• No results found

University of Groningen Gene-environment interactions in disruptive behaviors Ruisch, Hyun

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "University of Groningen Gene-environment interactions in disruptive behaviors Ruisch, Hyun"

Copied!
37
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

University of Groningen

Gene-environment interactions in disruptive behaviors

Ruisch, Hyun

DOI:

10.33612/diss.136546089

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date:

2020

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Ruisch, H. (2020). Gene-environment interactions in disruptive behaviors. University of Groningen.

https://doi.org/10.33612/diss.136546089

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the

author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately

and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the

number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

Chapter

2

Maternal substance use during

pregnancy and offspring conduct

problems: a meta-analysis

Published as:

Ruisch IH, Dietrich A, Glennon JC, Buitelaar JK, Hoekstra PJ. Maternal substance use during

pregnancy and offspring conduct problems: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience and

Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018; 84: 325-336.

(3)

Acknowledgements, funding & declarations

This work is supported by the European Community's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement no. 602805 (Aggressotype) and no. 603016 (MATRICS). Jan K. Buitelaar has been in the past years a consultant to / member of advisory board of / and/or speaker for Janssen Cilag BV, Eli Lilly, Medice, Shire, Roche, and Servier. He is not an employee of any of these companies, and not a stock shareholder of any of these companies. He has no other financial or material support, including expert testimony, patents, royalties. All other authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Abstract

We conducted meta-analyses of relationships between highly prevalent substance use

during pregnancy and offspring conduct disorder problems. In total 36 studies were

included. Odds ratios (ORs) were 2.06 (1.67-2.54, 25 studies) for maternal smoking, 2.11

(1.42-3.15, 9 studies) for alcohol use, and 1.29 (0.93-1.81, 3 studies) for cannabis use, while

a single study of caffeine use reported no effects. Our meta-analyses support an association

between smoking and alcohol use during pregnancy, and offspring conduct problems, yet

do not resolve causality issues given potential confounding by genetic factors,

gene-environment interactions, and comorbidity such as with attention deficit hyperactivity

disorders. Future studies should use genetically sensitive designs to investigate the role of

pregnancy substance use in offspring conduct problems and may consider more broadly

defined behavioral problems.

(4)

Introduction

Conduct disorder (CD) is a prevalent behavioral disorder characterized by a pattern of

severe antisocial behaviors in which the basic rights of others or major age-appropriate

societal norms or rules are violated, with an estimated prevalence between 4 and 16% in

school age boys and between 1 and 9% in school age girls (1). Symptoms include aggression

to people and animals, destruction of property, theft, and other rule-breaking behaviors (2).

Comorbid externalizing behaviors are frequent, in particular oppositional-defiant disorder

(ODD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (1–4). CD is considered a

precursor of antisocial personality disorder in adulthood and about 25% of girls and 40%

of boys with CD are estimated to eventually develop this disorder (4–6). Not surprisingly,

CD causes a considerable societal burden as well as significant negative functional outcomes

for affected individuals (4,7).

CD is a heterogeneous disorder and both genetic and environmental factors are involved in

its etiology (3,4). Early adversities, originating from the prenatal environment, may exert

programming influences on the developing fetus and thereby play a role in the etiology of

many mental disorders including CD (8). A widespread problem is substance use during

pregnancy with estimated prevalence’s of maternal smoking between 20 and 30%, alcohol

use around 15%, and cannabis use between 3 and 10% of pregnant women (9). Cigarette

smoke contains many potentially hazardous agents including carbon monoxide, nicotine,

polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons, and heavy metals (10,11). Maternal smoking during

pregnancy has been associated with abnormal development of the central nervous system

(12) and behavioral problems in the offspring (13,14). Ethanol, the principal alcohol in

alcoholic beverages, is a neurotoxic agent and intrauterine exposure leads to fetal alcohol

spectrum disorders and related behavioral problems (15,16). Cannabis is one of the most

commonly used recreational drugs (17), probably also during pregnancy (18). Cannabis use

during pregnancy may affect the neurocognitive development in the offspring (19–22).

Furthermore, caffeine is a psychoactive substance used daily by approximately 75 to 93%

of pregnant women (23,24). Human studies regarding caffeine intake during pregnancy and

offspring neuropsychiatric outcomes are mostly inconclusive (25).

Currently, no meta-analytic studies have focused yet on the role of the prenatal environment

in relation to CD. Our goal was therefore to summarize and expand current knowledge on

this topic by conducting meta-analyses of highly prevalent and preventable maternal

substance use during pregnancy including the use of cigarettes, alcohol, cannabis, and

caffeine, and offspring risk of conduct problems.

(5)

Methods

Search strategy

We conducted a systematic search in the databases of the U.S. National Library of Medicine

(PubMed/MEDLINE), EMBASE, and PsycINFO, using the following search terms: (“conduct

disorder” OR “conduct problems” OR “conduct symptoms”) AND (pregnancy OR prenatal OR

“intra uterine”) AND (smoking OR cigarettes OR nicotine OR alcohol OR marijuana OR

cannabis OR weed OR caffeine OR coffee). We limited the search to human observational

studies in English, published in peer reviewed journals between January 1

st

1990 and

November 1

st

2016. Review articles, conference abstracts, posters, whitepapers, and other

grey literature were not included. Table S1 (supplementary material) provides an

overview of the eligibility criteria for the included studies. We did not register a

meta-analysis protocol.

Data collection

Electronic searches and screening of titles and abstracts were conducted by two authors

independently (IHR and AD). Any discrepancies as well as any issues encountered during

literature review were resolved by discussion, if necessary with a third author (PJH). To

determine if a study met our eligibility criteria full text articles were read. Figure 1 provides

an overview of the literature search and review process. From a total of 360 unique citations,

a total of 36 unique studies were included in all meta-analyses.

Data extraction was performed by the first author. The following data was extracted

systematically: study design, sample, demographics, exposure, outcome measures, and

statistical analyses. Study quality was assessed using the Newcastle-Ottowa Scale (NOS) for

non-randomized studies (26). A detailed description of the NOS-criteria, as applied to our

specific sample of studies on pregnancy substance use, is provided in Table S2

(supplementary material). We considered a NOS-score < 6 (out of a maximum of 9) as an

indicator of low quality. Publication bias was graphically assessed by funnel plots when at

least ten studies were included in the particular analysis (27). Missing data was either

imputed, thereby resembling the actual sample as accurately as possible, or authors were

contacted to provide the missing information.

(6)

Fig. 1: Study flow diagram.

Data synthesis

Statistical heterogeneity was evaluated by the I

2

-statistic and the χ

2

-test for homogeneity.

The I

2

-statistic estimates the proportion of total variance that can be attributed to

between-studies dispersion, rather than sampling error within individual between-studies. The χ

2

-test for

homogeneity estimates the likelihood of true between-studies heterogeneity, yet has low

power when studies are few (27,28). When statistical heterogeneity was considered high

(I

2

> 75%) we investigated the source of this heterogeneity by conducting subgroup analyses

based on clinical and/or methodological diversity and study quality within the original pool

of included studies.

