• No results found

Reward and Punishment Sensitivity are Associated with Cross-disorder Traits

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Reward and Punishment Sensitivity are Associated with Cross-disorder Traits"

Copied!
10
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

University of Groningen

Reward and Punishment Sensitivity are Associated with Cross-disorder Traits

Portengen, Christel M; Sprooten, Emma; Zwiers, Marcel P; Hoekstra, Pieter J; Dietrich,

Andrea; Holz, Nathalie E; Aggensteiner, Pascal M; Banaschewski, Tobias; Schulze, Ulrike M

E; Saam, Melanie C

Published in:

Psychiatry Research

DOI:

10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113795

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date:

2021

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Portengen, C. M., Sprooten, E., Zwiers, M. P., Hoekstra, P. J., Dietrich, A., Holz, N. E., Aggensteiner, P.

M., Banaschewski, T., Schulze, U. M. E., Saam, M. C., Craig, M. C., Sethi, A., Santosh, P., Ouriaghli, I. S.,

Castro-Fornieles, J., Rosa, M., Arango, C., Penzol, M. J., Werhahn, J. E., ... Naaijen, J. (2021). Reward

and Punishment Sensitivity are Associated with Cross-disorder Traits. Psychiatry Research, 298, [113795].

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113795

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

Psychiatry Research 298 (2021) 113795

Available online 8 February 2021

0165-1781/© 2021 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

Reward and Punishment Sensitivity are Associated with

Cross-disorder Traits

Christel M. Portengen

1,2,3,*

, Emma Sprooten

1,2

, Marcel P. Zwiers

2

, Pieter J. Hoekstra

4

,

Andrea Dietrich

4

, Nathalie E. Holz

5

, Pascal M. Aggensteiner

5

, Tobias Banaschewski

5

, Ulrike M.

E. Schulze

6,7

, Melanie C. Saam

6

, Michael C. Craig

8

, Arjun Sethi

8

, Paramala Santosh

9,10

, Ilyas

Sagar Ouriaghli

9

, Josefina Castro-Fornieles

11

, Mireia Rosa

11

, Celso Arango

12

, María

Jos´e Penzol

12,13

, Julia E. Werhahn

14

, Daniel Brandeis

5,14,15,16

, Susanne Walitza

14,16

,

Marianne Oldehinkel

1,2,17

, Barbara Franke

18,19

, Jan K. Buitelaar

1,2,20

, Jilly Naaijen

1,2

1Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

2Radboud University, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Nijmegen, The Netherlands 3Department of Child and Adolescent Studies, Clinical Child and Family Studies, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands

4University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Hanzeplein 1 XA10, 9713 GZ Groningen, The

Netherlands

5Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/ Heidelberg University, Mannheim,

Germany

6Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry / Psychotherapy, University of Ulm, Germany 7Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Centre for Psychiatry Calw, B¨oblingen, Germany

8Department of Forensic & Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom 9Department of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom 10Centre for Interventional Paediatric Psychopharmacology and Rare Diseases (CIPPRD), National and Specialist Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services,

Maudsley Hospital, London, United Kingdom

11Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology, Clínic Institute of Neurosciences, Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, 2017SGR881, IDIBAPS, CIBERSAM,

University of Barcelona, Spain

12Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Mara˜n´on School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, IiSGM, CIBERSAM,

Madrid, Spain

13Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Mara˜n´on, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, IiSGM, CIBERSAM,

Madrid, Spain

14Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Switzerland 15Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich and ETH Zurich (ZNZ), Switzerland

16Zurich Center for Integrative Human Physiology (ZHIP), University of Zurich, Switzerland

17Brain & Mental Health Laboratory, Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences and School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Victoria,

Australia

18Radboud University Medical Center, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Department of Human Genetics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands 19Radboud University Medical Center, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Department of Psychiatry, Nijmegen, The Netherlands 20Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

A R T I C L E I N F O

Keywords:

Callous-unemotional traits oppositional defiant disorder attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder reversal learning

reward and punishment sensitivity

A B S T R A C T

Reversal learning deficits following reward and punishment processing are observed across disruptive behaviors (DB) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and have been associated with callous-unemotional (CU) traits. However, it remains unknown to what extent these altered reinforcement sensitivities are linked to the co-occurrence of oppositional traits, ADHD symptoms, and CU traits. Reward and punishment sensitivity and perseverative behavior were therefore derived from a probabilistic reversal learning task to investigate reinforcement sensitivity in participants with DB (n=183, ODD=62, CD=10, combined=57, age-range 8-18), ADHD (n=144, age-range 11-28), and controls (n=191, age-range 8-26). The SNAP-IV and Conners rating scales were used to assess oppositional and ADHD traits. The Inventory of CU traits was used to assess CU traits.

* Corresponding author: Department of Clinical Child and Family Studies. Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands.

E-mail address: c.m.portengen@uu.nl (C.M. Portengen).

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113795

(3)

Psychiatry Research 298 (2021) 113795

2

Decreased reward sensitivity was associated with ADHD symptom severity (p=0.018) if corrected for opposi-tional symptoms. ADHD symptomatology interacted with opposiopposi-tional behavior on perseveration (p=0.019), with the former aggravating the effect of oppositional behavior on perseveration and vice versa. Within a pooled sample, reversal learning alterations were associated with the severity of ADHD symptoms, underpinned by hyposensitivity to reward and increased perseveration. These results show ADHD traits, as opposed to opposi-tional behavior and CU traits, is associated with decreased reward-based learning in adolescents and adults.

