calculativetrust
and the factthat its
absolute value isvery
closeto
zero suggestthat relational
trust
completely mediates the effect of calculativetrust
on fear of terrorism. The lackof
significanceof
calculativetrust
in
thefull
model(Model
6) indicates a lackof
support
for
Hypothesis 3. Substantively, these results suggestthat
themore
citizenstrust
365in
the
government'sability
and performance regardingcounterterrorism, the more
theyfeel the government has the
right intentions
and,in turn,
the less fearful they are that they themselvesor their family will
be avictim of future terrorist
attacks.As shown
in Figure
1,two of
thecontrol
variables are alsosignificantly
relatedto
fearof terrorism.
In
thefull
modelwith all controls included (Model
6), the odds ofwomen
370being fearful
of
terrorism are
82.4o/ohigher than
thoseof
men
(B
=
0.60,p
<
.001).Furthermore,
aone-point
increasein
respondents' averagefear
of
other
hazards (mea-suredon
afour-point
scale) is associatedwith
7.3 timeshigher
oddsof
beingfearful
of
terrorism (B
=
1.98,p
<
.001). Thesefindings
arein
line
with
previous studieson risk
perception.
375Discussion
Our
findings confirm
that trust
in
government
is
negatively
associatedwith
fear
of
terrorism and add
to
the literature
that
someforms
of
trust matter more than
others. Citizens' general trustin
government consistently mitigates fearof
terrorism;their
trustin
the effectiveness
of
governmentcounterterrorism
policy matters less.More
precisely,and
380 perhapscounterintuitively,
matters of the head (calculativetrust)
seemto
affect mattersof
the heart
(relational trust) in how much
citizens fear aterrorist
attack. The good news isthat
building trust
could be an effective wayto
tackle public fearof terrorism-something
TERRORISM AND pOLtTtCAL VTOLENCE
@
11 Rclational trust Cahtiafvc bust Knowlcdgc Genoml fcar Fclnslc Agc lmome Sccondary cduca[fli Highcr edrcalion Outeh Manicd Houscho{d Childrcn Unomp&cycd Christlan Muslim Urbanlzation lntemct -101
Sizs of coaflicignts 2 3.
Signiflcant at 5%lcvcl
o
lnsignilicant at 5% Figure 1. Ordered logistic regression coefficients explaining fear of terrorismSiegrist, and Heinz Gutscher note,
"if
we understoodtrust,
andif
we could affect levelsof
385trust, then
wemight
also be ableto
affect levelsof
risk
perception," and,we may
add,levels of fear.so
In this
study, we havetried to
assess the influenceof trust
on fear by studying citizens'fear
of
terrorism
in
a national
context whereno
recentterrorist
attacks have occurred. Whereasmost
of
the literature
on
fear
of
terrorismsl looks
at
countries
with
recent
390 experiencesof terrorist
attacks (such as Canada, France, Israel,Norway, United
States),the
survey we used was conductedin the
Netherlands, where the last successfulterrorist
attack
took
placein
2004.In
addition
to
introducing
a
new context, studying fear
of
terrorism
in
the
Netherlandsoffers an analytical
advantage.It
helpsus
to
address the Potential problemthat what
we have observed isnot
an effectof trust
in
governmenton
395fear,
but
rather an effectof
fearon trust
in
government.In
termsof
relationaltrust,
sucha reversed relationship is most
likely
when aterrorist
attack hasjust
occurred and people tendto rally
around the government.Terrorist
attacks evoke unusuallyhigh
levelsof
fear amongthe
public,which temporarily
increases citizens' affectiverelationship to
govern-ment
in
order
to
reduce feelings
of
uncertainty.t'
At
the
sametime, the fact
that
400authorities could not preyent a terrorist
attackfrom
taking
placemay result
in
a
dropin
calculativetrust
in
government.By
contrast,in
situations whereno
recentterrorist
attacks have
occurred (such
asin
the
Netherlands),we may
expectit
leastlikely
that citizens' fearof terrorism
drives either typeof trust
in
government.Our
results showthat relational trust is robustly
relatedto
less fearof terrorism
and
405 tendsto
increasewith higher
levelsof
calculativetrust. This
has severalimplications for
security
governance.First, risk communication
by
authorities intended
to
reduce fearshould take
into
accounttrust-building
measures. Thosetrust-building
measures couldarguably
focus asmuch
on
enhancingrelational
trust
ason
boosting
calculative trust. However, attemptsto
directlybuild
relationaltrust in
government may be costly andtheir
410--.--*".+
-t-12
@
R. vAN DER DoEs ET AL.effects
will
dependpartly
on
citizens' political
preferences.s3It
therefore seems more effectiveto
emphasize strong governmentperformancetn-even more
so because recent evidence suggeststhat media
amplifr
state messagingon
counterterrorism
strengths.8sOur
findings indicate,in
linewith
a recent Belgian study on the effects of trust on citizens'perception
of
terrorist
threats,that strong
emphasison
government
performanceand
415public
perception of governmental expert efficacy could indeed contribute toboth
generaltrust
and fear reduction.86A second implication concerns information on how authorities combat terrorism. In a recent study, Aaron Hoffrnan and
William
Shelbyfind
that citizens who receive information on how government effectively combats terrorism have more calculative trustin
authorities thanthose
420 who do not receive such information. Those trusting citizens are less likely to show behavioralavoidance for instance in their willingness to travel. Respondents in their experimental study had
gained trust because
they-unlike
the control group-received information about how respon-sible authorities had disruptedterrorist
plots.87Both military
and criminal justicecounter-terrorism
performancehad
this
effect,while
the
former
produced
strongestresults.