Because observational, non-randomized studies cover different study types, samples,

sample sizes, methodology, and measures of CD, we expect more heterogeneity than when

assessing randomized controlled trials and we believe that the included studies do not

estimate one common effect size. Therefore we decided to use a random effects model in our

meta-analyses to calculate an average summary effect size to provide a global impression of

the magnitude of the effect. Note that when statistical heterogeneity (between-studies

variance) decreases, differences between fixed and random effects models decrease,

becoming eventually zero when there is no heterogeneity at all. In cases of very high

heterogeneity the random effects estimate is dominated by the between-studies variance

correction, levelling out study weights, and the summary effect will approach an arithmetic

mean. The between-studies variance (τ

2

-parameter) was estimated by the method of

DerSimonian and Laird (28,29). Since available precompiled software would restrict the

analyses to a small selection of studies, thereby introducing non-random selection,

aforementioned models were coded in MATLAB (30) to perform computations and generate

graphics.

349 unique records identified through database searching (PubMed, PsycINFO, EMBASE).

11 unique additional records identified through other sources (such as references of relevant papers).

287 records excluded due to non-relevance.

73 studies assessed for eligibility.

37 studies excluded with reasons: Non-relevance (N=3). No journal article (N=4).

Review article (N=2). Article not available (N=3). No CD or CD-symptoms outcome (N=8). Insufficient data for meta-analysis (N=13). Disordinal statistical interaction effects (N=2).

Overlapping sample (N=2). 360 total unique records screened.

(7)

Associations were quantitatively assessed by calculating odds ratio’s (OR) for dichotomous

outcome data and standardized mean differences (SMD) for continuous outcome data. Since

most studies natively provided dichotomous data, SMDs were converted to ORs. We choose

to convert effect sizes to avoid having to exclude a substantial number of studies, which

would potentially cause a substantial degree of selection bias (28).

Meta-analyses

A meta-analysis was performed when an effect size for two or more studies was available.

We combined the maximally adjusted (for the largest number of confounding variables, in

some cases only crude effects were available) effect sizes that were available for each study

(27). For smoking during pregnancy, we considered general, not dose-related effects, from

here on referred to as ‘overall effects’ (none versus any exposure), as well as ‘dose-related

effects’ (light versus heavy exposure). For dose-related effects, ‘light exposure’ was defined

as maternal smoking of ≤ 15 cigarettes per day during pregnancy, whereas ‘heavy exposure’

was defined as maternal smoking of > 15 cigarettes per day. Of note, for dose-related

analyses of smoking, we included one study that was excluded from the non dose-related

meta-analysis due to sample-overlap in that analysis (31). Therefore the total number of

studies regarding non dose-related effects of smoking equals 25 instead of 26.

Further of note is the existence of complex data structures within a number of studies

requiring the combining of subgroups to estimate an overall effect (28). In the few instances

where different gestational trimesters were compared it was decided to use only data from

exposure during the first trimester since the occurrence of fetal organogenesis causes this

to be a vulnerable period (15).

Strength of evidence assessment

To give a global impression of the overall strength of our evidence, we carried out a Grades

of Recommendation, Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) (32) assessment

for each risk factor. A detailed description of the GRADE-criteria, as applied to our sample

of observational studies, is available in Table S3 (supplementary material). Of note, the

default GRADE-level is ‘low’ for observational studies.

(8)

Results

Description of included studies

A total of 36 studies were included in our current analyses. Tables S4-S6 (supplementary

material) present an overview and description of all individual studies.

Samples

Sample sizes varied considerably, from 40 participants to approximately 52,000

participants. Average and total sample sizes were 4,612 and 115,297 (25 studies) for

smoking during pregnancy, 5,625 and 50,626 (9 studies) for alcohol use during pregnancy,

and 421 and 1,263 (3 studies) for cannabis use during pregnancy. The only study regarding

caffeine intake during pregnancy had a sample size of 3,439. Sixteen out of 25 studies with

data on smoking assessed a general population samples, while the other 9 studies assessed

samples preselected based on (psycho)pathological conditions such as ADHD or a familial

predisposition for substance abuse. For alcohol use, 6 out of 9 studies assessed general

population studies, while 3 studies assessed preselected samples. Studies regarding

cannabis use comprised only general population samples.

Demographics

Most studies used samples consisting of both males and females. The distribution of sex

varied roughly between 40 and 100% male, yet was not consistently reported across all

studies. Overall, males appeared to be more prevalent (clearly larger percentage males in at

least six mixed samples, and two male-only samples) in the total pool of analyzed studies.

The age of assessed offspring varied considerably, from approximately 5 to 18 years old.

Globally estimating, fourteen of the studies on smoking during pregnancy assessed offspring

at age 12 or below, five studies assessed conduct problems at an age >12, and the other

studies included an age range across both childhood and adolescence. In the case of alcohol

use during pregnancy four studies assessed offspring at age 12 or below, two studies

assessed at age >12, and the remaining studies included both age ranges.

Exposure to substance use during pregnancy

Data on substance use during pregnancy can be collected during the actual pregnancy

(prospective measurement) or after birth at any time in life (retrospective measurement).

Regarding smoking during pregnancy, five studies measured exposure prospectively, while

nineteen studies measured retrospectively. One study did not specify their method of

exposure measurement. Exposure proportions ranged from 13 to 68%. In the case of alcohol

use during pregnancy, seven studies measured exposure prospectively and three studies

used retrospective assessment methods. Proportions of exposed participants ranged from

13 to about 60%. Regarding cannabis use during pregnancy, all studies measured exposure

prospectively. Exposure proportions ranged from 40 to 50%. Caffeine intake was measured

prospectively and the exposure proportion was 72%.

(9)

Confounding variables in multivariable models

Table S7 (supplementary material) provides an overview of control variables and the

number of studies that adjusted for a particular variable. The majority of studies that

provided results adjusted for confounding variables adjusted for offspring age, sex, and

some form of social environmental disadvantage (e.g. low socioeconomic status, family

instability, crowdedness, harsh parenting style) in their multivariable statistical models.

Only few studies adjusted for comorbid ADHD-symptomatology in offspring, and other

maternal substance use during pregnancy.

Outcome measures

About half of the studies (12 studies regarding smoking, 5 studies regarding alcohol) used

diagnostic psychiatric interviews such as the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children

(33) or Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (34). About 40% of the

studies (10 studies regarding smoking, 4 studies regarding alcohol) used parent-on-child

questionnaires such as the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (35) or Child Behavior

Checklist (CBCL) (36,37), assessing a continuous measure of conduct problems. Regarding

the CBCL, conduct problems were scored using a combination of items from the delinquency

and aggression scales (36,37). In addition to diagnostic interviews and questionnaires,

Gilman et al. created a dimensional scale for conduct problems based on ratings for 15

different behavioral items (38,39). D’Onofrio et al. 2008 used CBCL-derived scales based on

DSM-constructs including seven specific items for conduct problems (40). Agrawal et al.

assessed DSM-IV CD-symptoms in the offspring by self-report and used a cut-off of three

symptoms for CD cases (41).