Reversal learning deficits based on altered reward sensitivity are hallmarks of disruptive behavior (DB) and attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Blair, Leibenluft, & Pine, 2014). How-ever, it remains unknown to what extent these altered reinforcement sensitivities are linked to the co-occurrence of oppositional behavior, ADHD symptomatology, and/or the presence of callous-unemotional (CU) traits. Additionally, literature on punishment sensitivity is limited. Reversal learning is a form of conditional associative learning in which individuals are required to adapt their behavior according to changes in stimulus-reward contingencies (Cools, Clark, Owen, & Rob-bins, 2002). Altered reinforcement sensitivity can lead to insufficient learning, resulting in behavior that cannot be adequately adjusted to changing environmental contingencies (Barkley, 1997). Most studies assessing reversal learning have used the amount of reversal errors as a measure of altered reinforcement sensitivity, associating more reversal errors to decreased reinforcement sensitivity. However, solely measuring the amount of reversal errors does not capture the differential effects of reward and punishment (Xue et al., 2013), nor does it explain which strategies are applied that result in increased reversal errors. Both reward and punishment sensitivity are often altered in individuals with DB (Byrd, Loeber, & Pardini, 2014; O’Brien & Frick, 1996) and/or ADHD (Furukawa et al., 2018; Ibanez et al., 2012; Li, 2018; Plichta & Scheres, 2014). Therefore, the current study aimed to examine reward and punishment sensitivity and perseverative behavior in relation to co-occurring traits within the externalizing cluster of disorders; more specifically, ADHD symptomatology, oppositional behavior, and CU traits.

Disruptive behavior is a hallmark of conduct disorder (CD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). Children diagnosed with CD violate basic rights of others, or otherwise disregard age-appropriate societal norms or rules (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). ODD is characterized by hostile, negative and defiant behavior towards au-thority, or irritable and disobedient attitudes towards others. Both dis-orders often occur together with ADHD, a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by attention-deficits and/or hyperactivity and impulsivity (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). One overlapping dimension that is present in both ADHD and DB is CU traits (lack of remorse/em-pathy, shallow, constricted affect, and limited prosocial emotions;

American Psychiatric Association, 2013); individuals with high levels of CU traits tend to have more severe, persistent, and more heritable antisocial behavior (Viding, Jones, Paul, Moffitt, & Plomin, 2008) and early criminal behavior (Frick & Dickens, 2006). With an overlap in symptoms and high comorbidity rates among this spectrum of disorders, researchers are increasingly moving away from categorical classifica-tions towards dimensional analyses of symptoms and disease-relevant traits (Herpers, Rommelse, Bons, Buitelaar, & Scheepers, 2012).

The few studies that assessed the relations between symptom di-mensions and reinforcement sensitivity have shown rather consistent results. Where oppositional behavior has been associated with hypo-sensitivity to reward (Matthys, Vanderschuren, Schutter, & Lochman, 2012), impulsivity relates to hypersensitivity to immediate reward and hyposensitivity to punishment (Plichta & Scheres, 2014). ADHD symp-toms in children were additionally related to increased sensitivity to both reward and punishment, depending on their parents’ reported parenting strategies (Li, 2018). Hyposensitivity to punishment has also been found in children with high levels of CU traits (Viding, Fontaine, & McCrory, 2012), which is proposed to lead to insufficient learning from

negative reinforcement and consequentially to disruptive and impulsive behavior (Byrd et al., 2014). With regards to perseverative behavior, it appears that response perseveration is present in both CD (Daughterty & Quay, 1991) and ODD (Matthys, van Goozen, Snoek, & van Engeland, 2004), and is associated with reward sensitivity in CD but punishment sensitivity in ODD. Thus, inconsistencies across studies are reported, possibly reflecting the complexity of reinforcement sensitivity across these symptom dimensions but may as well be due to different experi-mental paradigms that assessed reinforcement sensitivities separately. Also, previous studies have often focused on either reward processing or perseverance, but punishment processing has been largely overlooked. With studying developmental disorders, an important factor to consider is age. Age is often corrected for when studying (neuro) developmental disorders, yet it has been argued that ADHD is repre-sented by a delay in cortical maturation (Shaw et al., 2007). Whilst ADHD is presented as a lifelong neurodevelopmental disorder, hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms tend to decline with age in both clinical (Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, 2000; Mick, Faraone, & Bieder-man, 2004) and non-clinical samples (D¨opfner et al., 2015). Moreover, overall externalizing behavior tends to decrease with increasing age (Bongers, Koot, Van Der Ende, & Verhulst, 2004). Recent multimodal evidence showed age-dependent brain alterations in frontal regions within multiple networks, including those that have been frequently associated with ADHD symptoms (Wu et al., 2019). Additionally, meta-analytic evidence showed age-related effects when assessing in-hibition in ADHD (Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols, & Rubia, 2013). This study therefore additionally assessed the role of age in reinforce-ment sensitivity across clinical populations.

In the current study we aimed to assess the dimensional relationships between probabilistic reversal learning (PRL) response strategies and ADHD symptom severity, oppositional behavior, and CU traits in a sample of children, adolescents, and young adults with and without DB and ADHD, coming from two large (international) cohorts, NeuroIMAGE II and Aggressotype/MATRICS. Based on previous studies, expectations were that ADHD symptom severity and CU traits would be associated with increased reward and decreased punishment sensitivity (Ibanez et al., 2012). Oppositional behavior was expected to be associated with both decreased punishment sensitivity and decreased reward sensitivity (Matthys et al., 2012). Lastly, oppositional behavior and CU traits was expected to additionally relate to elevated perseverative behavior (Daughterty & Quay, 1991; Matthys et al., 2004; Byrd et al., 2014). We expected most of these relations to decline with increasing age, except the relations with ADHD symptom severity, because of the stable tra-jectory of inattentive symptomatology.

Methods

Participants

The original sample consisted of two clinical groups and healthy controls from two separate studies: children and adolescents aged 7-18 years with DB (n = 183), ADHD-diagnosed participants aged 11-28 years (n = 144), and healthy controls (n = 191) aged 8-26 years. Written consent was obtained from every participant above 18, from both parent and participant for adolescents aged 12-18, and from parents for chil-dren under the age of 12; chilchil-dren themselves gave oral assent. Data from participants with DB and from 95 healthy controls were collected

(4)

as part of the multi-center MATRICS/Aggressotype study (www. matrics-project.eu and www.aggressotype.eu), conducted among nine different research centers across Europe. Inclusion criteria for the DB sample were a CD or ODD diagnosis based on DSM-5 criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and/or presence of aggression in the clinical range (T > 70 on the aggression or delinquency subscale measured by the Youth Self Report [YSR], the Teacher Report Form [TRF], or the parent-reported Child Behavior Checklist [CBCL];

Achenbach, 1999), and being of Caucasian descent. Exclusion criteria for the MATRICS/Aggressotype study were an IQ below 80 as estimated with subtests (block design, vocabulary, picture completion and simi-larities) from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (WISC/WAIS; Wechsler, 2002), contra-indications for magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan-ning, and a primary DSM-5 diagnosis of psychosis, bipolar disorder, depression, or anxiety. Participants were included as healthy controls if they had no primary diagnosis of any DSM-5 Axis I disorder or any psychiatric diagnosis in their relatives, nor did they have clinically elevated levels of symptoms on any of the rating scales. Both medicated and unmedicated participants entered the study, and medicated par-ticipants were required to be on a stable dose of medication for two weeks. If participants were on medication during testing, interviews and questionnaires were based on (assumed) behavior and interference in daily life without medication.