425Meanwhile, communicating on counterterrorism did not increase feelings of insecurity among
target audiencesss-a conclusion that contrasts findings on the effects of terrorism warnings8e or
the
effectsof
visible counterterrorism measures such as "guns, guards and gates"in
publicplaces.e0 While both scholars and experts from practice disagree on the propenrlty of th" public to panic as a consequence of pre-event mitigation and risk communication campaigns,el
these
430 findings support the idea that concerns for social harm may be overstated.e2Hoffman
and Shelby warnthat
the effectson
increased calculative trust assume a basiclevel
of
trust
in
government.Our
results seemto
suggest insteadthat
calculativetrust
precisely mitigates fear because
of
its
impacton relational trust.
Yet, despiteour
carefulcase selection,
our
cross-sectional research designadmittedly
doesnot allow
usto
make
435firm
statements regarding causality. Future studieswould
ideally use panel survey dataor
(quasi-)experimental designs
to
verifr
the causallinks
betweenthe two
typesof trust in
government and fear of terrorism.
In-depth
qualitative case studies could shedlight
on the complex causal mechanismslinking
varioustlpes
oftrust
and fear of terrorism. Practically speaking,the
conclusion remainsthe
same: governments needto
communicate
proac-
440tively what
theydo in
combattingterrorism. To
increase calculative trust,it
makes sensefor public authorities to highlight their
successesin
disturbing
plots, preventing attacks,and
even rePort
on
what they
do
to
counter
radicalization
and
encourage
de-radicalization.
In
line
with
shifts
in
the
intelligence
world
since
the
attacks
on September 11,risk communication on terrorism
hasto
move from
"needto
know,"
to
445"responsibility to
share."e3To
conclude, relationaltrust
is the basisto build
on,but
calculativetrust
can indirectly helpto
reduce citizens' fear ofterrorism.
Even though "the war againstterror
ispotentially
interminable,"ea
the
mitigation
of
effectsof
terrorism can be
enhancedby
academic researchand
policy
practice.
The
implications
of
the
important
negativeimpact
of
450relational trust
in
mediatingthe
effectof
calculativetrust
will
enrich
thetoolbox
of
risk
communication
andcounterterrorism
policy makers.Disclosure statement
TERRoRTsM AND poltrcAl vroLENcE
@
r,
Notes
455I'
George Gray and David Ropeik, "Dealingwith the
Dangersof
Fear: The Roleof
RiskCommunication,"
Health
Afairs
2L,
no.
6
(2002): 108; HanneloreCrijns,
Veroline Cauberghe, and Liselot Hudders, "Terrorism Threatin
Belgium: The Resilienceof
BelgianCitizens and the Protection
of
Governmental Reputationby
Meansof
Communication,"Public Relations Review
43
(2017): 219-34; LeonieHuddy,
Stanley Feldman, Theresa 460Capelos, and Colin Provost, "The Consequences of Terrorism: Disentangling the Effects of
Personal and National Threat," Political Psychology 23, no.3 (2002):485-509.
2.
Gerd Gigerenzer,"Out of
the Frying Paninto
the Fire: Behavioral Reactionsto
TerroristAttacks," Risk Analysis 26, no.2 (2006):348.
3.
Gray and Ropeik (see note1).
4654.
Cass Sunstein, "Terrorism and Probability Neglect," The lournalof
Risk and Uncertainty 26,no.2-3
(2003): 12l-36.5.
Anja Gciritz and David Weiss, "Behavioral and Emotional Responses to Escalating Terrorism Threat," Mind6
Society 13, no.2 (2OtQ:.285-95.6.