Quality assessment of individual studies

Results of study quality assessment based on Newcastle-Ottowa Scale (NOS) scores are

presented in Tables S4-S6 (supplementary material) and a detailed breakdown of each

studies’ total score is provided in Table S8 (supplementary material). From 25 studies on

maternal smoking, 11 studies scored a quality rating < 6. For alcohol, five studies scored <

6. No studies on cannabis use scored < 6.

Meta-analysis of maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring conduct

problems

The overall, non dose-related summary OR for maternal cigarette smoking was 2.06 (95%

confidence interval [CI] 1.67-2.54; I

2

=93%, P<0.001; 25 studies; Figure 2). The funnel plot

(10)

Fig. 2: Forest plot of studies regarding maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring conduct problems. The

confidence interval is shown as a horizontal line. Study weight is proportional to the area of the boxes. The width of the summary diamond represents its confidence interval. Maximally adjusted effects. An overview of individual studies is presented in Table S4 (supplementary material).

Smoking during pregnancy (25 studies)

Study: OR (95%-CI): Weight:

Maughan 2004 1.13 (0.94-1.37) 5.38% Agrawal 2010 1.18 (0.75-1.86) 4.46% Brion 2010 1.29 (1.12-1.49) 5.50% Hutchinson 2010 1.42 (1.10-1.84) 5.19% Palmer 2016 1.49 (1.19-1.85) 5.31% Melchior 2015 1.56 (0.99-2.46) 4.45% Larkby 2011 1.56 (0.93-2.63) 4.18% Fergusson 1998 1.58 (1.10-2.27) 4.83% Murray 2010 1.73 (1.46-2.04) 5.44% Tanaka 2016 1.93 (1.15-3.24) 4.19% Chastang 2015 1.94 (1.51-2.49) 5.22% Huijbregts 2008 2.19 (0.68-7.10) 2.03% D’Onofrio 2008 2.25 (1.79-2.82) 5.28% Wakschlag 1997 2.30 (1.09-4.83) 3.30% Freitag 2012 2.32 (0.69-7.72) 1.97% Nigg 2007 2.32 (1.05-5.13) 3.12% Sengupta 2015 2.86 (1.79-4.55) 4.40% Arnold 2005 2.89 (1.60-5.20) 3.90% Gilman 2008 2.97 (2.87-3.07) 5.63% Langley 2007 3.14 (1.54-6.40) 3.41% Biederman 2009 3.30 (1.23-8.85) 2.50% Wakschlag 2002 3.38 (0.99-11.55) 1.91% Gatzke-Kopp 2007 4.59 (1.92-10.96) 2.85% Hill 2000 4.66 (1.38-15.72) 1.94% Wilson 2013 5.02 (2.60-9.69) 3.62% Summary effect 2.06 (1.67-2.54) 100% 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10 20 50 OR for offspring conduct problems

Summary effect: 𝑍𝑍 = 6.76, 𝑃𝑃 < 0.001

(11)

Fig. 3: Funnel plot of studies regarding maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring conduct problems.

Smoking during pregnancy (25 studies)

(12)

Dose-related analyses yielded for light exposure an OR of 1.40 (95%-CI 1.25-1.57; I

2

=26%,

P=0.238; six studies; Figure 4A) and for heavy exposure an OR of 1.78 (95%-CI 1.37-2.32;

I

2

=73%, P=0.002; six studies; Figure 4B). The 95% CI’s of the dose-effects showed clear

overlap, indicating that effects of light and heavy exposure do not differ significantly.

Fig. 4: Forest plots of studies regarding maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring conduct problems for light (A) and heavy (B) exposure. The confidence interval is shown as a horizontal line. Study weight is proportional to the

area of the boxes. The width of the summary diamond represents its confidence interval. Maximally adjusted effects. ‘Light exposure’ was defined as maternal smoking of 15 or less cigarettes per day during pregnancy, whereas ‘heavy smoking’ was defined as maternal smoking of 15 or more cigarettes in pregnancy. An overview of individual studies is presented in Table

S4 (supplementary material).

A. Light smoking during pregnancy (6 studies)

Study: OR (95%-CI): Weight:

Maughan 2004 1.07 (0.82-1.40) 13.61% Hutchinson 2010 1.27 (0.97-1.66) 13.72% Fergusson 1998 1.43 (1.02-2.01) 9.58% Maughan 2001 1.48 (1.18-1.86) 17.79% D’Onofrio 2008 1.52 (1.38-1.67) 44.26% Wakschlag 1997 1.60 (0.53-4.83) 1.02% Summary effect 1.40 (1.25-1.57) 100% Summary effect: 𝑍𝑍 = 5.83, 𝑃𝑃 < 0.001 Heterogeneity: 𝑇𝑇2= 0.01, 𝜒𝜒2= 6.77, 𝑑𝑑𝑑𝑑 = 5, 𝑃𝑃 = 0.238, 𝐼𝐼2= 26.16% B. Heavy smoking during pregnancy (6 studies)

Study: OR (95%-CI): Weight:

Maughan 2004 1.27 (1.00-1.60) 20.98% Maughan 2001 1.53 (1.17-2.00) 19.90% Hutchinson 2010 1.60 (1.23-2.09) 20.00% Fergusson 1998 1.92 (1.24-2.97) 14.84% D’Onofrio 2008 2.74 (2.03-3.68) 18.99% Wakschlag 1997 3.30 (1.20-9.08) 5.29% Summary effect 1.78 (1.37-2.32) 100% Summary effect: 𝑍𝑍 = 4.34, 𝑃𝑃 < 0.001 Heterogeneity: 𝑇𝑇2= 0.07, 𝜒𝜒2= 18.69, 𝑑𝑑𝑑𝑑 = 5, 𝑃𝑃 = 0.002, 𝐼𝐼2= 73.25% 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10 20 50 OR for offspring conduct problems

0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10 20 50 OR for offspring conduct problems

(13)

Meta-analysis of maternal alcohol use during pregnancy and offspring conduct

problems

The summary OR for maternal alcohol use was 2.11 (95%-CI 1.42-3.15; I

2

=76%, P<0.001; 9

studies; Figure 5).

Fig. 5: Forest plot of studies regarding maternal alcohol use during pregnancy and offspring conduct problems. The

confidence interval is shown as a horizontal line. Study weight is proportional to the area of the boxes. The width of the summary diamond represents its confidence interval. Maximally adjusted effects. An overview of individual studies is presented in Table S5 (supplementary material).

Alcohol use during pregnancy (9 studies)

Study: OR (95%-CI): Weight:

Kelly 2009 0.81 0.54-1.20) 15.52% Niclasen 2014 1.27 (0.96-1.68) 16.79% Disney 2008 1.75 (1.30-2.36) 16.60% Sood 2001 2.04 (1.05-3.95) 12.18% Larkby 2011 2.74 (1.50-5.01) 12.92% Alvik 2013 4.10 (1.50-11.21) 8.48% Hill 2000 4.42 (1.35-14.47) 7.00% Fryer 2007 6.34 (0.74-54.72) 2.89% Ware 2013 7.68 (2.54-23.22) 7.61% Summary effect 2.11 (1.42-3.15) 100% 0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10 20 50 OR for offspring conduct problems

Summary effect: 𝑍𝑍 = 3.67, 𝑃𝑃 < 0.001

(14)

Meta-analysis of maternal cannabis use during pregnancy and offspring conduct

problems

The summary OR for cannabis use was 1.29 (95%-CI 0.93-1.81; I

2

=0; three studies; Figure

6). For this meta-analysis, there were no studies with a NOS-score < 6.