Data from all participants with ADHD and the remaining 96 partic-ipants in the control group were collected in the NeuroIMAGE2 study, a follow up study of the NeuroIMAGE cohort of Caucasian families with probands with ADHD, their (un)affected siblings, and healthy controls (von Rhein et al., 2015). Unaffected siblings were excluded in this study but affected siblings remained as cases. Participants had an ADHD diagnosis if they had ≥ 6 inattentive and/or hyperactive-impulsive symptoms below the age of 18 years, or ≥ 5 symptoms for partici-pants older than 18 years on the Conners rating scales. Additionally, they had to meet the DSM criteria for pervasiveness and impairment and an age of onset before 12 years, as determined by diagnostic interviews held by trained researchers under the supervision of a clinician and trained child and adolescent psychiatrist. Exclusion criteria for the NeuroIMAGE2 study were an IQ below 70 (as measured with the block design and vocabulary subtests from the WISC/WAIS), contra-indications for MRI scanning, and a diagnosis of autism, epilepsy, learning difficulties, brain disorders, or known genetic disorders. Medicated participants were required to be on a stable dose of cation for two weeks and were asked to abstain from stimulant medi-cation 48 hours prior to the testing day. Again, if participants had used medication prior to testing, interviews and questionnaires were based on (assumed) behavior and interference in daily life when no medication was used.

Phenotypic measures

The YSR, TRF, and the CBCL (Achenbach, 1999) were used for the assessment of aggression and rule-breaking behavior. To confirm the diagnosis of ADHD and disruptive behavior disorder and for the assessment of comorbid disorders, the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS-PL; Kaufman et al., 1997) was administered by trained researchers. ADHD diagnosis was additionally confirmed by Conners rating scales (Conners, Erhardt, & Sparrow, 1999; CTRS-R:L parent-report for participants under the age of 18 years and the CAARS-S:L self-report for participants 18 years and older). Subscales from these questionnaires in the NeuroIMAGE2 study and subscales from the parent-reported SNAP-IV (Swanson, Nolan and Pelham-IV questionnaire; Swanson, 1981) in the MATRICS/Aggressotype study were used for the assessment of ADHD and oppositional symptom severity. Items were scored on a 4-point scale, and the sum of these scores belonging to these dimensions were added to create a severity score. Overlapping questions between the SNAP-IV and Conners were summed to calculate symptom severity across the studies. Both

questionnaires have the same measures of symptom dimensions, with the Conners using multiple questions to measure the same constructs. The self-reported Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits (Kimonis et al., 2008) (ICU) was used for the measurement of CU traits in both cohorts. IQ was estimated using subtests from the WISC/WAIS (Wechsler, 2002). Handedness was assessed by asking whether the participant was left- or right-handed during screening. Lastly, de-mographic variables such as age and (biological) sex were gathered through self-report.

Probabilistic Reversal Learning task

A PRL task (see Figure 1) was administered to investigate strategies in processing of reward and punishment and perseverative behavior (Cools et al., 2002; Lawrence, Sahakian, Rogers, Hodges, & Robbins, 1999). Win-stay scores indicated the proportion of trials where the subject remained with the previously rewarding stimulus (i.e. reward sensitivity), whereas lose-shift scores indicated the proportion of trials on which the subjects altered their response after punishment (i.e. punishment sensitivity; den Ouden et al., 2013). Perseveration errors were defined as the sequence of two or more errors during the reversal phase and reflect the inability to adapt response choice after reversal. The task also measured a learning criterion of eight consecutive trials, to account for learning effects. Two sites did not administer the PRL in the MATRICS/Aggressotype study and were therefore excluded from the analyses.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed with the R statistical program (R Core Team, 2013). Reward, punishment, and perseveration summary scores of the PRL task were extracted with MATLAB R2016b (The MathWorks Inc., 2016) using in-house developed scripts.

Data were analyzed by applying a multilevel linear model with a maximum likelihood (ML) fit using the nlme package (Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, Sarkar, & Team, 2007) (see supplementary materials for model statements). The PRL summary scores (win-stay, lose-shift, and persev-eration errors) were outcome variables in these models, and ADHD symptom severity, oppositional symptom severity, and ICU scores were added as predictors corrected for the other domains. Interactions be-tween ADHD symptom severity, oppositional symptom severity, and ICU scores were also tested in a separate model. In all analyses, site was added as a first-level random factor. Family relatedness (only applicable for NeuroIMAGE2) was added as a random factor nested in site. Age was separately added to assess its influence on the relations between vari-ables of interest. Varivari-ables of non-interest (IQ, sex, medication, and the learning criterion of 8 consecutive trials) were added, when the base model yielded a significant effect of the variable of interest on the PRL summary scores. Results of the effects of covariates of non-interest can be found in the supplementary materials. Model fits were compared with the anova function in R, and the log likelihood ratio test was used to assess model significance. For an overview of the average PRL summary scores per site, see Figure S1. To control for multiple testing the effective number of tests method was used (Galwey, 2009; Nyholt, 2004), calculating an adjusted α of 0.0193 taking into account the dependency

between the variables of interest (original n = 3 tests). Effect sizes were estimated by calculating the Pearson correlations, with r = 0.1 repre-senting a small effect size, r = 0.3 a medium effect size, and r =0.5 a

large effect size (Cohen, 2013).

