• No results found

University of Groningen Gene-environment interactions on the course of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms Brinksma, Djûke Maaike

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "University of Groningen Gene-environment interactions on the course of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms Brinksma, Djûke Maaike"

Copied!
21
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Gene-environment interactions on the course of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

(ADHD) symptoms

Brinksma, Djûke Maaike

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date: 2018

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Brinksma, D. M. (2018). Gene-environment interactions on the course of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms: From early into late adolescence. Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

Chapter 1

General introduction

(3)
(4)

1

This PhD thesis focuses on the associations of several candidate genes, environmen-tal factors, and their interaction (i.e., gene-environment [G×E] interactions) with the course of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms from early into late adolescence. While ADHD symptoms often persist throughout adolescence and even adulthood, relatively little is known about determinants of the course of ADHD symptoms across adolescence. This PhD thesis aims to advance our understanding why ADHD symptoms persist into late adolescence in some individuals but not in others. Furthermore, in this PhD thesis it will be analyzed whether the contribution of genes, environments, and G×E’s on ADHD symptoms may differ across various stages of ado-lescence.

This chapter begins with an introduction of the clinical features and course of ADHD. In the following sections I will discuss what is currently known about the role of genetic and environmental influences as well as gene-environment interactions on the course of ADHD symptoms. Then I will emphasize why it is important to examine the influence of genes, environments, and their interaction on ADHD symptoms across adolescent development. Finally, the chapter ends with an overview of the aims and an outline of this PhD thesis.

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that affects individuals across the life span and is characterized by symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000). An ADHD diagnosis requires that six or more symptoms of inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity should be present in multiple settings of which some symptoms were present before the age of 7 and are associated and with clinically significant impairment (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000). Based on the symptoms, three types of ADHD are distinguished: predomi-nantly inattentive, predomipredomi-nantly hyperactive-impulsive, or a combined type of both inattention and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (DSM-IV-TR; APA, 2000). Importantly, in 2013 the DSM-IV-TR was replaced by the DSM-V (APA, 2013); although the diagnostic criteria remained similar between the two DSM versions, some minor changes have been made. For example, the onset criterion has been changed from 7 to 12 years of age and a reduction from six to five in the minimum number of symptoms in either symptom domain required for older adolescents and adults (DSM-V, 2013). For the present thesis I stuck to the DSM-IV-TR to maximize comparability to previous studies.

The worldwide prevalence of ADHD in childhood is 5.3% (Polanczyk, De Lima, Horta, Biederman, & Rohde, 2007). The estimated prevalence of ADHD in adulthood lies in the range of 2.5%-4.4% (Kessler et al., 2006; Simon, Czobor, Balint, Meszaros, & Bitter, 2009). Moreover, recent longitudinal studies have shown that 15% of patients diagnosed with ADHD in childhood will have full syndromic persistence into adolescence and

(5)

adult-hood (Faraone, Biederman, & Mick, 2006; Faraone et al., 2015). Notably, the prevalence of ADHD is higher in boys than in girls in both clinical (9:1; Biederman et al., 2002; Gaub & Carlson, 1997) and community samples of children and adolescents (3:1; Barkley, 2006; Nøvik et al., 2006).

Clinical Features of ADHD

Individuals with ADHD vary widely in their clinical profile and course of the disorder. The distinction of ADHD types is still under debate since it has been suggested that ADHD types may not be stable over time (Coghill & Seth, 2011). Another point of dis-cussion in the ADHD literature is whether ADHD should be better viewed as a distinct entity or as the extreme negative end of a continuous distribution of ADHD symptoms in the population (Asherson & Trzaskowski, 2015; Larsson, Anckarsater, Rastam, Chang, & Lichtenstein, 2012). The last point of view is supported by twin studies showing a substantial overlap between the genetic factors for a clinical diagnosis of ADHD and continuous measures of ADHD symptoms in the general population (Larsson et al., 2012; Levy, Hay, McStephen, Wood, & Waldman, 1997). Therefore, the focus in this PhD thesis lies on the dimensional nature of ADHD, rather than on the distinction between ADHD and individuals without the disorder.

ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder that is associated with tremendous financial bur-den for both the individual (Swensen et al., 2003) and the society (Le et al., 2014), stress on families (Johnston & Mash, 2011), social impairments (Das, Cherbuin, Butterworth, Anestey, & Easteal, 2012; Wehmeier, Schacht, & Barkley, 2010), and adverse academic and vocational outcomes (Loe & Feldman, 2007; Polderman, Boomsma, Bartels, Verhulst, & Huizink, 2010) on the long term. Another worrisome issue is that individuals with ADHD may develop high comorbidity with substance use disorders (Van Emmerik-Van Ootmerssen et al., 2012), antisocial behaviors, and depression (Secnik, Swensen, & Lage, 2005) and anxiety (Michielsen, et al., 2013) into adulthood. Recent studies suggest that functional outcomes differ between participants with persisting versus decreasing ADHD symptoms (Barkley, Murphy, & Fisher, 2008; Swanson, Owens, & Hinshaw, 2014). Therefore, it is important to know what determines the course of ADHD symptoms across adolescence since ADHD symptoms can have severe long-term consequences on many domains of an individual’s life.

The Course of ADHD Symptoms across Adolescence

In general, ADHD symptoms tend to decrease during adolescence (Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, 2000), but the course of symptoms differs between individuals. Studies showed that of all children with ADHD, 45% to 85% are still symptomatic in adolescence and 50% to 60% continue to show symptoms in adulthood (Faraone et al., 2006). Thus, the rate of persistence of ADHD is especially high when persistence is defined as functional

(6)

1

impairment or the persistence of subthreshold (three or fewer) impairing symptoms (Faraone et al., 2015). Various ADHD symptom trajectories have been described across adolescence, in both clinical and population samples, and most studies reported a low stable and a high persistent ADHD symptom trajectory (e.g., Döpfner et al., 2015; Muss-er, Karalunas, Dieckman, Peris, & Nigg, 2016; SassMuss-er, Kalvin, & Bierman, 2016; Tandon, Tillmann, Argrawal, & Luby, 2016). However, some studies also described an increasing ADHD symptom trajectory (Döpfner et al., 2015; Riglin et al., 2016; Van Lier, Van der Ende, Koot, & Verhulst, 2007). The rate of adolescents following a high persistent ADHD symptom trajectory is 2.8%-5% in population-based samples (Döpfner et al., 2015; Van Lier et al., 2005) and 17.5%-22% in clinical samples (Musser et al., 2016; Tandon et al., 2016). Overall, there is large heterogeneity in the course of ADHD symptoms, highlight-ing the need to find the causes of this heterogeneity.