Fig. 6: Forest plot of studies regarding maternal cannabis use during pregnancy and offspring conduct problems. The

confidence interval is shown as a horizontal line. Study weight is proportional to the area of the boxes. The width of the summary diamond represents its confidence interval. Maximally adjusted effects. An overview of individual studies is presented in Table S6 (supplementary material).

Maternal caffeine use during pregnancy and offspring conduct problems

Due to a lack of studies, we were unable to conduct a meta-analysis and report here the

results of the only identified study on caffeine use during pregnancy. Reported unadjusted

ORs were between 0.62 (95%-CI 0.32-1.17) and 1.12 (95%-CI 0.43-2.94) as well as adjusted

ORs between 0.67 (95%-CI 0.34-1.35) and 1.16 (95%-CI 0.42-3.16) (25).

Analyses investigating heterogeneity

Given high heterogeneity in the meta-analysis of smoking during pregnancy, we stratified

the pool of included studies based on the type of study sample. We distinguished general

population samples, and samples that were preselected based on clinical conditions (such

as ADHD or a familial predisposition for substance abuse). The summary OR for general

population samples was 1.78 (95% CI 1.38-2.29; I

2

=96%, P<0.001; 16 studies) and the

summary OR for clinical samples was 3.02 (95% CI 2.35-3.89; I

2

=0; 9 studies). The summary

OR’s CI’s did not overlap, indicating a significant difference between general population and

clinical samples.

Since the general population subgroup meta-analyses of smoking during pregnancy still

showed high heterogeneity, we investigated the source of the heterogeneity further by

removing the study of Gilman et al. 2008. This study drew our attention because it appeared

as an outlier (large effect size and one to three orders of magnitude smaller variance than

most studies) and because of its use of a self-constructed outcome measure. The summary

Cannabis use during pregnancy (3 studies)

Study: OR (95%-CI): Weight:

Larkby 2011 1.18 (0.71-1.98) 42.30% Goldschmidt 2000 1.20 (0.73-1.97) 45.60% O’Connel 1991 2.34 (0.89-6.14) 12.10%

Summary effect 1.29 (0.93-1.81) 100%

0.2 0.5 1 2 5 10 20 50 OR for offspring conduct problems

Summary effect: 𝑍𝑍 = 1.51, 𝑃𝑃 = 0.131

(15)

OR for the subgroup of general population studies after removal of this study was 1.64 (95%

CI 1.42-1.90; I

2

=71%, P<0.001; 14 studies).

Because of substantial heterogeneity in the meta-analysis of alcohol use during pregnancy,

we stratified the studies investigating alcohol use the same way as with smoking during

pregnancy. The summary OR for general population samples was 1.66 (95% CI 1.14-2.40;

I

2

=75%, P=0.001; 6 studies) while the summary OR for clinical samples was 5.99 (95% CI

2.81-12.77; I

2

=0; 3 studies). The summary OR’s CI’s did not overlap, indicating a significant

difference between general population and clinical samples.

Sensitivity analyses investigating the influence of low quality studies

After excluding studies with low quality (NOS-score < 6) we reported for maternal smoking

during pregnancy a summary OR of 1.94 (95%-CI 1.48-2.54; I

2

=96%, P<0.001; 14 studies;

note the substantial heterogeneity), and for alcohol use during pregnancy a summary OR of

1.61 (95%-CI 1.06-2.45; I

2

=79%, P<0.001; 5 studies; note the substantial heterogeneity).

GRADE-assessment for each exposure

Taking into account that for observational studies the default GRADE-assessment level is

‘low’, we considered the global strength of evidence as baseline (‘low’) for maternal

smoking, below baseline (‘low to very low’) for maternal alcohol use during pregnancy, and

below baseline (‘low to very low’) for cannabis use during pregnancy. We did not rate the

single study on caffeine, since its quality was already assessed by the NOS (see section

‘Quality assessment of individual studies’). The evidence for smoking and alcohol use

during pregnancy was downgraded because we could only partially explain inconsistency

in results. Results of the GRADE-assessment are shown in Table S9 (supplementary

(16)

Discussion

Meta-analyses

We conducted meta-analyses of observational studies concerning substance use during

pregnancy in relation to offspring CD problems. The various analyses were represented by

subgroups of the total pool of included studies of maternal substance use during pregnancy.

Associations were observed between CD problems and both cigarette smoking and alcohol

consumption during pregnancy, while for cannabis use and caffeine intake insufficient

studies were available to draw sensible conclusions yet.

Maternal smoking during pregnancy

We observed an overall, not dose-related, association between maternal smoking during

pregnancy and offspring conduct problems. However, high heterogeneity was noted and

therefore further analysis was required. We found that effects reported in clinical study

samples were clearly higher than those reported in general population studies. Five of the

nine clinical samples involved ADHD-samples. Within these five studies, the two studies

reporting the largest effects controlled for comorbid ADHD, which suggests that comorbid

ADHD does at least not solely drive the higher effect of pregnancy smoking found in clinical

samples. Moreover, in these clinical samples, offspring CD was mostly diagnosed by using a

DSM diagnostic interview, while most general population studies used a screening

questionnaire to assess conduct problems. Expected milder levels of conduct problems in

the general population might explain the lower magnitude of effect when compared to more

severe psychopathology involving a DSM diagnosis of CD.

Furthermore, it should be noted that studies have mostly failed to take into account the

possible role of passive smoking during pregnancy and postnatal smoke exposure (42,43).

Gatzke-Kopp and Beauchaine reported associations of passive smoking during pregnancy

with increased offspring externalizing psychopathology including CD. These associations

were observed in women who did not smoke themselves and thereby indicated an effect of

passive smoking (43). After controlling for multiple confounding variables including

exposure to smoking after birth, Hutchinson et al. reported attenuated but still small effects

of maternal smoking during pregnancy, suggesting that postnatal exposure to smoking

explains only a limited part of the reported association between smoking during pregnancy

and offspring conduct problems (44). Thus, these studies indicate a true effect of exposure

to smoking during pregnancy.

Regarding possible underlying mechanisms, smoking during pregnancy might be related to

abnormal fetal development by inducing hypoxia, nutritional abnormalities, teratogenic

effects, and DNA-damage by exposure to the multitude of toxicants in cigarette smoke (45–

49). More specifically, it has been shown that early nicotine exposure in rodents, equivalent

to gestational exposure in humans, affects development of catecholaminergic and brainstem

autonomic nuclei as well as development of the neocortex, hippocampus and cerebellum.

These observations are supported by clinical data including increased incidence of

disruptive behavior and substance abuse during childhood (14,50). Moreover, another issue

of concern are the adverse effects of tobacco smoking on breastfeeding as many women who

(17)

smoke during pregnancy continue to do so after birth. In addition to decreased milk

production and a shorter lactation period, the composition of breast milk as well as the

response of the infant are also affected by maternal smoking, which poses a further health

risk for the infant (51).