Results

Demographics

The final analysis, after excluding participants and sites without PRL administration, included 438 participants, including 145 participants

(5)

Psychiatry Research 298 (2021) 113795

4

with DB (either above cut-off aggression scores or a primary diagnosis of ODD/CD), 123 participants with a primary diagnosis of ADHD, and 170 controls. The three groups (DB ADHD, and controls) differed signifi-cantly regarding age, IQ, and sex (see Table 1 for details). Among the DB

group, 26.9% had comorbid ADHD, and 19.5% of the ADHD sample had comorbid ODD/CD. Figure S2 shows the division of the parameters per group.

Associations of PRL with ADHD symptoms, oppositional behavior, and CU traits

ADHD symptom severity was negatively associated with win-stay scores (β = -0.004, t(113) = -3.085, p = 0.004, r = 0.278) and posi-tively associated with lose-shift scores (β = 0.004, t(113) = 3.109, p = 0.004, r = 0.281). After correction for oppositional symptom severity and CU traits, a trend for the ADHD effect on lose-shift remained (β = 0.003, t(113) = 1.781, p = 0.083, r = 0.165), and ADHD symptom severity remained significantly related to decreased win-stay scores (β = -0.004, t(113) = -2.476, p = 0.018, r = 0.227; see Figure 2).

Oppositional symptom severity and ICU scores did not significantly relate to win-stay scores, lose-shift scores, or perseveration errors (all p- values > 0.0193). Contrary to expectations, none of the variables of interest were associated with perseveration errors (all p-values > 0.0193).

Interactions between PRL and ADHD symptoms, oppositional traits, and CU traits

There was no three-way interaction-effect of ADHD symptoms, oppositional traits and CU traits on any of the PRL parameters (p-value > 0.0193). There was a significant interaction-effect of ADHD severity with oppositional behavior on perseveration errors (β = -0.029, t(113) =-2.436, p = 0.0192, r = 0.223; see Figure 3). With an increase of ADHD severity, the effect of oppositional behavior on perseveration decreased. Both main effects showed a non-significant negative slope (ADHD severity: β = -0.151, t(113) = 1.486, p = 0.146, r = 0.138; oppositional:

β =-0.646, t(113) = 2.007, p = 0.052, r = 0.186). There were no

in-teractions between any of the other continuous measures on any of the other PRL parameters (all p-values > 0.0193).

Age and the relations between symptom dimensions and PRL parameters

We additionally assessed whether the linear and interaction effects were driven by age. First, we investigated whether age interacted with any of the predictors in all models. Age did not significantly interact

Figure 1. A probabilistic reversal learning task

as administered to the participants. In this task, two visual stimuli were presented to the par-ticipants on a computer in two out of four randomly selected locations. Participants were instructed to select the usually rewarded stim-ulus and had to respond to these stimuli by clicking on the right object. After their response, feedback was probabilistically given in either a green ‘correct’ for reward or a red ‘incorrect’ in case of punishment. Choosing the correct stimulus (the stimulus chosen on the first trial) led to reward in 70% of the trials and to (misleading) punishment in 30% of the trials (acquisition phase). After 40 trials the rein-forcement contingencies changed, and the frequently rewarded stimulus became the frequently punished stimulus (reversal phase) in 70% of the trails. Subjects were instructed that the identity of the rewarded stimulus could change but did not know when or how often this change could occur. Adapted from Cools et al. (2002) Lawrence et al. (1999), and den Ouden et al. (2013).

Table 1

Demographic characteristics of the full sample (N = 438).

DBa (n

=145)

ADHDb

(n = 123) control (n = 170) Test statistic p-value Sex, n (%) Male Female 119 (82.1) 26 (17.9) 69 (56.1) 54 (43.9) 102 (60.0) 68 (40.0) χ2(2) = 463.91 <0.001** Age, M (SD) 13.03 (2.71) 19.61 (3.78) 16.49 (4.37) F(2, 437) =104.9 <0.001** IQ, M (SD) 100.2 (11.2) 98.7 (17.9) 109.0 (12.1) F(2, 432) =25.17 <0.001** Handedness, n (%) Left Right 19 (13.1) 116 (80.0) 13 (10.6) 109 (88.6) 22 (12.9) 146 (85.9) χ2(2) = 0.713 0.700 ODD severity, M (SD) 14.16 (5.46) 7.08 (4.26) 2.17 (2.99) F(2, 364) =506.8 <0.001** ADHD severity, M (SD) 31.12 (11.83) 22.62 (7.99) 7.10 (6.55) F(2, 362) =468.3 <0.001** CU traits, M (SD) 29.67 (9.52) 23.91 (7.49) 20.97 (6.92) F(2, 392) =40.83 <0.001** PRL win-stay, M (SD) 0.70 (0.172) 0.79 (0.149) 0.82 (0.149) F(2, 351) =17.61 <0.001** lose-shift, M corr (SD) 0.49 (0.157) 0.47 (0.147) 0.40 (0.178) F(2, 351) =10.43 <0.001** perseveration errors, M (SD) 11.6 (7.6) 10.0 (7.2) 8.9 (7.4) F(2, 351) =3.79 0.024* Note: *p < 0.05, two-tailed.

**p < 0.01, two-tailed. DB = disruptive behaviors, ADHD = attention-deficit/

hyperactivity disorder, ODD = oppositional defiant disorder, CU = callous-un-emotional, PRL = probabilistic reversal learning, M = mean, SD = standard deviation. ADHD total scores were calculated with the use of the SNAP-IV and Conners rating scales.

a39 participants with DB had comorbid ADHD diagnosis. b 24 ADHD participants had comorbid ODD diagnosis.

(6)

with ADHD symptoms nor oppositional behavior (lowest p-value = 0.651). However, age itself was positively associated with win-stay scores (β = 0.015, t(113) = 4.231, p = 0.001, r = 0.370). Adding age to the ADHD severity model only changed the slope but not the signif-icance of ADHD symptom severity on win-stay (β = -0.003, t(113) = -2.769, p = 0.008, r = 0.252).