Determinants of the Course of ADHD Symptoms

Relatively little is known about determinants of the course of ADHD symptoms in adolescents in the transition towards adulthood. As mentioned above, it is important to understand why symptoms persist in some individuals but not in others, since ADHD symptoms may lead to impairments in functioning in several domains. Moreover, it is possible that determinants may operate through time-sensitive windows during devel-opment, playing a role at one time but not another. Thereby it is important to recognize that the same risk factors that influence the onset of ADHD may be involved in the course of the disorder, but it may also be that a different set of risk and protective factors influ-ence the course of ADHD, with varying influinflu-ences over time (Thapar, Langley, Asherson, & Gill, 2007b). For example, in a twin study with individuals of whom symptoms were obtained from the ages 8 to 20 it was found that genetic effects operating in child-hood explained less of the total variance in early adultchild-hood than during childchild-hood, thus pointing to decreasing genetic influences over time (Chang, Lichtenstein, Asherson, & Larsson, 2013). Yet, new sets of genetic risk factors emerged at different stages across adolescence in this study.

In this PhD thesis, I made a selection of genes and environments which have been implicated in the etiology of ADHD, but which might also be relevant determinants for the course of ADHD symptoms across adolescence. In the following, I will also discuss a number of G×E interactions that may be related to the course of ADHD symptoms. Genetic Factors

It has been suggested that 70 to 90% of the ADHD symptoms can be accounted for by genetic factors (Faraone & Mick, 2010; Nikolas & Burt, 2010). ADHD symptoms are likely to be caused by multiple genes of small effects (Asherson, Kuntsi, & Taylor, 2005), as well as rare genetic variants of large effects (Elia et al., 2010). Nearly 180 candidate

(7)

genes for the etiology of ADHD have been implicated (Li, Chang, Zhang, Gao, & Wang, 2014), which mainly involve genes in the functioning and synthesis of neurotransmitters in the dopaminergic and serotonergic systems, and the degradation of these enzymes (Brookes et al., 2006; Gizer, Ficks, & Waldman, 2009). The dopamine system has been implicated in ADHD because of its central role of planning and initiation of motor re-sponses, activation, switching, reaction to novelty, and processing of rewards (Faraone et al., 2015). Genes involved in the serotonin system are plausible candidates for ADHD studies since experimentally manipulated serotonin levels lead to ADHD-like behaviors such as impulsive choices, increased motor activity, and delay aversion in both humans and animal models (Brewer & Potenza, 2008). Therefore, based on meta-analyses inves-tigating the etiology of ADHD (Brookes et al., 2006; Gizer, Ficks, & Waldman, 2009), the five selected relevant candidate genes for ADHD that were available in our dataset were the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4), dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2), serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR), catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT), and monoamine oxidase A (MAOA). The 7-repeat of the DRD4 (Gizer et al., 2009; Li, Sham, Owen, & He, 2006) and the long version of the 5-HTTLPR genotype (Brookes et al., 2006; Gizer et al., 2009) have been repeatedly associated with ADHD; less robust associations have been suggested for the A1 allele of the DRD2 (Kopečková et al., 2008), the Val-allele of the COMT, (Gizer et al., 2009; Sun, Yuan, Shen, Xiong, & Wu, 2014) and high-activity of the MAOA genotype with ADHD (Gizer et al., 2009). Still, definite conclusions on which genes influence ADHD will have to await further research, given the large heterogeneity across genetic studies and the complex genetic architecture of neuropsychiatric disorders, which requires large samples (Li et al., 2014).

In contrast to the wealth of genetic studies on the etiology of ADHD, there is far less literature on the genetic influences on the course of ADHD symptoms; only a handful of candidate gene studies is currently available. For example, the DRD4 7-repeat was found to be associated with a more persistent course of ADHD symptom severity over time by two studies (Biederman et al., 2009; Langley et al., 2009), whereas the presence of the long version of the 5-HTTLPR was not associated with the course of ADHD (Langley et al., 2009). Furthermore, the low activity MAOA genotype was associated with stable attention problems during adolescence (Zohsel et al, 2015), rather than with the high activity MAOA that has been related to the onset of ADHD (Gizer et al., 2009). Moreover, as mentioned above, it is important to be aware that genetic influences on the course of ADHD symptoms may change over time (Chang et al., 2013). For example, the dopamine

active transporter 1 gene (DAT1), although not available in our dataset, was found to be

differentially associated with ADHD in children and in adults, suggesting that the DAT1 gene interacts with developmental factors (Franke et al., 2008; 2009). By focusing on genetic underpinnings of changes in ADHD symptoms across adolescence, there might

(8)

1

be new insights into neurobiological mechanisms that underlie the course of ADHD symptoms.

Environmental Factors

Just as genetic factors are involved in the etiology and likely also in the course of ADHD, non-genetic factors may play a role in its onset and course. Indeed, it has been suggested that change over time of ADHD symptoms is markedly influenced by environmental fac-tors (Kan et al., 2013). More specifically, in a twin study with 8-9 year old children who were examined four years later it was found that change in symptoms between child-hood and early adolescence was partly due to new nonshared environmental effects that became important during adolescence (Larsson, Larsson, & Lichtenstein, 2004). Yet our current knowledge of developmental processes is much more limited compared to the wider literature that has focused on a variety of environmental factors in relation to ADHD (e.g., Banerjee, Middleton, & Faraone, 2007; Kim & Yoo, 2013; Sagiv, Epstein, Bellinger, & Korrick, 2013; Wirth et al., 2017). Well replicated environmental risk factors of ADHD involve, for instance, pre- and perinatal adversities (Banerjee et al., 2007; Gustafsson & Källén, 2011; Silva, Colvin, Hagemann, & Bower, 2014); these are theoreti-cally attractive as a causal factor for neuropsychiatric disorders such as ADHD because of their potential influence on the developing brain (Marsh, Gerber, & Peterson, 2008). In particular, prenatal exposure to maternal smoking, alcohol or cannabis use have been linked to behavioral characteristics of ADHD (Huizink & Mulder, 2006). However, it should be noted that this effect of the environment might be caused by underlying causal genetic factors that have elicited the exposure to these environmental factors. Although these events occur before birth, the effects of pre- and perinatal adversities should be considered in the light of postnatal environment conditions, since neurode-velopment continues into young adulthood (Toga, Thompson, & Sowell, 2006). While these pre- and perinatal adversities seem risk factors for ADHD during the early stages of life with possible prolonged effects in later life, still little is known about their role on the course of ADHD symptoms (Galéra et al., 2011; Halmøy et al., 2012).