Summarizing, we found an association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and

offspring conduct problems, which appears to be stronger in clinical cases of CD. While

hypothesized underlying biological mechanisms underlying effects of smoking are plausible,

the role of various types of confounding variables remains an important issue which needs

to be addressed by future studies.

Alcohol use during pregnancy

Our meta-analyses regarding prenatal ethanol exposure indicated elevated odds on

offspring conduct problems for mothers who consumed alcohol while pregnant.

Furthermore, while the number of studies was relatively low for subgroup analyses, we

found that effects were clearly higher in clinical samples. The studies assessing clinical

samples used diagnostic measures of CD, rather than screening questionnaires. Of further

interest, one study reported that exposure to alcohol during the first but not third trimester

is a risk factor for offspring conduct problems (52). Alcohol use was assessed during

multiple visits in pregnancy in this study and while social desirability might have affected

the reported exposures, it should not have affected trimester-specific reports differently.

Therefore, this finding may indicate more harmful influences of alcohol use early in

pregnancy, reflecting an embryological vulnerable period. Also, early exposure may be more

prevalent because future mothers might be still unaware of their pregnancy, whereas

alcohol consumption after the first gestational trimester decreased in most pregnant women

(53).

Ethanol is a notorious teratogenic substance. It is known to cross both the placenta and fetal

blood-brain barrier and may have detrimental effects on neural development (54–56). In

animal studies structural anomalies are observed in the cerebral cortex in particular (57),

yet also developmental alterations in other structures including the hippocampus and

corpus callosum have been reported (58–60). Human neuro-imaging studies revealed also

abnormalities in these brain regions (61–69) and it is theorized that abnormal hippocampal

and cortical information processing is a major contributing factor for clinical

neuropsychiatric symptoms (70–72). These include a pattern of behavioral disinhibitions

seen in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders, resulting at different ages in symptoms including

irritability, conduct problems, and delinquency (16).

Concluding, we report an effect of alcohol use during pregnancy on offspring conduct

problems. Similar to maternal smoking during pregnancy, this effect appears to be stronger

(18)

problems. Upon more close inspection of individual results one study reported a significant

overall association (22) and for another study significant sub effects for different raters and

gestational trimesters were reported, although we did not compute a significant overall

effect for that study (73). However, these results apply to unadjusted data. The only

available confounder-adjusted data did not reveal a significant association and, moreover, it

was suggested by the authors that their unadjusted results may reflect differences in

parental tolerance for problem behavior instead of true offspring behavioral differences

(22). Therefore a more independent rater such as the school teacher was suggested. A

further point of consideration raised by O’Connel et al. is the increase in potency of cannabis

preparations over the last decades (22). A particular increase in ∆

9

—tetrahydrocannabinol

(THC), the main psychoactive ingredient in cannabis, has been observed (74). In addition, a

general problem in isolating effects of cannabis is the strong association with use of other

intoxicating substances (75).

Concerning potential biological effects, it is known that cannabinoids cross the placenta (76)

and cannabis use during pregnancy may affect offspring neurocognitive development (19–

22). THC has been shown to disrupt endocannabinoid signaling, resulting in altered fetal

cortical wiring (77) and two large prospective cohort studies showed affected executive

functioning in heavily exposed offspring (78), however, no difference in IQ (79).

Summarizing, we found no overall effect of cannabis use during pregnancy on offspring

conduct problems, however, we suggest that further research is needed considering the

scarcity and somewhat conflicting nature of current evidence.

Caffeine use during pregnancy

The only identified study investigating maternal caffeine intake during pregnancy reported

no association with offspring conduct problems (25). The study measured different doses of

caffeine intake prospectively around the 16

th

week of gestation and controlled for pre-, peri-,

and postnatal confounders. Caffeine (1,3,7-trimethylxanthine) acts primarily as an

adenosine receptor antagonist and stimulates the central nervous system. Neural and

cardiovascular teratogenic effects following prenatal caffeine exposure have been reported

in animal studies (80–84). Moreover, a study by Silva et al., hypothesized that adenosine

antagonism may affect cognitive functioning in mice (85).

To summarize, we identified one study which did not report an effect of caffeine intake

during pregnancy on offspring conduct problems. Although no effects were suggested by

this single study, more studies are probably needed to further investigate potential effects

of caffeine use during pregnancy.

Considerations in current research

In the light of our present results, it is essential to discuss a number of pitfalls emerging from

current research on the role of the prenatal environment in multifactorial behavioral

disorders such as CD. The most important issue is that maternal substance use during

pregnancy does not necessarily exert a causal influence on offspring behavior, but may

merely represent an association, confounded by shared genetic and environmental factors

(39,40).

(19)

Genetic confounding

A key issue when investigating the prenatal environment is the possibility that reported

statistical associations are substantially confounded by the maternal genome. More

specifically, this means that the genetic make-up of the mother predisposes for both the

environmental factors (maternal substance use during pregnancy) and the outcome

(offspring conduct problems) (40). Classic approaches to disentangle genetic and

environmental influences use twin study designs, for example twins who grew up in

different environments due to adoption (86). However, to more sophistically test the

hypothesis of genetic confounding for maternal substance use during pregnancy, it would

be necessary to implement a study design in which the prenatal environment was provided

by both genetically related and unrelated mothers. An example of such a design includes the

use of in vitro fertilization (IVF) techniques and surrogacy pregnancies (87,88). A study by

Rice et al. using medical records of children born through IVF (the Cardiff IVF sample)

reported only associations between smoking during pregnancy and offspring antisocial

behavior in genetically related mothers and offspring, thereby providing evidence for

genetic mediation in this association. Cell sizes were, however, unbalanced due to a

relatively low number of mothers who reported smoking during pregnancy (87). Another,

perhaps more feasible approach, may be to correct for polygenic risk scores for CD, although

polygenic risk scores still only capture a limited amount of phenotypic heritability (89,90).

With regard to future studies, the use of genetically sensitive designs is essential, as this will

eventually allow us to gain more insight into the true nature of observed statistical

relationships regarding prenatal adversities and offspring behavioral problems.

Gene x environment interactions

Genetic factors may interact with influences from the environment and subsequently

predispose an individual to a certain condition, also referred to as gene x environment (G x

E) interactions (91,92). Wakschlag et al. reported a sex-specific G x E interaction for a

functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene and smoking during

pregnancy for youth antisocial behavior.(93) Another study by O’Brien et al. reported also

sex-specific patterns for a genetic marker in the dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene and

smoking during pregnancy for externalizing behavior (94). Sex-specific G x E interaction

patterns may reflect effects of the genotype on sex-specific prenatal brain development (95).

No gene-environment correlations (rGE) were reported by both studies (93,94). One would

expect rGE if the genetic factor would be responsible for both the environmental factor and

outcome, as discussed in the previous paragraph.

(20)

factors such as socioeconomic status, parental psychopathology, and parenting style,

significant confounding likely also comes from unmeasured familial factors (39,40,86),

which are not being taken into account by the propensity scoring. Two studies addressed

this issue by using a sibling-matched design and reported no link between maternal smoking

and offspring behavior when comparing siblings with different prenatal exposure (39,40).