Both the linear and interaction analyses were then repeated within the two cohorts (MATRICS/Aggressotype and NeuroIMAGE2) sepa-rately, since both cohorts recruited matched controls in their samples. Repeating the analyses within the MATRICS/Aggressotype cohort led to non-significance in the relations between win-stay and ADHD severity (β = -0.001, t(140) = -0.600, p = 0.549, r = 0.051) and the interaction of

ADHD severity and oppositional behavior on perseveration (β = -0.001, t (128) = 0.247, p = 0.805, r = 0.022). Both effects remained significant in the NeuroIMAGE2 cohort (win-stay: β = -0.004, t(113) = 2.156, p = 0.016, r = 0.199; perseveration: β = -0.029, t(110) = -2.242, p = 0.0192,

r = 0.209), indicating that these effect were apparently more

pro-nounced in the older age ranges.

The effects of variables of non-interest

Regarding the variables of non-interest, the learning criterion of 8 consecutive trials was significantly associated of all parameters (all p- values < 0.001). IQ and sex were no significant correlates of any of the

(7)

Psychiatry Research 298 (2021) 113795

6

variables (all p-values > 0.0193). Medication was not significantly associated with win-stay scores, lose-shift scores, or perseveration (lowest p-value = 0.290 for lose-shift). Since IQ inclusion criteria differed by 10 points between the two cohorts, all analyses were repeated without the participants with an estimated IQ of below 80 (n = 10). This did not change the relation between ADHD symptom severity and win-stay adoption (β = -0.004, t(103) = -2.498, p = 0.017, r = 0.227), and strengthened the interaction between ADHD symptom severity and ODD symptom severity on perseverative behavior (β = -0.032, t(103) = -2.636, p = 0.0126).

Post-hoc analyses

To see whether the ADHD symptom relations with win-stay and lose- shift scores were driven by hyperactivity-impulsivity or inattention, the scores were divided into these two subcategories. Inattention was significantly associated with both win-stay (β = -0.006, t(113) = -2.637,

p = 0.012, r = 0.241) and lose-shift scores (β = 0.007, t(113) = 3.182, p

= 0.002, r = 0.287). Hyperactivity-impulsivity was also significantly related to win-stay (β = -0.007, t(113) = -3.089, p = 0.003, r = 0.279) and lose-shift scores (β = 0.006, t(113) = 2.534, p = 0.015, r = 0.232). Correcting for the other domains led to both effects becoming non-

significant (smallest p-value = 0.079).

To examine if the win-stay scores were driven by strategies adopted in the acquisition phase or reversal phase, the model was repeated in both phases. Win-stay scores were negatively related with ADHD symptom severity in the acquisition phase (β = -0.005, t(113) = -2.675,

p = 0.011, r = 0.244) but not the reversal phase (β = -0.003, t(113) =

-1.640, p = 0.110, r = 0.152), indicating that, with increasing ADHD severity, the tendency to switch stimulus after reward was no longer apparent in the presence of the reward reversal manipulation.

Discussion

This study examined the associations between reinforcement sensi-tivities and ADHD symptoms, oppositional behavior, and CU traits across a diagnostic population of children, adolescents and adults with ADHD and DB. ADHD symptom severity was associated with decreased reward sensitivity independent of oppositional symptom severity and CU traits. Severity of oppositional symptoms and CU traits on itself were not related to any of the reversal learning strategies. Adding interaction terms showed that ADHD severity alleviated the effect of oppositional behavior on perseveration, whereas the effect of ADHD on perseveration was opposite depending on the degree of oppositional symptoms.

Figure 3. The effect of oppositional

traits on perseverative behavior de-pends on the severity of ADHD and vice versa. Note: For visual purposes, ADHD severity (top panel) and oppositional severity (bottom panel) scores are here divided in three groups: One group with symptom scores around 1 standard de-viation or more above the mean, one group with symptom scores around the mean, and one group with symptom scores around 1 standard deviation below the mean. In the analyses and the subsequent results, however, they were entered as continuous measures.

(8)

Our finding of an association between ADHD symptom severity and hyposensitivity to reward, independent of oppositional and CU traits aligns with some of the previous studies on reward-sensitivity in ADHD samples (Furukawa et al., 2018; Furukawa, Shimabukuro, Alsop, & Tripp, 2017; Geurts, Van der Oord, & Crone, 2006). Moreover, they suggest that the reduced sensitivity to reward observed in previous ADHD studies may partly be related to ADHD symptom severity in both clinical and non-clinical populations. Adding age only changed the slope but not the significance of this relation, yet repeating analyses showed that this effect was only present in the NeuroIMAGE2 cohort. Age in itself was however significantly associated with increased win-stay adoption, and repeating the analyses showed that in the older cohort, which consisted of ADHD-diagnosed participants and healthy controls, the association between ADHD symptom severity and win-stay scores remained. The reason that this effect was only visible in the ADHD sample may be because the relation between ADHD symptom severity and sensitivity to reward is mostly driven by inattentive symptoms rather than impulsivity. Inattentive symptoms are relatively static across the lifespan, whereas impulsive-hyperactive symptoms often decrease with increasing age (Biederman et al., 2000; D¨opfner et al., 2015). It is possible that in the older age groups, ADHD symptom severity was mostly driven by the inattention subtype rather than the combined subtype.

The negative association between ADHD severity and reward sensi-tivity was present in the acquisition phase but not the reversal phase, suggesting that sensitivity to reward increased during the task. Partici-pants with DB and ADHD reached the learning criterion of 8 consecutive trials less often than controls (see Figure S4), perhaps suggesting that children with DB and ADHD needed a longer time to adjust to the task (Luman, Oosterlaan, Knol, & Sergeant, 2008). The delayed win-stay adoption may in part reflect the well-documented prefrontal and orbi-tofrontal metabolic dysfunction during cognitive tasks in externalizing and disruptive disorders (Finger et al., 2008; Itami & Uno, 2002; Rubia et al., 2009; Zametkin et al., 1993; Zametkin et al., 1990). Further research is required to assess whether the delayed win-stay adoption is caused by stabilization of metabolic rates in the frontal lobes by measuring PRL-related brain activity in clinical and non-clinical samples with the use of fMRI.