Adolescence is a unique period that might influence the course of ADHD symptoms. Adolescence represents a particularly sensitive period of life, characterized by increasing peer influences and social relations, self-awareness, sensitivity to social environmental cues (particularly social acceptance), inter-personal conflicts, and mental health prob-lems, resulting from personal experiences, maturation, and neurobiological changes (Arnett, 1999; Schriber & Guyer, 2016). It is important to acknowledge that, when exam-ening the course of ADHD symptoms, the impact of environmental factors may change during development. For example, parents’ contribution to ADHD symptoms might be different in childhood than in adolescence as youths strive to gain independence from their families and become more engaged with their peers (Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker,

(9)

2006). To conclude, the influence of the pre- and perinatal environment (‘Chapter 2’), parenting environment (see ‘Chapter 3’ and ‘Chapter 5’), stressfull environment (‘Chapter 4’), and school environment (‘Chapter 5’) across adolescence will be examined in rela-tion to the course of ADHD symptoms in my studies.

Gene-Environment Interactions

Individuals differ in the extent to which they are influenced by the environment. More specifically, the impact of the environment may vary depending on the individual’s genetic make-up. G×E interactions can be defined as genetically modulated sensitiv-ity to environmental factors (e.g., Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van IJzendoorn, 2011; Caspi, Hariri, Holmes, Uher, & Moffitt, 2010; Pluess & Belsky, 2010). G×E interactions are crucial to understand the mechanisms underlying ADHD symptoms (Buitelaar, 2005; Nikolas, Klump, & Burt, 2012). More specifically, through examining G×E interactions it is possible to get a more accurate estimate of the importance of both the genetic and environmental risk factors involved, and identify those individuals most susceptible to environmental influences (Moffitt, Caspi, & Rutter, 2005).

The interplay between genes and environmental factors is thought to play a pivotal role in both the etiology (Thapar, Cooper, Jefferies, & Stergiakouli, 2012) and also the course of ADHD symptoms (Nigg, Nikolas, & Burt, 2010; Thapar, Harold, Rice, Langley, & O’Donovan, 2007a), although still few studies have examined G×E in relation to the course. One of the first G×E studies in ADHD looked into possible interactions of DRD4 with prenatal exposure to alcohol or smoking (Neuman et al., 2007). Neuman and col-leagues found evidence that individuals with the 7-repeat DRD4 genotype and whose mother smoked during pregnancy had higher odds of having ADHD. However, this association was not replicated in other studies (Altink et al., 2008; Langley et al., 2008). A well-replicated G×E interaction concerns the interaction between 5-HTTLPR and psy-chosocial stress. Individuals carrying the S-allele have been found to be more sensitive to the negative effects of long-term stress exposure for a range of psychiatric disorders such as depression (e.g., Caspi et al., 2003), substance use (e.g., Covault et al., 2007), and also ADHD (Müller et al., 2008; Retz et al., 2008; Van der Meer et al., 2014).

The differential susceptibility theory posits that individuals are more susceptible to both positive and negative influences of the environment depending on their genetic background (Belsky & Pluess, 2009, 2013; Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, 2011). Several studies have indicated that the DRD4 genotype may render children susceptible to environmental influences ‘for better and for worse’ (Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van IJzendoorn, 2006). That is, children carrying the 7-repeat allele of the DRD4 genotype were susceptible to the effects of both maternal sensitivity and maternal insensitivity: these children showed the highest levels of externalizing behavior when their mothers were insensitive, and the lowest levels of externalizing

(10)

1

behavior when their mothers were sensitive. Also serotonergic genes have been found to contribute to individual differences in response to environmental influences. For example, for S-allele homozygotes of the 5-HTTLPR genotype, family conflict predicted increased inattention scores, while family cohesion predicted decreased inattention scores (Elmore, Nigg, Friderici, Jernigan, & Nikolas, 2016).

In sum, the majority of the existing studies that examined G×E interactions in relation to ADHD are cross-sectional in nature and focused on the onset of ADHD symptoms rather than the course over time (e.g., Elmore et al., 2016; Van der Meer et al., 2014; Nikitopoulos et al., 2014). Longitudinal designs examining G×E interactions are impor-tant for two reasons. First, the relative contribution of genes and environment on ADHD symptoms may differ across age (Chang et al., 2013; Larsson et al., 2004; Pingault et al., 2015), suggesting that G×E interactions are not always stable over time (Belsky & Pluess, 2013; Berry, Deater-Deckard, McCartney, Wang, & Petrill, 2013), pointing to the importance to consider change over time. Second, the expression of ADHD symptoms of adolescents is present within a complex and changing social context (we will discuss this in more detail below). In this PhD thesis I examined whether G×E interactions that have been implicated in the etiology of ADHD are also relevant to the course of ADHD symptoms across adolescence focusing on G×Pre- and perinatal adversities, G×Parenting, G×Stress, and G×Family and School climate.