Kevin Grosskopi "Evaluating the Societal Responseto
Antiterrorism Measures," Journalof
470Homeland Security and Emergency Management 3, no. 2 (2006):76-56.
7.
e.9., Leonie Huddy, Stanley Feldman, Charles Taber, and Gallya Lahav, "Threat, Anxiety, andSupport
of
Antiterrorism Policies," American Journal of Political Science 49, no.3
(2005): 593-608; Peter Dinesen and Mads jeger, "The Effect of Terror on Institutional Trust: New Evidencefrom the
3/ll
Madrid Terrorist Attack," Political Psychology 34,no.6
(2013): 475917-926; Dag Wollebaek, Bernard Enjolras, Kari Steen-Johnsen, and Gure Odegird, "After Utoya: How a High-Trust Society Reacts
to
Terror-
Trust and Civic Engagementin
theAftermath of fuly 22," PS: Political Science
6
politics 45, no.I
(20t2):32-3i.
The wider literature on the determinants of fear of terrorism has largely neglected the role
of
trustin
government.It
draws on workin
the field of criminology to examine to what degree 480 prior victimization and socio-economic background characteristics, such as sex, age, ethnicity, income, education, and occupation, influence fear. Other factors that are commonly studiedinclude geographic location, distance
to
prior
sitesof
terrorist attacks, and exposure to(certain) news media.
See:Tilman
Briick and
CathdrineMi.iller, "Compiring
theDeterminants
of
Concern about Terrorism and Crime," Global Crime 11,no. 1
(2010): 485l-15;
David May, Ioe Herbert, Kelly Cline, and Ashley Nellis, "Predictorsof
Fear and Riskof Terrorism
in
a Rural State," International Journal of Rural Criminologyl,
no.I
(2011):1-22; Ashley
Mar,
"Gender Differencesin
Fearof
Terrorism," Journalof
ContemporaryCriminal lustice 25, no.
3
(2009): 322-40; Mally Shechory-Bitton and Keren Cohen-Louck,"An
Israeli Modelfor
Predicting Fearof
Terrorism Basedon
Community and IndividuaI
490Q6
Factors," Iournal of Interpersonal Violence (2017); Mally Shechory-Bitton and KerenCohen-Louck, "Does Fear of Terrorism Differ from Fear of Crime and Sexual Assault:
A
Questionof
Geographical Location and Residential Area," International Journal of Ofender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 62, no.3 (2018): 806-26; Mally Shechory Bitton and Yousef Silawi,"Do |ews and Arabs Differ
in
Their Fear of Terrorism and Crime?" Journal of Interpersonal 495Q7
Violence (2016); Konstantinos Drakosand
Cathdrine Mi.iller,"On the
Determinantsof
Terrorism
Risk
Concernin
Europe," Defenceand
Peace Economics25, no.
3
(2014):291-3lO; Shelly McArdle, Heather Rosofl and Richard
john,
"The Dynamicsof
EvolvingBeliefs, Concerns, Emotions, and Behavioral Avoidance
following
glll:
A
LongitudinalAnalysis of Representative Archival Samples," Risk Analysis: An International Journal32,
no.
5004
(2012): 744-61 Ashley Marie Nellis and |oanne Savage, "Does Watching the News AffectFear
of
Terrorism?The
Importanceof
Media Exposureon
Terrorism Fear," Crime(t
Delinquency 58, no. 5 (2012):748-68: Alan Rubin, Paul Haridakis, Gwen Hullman, Shaojing Sun, Pamela Chikombero, and Vikanda Pornsakulvanich, "Television Exposure not Predictive
of Terrorism Fear," Newspaper Research Journal 24, no.
I
(2003): 128.,45; Mally Shechory- 505Q8
14
@
R. vAN DER DoEs Er AL.Journal of Ofender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 62,
no.3
(2018): 806-26; PamelaWilcox, Murat Ozer, Murat Gunbeyi, and Tarkan Gundogdu, "Gender and Fear of Terrorism in Turkey," lournal of Contemporary Criminal Justice 25, no. 3 (2009): 341-57
.
5l0
8.
e.g. Huddy, Feldman, Taber, and Lahav; Dinesen and feger; Wollebaek, Enjolras,Steen-|ohnsen, and Odeg6rd (see note 7); Andrew Perrin and Sondra J. Smolek, "Who Trusts? Race, Gender, and
the
Septemberll
Rally Effect among YoungAdults,"
Social Science Research 38,no.