No associations were detected between CU traits and reversal learning. The current results, using CU traits across disorders, are inconsistent with previous research that assessed CU traits categorically with a cut-off at the median (Budhani & Blair, 2005; Finger et al., 2008). Since high CU traits are highly comorbid with oppositional behavior and ADHD (Herpers et al., 2012), a possible explanation for the current finding would be that most of the effects found in previous studies (Byrd et al., 2014; O’Brien & Frick, 1996; Rubia et al., 2009) may have been due to other overlapping traits or a combination of traits that defined these diagnoses.

Where several studies associated CU traits with increased reversal errors in previous literature (Byrd et al., 2014; O’Brien & Frick, 1996;

Rubia et al., 2009), we found a small moderating effect of oppositional behavior and ADHD symptomatology explaining perseveration in this study. For high-oppositional people, more ADHD-symptoms reduced perseveration errors, whereas for the low oppositional group, ADHD severity increased perseveration. It has been discussed that ODD was associated with deficits in inhibition, especially when motivational factors are involved, leading children with ODD and CD experiencing greater difficulty to learn to make appropriate decisions (Matthys et al., 2012). Perhaps diagnostic categorization of previous studies has masked the conflicting role of ADHD symptomatology in ODD studies. Published research on oppositional traits is scarce, and could benefit from subse-quent studies trying to disentangle the roles of oppositionality and ADHD symptomatology in relation to perseverance in probabilistic learning tasks.

A limitation of the current study was the substantial age difference across the samples. Linearly correcting for age, checking for age

interactions and repeating analyses in each cohort ensured that any differences between ADHD and DB were unlikely due to age alone, but on the other hand may have led to false negative findings to the extent that age and group effects of interest explained the same variance. Sensitivity analyses showed that the results only held up for the Neu-roIMAGE2 cohort with an age range of 11-28 years. Lastly, our findings had small to medium effect sizes, meaning that the results should be interpreted relative to its contributions. Nonetheless, by combining data from two cohorts, it was possible, for the first time, to investigate reward and punishment processing in relation to ADHD and DB symptom-atology across different disorders. Previous studies that found reward and punishment processing deficits in both diagnoses failed to investi-gate the deficits in the context of its potential associations.

Conclusions

This study suggests that reduced reward sensitivity as measured by a probabilistic reversal-learning task is unrelated to oppositional behavior and CU traits but rather depends on ADHD hyposensitivity to reward across disorders and in healthy controls. ADHD severity alleviated the effect of oppositional behavior on perseveration, whereas the effect of ADHD on perseveration is opposite depending on the degree of oppo-sitional symptoms. The dissociation between results from categorical and dimensional analyses provide evidence that different traits may relate to different reward processing deficits that appear trans- diagnostic.

Disclosures

T Banaschewski served in an advisory or consultancy role for Acte-lion, Hexal Pharma, Lilly, Medice, Novartis, Oxford outcomes, PCM scientific, Shire and Viforpharma. He received conference support or speaker’s fee by Medice, Novartis and Shire. He is/has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Shire & Viforpharma. The present work is unrelated to the grants and relationships noted earlier. C Arango has been a consultant to or has received honoraria or grants from Acadia, Ambrosseti, Caja Navarra, CIBERSAM, Fundaci´on Alicia Koplowitz, Forum, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Gedeon Richter, Janssen Cilag, Lundbeck, Merck, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovaci´on, Ministerio de Sanidad, Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad, Mutua Madrile˜na, Otsuka, Roche, Servier, Shire, Schering Plough, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Sunovio and Takeda. S. Walitza has received in the last 5 years royalties from Thieme Hogrefe, Kohlhammer, Springer, Beltz. S. Walitza has received lecture honoraria from Opopharma in the last 5 years. Her work was supported in the last 5 years by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNF), diff. EU FP7s, HSM Hochspezialisierte Medizin of the Kanton Zurich, Switzerland, Bfarm Germany, ZInEP, Hartmann Müller Stiftung, Olga Mayenfisch, Gertrud Thalmann Fonds. D Brandeis serves as an unpaid scientific advisor for an EU-funded Neurofeedback trial unrelated to the present work. JC Glennon has acted as a consultant for Boehringer Ingelheim GmbH. B Franke received an educational speaking fee from Medice. JK Buitelaar has been consultant to/member of advisory board of and/or speaker for Janssen Cilag BV, Eli Lilly, Bristol-Myer Squibb, Shering Plough, UCB, Shire, Novartis, and Servier. He is not an employee of any of these companies, nor a stock shareholder of any of these companies. He has no other financial or material support, including expert testimony, patents, and royalties. The other authors do not report any biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Christel M. Portengen: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal

analysis, Writing - original draft. Emma Sprooten: Software, Writing - review & editing. Marcel P. Zwiers: Software, Writing - review & editing. Pieter J. Hoekstra: Project administration, Investigation,

(9)

Psychiatry Research 298 (2021) 113795

8

Writing - review & editing. Andrea Dietrich: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Nathalie E. Holz: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Pascal M.

Aggensteiner: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review &

editing. Tobias Banaschewski: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Ulrike M.E. Schulze: Project administra-tion, Investigaadministra-tion, Writing - review & editing. Melanie C. Saam: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing.

Michael C. Craig: Project administration, Investigation, Writing -

re-view & editing. Arjun Sethi: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Paramala Santosh: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Ilyas Sagar Ouriaghli: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Josefina

Castro-Fornieles: Project administration, Investigation, Writing -

re-view & editing. Mireia Rosa: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Celso Arango: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. María Jos´e Penzol: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Julia E.

Werhahn: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review &

editing. Daniel Brandeis: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Susanne Walitza: Project administration, Investi-gation, Writing - review & editing. Marianne Oldehinkel: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing. Barbara

Franke: Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review &

edit-ing. Jan K. Buitelaar: Conceptualization, Project administration, Investigation, Writing - review & editing, Supervision. Jilly Naaijen: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing - review & editing, Supervision.