Exploring ADHD Symptoms across Adolescence

Adolescents as a Unique Developmental Period in Examening ADHD Symptoms

Although the severity of ADHD symptoms peaks during early elementary school (Schmidt & Petermann, 2009), adolescence is an important developmental period to study G×E interactions in relation to ADHD symptoms. Adolescence differs fundamentally from childhood. With increasing age, both the individual and its environment change and it is, therefore, key to acknowledge that predictors (genes, environment, and gene-environ-ment interactions) of ADHD symptoms may differ across the life course. Adolescence is a core developmental period setting the stage for the course of ADHD symptoms in later life for several reasons. First, in adolescence the ability to focus and regulate behavior are put increasingly into practice in order to meet progressing demands of the environ-ment. For example, in secondary education individuals have to make their homework, which might overwhelm and frustrate students with ADHD who often struggle with executive functions, focus, and organization. Indeed, individuals with ADHD frequently lose homework assignments or fail to turn them in on-time, misplace school materials such as books, pencils, and classwork, and procrastinate and fail to adequately prepare for tests (Evans et al., 2009; Langberg et al., 2011). But more importantly, in adolescence social skills are strongly put to the test since peers become more important than parents and become the main domain of social interaction of adolescents (Véronneau, Trempe,

(11)

& Paiva, 2014). However, symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity of individuals with ADHD may result in more irritating, socially inappropriate, and aversive behaviors when they are interacting with peers (Whalen & Henker, 1985). As a result, adolescents with high levels of ADHD symptoms may be rejected and are then more likely to be excluded from social activities with peers (Buhs & Ladd, 2001). This may lead to social isolation and deprivation of important socialization experiences, of opportunities to develop and refine social skills, and of important sources of social support (Parker, Rubin, Erath, Wojslawowicz, & Buskirk, 2006). Over time this may ultimately lead to continuation or exacerbation of ADHD symptoms.

Second, recent research has drawn attention to the development of the adolescent brain (Giedd, 2008; Giedd & Rapoport, 2008). While there is evidence that ADHD is as-sociated with a different brain structure (Hoogman et al., 2017), adolescence is a period of time where brain maturation is dynamic. This might possibly explain the significant change in ADHD symptoms that take place in a proportion of patients during this time window. On the other hand, this developmental plasticity may also make the adolescent brain amenable to interventions that help mitigate environmental adversities of emo-tional and/or physical nature (Andersen, 2003).

Third, current treatment strategies for ADHD are not optimally attuned to the needs and vulnerabilities of adolescents (Buitelaar, 2017). Many adolescents with ADHD have poor medication adherence or even stop medication treatment, and use of behavioral interventions is also suboptimal. This is related to characteristics related to adolescence: poor decision making, poor insight into one’s own functioning, and peer pressure (for a more elaborated overview, see Chan, Fogler, & Hammerness, 2009). Examining how genes, environments and their interactions are related to the course of ADHD symptoms in adolescence might help targeting treatment strategies to those who need it most. Longitudinal GxE Research Designs in Examining the Course of ADHD Symptoms As I have outlined above, it is important to include a developmental aspect in G×E research. Nonetheless, most studies tested the interplay between genes and the envi-ronment cross-sectionally or on a single or one combined outcome measure over time. When examining the longitudinal course of ADHD symptoms across adolescence it is desirable to include multiple measures across time. Many research designs can be cho-sen when examining G×E interactions longitudinally and it is important to think about their implications in defining the course of ADHD symptoms. For example, pre- and perinatal adversities (see ‘Chapter 2’), which are low-prevalent, might have long-term effects on ADHD symptoms that may vary over the course of adolescence. This requires a large sample size and justifies estimating the course of ADHD symptoms as a single curve over time. On the other hand, the family and school climate (see ‘Chapter 5’) might be different at the age of 11 than at 16 years and then it is important to include each

(12)

1

outcome measure separately, providing the possibility to estimate bidirectional relation-ships between different ages. It is also possible to define the course of ADHD symptoms by creating homogenous groups (i.e., individuals within these groups are more alike compared to individuals from another group) over time based on the outcome measure; these so-called ‘trajectories’ allow for studying symptom persistence, remission, and stability (see ‘Chapter 3’). In sum, it is of great importance to include a developmental perspective when studying the role of G×E interactions on ADHD symptoms over time, while different research designs will address different aspects across the studies in this PhD thesis.

Study Design

We used data from the first three measurement waves (mean ages from about 10 to 18 years) of the Dutch ‘TRacking Adolescents’ Individual Lives Survey’ (TRAILS; e.g., De Winter et al., 2005; Huisman et al., 2008; Oldehinkel et al., 2015). TRAILS aims to contribute to the understanding of the course of ADHD symptoms across adolescence following 10-12 year-old Dutch children biennially into adulthood. Data from both the general-population cohort and the parallel clinic referred cohort were pooled to obtain results reflecting the entire spectrum of ADHD symptom levels. In brief, for the population-based cohort TRAILS approached 135 primary schools in five municipalities in the Northern Netherlands. Of these schools, 90.4% agreed to participate. TRAILS con-tacted eligible students (n = 3,145) and their parents, enrolling 76% (n = 2,230) of those contacted in the study. The three data waves included in this study ran from March 2001 to July 2002 (T1), September 2003 to December 2004 (T2), and September 2005 to Au-gust 2007 (T3), with response rates consistently above 80%. The clinic-referred sample (n = 543) consisted of pre-adolescents who had been referred to a child and adolescent psychiatric outpatient clinic in the northern of the Netherlands at any point in their life (20.8 % ≤5 years, 66.1 % 6-9 years, 13.1 % 10-12 years) for consultation or treatment. The first three data waves in the clinic-referred cohort ran two years behind those of the population cohort: From September 2004 to December 2005 (T1), September 2006 to November 2007 (T2), and September 2009 to February 2011 (T3). The measurement instruments and design for the clinic-referred cohort were the same as those of the population cohort. Of the 1264 eligible pre-adolescents, 543 were enrolled in the study and finished baseline measurements (T1). Further details of the included measurements across the different time points, sampling procedures, and response rates of both co-horts are well-documented elsewhere (e.g., De Winter et al., 2005; Huisman et al., 2008; Oldehinkel et al., 2015).

(13)

Aims and Outline of this Thesis

The overall aim of this PhD thesis is to elucidate the role of genetic and environmental influences as well as their interplay in the course of ADHD symptoms across adoles-cence. For that purpose, different environmental contexts were examined to obtain a more comprehensive picture of how ADHD candidate genes, environment, and their interaction may be involved in adolescent development in the transition into adulthood. The general hypothesis of this PhD thesis was that G×E interactions (partly) explain the course of ADHD symptoms across adolescence.