Similar results were reported in more recent studies using sibling-matched designs to

investigate associations between smoking during pregnancy and other offspring

psychopathology such as ADHD or schizophrenia in very large population samples (99,100).

Given that aforementioned studies investigated well-powered general population samples

this could imply that our current results may be explained by familial confounding, resulting

from both shared social environmental factors and genetic factors (as discussed previously).

Further pregnancy factors

In addition to substance use, other maternal factors during pregnancy such as medication

use, somatic health issues and anxiety could also contribute to offspring risk for CD related

problems (101–103), and might also be related to unhealthy maternal behaviors such as

smoking (104). It would therefore be important for future studies to concurrently consider

a broader range of factors during pregnancy to adjust for potential confounding effects. Only

a few studies adjusted for some form of medication use or life events stressors during

pregnancy, which could imply a slight overestimation of present results.

Furthermore, most studies in our meta-analyses did not control for potential effects of

maternal parity or birth order of the child. While literature findings show that risk for

emotional disorders and attention problems appears to be higher for the first child, no such

effect has been observed for conduct disorders (105,106). Regarding substance use during

pregnancy, both maternal smoking and alcohol consumption during pregnancy have been

associated with grand multiparity (more than five pregnancies), which itself is also related

to a number of pregnancy complications (107). Such highly multiparous pregnancies

constitute, however, only a minority of all births in developed countries (108). Therefore,

we conclude that potential confounding effects of maternal parity in the relationship

between substance use during pregnancy and offspring conduct problems are likely to be

minor.

Comorbidity among behavioral disorders

As already covered in the discussion about smoking during pregnancy, high rates of

comorbidity among behavioral disorders may constitute another general, important issue

and reflect a more fundamental problem of psychiatric classification in general. Descriptions

of mental illnesses are merely based on clinical symptomatology and therefore do not

necessarily infer etiologically separate entities. This may imply a common underlying

etiology among a broader spectrum of disruptive behavioral disorders, as suggested by a

case-control study on ADHD, that reported substantial comorbidity among ADHD, ODD, and

CD (42). Two other studies implied a common underlying etiology for CD and ODD

(109,110). Furthermore, assessing comorbid disruptive behavior can, in addition to

revealing more generic effects, also aid in investigating whether or not observed

(21)

associations with CD may be better explained by mediation effects, for example due to ADHD

(111–114). Goldschmidt et al. reported that inattention symptoms mediated the association

between maternal cannabis use during pregnancy and offspring delinquency (73). However,

three other studies that investigated maternal smoking during pregnancy did not report

such effects (31,115,116). Unfortunately, in general, most studies did not assess comorbid

disruptive behavior in the offspring. Therefore, to summarize, present meta-analytic results

are not yet conclusive with regard to the specificity of effects of maternal substance use on

CD only versus their role within the broader domain of disruptive behavioral disorders.

Methodological considerations of this study

Given limited time and resources we had to restrict our search parameters to CD specific

outcome measures, and thereby excluded potentially relevant and related constructs such

as delinquency, aggression, affective dysregulation and the broader domain of externalizing

problems. Also of note is that the prevalence of CD is higher in males than females, which

may lead to an overrepresentation of boys in clinical samples (1,4). While in the present

selection of studies most samples covered both sexes, males appeared to dominate in

general in our meta-analytical sample. Sex-specific effects were not analyzed due to

insufficient available data. Further considerations include the onset-age of conduct

problems. It has been suggested that an early onset-age is predictive for the persistence of

conduct problems (1), and recent research also suggested different correlates for a different

age of onset (117). We were unable to analyze the role of the age of onset given the lack of

data. Moreover, while CD has been suggested to be strongly related to instrumental

aggression and callous-unemotional traits (118), subphenotypes of aggression were also not

available and should be included in future studies (1–3).

Most studies assessed maternal substance use using retrospective methods, which are

subject to recall bias, thereby potentially causing additional inaccuracy and/or error of

exposure measurement. Although recall delays were reported up to multiple years, previous

studies have indicated satisfactory long-term recall of maternal smoking (119). Another

potential issue with self-reports of pregnancy substance use is social desirability bias, which

may even occur in computerized surveys (120), potentially resulting in underestimates of

exposure. A possible solution could be the inclusion of biomarkers in exposure

measurement, such as maternal or fetal cotinine levels (121). Furthermore, the outcome

measure was based on both diagnostic psychiatric interviews and parental or self-report

questionnaires such as the child behavior checklist (CBCL) (36,37) and SDQ (35). Although

the SDQ has shown high sensitivity and specificity for detecting CD (122), it contains only a

limited number of items for conduct problems, and it probably captures the broader domain

of CD and ODD. Further of note is the inclusion of samples, whose subjects were preselected

(22)

clinical samples, and within general population samples one particular study ((39))

explained about 25% of heterogeneity. This study of Gilman et al. is a good quality study

concerning a very large (50,000+) total sample size, which makes it of considerable value.

However, the outcome measure used to assess CD symptoms was self-constructed and also

screened for more broadly defined disruptive behavior. We suspect that the use of this

outcome measure might be a possible explanation for the heterogeneity caused by this

study. Furthermore, remaining, unexplained heterogeneity is still considerable (about

70%), and we suspect that unexplored factors such as differences in control variables,

sex-differences, or exposure ascertainment methods at least partially explain the remaining

inconsistency. Because we were not able to further investigate these factors we downgraded

our strength of evidence assessment. However, as all effects of the individual studies were

in the same direction (with by far most effects also being significant), and summary

estimates were not affected by low quality studies, we consider our results of sufficient

robustness and meaningful as an average effect estimate.

Furthermore, we analyzed effect sizes based on multivariable statistical models with the

largest number of confounding variables (27). As mentioned before, it was, however, not

possible to adjust for individual differences in number or types of confounding variables. Of

further consideration is that some potentially important results may have been excluded

because the respective studies did not provide sufficient data for our meta-analyses. In

addition, except for cigarette smoking during pregnancy, a more general issue was the low

availability of studies, resulting in limited power and sample representability for

meta-analytic data synthesis, and highlighting the need for more studies (28). Finally, for a

number of studies it was necessary to combine multiple subgroups to obtain an overall

estimate. It is important to recall that the subgroups represent in fact adjusted data for a

particular moderating variable, such as sex or age. By combining these effects we computed

an approximation of a general effect, which potentially could have slightly affected the

accuracy of our results (28).

Conclusion

This study provides evidence for a link between both maternal cigarette smoking and

alcohol use during pregnancy, and offspring conduct disorder problems. We did not

encounter clear effects of cannabis use and caffeine intake during pregnancy. However, it

must be emphasized that the paucity of studies does not yet allow for drawing firm

conclusions. Future research needs to address important unresolved issues concerning

confounding by shared genetic and socio-environmental factors, the role of possible G x E

interactions, and comorbidity among behavioral disorders to advance our understanding of

true effects of maternal substance use during pregnancy on offspring conduct problems.