I
(2009): 134-45; Paschalis Arvantidis, Athina Economou, and ChristosKollias, "Terrorism's Effects
on
Social Capitalin
European Countries," Public Choice 169 515 (2016): 231-st.9.
Examples of what we refer to as "calculative" trust include: Kimberly Gross, Paul R. Brewer,ll
and Sean Aday, "Confidence
in
Government and Emotional Responsesto
Terrorism after Septemberll,
2001," American Politics Research 37, no.I
(2009): 107-128; Darren W. Davis and Brian D. Silver, "Civil Liberties vs. Security: Public Opinion in the Context of theTerrorist
520 Attacks on America," American Journal of Political Science 48,no.
l:
28-46; Dinesen and faeger (see note 7); Arvantidis, Economou, and Kollias (see note 8); Wollebaek, Enjolras, Steen-|ohnsen, and Odegird (see note 7). Examples of what we refer to as "relational" trust include: Samuel|.
Sinclair and Alice LoCicero,"Do
Fearsof
Terrorism Predict Trust inGovernment?" Journal of Aggression, ConJlict and Peace Research 2,
no.
I
(2010):57-68;
525McArdle, Rosoff, and fohn (see note 7).
See the review in Timothy Earle, Michael Siegrist, and Heinz Gutscher, "Trust, Risk Perception and the TCC Model of Cooperation," Trust
in
Risk Management: (Jncertainty and Scepticismin
the PublicMind
(2010):l-50.
e.g., Crijns, Cauberghe, and Hudders (see note
l);
Nellis and Savage (see note 7); Jennifer Lee, 530Louise Lemyre, and Daniel Krewski, "A Multi-Method, Multi-Hazard Approach to Explore the Uniqueness of Terrorism Risk Perceptions and Worry," Journal of Applied Social Psychology 40, no.
I
(2010):241-72; Michael Siegrist, Carmen Keller, Hans Kastenholz, Silvia Frey, andArnim Wiek,
"Laypeople'sand
Experts' Perceptionof
Nanotechnology Hazards," RiskAnalysis:
An
International Journal27, no.
|
(2007):59-69;
MarcosL.
Misis,Michael
535D. Bush, and Nicole Hendrix, "An Examination
of
College Students' Fears about Terrorism and the Likelihoodof
a
TerroristAttack"
Behavioral Sciencesof
Terrorism and Political Aggression 9, no. 2 (2017): 125-38.e.g. Huddy, Feldman, Capelos, and Provost (see note 1); Lee, Lemyre, and Krewski (see note
ll);
Shechory-Bitton&
Cohen-Louck,2018 (seenote 7); Wilcox,
Ozer, Gunbeyi,and
540Gundogdu (see note 7).
Dinesen and feger (see note 7); Perrin and Smolek (see note 8).
Arvantidis, Economou, and Kollias (see note 8); Dinesen and |aeger (see note 7); Perrin and Smolek (see note 8); Wollebaek, Enjolras, Steen-fohnsen, and Odegird (see note 7); Virginia
A.
Chanley,"Trust
in
Governmentin
the
Aftermath
of 9.ll:
Determinantsand
545 Consequences," P olitical P sycholo gy 23, no. 3: 469 -83.Ibid. Note that Arvantidis and colleagues attribute the incongruences they observe with this
pattern
in
France and Norwayto
the limited availabilityof
empirical data (rather than tointerpret these incongruences as evidence of a different type of pattern). In line with patterns observed in other countries, Wollebaek and colleagues do find at least that trust in government 550
increased shortly after the attacks in Norway, Sinclair and LoCicero (see note 9),65. rbid., s8.
Gross, Brewer, and Aday (see note 9),
l2l.
Denise M. Rousseau, Sim B. Sitkin, Ronald S. Burt, and Colin Camerer, "Not So Different
After
555All: A Cross-discipline View of Trust," Academy of Management Review 23, no. 3 (1998): 395.
e.g.
Timothy
Earle,"Trust
in
Risk Management:A
Model-based Reviewof
EmpiricalResearch," Risk Analysis: An International Journal 30, no. a (2010): 541-74; David Houston
and
Lauren Howard Harding, "PublicTrust
in
Government Administrators: ExplainingTERRoRTsM AND poLtlcAL vroLENcE
O ,t
21.
Earle,2010 (see note 20).22.
Timothy Earle, "Trust, Confidence, and the 2008 Global Financial Crisis," Risk Analysis: AnInternational lournal 29, no. 6 (2009):786.
23'
Earle, 2010 (see note 20),542 see also Roger Mayer, fames H. Davis, and F. David Schoorman, 565"An
Integrative Model of Organizational Trust," Academy of Management Reyiew 20, no. 3 (1995): 709-34. p.713.24.