In Chapter 2, I examined whether candidate genes (DRD4, 5-HTTLPR, DRD2, COMT, and

MAOA), a set of pre- and perinatal factors (i.e., index measure of various pregnancy and

delivery complications, maternal smoking, maternal drinking, and low birth weight), and their interactions were associated during the course of ADHD symptoms from early to late adolescence, using Linear Mixed Models (LMM). While these factors have been implicated in the etiology of ADHD, it is currently unknown whether they have an endur-ing effect on the course of ADHD symptoms across adolescence.

Chapter 3 explored whether perceived parenting styles, both independent and

de-pendent of candidate genes (i.e., DRD4, 5-HTTLPR, and MAOA), predicted different ADHD symptom trajectories across adolescence, using Growth Mixture Modeling (GMM). This might serve as a starting point for treatment to prevent adolescents following at-risk (persistent) ADHD symptom trajectories.

In Chapter 4, I investigated whether the 5-HTTLPR genotype moderated associations between two types of life stressors (i.e., environment-related and person-related life stressors) and ADHD symptoms between early, mid, and late adolescence. Using a bidirectional longitudinal design with Structural Equation Modeling [SEM] helps to de-termine whether exposure to life stressors contributed to adolescents’ ADHD symptoms and vice versa, all in the context of the 5-HTTLPR genotype. This elucidates the direction-ality (ADHD <-> life stressors), specificity (independent vs. dependent life stressors), and time dependency of the associations.

In Chapter 5, by using an innovative analysis method (Random Intercept Cross Lagged Path Model [RI-CLPM]), genetic moderation by the DRD4 and 5-HTTLPR genotypes were examined on longitudinal bidirectional associations of the family climate and school climate with ADHD symptoms across adolescence. The importance of the family and school context changes across adolescence and since adolescents may be differently af-fected by the environment because of their genetic make-up, it would be interesting to see how susceptibility genes, the family and school environment, and their interaction are related to ADHD symptoms across adolescence.

Finally, in Chapter 6 I will discuss the main findings and integrate these within current research, concluding with directions for future research.

(14)

1

REFERENCES

Altink, M.E., Arias-Vasquez, A., Franke, B., Slaats-Willemse, D.I., Buschgens, C.J., Rommelse, N.N., Buitelaar, J.K. (2008). The dopamine receptor D4 7-repeat allele and prenatal smoking in ADHD-affected children and their unaffected siblings: no gene-environment interaction. Journal of Child

Psychol-ogy and Psychiatry, 49(10), 1053-1060.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental health disorders:

DSM-5 (4th ed., text rev.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental health disorders:

DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Andersen, S.L. (2003). Trajectories of brain development: point of vulnerability or window of opportunity.

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Review, 27(1-2), 3-18.

Arnett, J.J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. American Psychologist, 54(5), 317-326. Asherson, P., & Trzaskowski, M. (2015). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder is the extreme and

im-pairing tail of a continuum. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

54(4), 573-583.

Asherson, P., Kuntsi, J., & Taylor, E. (2005). Unravelling the complexity of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: a behavioural genomic approach. British Journal of Psychiatry, 187, 103-105.

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to rearing environ-ment depending on dopamine-related genes: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Developenviron-ment

and Psychopathology, 23(1), 39-52.

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2006). Gene-environment interaction of the dopa-mine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(5), 406-409.

Banerjee, T.D., Middleton, F., & Faraone, S.V. (2007). Environmental risk factors for Attention-Deficit Hyper-activity Disorder. Acta Paediatrica, 96(9), 1269-1274.

Barkley, R. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: The Guilford Press.

Barkley, R.A., Murphy, K.R., & Fischer, M. (2008). ADHD in adults: what the science says. New York: Guilford. Belsky J., & Pluess, M. (2013). Genetic moderation of early child-care effects on social functioning across

childhood: a developmental analysis. Child Development, 84(4), 1209-1225.

Belsky J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influ-ences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885-908.

Berry, D., Deater-Deckard, K., McCartney, K., Wang, Z., & Petrill, S. A. (2013). Gene-environment interaction between dopamine receptor D4 7-repeat polymorphism and early maternal sensitivity predicts inattention trajectories across middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 25(2), 291-306.

Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S.V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 157(5), 816-818.

Biederman, J., Mick, E., Faraone, S.V., Braaten, E., Doyle, A., Spencer, T., Wilens, T.E., Frazier, E., & Johnson, M.A. (2002). Influence of gender on Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in children referred to a psychiatric clinic. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(1), 36-42.

Biederman, J., Petty, C.R., Ten Haagen, K.S., Small, J., Doyle, A.E., Spencer, T., ... Faraone, S.V. (2009). Effect of candidate gene polymorphisms on the course of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.

(15)

Brewer, J.A., & Potenza, M.N. (2008). The neurobiology and genetics of impulse control disorders: relation-ships to drug addictions. Biochemical Pharmacology, 75(1), 63-75.

Brookes, K., Xu X., Chen, W., Zhou, K., Neale, B., Lowe, N., ... Johansson, L. (2006). The analysis of 51 genes in DSM-IV combined type Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: association signals in DRD4, DAT1 and 16 other genes. Molecular Psychiatry, 11(10), 934-953.

Buhs, E.S., & Ladd, G.W. (2001). Peer rejection as an antecedent of young children’s school adjustment: an examination of mediating processes. Developmental Psychology, 37(4), 550-560.

Buitelaar, J.K. (2017). Optimising treatment strategies for ADHD in adolescence to minimise ‘lost in transi-tion’ to adulthood. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 26(5), 448-452.

Buitelaar, J.K. (2005). ADHD: Strategies to unravel its genetic architecture. Journal of Neural Transmission.

Supplementum, 69, 1-17.

Caspi, A., Hariri, A.R., Holmes, A., Uher, R., & Moffitt, T.E. (2010). Genetic sensitivity to the environment: the case of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases and traits. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(5), 509-527.

Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T.E., Taylor, A., Craig, I.W., Harrington, H., ... Poulton, R. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301(5631), 386-389.

Chan, E., Fogler, J.M., & Hammerness, P.G. (2009). Treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in adolescents: A systematic review. Clinical Review & Education, 315(18), 1999-2008.