Acknowledgements

This project has received funding from the European Union’s Sev-enth Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement no 602805 (Aggressotype), 603016 (MATRICS), 728018 (Eat2beNICE) and 667302 (CoCA). This work reflects only the authors’ views, and the European Union is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained herein. E.S. is supported by a Hypatia Fellowship (Radboudumc) and a NARSAD Young Investigator Grant (ID:25034). B.F. is supported by the Dutch National Science Agenda through the NWA-NeurolabNL project (grant 400 17 602). J.N. is supported by a VENI grant of the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) (V1.Veni.194.032). We gratefully thank Hanneke den Ouden for the task code and basic analysis code. We gratefully acknowledge and thank all the participants and their families for their enthusiastic participation in the study. The authors would also like to thank all other PhD students, post-docs, and research assistants for their involvement in data-collection.

Supplementary materials

Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113795.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1999). The child behavior checklist and related instruments. In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcome

assessment (pp. 429-466). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (dsm-5®): American Psychiatric Pub.

Barkley, R.A., 1997. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of adhd. Psychological Bulletin 121 (1), 65. https:// doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65.

Biederman, J., Mick, E., Faraone, S.V., 2000. Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of Psychiatry 157 (5), 816–818. https://doi.org/10.1176/ appi.ajp.157.5.816.

Blair, R.J.R., Leibenluft, E., Pine, D.S, 2014. Conduct disorder and callous–unemotional traits in youth. New England Journal of Medicine 371 (23), 2207–2216. https://doi. org/10.1056/nejmra1315612.

Bongers, I.L., Koot, H.M., Van Der Ende, J., Verhulst, F.C., 2004. Developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence. Child Development 75 (5), 1523–1537. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 8624.2004.00755.x.

Budhani, S., Blair, R.J.R., 2005. Response reversal and children with psychopathic tendencies: Success is a function of salience of contingency change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 46 (9), 972–981. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469- 7610.2004.00398.x.

Byrd, A.L., Loeber, R., Pardini, D.A., 2014. Antisocial behavior, psychopathic features and abnormalities in reward and punishment processing in youth. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 17 (2), 125–156. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-013- 0159-6.

Cohen, J., 2013. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic press. Conners, C.K., Erhardt, D., Sparrow, E.P., 1999. Conners’ adult adhd rating scales

(caars): Technical manual. Multi-Health Systems, North Tonawanda (NY). Cools, R., Clark, L., Owen, A.M., Robbins, T.W., 2002. Defining the neural mechanisms of

probabilistic reversal learning using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience 22 (11), 4563–4567. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.22-11-04563.2002.

Daughterty, T.K., Quay, H.C., 1991. Response perseveration and delayed responding in childhood behavior disorders. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 32 (3), 453–461. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00323.x.

... den Ouden, H.E., Daw, N.D., Fernandez, G., Elshout, J.A., Rijpkema, M., Hoogman, M., Cools, R., 2013. Dissociable effects of dopamine and serotonin on reversal learning Neuron 80 (4), 1090–1100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.030. D¨opfner, M., Hautmann, C., G¨ortz-Dorten, A., Klasen, F., Ravens-Sieberer, U., Group, B.

S., 2015. Long-term course of adhd symptoms from childhood to early adulthood in a community sample. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 24 (6), 665–673. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-014-0634-8.

... Finger, E.C., Marsh, A.A., Mitchell, D.G., Reid, M.E., Sims, C., Budhani, S., Leibenluft, E., 2008. Abnormal ventromedial prefrontal cortex function in children with psychopathic traits during reversal learning Archives of general psychiatry 65 (5), 586–594. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.586.

Frick, P.J., Dickens, C., 2006. Current perspectives on conduct disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports 8 (1), 59–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-006-0082-3. ... Furukawa, E., Alsop, B., Caparelli-D´aquer, E.M., Casella, E.B., da Costa, R.Q.M., de

Moura Queiroz, P., Tripp, G., 2018. Behavioral adjustment to asymmetric reward availability among children with and without adhd: Effects of past and current reinforcement contingencies ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-018-0265-x.

Furukawa, E., Shimabukuro, S., Alsop, B., Tripp, G., 2017. Behavioral sensitivity of japanese children with and without adhd to changing reinforcer availability: An experimental study using signal detection methodology. Behavioral and Brain Functions 13 (1), 13. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-017-0131-6.

Galwey, N.W., 2009. A new measure of the effective number of tests, a practical tool for comparing families of non-independent significance tests. Genetic Epidemiology: The Official Publication of the International Genetic Epidemiology Society 33 (7), 559–568. https://doi.org/10.1002/gepi.20408.

Geurts, H.M., Van der Oord, S., Crone, E.A., 2006. Hot and cool aspects of cognitive control in children with adhd: Decision-making and inhibition. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 34 (6), 811–822. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-006-9059-2. Hart, H., Radua, J., Nakao, T., Mataix-Cols, D., Rubia, K., 2013. Meta-analysis of

functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA psychiatry 70 (2), 185–198. https://doi.org/ 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277.

Herpers, P.C., Rommelse, N.N., Bons, D.M., Buitelaar, J.K., Scheepers, F.E., 2012. Callous–unemotional traits as a cross-disorders construct. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 47 (12), 2045–2064. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127- 012-0513-x.

... Ibanez, A., Cetkovich, M., Petroni, A., Urquina, H., Baez, S., Gonzalez-Gadea, M.L., Strejilevich, S., 2012. The neural basis of decision-making and reward processing in adults with euthymic bipolar disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (adhd) PloS One 7 (5), e37306. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0037306. Itami, S., Uno, H., 2002. Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder revealed by reversal and extinction tasks. Neuroreport 13 (18), 2453–2457.

... Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., Ryan, N., 1997. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (k-sads-pl): Initial reliability and validity data Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 36 (7), 980–988. https://doi. org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021.

... Kimonis, E.R., Frick, P.J., Skeem, J.L., Marsee, M.A., Cruise, K., Munoz, L.C., Morris, A. S., 2008. Assessing callous–unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: Validation of the inventory of callous–unemotional traits International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 31 (3), 241–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2008.04.002. Lawrence, A., Sahakian, B., Rogers, R., Hodges, J., Robbins, T., 1999. Discrimination,

reversal, and shift learning in huntington’s disease: Mechanisms of impaired response selection. Neuropsychologia 37 (12), 1359–1374. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00035-4.