Earle, Siegrist, and Gutscher (see note l0), 4; see also Wouter Poortinga and Nick F. Pidgeon,"Exploring the Dimensionality
of
Trustin
Risk Regulation," Risk Analysis: An InternationalJournal 23,
no.5
(2003):961-72.
57025.
Earle,2009 (see note 22),786; Earle, Siegrist, and Gutscher (see notel0),5.
26.
Earle, 2010 (see note 20), 542; Paul Slovic, Melissa Finucane, Ellen Peters, and Donald G. MacGregor, "Risk as Analysis and Risk as Feelings: Some Thoughts about Affect, Reason, Risk, and Rationality," Risk Analysis 24,no.2 (200Q:3ll-22.
27.
cf. Seymour Epstein, "Integrationof
the Cognitive and the Psychodynamic Unconscious," 575 American Psychologist 49, no. 8 (199a): 710-11.28.
See notell.
29.
Earle, Siegrist, and Gutscher (see note 10).30.
Gisela Wachinger, Ortwin Renn, Chloe Begg, and Christian Kuhlicke, "The Risk PerceptionParadox
-
Implicationsfor
Governance and Communicationof
Natural Hazards," Rtsk 580Analysis 33, no. 6 (2013): 1053.
31.
Michael Siegrist, George Cvetkovich, and Claudia Roth, "salient Value Similarity, Social Trust,and Risk/benefit Perception," Risk Analysis 20, no. 3 (2000): 353-62.
32.
Nicol6s Bronfman, Esperanza L6pezY6zquez, and Gabriel Dorantes, "An Empirical Study for the Direct and Indirect Links between Trustin
Regulatory Institutions and Acceptabilityof
585Hazards," Safety Science 47,
no.5
(2009): 686-92. For some studiesit
is unclear what the measure of trustin
government and/or authorities seeks to tap (Siegrist, Keller, Kastenholz, Frey, and Wiek (see notell);
Stephen C. Whitfield, Eugene A. Rosa, Amy Dan, and Thomas Dietz, "The Future of Nuclear Power: Value Orientations and Risk Perception," Rlsk Analysis:An International Journal 29, no. 3 (2009): 425-37) but the reported results by and large
point
590 into the same direction. For partial exceptions see Lennart Sj<iberg, "Limits of Knowledge and the Limited Importance of Trust," Risk Analysis 21, no.I
(2001): 189-98; Ziqiang Han, Xiaoli Lu, ElisaI.
Htirhager, andjubo
Yan, "The Effects of Trustin
Government on Earthquake Survivors' Risk Perception and Preparednessin
China," Natural Hazards 86 (2017): 437-52.33.
Teun Terpstra, "Emotions, Trust, and Perceived Risk Affective and Cognitive Routes toFlood
595 Preparedness Behavior," Risk Analysis: An International Journal 31, no.l0
(2011): 1658-75.34.
Siegrist, Cvetkovich, and Roth (see note3l).
35.
David Pijawka andAlvin
H. Mushkatel, "Public Oppositionto
the Siting of the HighJevel Nuclear Waste Repository: The Importanceof
Trust," Review of Policy Research 10, no. 4 (1991): 180-94; Yeonjae Ryu, Sunhee Kim, and Seoyong Kim, "Does Trust Matter? Analyzing 600the
Impactof
Trust on the
Perceived Risk and Acceptanceof
Nuclear Power Energy,"Sustainability 10, no. 3 (2018): 758; Lennart Sjdberg and Misse Wester Herber, "Too Much Trust
in
(Social) Trust? The Importance of Epistemic Concerns and Perceived Antagonism,"International Journal of Global Enyironmental Issues 8, no.
l-2
(2005):30-44.36.
Wouter Poortinga and Nick F. Pidgeon, "Trustin
Risk Regulation: Cause or Consequenceof
605the Acceptability
of GM
Food?" Risk Analysis:An
Internationallournal
25, no.I
(2005): 199-209.37
.
Peter Groothuis and Gail Miller, "The Role of Social Distrust in Risk-benefit Analysis: A Study of the Siting of a Hazardous Waste Disposal Facility," lournal of Rkk and Uncertainty 15, no. 3(1997):24t-s7.
61038.
Sjdberg and Herber (see note 35).39.
Diana van Dongen, Liesbeth Claassen, Tjabe Smid, and Danielle Timmermans, "People's Responsesto
Risks of Electromagnetic Fields and Trustin
Government Policy: The Role ofPerceived Risk, Benefits and Control," Journal of Rkk Research 16, no. 8 (2013): g4S-57.