Chang, Z., Lichtenstein, P., Asherson, P.J., & Larsson, H. (2013). Developmental twin study of attention problems: high heritabilities throughout development. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(3), 311-318. Coghill, D., & Seth, S. (2011). Do the diagnostic criteria for ADHD need to change? Comments on the

pre-liminary proposals of the DSM-5 ADHD and disruptive behavior disorders committee. European

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 20(2), 75-81.

Covault, J., Tennen, H., Armeli, S., Conner, T.S., Herman, A.I., Cillessen, A.H., & Kranzler, H.R. (2007). Inter-active effects of the serotonin transporter 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and stressful life events on college student drinking and drug use. Biological Psychiatry, 61(5), 609-616.

Das, D., Cherbuin, N., Butterworth, P., Anstey, K.J., & Easteal, S. (2012). A population-based study of Atten-tion Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder symptoms and associated impairment in middle-aged adults.

PLoS One, 7, e31500.

De Winter, A., Oldehinkel, A.J., Veenstra, R., Brunnekreef, J.A., Verhulst, F.C., & Ormel, J. (2005). Evaluation of non-response bias in mental health determinants and outcomes in a large sample of pre- ado-lescents. European Journal of Epidemiology, 20(2), 173-181.

Döpfner, M., Hautmann, C., Görtz-Dorten, A., Klasen, F., Ravens-Sieberer, U., & The BELLA study group (2015). Long-term course of ADHD symptoms from childhood to early adulthood in a community sample. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 24(6), 665-673.

Elia, J., Gai, X., Xie, H.M., Perin, J.C., Geiger, E., Glessner, J.T., ... White, P.S. (2010). Rare structural variants found in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder rare preferentially associated with neurodevel-opmental genes. Molecular Psychiatry, 15(6), 637-646.

Ellis, B.J., Boyce, W.T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: an evolutionary--neurodevelopmental theory. Developmental

Psychopathology, 23(1), 7-28.

Elmore, A.L., Nigg, J.T., Friderici, K.H., Jernigan, K., & Nikolas, M.A. (2016). Does 5HTTLPR genotype moder-ate the association of family environment with child Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder symptomatology? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 45(3), 348-360.

(16)

1

Evans, S.W., Schultz, B.K., White, L.C., Brady, C., Sibley, M.H., & Van Eck, K.A. (2009). School-based organiza-tion intervenorganiza-tion for young adolescents with Attenorganiza-tion-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. School

Mental Health, 1(2), 78-88.

Faraone, S.V., Asherson, P., Banaschewski, T., Biederman, J., Buitelaar, J.K., Ramos-Quiroga, J.A., ... Franke, B. (2015). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 15020.

Faraone, S.V., Biederman, J., & Mick, E. (2006). The age-dependent decline of Attention Deficit Hyperactiv-ity Disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychological Medicine, 36(2), 159-165. Faraone S.V., & Mick, E. (2010). Molecular genetics of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Psychiatric

Clinics of North America, 33(1), 159-180.

Franke, B., Vasquez, A.A., Johansson, S., Hoogman, M., Romanos, J., Boreatti-Hümmer, A., ... Reif, A. (2010). Multicenter analysis of the SLC6A3/DAT1 VNTR haplotype in persistent ADHD suggests differ-ential involvement of the gene in childhood and persistent ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology,

35(3), 656-64.

Franke, B., Hoogman, M., Arias Vasquez, A., Heister, J.G., Savelkoul, P.J., Naber, M., ... Buitelaar, J.K. (2008). Association of the dopamine transporter (SLC6A3/DAT1) gene 9-6 haplotype with adult ADHD.

American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B(8), 1576-1579.

Galéra, C., Côté, S.M., Bouvard, M.P., Pingault, J., Melchior, M., Michel, G., ... & Tramblay, R.E. (2011). Early risk factors for hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(12), 1267-1275.

Gaub, M., & Carlson, C.L. (1997). Gender differences in ADHD: a meta-analysis and critical review. Journal

of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(8), 1036-1045.

Giedd, J.N. (2008). The teen brain: insights from neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42(4), 335-343.

Giedd, J.N., & Rapoport, J.L. (2010). Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron, 67(5), 728-734.

Gizer, I.R., Ficks, C., & Waldman, I.D. (2009). Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review.

Human Genetics, 126(1), 51-90.

Gustafsson, P., & Källén, K. (2011). Perinatal, maternal, and fetal characteristics of children diagnosed with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: results from a population-based study utilizing the Swedish Medical Birth Register. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 53(3), 263-268. Halmøy, A., Klungsøyr, K., Skjærven, R., & Haavik, J. (2012). Pre- and perinatal risk factors in adults with

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 71(5), 474-481.

Hoogman, M., Bralten, J., Hibar, D.P., Mennes, M., Zwiers, M.P., Schweren, L.S., ... Franke, B. (2017). Sub-cortical brain volume differences in participants with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in children and adults: a cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry, 4(4), 310-319.

Huisman, M., Oldehinkel, A.J., De Winter, A., Minderaa, R.B., De Bildt, A., Huizink, ... F.C., Ormel, J. (2008). Cohort profile: the Dutch ‘TRacking Adolescents’ Individual Lives’ Survey’; TRAILS. International

Journal of Epidemiology, 37(6), 1227-1235.

Huizink, A.C., & Mulder, E.J. (2006). Maternal smoking, drinking or cannabis use during pregnancy and neurobehavioural and cognitive functioning in human offspring. Neuroscience and

Biobehav-ioural Reviews, 30(1), 24-41.

Johnston, C., & Mash, E.J. (2001). Families of children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: review and recommendations for future research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review,

4(3), 183-207.

Kan, K.J., Dolan, C.V., Nivard, M.G., Middeldorp, C.M., Van Beijsterveldt, C.E.M., Willemsen, G., & Boomsma, D.I. (2013). Genetic and environmental stability in attention problems across the lifespan:

(17)

evi-dence from the Netherlands twin register. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 52(1), 12-25.

Kessler, R.C., Adler, L., Barkley, R., Biederman, J., Conners, C.K., Demler, O., ... Zaslavsky, A.M. (2006). The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the national comor-bidity survey replication. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(4), 716-723.