Li, J.J., 2018. Children’s reward and punishment sensitivity moderates the association of negative and positive parenting behaviors in child adhd symptoms. Journal of

(10)

Abnormal Child Psychology 46 (8), 1585–1598. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802- 018-0421-y.

Luman, M., Oosterlaan, J., Knol, D.L., Sergeant, J.A., 2008. Decision-making in adhd: Sensitive to frequency but blind to the magnitude of penalty? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49 (7), 712–722. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469- 7610.2008.01910.x.

Matthys, W., van Goozen, S.H.M., Snoek, H., van Engeland, H., 2004. Response perseveration and sensitivity to reward and punishment in boys with oppositional defiant disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 13, 362–364. https://doi. org/10.1007/s00787-004-0395-x.

Matthys, W., Vanderschuren, L.J., Schutter, D.J., Lochman, J.E., 2012. Impaired neurocognitive functions affect social learning processes in oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: Implications for interventions. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 15 (3), 234–246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-012- 0118-7.

Mick, E., Faraone, S.V., Biederman, J., 2004. Age-dependent expression of attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Psychiatric Clinics 27 (2), 215–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2004.01.003.

Nyholt, D.R., 2004. A simple correction for multiple testing for single-nucleotide polymorphisms in linkage disequilibrium with each other. The American Journal of Human Genetics 74 (4), 765–769. https://doi.org/10.1086/383251.

O’Brien, B.S., Frick, P.J., 1996. Reward dominance: Associations with anxiety, conduct problems, and psychopathy in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 24 (2), 223–240. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01441486.

Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., Team, R.C., 2007. Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3 (57), 1–89.

Plichta, M.M., Scheres, A., 2014. Ventral–striatal responsiveness during reward anticipation in adhd and its relation to trait impulsivity in the healthy population: A meta-analytic review of the fmri literature. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 38, 125–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.012.

R Core Team, 2013. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Rubia, K., Smith, A.B., Halari, R., Matsukura, F., Mohammad, M., Taylor, E.,

Brammer, M.J., 2009. Disorder-specific dissociation of orbitofrontal dysfunction in boys with pure conduct disorder during reward and ventrolateral prefrontal dysfunction in boys with pure adhd during sustained attention. American Journal of Psychiatry 166 (1), 83–94. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08020212.

... Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J., Greenstein, D., Rapoport, J., 2007. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 (49), 19649–19654. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707741104.

Swanson, J.M., 1981. The snap rating scale for the diagnosis of the attention deficit disorder.

The MathWorks Inc, 2016. Matlab and statistics toolbox release 2016b. Natick, Massachusetts, United States. Retrieved from. https://nl.mathworks.com/p roducts/matlab.html.

Viding, E., Fontaine, N.M., McCrory, E.J., 2012. Antisocial behaviour in children with and without callous-unemotional traits. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 105 (5), 195–200. https://doi.org/10.1258/jrsm.2011.110223.

Viding, E., Jones, A.P., Paul, J.F., Moffitt, T.E., Plomin, R., 2008. Heritability of antisocial behaviour at 9: Do callous-unemotional traits matter? Developmental Science 11 (1), 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00648.x. ... von Rhein, D., Mennes, M., van Ewijk, H., Groenman, A.P., Zwiers, M.P.,

Oosterlaan, J., Faraone, S.V., 2015. The neuroimage study: A prospective phenotypic, cognitive, genetic and mri study in children with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Design and descriptives European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 24 (3), 265–281. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-014-0573-4. Wechsler, D., 2002. Wisc-iii handleiding. The Psychological Corporation, London (UK). ... Wu, Z.-M., Llera, A., Hoogman, M., Cao, Q.-J., Zwiers, M.P., Bralten, J., Yang, B.-R.,

2019. Linked anatomical and functional brain alterations in children with attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder NeuroImage: Clinical 23, 101851 https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101851.

Xue, G., Xue, F., Droutman, V., Lu, Z.-L., Bechara, A., Read, S., 2013. Common neural mechanisms underlying reversal learning by reward and punishment. PloS One 8 (12), e82169. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082169.

... Zametkin, A.J., Liebenauer, L.L., Fitzgerald, G.A., King, A.C., Minkunas, D.V., Herscovitch, P., Cohen, R.M., 1993. Brain metabolism in teenagers with attention- deficit hyperactivity disorder Archives of General Psychiatry 50 (5), 333–340. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820170011002.

... Zametkin, A.J., Nordahl, T.E., Gross, M., King, A.C., Semple, W.E., Rumsey, J., Cohen, R.M., 1990. Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with hyperactivity of childhood onset New England Journal of Medicine 323 (20), 1361–1366. https:// doi.org/10.1056/nejm199011153232001.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Tot slot zijn verklaringen voor het niet vinden van een effect voor het huilen per nacht dat de white-noise en THB ‘s nachts niet vaak door ouders zijn toegepast en dat de baby’s

Chapter 5 was financially supported by Nationaal Regieorgaan Onderwijsonderzoek (NRO): Programma voor onderwijsonderzoek (PROO) - Review Studies. ISBN digital

Considering that motivational deficiencies are likely to contribute to the educational difficulties of students with ADHD (see Chapter 4), Chapter 5 describes a meta-analytic

positive findings ([+], i.e., increased risk-taking performance in ADHD compared to TDCs) and studies with null findings ([0], i.e., no ADHD-TDCs difference in risk-taking

Most studies found intact social reward processing in ADHD and only a few studies found hyperresponsiveness to social rewards in ADHD relative to TDCs (two out of five

In contrast to intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation is the desire to engage in certain behaviors in order to earn a reward or avoid punishment (see General

Based on the present study, it can be concluded that the different classroom interventions performed had (small to large) positive effects on off-task and disruptive classroom

The present survey study among Dutch general education teachers examines: (a) primary and secondary school teachers’ reported frequency of use and the perceived effectiveness