(23)

References

1. Olsson M. DSM diagnosis of conduct disorder (CD)--a review. Nord J Psychiatry. 2009;63(Cd):102–12.

2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

3. Blair RJR, Leibenluft E, Pine DS. Conduct Disorder and Callous–Unemotional Traits in Youth. N Engl J Med. 2014;371:2207–16. 4. Martin A, Volkmar FR, editors. Lewis’s child and adolescent psychiatry: a comprehensive textbook. 4th ed. Wolters Kluwer: Lippincott

Williams & Wilkins; 2007.

5. Black D. The Natural History of Antisocial Personality Disorder. Can J Psychiatr. 2015;60(7):309–14.

6. Robins L. Epidemiology of antisocial personality disorder. In: Michels R, CavenarJO, editors. Psychiatry. Volume 3. Philadelphia (PA): Lippincott; 1987. p. 1–14.

7. Olsson M, Hansson K, Cederblad M. A long-term follow-up of conduct disorder adolescents into adulthood. Nord J Psychiatry. 2006;60(6):469–79.

8. Kim DR, Bale TL, Epperson CN. Prenatal Programming of Mental Illness: Current Understanding of Relationship and Mechanisms. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2015;17(2):5.

9. Lamy S, Thibaut F. Psychoactive substance use during pregnancy: a review [article in French]. Encephale. 2010;36(1):33–8. 10. National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion; Office on Smoking and Health (U.S.). How Tobacco Smoke

Causes Disease: The Biology and Behavioral Basis for Smoking-Attributable Disease: A Report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta (GA): Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (US); 2010. Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/

11. Rodgman A, Perfetti TA, editors. The Chemical Components of Tobacco and Tobacco Smoke. 1st ed. CRC Press, Taylor & Francis Group; 2009.

12. Honein MA, Paulozzi LJ, Watkins ML. Maternal smoking and birth defects: validity of birth certificate data for effect estimation. Public Health Rep. 2001;116(4):327–55.

13. Haustein K. Cigarette smoking, nicotine and pregnancy. Int J Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1999;37(9):417–27.

14. Wakschlag LS, Pickett KE, Cook Jr E, Benowitz NL, Leventhal BL. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and severe antisocial behavior in offspring: a review. Am J Public Health. 2002 Jun;92(6):966–74.

15. Kumar P, Clark M, editors. Clinical Medicine. 6th ed. Elsevier Saunders; 2005.

16. Dörrie N, Föcker M, Freunscht I, Hebebrand J. Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23(10):863–75. 17. United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. 2006 World Drug Report, Volume 1: Analysis. United Nations Publications; 2006. 18. National Institue on Drug Abuse (U.S.). National pregnancy and health survey. Rockville, MD; 1996. Available from:

https://www.drugabuse.gov/

19. Day N, Richardson G, Goldschmidt L, Robles N, Taylor P, Stoffer D, et al. Effect of prenatal marijuana exposure on the cognitive development of offspring at age three. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1994;16(2):169–75.

20. Fried P, Watkinson B. 36- and 48-month neurobehavioral follow-up of children prenatally exposed to marijuana, cigarettes, and alcohol. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1990;11(2):49–58.

21. Fried P, Watkinson B, Gray R. A follow-up study of attentional behavior in 6-year-old children exposed prenatally to marihuana, cigarettes, and alcohol. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1992;14(5):299–311.

22. O’Connell CM, Fried PA. Prenatal exposure to cannabis: A preliminary report of postnatal consequences in school age children. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1991 Nov;13(6):631–9.

23. Frary C, Johnson R, Wang M. Food sources and intakes of caffeine in the diets of persons in the United States. J Am Diet Assoc. 2005;105(1):110–3.

24. Kaiser L, Allen L. Position of the American Dietetic Association: nutrition and lifestyle for a healthy pregnancy outcome. J Am Diet Assoc. 2008;108(3):553–61.

25. Loomans EM, Hofland L, Van der Stelt O, Van der Wal MF, Koot HM, Van den Bergh BR, et al. Caffeine intake during pregnancy and risk of problem behavior in 5- to 6-year-old children. Pediatrics. 2012 Jan;130(2):e305–13.

26. Wells G, Shea B, O’Connell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. The Ottawa Hospital - Research Institute. Available from: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp

27. Higgins JPT, Green S, editors. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (online version). 5.1.0. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2011.

28. Borenstein M, Hedges L V., Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR, editors. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. 1st ed. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2009. 29. DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177–88.

30. The Mathworks Inc. MATLAB and Statistics Toolbox Release 2014a. Natick, Massachusetts, United States: The Mathworks, Inc.; 2014. 31. Maughan B, Taylor C, Taylor A, Butler N, Bynner J. Pregnancy smoking and childhood conduct problems: A causal association? J Child

Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(8):1021–8.

32. Schunemann H, Brozek J, Oxman A. GRADE handbook for grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendation. Version 3. The Grade Working Group; 2009. Available from: http://www.gradeworkinggroup.org/index.html

33. Shaffer D, Fisher P, Dulcan MK, Davies M, Piacentini J, Schwab-Stone ME, et al. The NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version 2.3 (DISC-2.3): description, acceptability, prevalence rates, and performance in the MECA Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35(7):865–77.

34. Orvaschel H. Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children Epidemiologic Version. 5th editio. Lauderdale: Nova Southeastern University, Center for Psychological Studies; 1994.

35. Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1337–45.

36. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991.

(24)

44. Hutchinson J, Pickett KE, Green J, Wakschlag LS. Smoking in pregnancy and disruptive behaviour in 3-year-old boys and girls: An analysis of the UK millennium cohort study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2010;64(1):82–8.

45. Chernoff N. Teratogenic effects of cadmium in rats. Teratology. 1973;8(1):29–32.

46. Mochizuki M, Maruo T, Masuko K, Ohtsu T. Effects of smoking on fetoplacental-maternal system during pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1984;149(4):413–20.

47. Lammer EJ, Shaw GM, Iovannisci DM, Van Waes J, Finell RH. Maternal smoking and the risk of orofacial clefts: susceptibility with NAT1 and NAT2 polymorphisms. Epidemiology. 2004;15(2):150–6.

48. Munger RG, Sauberlich HE, Corcoran C, Nepomuceno B, Baack-Hirsch S, Solon FS. Maternal vitamin B-6 and folate status and risk of oral cleft birth defects in the Philippines. Birth Defects Res Part A, Clin Mol Teratol. 2004;70(7):464–71.

49. Ziaei S, Nouri K, Kazemnejad A. Effects of carbon monoxide air pollution in pregnancy on neonatal nucleated red blood cells. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2005;19(1):27–30.

50. Dwyer J, McQuown S, Leslie F. The dynamic effects of nicotine on the developing brain. Pharmacol Ther. 2009;122(2):125–39. 51. Napierala M, Mazela J, Merritt TA, Florek E. Tobacco smoking and breastfeeding : Effect on the lactation process , breast milk

composition and infant development . A critical review. Environ Res. 2016;151:321–38.

52. Larkby CA, Goldschmidt L, Hanusa BH, Day NL. Prenatal alcohol exposure is associated with conduct disorder in adolescence: Findings from a birth cohort. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(3):262–71.

53. Hill SY, Lowers L, Locke-Wellman J, Shen S. Maternal smoking and drinking during pregnancy and the risk for child and adolescent psychiatric disorders. J Stud Alcohol. 2000;61(5):661–8.

54. Taléns-Visconti R, Sanchez-Vera I, Kostic J, Perez-Arago MA, Erceg S, Stojkovic M, et al. Neural differentiation from human embryonic stem cells as a tool to study early brain development and the neuroteratogenic effects of ethanol. Stem cells Dev. 2010;20(2):327– 39.

55. Guizzetti M, Zhang X, Goeke C, Gavin DP. Glia and neurodevelopment: focus on fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Front Pediatr. 2014;11(2):123.

56. Rubert G, Miñana R, Pascual M, Guerri C. Ethanol exposure during embryogenesis decreases the radial glial progenitorpool and affects the generation of neurons and astrocytes. J Neurosci Res. 2006;84(3):483–96.

57. Abbott CW, Kozanian OO, Kanaan J, Wendel KM, Huffman KJ. The Impact of Prenatal Ethanol Exposure on Neuroanatomical and Behavioral Development in Mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2016;40(1):122–33.

58. Livy D, Elberger A. Alcohol exposure during the first two trimesters- equivalent alters the development of corpus callosum projection neurons in the rat. Alcohol. 2008;42:285–93.

59. Livy D, Miller E, Maier S, West J. Fetal alcohol exposure and temporal vulnerability: effects of binge-like alcohol exposure on the developing rat hippocampus. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2003;25:447–58.

60. Maier S, West J. Regional differences in cell loss associated with bing-like alcohol exposure during the first two trimesters equivalent in the rat. Alcohol. 2001;23:49–57.

61. Bookstein F, Sampson P, Connor P, Streissguth A. Midline corpus callosum is a neuroanatomical focus of fetal alcohol damage. Anat Rec. 2002;269(3):162–74.

62. Bookstein F, Connor P, Huggins J, Barr H, Pimentel K, Streissguth A. Many infants prenatally exposed to high levels of alcohol show one particular anomaly of the corpus callosum. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31(5):868–79.

63. Mattson S, Riley E, Sowell E, Jernigan T, Sobel D, Jones K. A decrease in the size of the basal ganglia in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20(6):1088–93.

64. Archibald S, Fennema-Notestine C, Gamst A, Riley E, Mattson S, Jernigan T. Brain dysmorphology in individuals with severe prenatal alcohol exposure. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43(3):148–54.

65. Riikonen R, Salonen I, Partanen K, Verho S. Brain perfusion SPECT and MRI in foetal alcohol syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1999;41:652–9.

66. Sowell E, Thompson P, Mattson S, Tessner K, Jernigan T, Riley E, et al. Regional brain shape abnormalities persist into adolescence after heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Cereb cortex. 2002;12(8):865–65.

67. Sowell E, Thompson P, Peterson B, Mattson S, Welcome S, Henkenius A, et al. Mapping cortical gray matter asymmetry patterns in adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Neuroimage. 2002;17(4):1807–19.

68. Zhou D, Lebel C, Lepage C, Rsmussen C, Evans A, Wyper K, et al. Developmental cortical thinning in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Neuroimage. 2011;58(16–25).

69. Yang S, Fombonne E, Kramer MS. Duration of gestation, size at birth and later childhood behaviour. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2011 Jul;25(4):377–87.

70. El Shawa H, Abbott C, Huffman K. Prenatal ethanol exposure disrupts intraneocortical circuitry, cortical gene expression, and behavior in a mouse model of FASD. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2013;33(48):18893–905.

71. Granato A. Altered organization of cortical interneurons in rats exposed to ethanol during neonatal life. Brain Res. 2006;1069(1):23– 30.

72. Streissguth A, Barr H, Sampson P, Bookstein F. Prenatal alcohol and offspring development: the first fourteen years. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1994;36(2):89–99.

73. Goldschmidt L, Day NL, Richardson GA. Effects of prenatal marijuana exposure on child behavior problems at age 10. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22(3):325–36.

74. Trimbos Instituut. THC, CBD en gezondheidseffecten van wiet en hasj: recente inzichten. april 2012. Niesink R, Van Laar M, editors. Trimbos Instituut; 2012. Available from: http://www.trimbos.nl

75. Hill M, Reed K. Pregnancy, Breast-feeding, and Marijuana: A Review Article. Obstet Gynecol Surv. 2013;68(10):710–8.

76. Tennes K, Avitable N, Blackard C, Boyles C, Hassoun B, Holmes L, et al. Marijuana: prenatal and postnatal exposure in the human. NIDA Res Monogr. 1985;59:48–60.

77. Tortoriello G, Morris C V, Alpar A, Fuzik J, Shirran SL, Keimpema E, et al. Miswiring the brain: Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol disrupts cortical development by inducing an SCG10/stathmin-2 degradation pathway. EMBO J. 2014;33(7):668–85.

78. Fried P, Watkinson B. Differential effects on facets of attention in adolescents prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2001;23(5):421–30.

79. Fried P, Watkinson B, Gray R. Differential effects on cognitive functioning in 9- to 12-year olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1998;20(3):293–306.

80. Bruyere H, Michaud B, Gilbert E, Folts J. The effects of cardioteratogenic doses of caffeine on cardiac function in the 3-day chick embryo. J Appl Toxicol. 1987;7(3):197–203.

81. Ma Z, Qin Y, Wang G, Li X, He R, Chuai M, et al. Exploring the caffeine-induced teratogenicity on neurodevelopment using early chick embryo. PLoS One. 2012;7(3):e34278.

82. Ma Z, Wang G, Cheng X, Chuai M, Kurihara H, Lee K, et al. Excess caffeine exposure impairs eye development during chick embryogenesis. J Cell Mol Med. 2014;18(6):1134–43.

83. Nikoui V, Ostadhadi S, Kazare N, Nabavi S, Giorgi M, Bakhtiarian A. The role of clomipramine in potentiating the teratogenic effects of caffeine in pregnant rats: a histopathological study. ScientificWorldJournal. 2013;2013:382434.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Vanuit observationele studies zijn er aanwijzingen voor een beperkt effect van alfa 1 -proteïnaseremmer op de progressie van de luchtwegobstructie en op de mortaliteit

Chapter 5 | Genome-wide study of gene-based interactions with four environmental risk factors for disruptive behavior in children and adolescents 99 Chapter 6 | Aggression

In addition to extensive phenotypic (including different measures of ADHD symptomatology and disruptive behavior comorbidity) and environmental data (including pregnancy- related

We selected those pregnancy related risk factors from the ALSPAC database that had been linked in the literature to disruptive behavior and/or more general neurodevelopment:

The GxE between rs11215217-TC and maltreatment only reached nominal significance after adjustment for comorbid ADHD symptoms (P=0.0074, females). GxE: gene by environment

6 | Aggression based genome-wide, glutamatergic, dopaminergic and neuroendocrine polygenic risk scores predict callous-unemotional traits Results.. Descriptive statistics

In Chapter 4, using data from ALSPAC, I investigated GxE’s at the genetic variant level, using SNPs implicated through sex-stratified GWASs of antisocial behavior, and

Na het corrigeren van de effecten van meerdere omgevingsfactoren voor elkaar, het corrigeren voor genetische risicofactoren voor disruptief gedrag, en het corrigeren voor