Kim, D.H., & Yoo, I.Y. (2013). Relationship between Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder symptoms and perceived parenting practices of school-age children. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 22(7-8), 1133-1139.

Kopečková, M., Paclt, I., Petrášek, J., Pacltová, D., Malíková, M., & Zagatová, V. (2008). Some ADHD poly-morphisms (in genes DAT1, DRD2, DRD3, DBH, 5-HTT) in case-control study of 100 subjects 6-10 age. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 29(2), 246-251.

Langberg, J.M., Epstein, J.N., Girio-Herrera, E., Becker, S.P., Vaughn, A.J., & Altaye, M. (2011). Materials organization, planning, and homework completion in young adolescents with ADHD: impact on academic performance. School Mental Health, 3(2), 93-101.

Langley, K., Fowler, T.A., Grady, D.L., Moyzis, R.K., Holmans, P.A, Van den Bree, M.B.M., ... Thapar, A. (2009). Molecular genetic contribution to the developmental course of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 18(1), 26-32.

Langley, K., Turic, D., Rice, F., Holmans, P., Van den Bree, M.B., Craddock, N., ... Thapar, A. (2008). Testing for gene x environment interaction effects in Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and associ-ated antisocial behaviour. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics,

147B(1), 49-53.

Larsson, H., Anckarsater, H., Rastam, M., Chang, Z., & Lichtenstein, P. (2012). Childhood adult Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 73-80.

Larsson, J.O., Larsson, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2004). Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change of ADHD symptoms between 8 and 13 years of age: a longitudinal twin study. Journal

of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(10), 1267-1275.

Le, H.H., Hodgkins, P., Postma, M.J., Kahle, J., Sikirica, V., Setyawan, J., ... Doshi, J.A. (2014). Economic im-pact of childhood/adolescent ADHD in a European setting: the Netherlands as a reference case.

European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(7), 587-589.

Levy, F., Hay, D.A., McStephen M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(6),737-744.

Li, D., Sham, P.C., Owen, M.J., & He, L. (2006). Meta-analysis shows significant association between do-pamine system genes and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Human Molecular Genetics, 15(14), 2276-2284.

Li, Z., Chang, S., Zhang, L., Gao, L., & Wang. (2014) Molecular genetic studies of ADHD and its candidate genes: a review. Psychiatry Research, 219(1), 10-24.

Loe, I.M., & Feldman, H.M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD.

Ambula-tory Pediatrics, 7(1), 82-90.

Marsh, R., Gerber, A.J., & Peterson, B.S. (2008). Neuroimaging studies of normal brain development and their relevance for understanding childhood neuropsychiatric disorders. Journal of American

Acadamy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(11), 1233-1251.

Michielsen, M., Comijns, H.C., Semeijn, E.J., Beekman, A.T., Deeg, D.J., & Kooij, S.J.J. (2013). The comor-bidity of anxiety and depressive symptoms in older adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: a longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 148(2-3), 220-227.

(18)

1

Moffit, T.E., Caspi, A., & Rutter, M. (2005). Strategy for investigating interactions between measured genes and measured environments. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(5), 473-481.

Müller, D.J., Mandelli, L., Serretti, A., DeYoung, C.G., De Luca, V., Sicard, T., ... Kennedy, J.L. (2008). Serotonin transporter gene and adverse life events in adult ADHD. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part

B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B(8), 1461-1469.

Musser, E.D., Karalunas, S.L., Dieckman, N., Peris, T.S., & Nigg, J.T. (2016). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder developmental trajectories related to parental expressed emotion. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 125(2), 182-195.

Neuman, R.J., Lobos, E., Reich, W., Henderson, C.A., Sun, L.W., & Todd, R.D. (2007). Prenatal smoking expo-sure and dopaminergic genotypes interact to cause a severe ADHD subtype. Biological Psychiatry,

61(12), 1320-1328.

Nigg, J.T., Nikolas, M., & Burt, S.A. (2010). Measured gene-by-environment interaction in relation to Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 49(9), 863-73.

Nikitopoulos, J., Zohsel, K., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A.F., Schmid, B., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., ... Laucht, M. (2014). Are infants differentially sensitive to parenting? Early maternal care, DRD4 genotype and externalizing behavior during adolescence. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 59(9), 53-59.

Nikolas, M., Klump, L.K., & Burt, S.A. (2012). Youth appraisals of inter-parental conflict and genetic and environmental contributions to Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: examination of GxE effects in a twin sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(4), 543-554.

Nikolas, M.A., & Burt, S.A. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(1), 1-17. Nøvik, T.S., Hervas, A., Ralston, S.J., Dalsgaard, S., Rodrigues Pereira, R., Lorenzo, M.J., & ADORE Study

Group (2006). Influence of gender on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Europe – ADORE. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 15-24.

Oldehinkel, A.J., Rosmalen, J.G.M., Buitelaar, J.K., Hoek, H.W., Ormel, J., Raven, D., ... Hartman, C.A. (2015). Cohort profile update: the Tracking Adolescents’ Individual Live Survey (TRAILS). International

Journal of Epidemiology, 44(1), 76-76n.

Parker, J., Rubin, K.H., Erath, S., Wojslawowicz, J.C., & Buskirk A.A. (2006). Peer relationships and develop-mental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developdevelop-mental psychopathology: risk,

disorder, and adaptation (pp. 419-493). New York: Wiley.

Pingault, J., Viding, E., Galéra, C., Greven, C.U., Zhen, Y., Plomin, R., & Rijsdijk, F. (2015). Genetic and envi-ronmental influences on the developmental course of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder symptoms from childhood to adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(7), 651-658.

Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Children’s differential susceptibility to effects of parenting. Family Science,

1(1) 14-25.

Polanczyk, G., De Lima, M.S., Horta, B.L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L.A. (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 942- 948.

Polderman, T., Boomsma, D., Bartels, M., Verhulst, F., & Huizink, A. (2010). A systematic review of prospec-tive studies on attention problems and academic achievement. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,

122(4), 271-284.

Retz, W., Freitag, C.M., Retz-Junginger, P., Wenzler, D., Schneider, M., Kissling, C., ... Rosler, M. (2008). A functional serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism increases ADHD symptoms in de-linquents: interaction with adverse childhood environment. Psychiatry research, 158(2), 123-131.

(19)

Riglin, L., Collishaw, S., Thapar, A.J., Dalsgaard, S., Langley, K., Smith, G.D., ... Thapar, A. (2016). Association of genetic risk variants with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder trajectories in the general population. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(12), 1285-1292.

Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. (2006). Peer inter actions, relationships, and groups. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 571-645). New York , NY: Wiley.

Sagiv, S.K., Epstein, J.N., Bellinger, D.C., & Korrick, S.A. (2013). Pre- and postnatal risk factors for ADHD in a nonclinical pediatric population. Journal of Attention Disorders, 17(1), 47-57.

Sasser, T.R., Kalvin, C.B., & Bierman, K.L. (2016). Developmental trajectories of clinically significant Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms from grade 3 through 12 in a high-risk sample: predictors and outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 207-219.

Schmidt, S., & Petermann, F. (2009). Developmental psychopathology: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). BMC Psychiatry, 56, 265-274.

Schriber, R.A., & Guyer, A.E. (2016). Adolescent neurobiological susceptibility to social context.

Develop-mental Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1-18.

Secnik, K., Swensen, A., & Lage, M.J. (2005). Comorbidities and costs of adult patients diagnosed with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Pharmacoeconomics, 23(1), 93-102.

Silva, D., Colvin, L., Hagemann, E., & Bower, C. (2014). Environmental risk factors by gender associated with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Pediatrics, 133(1), e14-e22.

Simon, V., Czobor, P., Balint, S., Meszaros, A., & Bitter, I. (2009). Prevalence and correlates of adult Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 194(3), 204-211. Sun, H., Yuan, F., Shen, X., Xiong, G., & Wu, J. (2014). Role of COMT in ADHD: a systematic review. Molecular

Neurobiology, 49(1), 251-261.

Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2006). Mapping brain maturation. TRENDS in Neurosciences,

29(3), 148-159.

Swanson, E.N., Owens, E.B., & Hinshaw, S.P. (2014). Pathways to self-harmful behaviors in young women with and without ADHD: a longitudinal examination of mediating factors. Journal of Child

Psy-chology and Psychiatry, and allied sciences, 55(5), 505-515.

Swensen, A.R., Birnbaum, H.G., Secnik, K., Marynchenko, M., Greenberg, P., & Claxton, A. (2003). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: increased costs for patients and their families. Journal of American

Acadamy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(12), 1415-1423.

Tandon, M., Tillmann, R., Agrawal, A., & Luby, J. (2016). Trajectories of ADHD severity over 10 years from childhood into adulthood. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 8(3), 121-130.

Thapar, A., Cooper, M., Jefferies, R., & Stergiakouli, E. (2012). What causes Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder? Archives of Disease in Childhood, 97(3), 260-265.

Thapar, A., Harold, G., Rice, F., Langley, K., & O’Donovan, M. (2007a). The contribution of gene-environment interaction to psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 19(4), 989-1004.

Thapar, A., Langley, K., Asherson, P., & Gill, M. (2007b). Gene-environment interplay in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and the importance of a developmental perspective. The British Journal of

Psychiatry, 190(1), 1-3.

Van der Meer, D., Hartman, C.A., Richards, J., Bralten, J.B., Franke, B., Oosterlaan, J., ... Hoekstra, P.J. (2014). The serotonin transporter gene polymorphism 5-HTTLPR moderates the effects of stress on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(12), 1363-1371.

Van Emmerik-Van Oortmerssen, K., Van de Glind, G., Van den Brink, W., Smit, F., Crunelle, C.L., Swets, M., & Schoevers, R.A. (2012). Prevalence of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in substance use

(20)

1

disorder patients: a meta-analysis and meta- regression analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,

122(1-2), 11-19.

Van Lier, P.A.C., Van der Ende J., Koot, H.M., & Verhulst, F.C. (2007). Which better predicts conduct prob-lems? The relationship of trajectories of conduct problems with ODD and ADHD symptoms from childhood into adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(6), 601-608.

Véronneau, M.H., Trempe, S.C., & Paiva, A.O. (2014). Risk and protective factors in the peer context: how do other children contribute to the psychosocial adjustment of the adolescent? Ciencia & Saude

Coletiva, 19(3), 695-705.

Wehmeier, P.M., Schacht, A., & Barkley, R.A. (2010). Social and emotional impairment in children and ado-lescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of life. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46(3), 209-217. Whalen, C.K., & Henker, B. (1985). The social worlds of hyperactive (ADHD) children. Clinical Psychology

Review, 5(5), 447-478.

Wirth, A., Tilman, R., Gawrilow, C., Schwenck, C., Freitag, C.M., & Rauch, W.A. (in press). Examining the relationship between children’s ADHD symptomatology and inadequate parenting: the role of household chaos. Journal of Attention Disorder.

Zohsel, K., Bianchi, V., Mascheretti, S., Hohm, E., Schmidt, M.H., Esser, G., ... Laucht, M. (2015). Monoamine oxidase A polymorphism moderates stability of attention problems and susceptibility to life stress during adolescence. Genes, Brain & Behavior, 14(8), 565-572.

(21)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Gene-environment interactions on the course of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms: From early into late adolescence..

The simultaneous presence of the low activity MAOA genotype and low birth weight (p &lt; .001) and of the 5-HTTLPR LL-allele and respectively pregnancy and delivery complications

In the present study, we examined perceived parental rejection, overprotection, and emotional warmth, as well as their interaction with three plasticity genes as predictors

life stressors would predict ADHD symptom levels only in S-allele carriers but not in L-allele homozygotes of the 5-HTTLPR genotype; (2) ADHD symptom levels would

Inde- pendent from the DRD4 and 5-HTTLPR genotype, we found important trait associations (i.e., between-person level) between ADHD and the family and school climate which

• S-allele homozygotes of the 5-HTTLPR genotype with higher levels of ADHD symptoms were more vulnerable to becoming exposed to person-related (i.e., dependent) life

Na het corrigeren van de effecten van meerdere omgevingsfactoren voor elkaar, het corrigeren voor genetische risicofactoren voor disruptief gedrag, en het corrigeren voor

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright