• No results found

Making sense of numbers - Early mathematics achievement and working memory in primary school children

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Making sense of numbers - Early mathematics achievement and working memory in primary school children"

Copied!
310
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

1

Making sense of numbers

Early mathematics achievement and working memory in primary

school children

(2)

2

Cover photo: © Dvmsimages | Dreamstime.com

Cover design: Michel Friso

Printed by: GVO drukkers & vormgevers B.V.

ISBN: 978-90-393-6197-9

© 2014 I. Friso-van den Bos

All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the author. The copyright of the articles that have already been published has been transferred to the respective journals.

(3)

3

Making sense of numbers

Early mathematics achievement and working memory in primary

school children

Inzicht in getallen

Vroege rekenvaardigheden en werkgeheugen bij basisschoolkinderen (met een samenvatting in het Nederlands)

Proefschrift

ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Universiteit Utrecht op gezag van de rector magnificus, prof.dr. G.J. van der Zwaan, ingevolge het besluit van het college voor promoties in het openbaar te verdedigen op vrijdag 12 september 2014 des middags te 4.15 uur

door

Ilona Friso-van den Bos

geboren op 3 januari 1985 te Sliedrecht

(4)

4

Copromotor: Dr. E.H. Kroesbergen

Studies reported in this dissertation were funded by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO).

(5)

5

Table of contents

Chapter 1: Introduction 7

Section 1: Meta-analyses 29

Chapter 2: Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A

meta-analysis

31

Chapter 3: The relation between executive functions and number sense in primary

school children: A meta-analysis

79

Section 2: Studies of parallel performance 99

Chapter 4: Explaining variability: Numerical representations in 4- to 8-year old

children

101

Chapter 5: Number sense in kindergarten children: Factor structure and working

memory predictors

129

Section 3: Longitudinal studies 151

Chapter 6: Number line development in children at-risk for dyscalculia: Predictors

and mathematical proficiency

153

Chapter 7: Longitudinal development of number line estimation in primary school

children

175

Section 4: Training studies 205

Chapter 8: Number sense in kindergartners at-risk: Number sense and working

memory training

207

Chapter 9: A comparison of counting and number line training in kindergarten 243

Chapter 10: General discussion 263

Samenvatting in het Nederlands 293

Acknowledgements 303

(6)
(7)

7

Chapter 1

(8)

8

General introduction

“You see, if there are four apples, and only three oranges – four is a bigger number than three, so that means there are more apples on the table than there are oranges.”

~ Benedict Cumberbatch on Sesame Street, in response to the mystery of apples and oranges.

Understanding numbers and quantities, interpreting them correctly, and using them for making informed decisions is an important task in the lives of young children. Children are confronted with numbers and quantities at school, but also during play, and while doing chores. For example, when distributing marbles for a game, children would want to be able to determine the number of marbles that they have, the number of marbles their friends have, and whether a fair distribution has been made. To do so, they would need to take the same steps as Benedict took in the example above: they would need to count accurately and keep track of counted items, understand what the result meant, remember the result whilst counting the next set, compare both sets, and have sufficient knowledge of numbers to be able to determine that one number is indeed bigger than the other. Later in life, children start using money, comparing prices, and making calculations with time, numbers, or any other quantities, and they continue using number facts for all kinds of decision making. It is thought that a sound grasp on basic number skills, such as counting the number of apples and oranges and comparing between them, constitutes the foundation of more advanced number skills needed for personal and professional goals (e.g., Dehaene, 2001; Jordan, Kaplan, Oláh, & Locuniak, 2006).

This dissertation focuses on these basic number skills, which are often joined under the term number sense (Berch, 2005; Dehaene, 1992, 2001; Gersten & Chard, 1999), as well as the ability to remember and process pieces of information (such as the quantity of a previously counted set) for short amounts of time, also known as working memory (Alloway, Gathercole, Willis, & Adams, 2004; Baddeley, 2007; Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). Number sense and working memory capacity are both thought to be of vital importance for mathematics achievement (Cowan & Powell, 2014; Geary, 2010, 2011; Jordan et al., 2013). In this dissertation, associations between number sense, working memory, and mathematics achievement are investigated using various approaches.

(9)

9 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

Mathematics and number sense

Number sense components and their relation with mathematics

The term number sense refers to a general understanding of number, quantity, and magnitude. The term is difficult to define, and different authors have given different definitions, each highlighting different aspects, and each employing a different delineation of the skills involved in number sense. Broadly speaking, some authors have highlighted the conceptual, abstract part of number processing to define number sense, such as Dehaene (1992, 2001) who stated that number sense is the ability to mentally represent and manipulate number and quantity, or Gersten and Chard (1999), who highlighted fluidity and flexibility with numbers, and a general sense of meaning of numbers. Other researchers, by contrast, have employed operational definitions, focusing on performance that is facilitated by this conceptual understanding of number, such as counting, number identification, number knowledge, estimation, measurement concepts, the ability to perform mental operations with numbers, and to compare between them (e.g., Berch, 2005; Gersten & Chard, 1999; Jordan, Glutting, & Ramineni, 2008; Lago & DiPerna, 2010; Passolunghi, Vercelloni, & Schadee, 2007). Other terms have been used as well when referring to these operational definitions, such as number knowledge, early numeracy, numerical competence, and other terms (Lago & DiPerna, 2010). In this dissertation, the term number sense is used to refer to both the abstract understanding of numbers, mentioned in the conceptual definitions of the construct, and performance on various tasks in which this conceptual understanding is implicit, which is the focus of the operational definitions of number sense.

One of the topical issues in published literature, and one of the themes under investigation in this dissertation, is whether number sense should be seen as a unitary construct, or whether it consists of different components, and if so, which components should be included in a theoretical model. Previous reports include a number of subdivisions of the construct, most of them based on an influential theoretical model known as the ‘triple code model for numerical cognition’ (Dehaene, 1992; Dehaene & Cohen, 1995). This is an information processing model based on the premise that numbers are mentally represented using three different codes. First, there is the auditory verbal word frame, which is the representation of a number word (for example: the number 64 consists of the elements ‘sixty’ and ‘four’, and is processed with the use of these elements). Second, there is the visual Arabic number form, in which numbers are processed in their Arabic form, and recognised as sets of digits. Third, there is the analogue magnitude code, which is the representation of

(10)

10

quantities with no exact notation. This last numerical code is the most hypothetical, and assumed to be associated with numerical estimation and subitising processes (Dehaene, 1992). The triple code model has formed the foundation for a pertinent number of studies targeting number processing (e.g., De Smedt & Boets, 2010; Moll, Göbel, & Snowling, 2014).

By far the most common distinction between components of number sense is a distinction between symbolic number sense and non-symbolic number sense (e.g., Cirino, 2011; Desoete, Ceulemans, De Weerdt, & Pieters, 2012; Göbel, Watson, Lervåg, & Hulme, 2014; Noël & Rousselle, 2011; Sasanguie, Göbel, Moll, Smets, & Reynvoet, 2013). Non-symbolic number sense, in this framework, refers to the ability to evaluate quantities when these are represented in a non-symbolic way, such as a collection of dots or series of beeps. No formal education is needed to acquire the ability to discriminate between non-symbolic quantities, and it can be compared to the analogue magnitude code (Dehaene, 1992). It is often associated with the approximate number system, which is thought to be an innate capacity to process quantity (Dehaene, 2001; Lipton & Spelke, 2003), although the idea of innateness has been disputed as well (Verguts & Fias, 2004). Symbolic number sense, on the other hand, refers to the ability to interpret and manipulate quantities with the use of culturally taught symbols, such as words and digits. Symbolic number sense manifests itself in tasks in which number words and symbols are actively used to represent quantity, such as counting tasks, number recognition tasks, or number writing tasks. This component can be seen as a combination of the auditory verbal word frame and the visual Arabic code (Dehaene, 1992). Number words and symbols are thought to acquire meaning through their connection with their non-symbolic counterparts, also referred to as mapping (Geary, 2013; Holloway & Ansari, 2009).

A recent debate has emerged concerning the respective roles of symbolic and non-symbolic number sense in the development of mathematical skill. Number sense has been argued to be foundational to the development of mathematical cognition (Dehaene, 2001; Jordan et al., 2006; Kroesbergen, Van Luit, Van Lieshout, Van Loosbroek, & Van de Rijt, 2009; Starr, Libertus, & Brannon, 2013). Traditional theoretical assumptions included non-symbolic number sense as a core system, responsible for successful acquisition of non-symbolic skills as well as mathematical proficiency (e.g., Lipton & Spelke, 2003; Starr et al., 2013). It was argued that symbolic quantities were only meaningful to a person through association with a pre-existing non-symbolic quantity representation (Mundy & Gilmore, 2009; Xenidou-Dervou, De Smedt, Van der Schoot, & Van Lieshout, 2013) and that individual differences in

(11)

11 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

non-symbolic number sense could therefore account for individual differences in mathematics achievement. Alternatively, it has been argued that non-symbolic number processing becomes more precise as a result of a connection with symbolic number sense (Kolkman, Kroesbergen, & Leseman, 2013; Noël & Rousselle, 2011; Verguts & Fias, 2004), which would imply that symbolic processing is a catalyser for more advanced non-symbolic number sense as well as mathematics achievement. Either of these views can account for the finding that it is the symbolic system that is directly associated with mathematical achievement (e.g., Göbel et al., 2014; Kolkman, Kroesbergen et al., 2013; Sasanguie et al., 2013).

Measuring number sense

As a result of the variety in definitions of number sense, tasks employed to index the construct have also been diverse. Three types of tasks are especially prevalent in the literature: counting and number recognition tasks, comparison tasks, and number line estimation tasks. All three types of tasks are used to measure number sense in the various chapters of this dissertation, with an emphasis on number line estimation in some chapters.

Counting is an activity that is considered fundamental to learning about relations between numbers, and difficulties with mathematics have been linked to delays in counting skills (Geary, 2003; Geary, Hoard, Byrd-Craven, & DeSoto, 2004). Counting skills can be measured using a number of tasks: 1) enumeration – a task in which a child is asked to determine the number of a set of objects through counting, 2) counting sequence or rote counting – a task in which a child is asked to count as far as they can, or up to a certain number, and 3) the counting principles task – a task in which children detect violations in counting principles as defined by Gelman and Gallistel (1978), usually with the use of a puppet (Chu, VanMarle, & Geary, 2013; Jordan et al., 2006). Counting is considered a basic number skill, together with number recognition, which is the ability to verbalise written numbers (Jordan et al., 2006) and draws upon symbolic number sense (Kolkman, Kroesbergen et al., 2013).

Comparison tasks are found in two versions: non-symbolic versions and symbolic versions. In non-symbolic versions, participants are presented with two arrays of dots, squares, or other shapes, and asked to decide which of the arrays contains a larger number of shapes. Dot size and array diameter are typically controlled for, to prevent participants from basing their answers on visual properties instead of an evaluation of quantity (see Gebuis &

(12)

12

Reynvoet, 2011). Performance on these non-symbolic comparison tasks has been found to be correlated with mathematics achievement in children (e.g., Desoete et al., 2012; Libertus, Feigenson, & Halberda, 2011; Mazzocco, Feigenson, & Halberda, 2011), although claims have been made that this association is moderated by domain-general cognitive skills (Gilmore et al., 2013). In symbolic versions of the comparison task, participants are asked which of two symbolically presented numbers is the larger of the two. Performance on this task has also been found to predict mathematics achievement at a later age (De Smedt, Verschaffel, & Ghesquière, 2009; Holloway & Ansari, 2009; Mundy & Gilmore, 2009; Sasanguie et al., 2013).

Finally, number line estimation is usually measured with the Number-to-Position task (Siegler & Opfer, 2003). In this task, children are shown an empty number line with a marked beginning- and endpoint, and are asked to indicate the position of a certain number on this line by drawing a hatch mark on the location or pointing to the intended location. As children get older, their number line placements become increasingly accurate, as indexed by a linear association between presented and estimated number (e.g., Ebersbach, Luwel, Frick, Onghena, & Verschaffel, 2008; Holloway & Ansari, 2008; Laski & Siegler, 2008). Before placements show a linear association with their actual values, they have been argued to be distributed logarithmically, with the lower numbers spaced far apart and the higher numbers compressed at the end of the line (Ashcraft & Moore, 2012; Dehaene, 2003; Opfer, Siegler, & Young, 2011; Siegler & Booth, 2004), or according to a power function, with both the lower and higher numbers spaced far apart and small spaces between numbers around the midpoint of the scale (Barth & Paladino, 2011; Hollands & Dyre, 2000; Slusser, Santiago, & Barth, 2013). No consensus with regard to pre-linear models has yet been reached. Linear and accurate placements of numbers on a number line have been shown to be related to higher mathematics achievement (Geary, 2011; Halberda, Mazzocco, & Feigenson, 2008; Sasanguie, De Smedt, Defever, & Reynvoet, 2012; Sasanguie et al., 2013; Sasanguie, Van den Bussche, & Reynvoet, 2012; Siegler & Booth, 2004) and to be impaired in children with mathematical learning disorder (Geary, Hoard, Nugent, & Byrd-Craven, 2008; Landerl, 2013). The relationship between number line estimation and mathematics achievement, however, has been suggested to be bidirectional (LeFevre et al., 2013).

(13)

13 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

Training of number sense

It is generally accepted that delays in number sense early in life can have a negative impact on mathematics achievement at a later age. As a result, a variety of intervention programmes has been developed and examined, both for children with low performance on number sense tasks (e.g., Aunio, Hautamäki, & Van Luit, 2005; Toll & Van Luit, 2012; Van Luit & Schopman, 2000) and for children with other risk-factors that are generally associated with mathematical performance, such as working memory deficits (Toll & Van Luit, 2013b), specific language impairment (Mononen, Aunio, & Koponen, 2014), and low socio-economic status (e.g., Baroody, Eiland, & Thompson, 2009; Jordan, Glutting, Dyson, Hassinger-Das, & Irwin, 2012). Moreover, effects of early training on number sense achievements of typically performing children have been investigated (Krajewski, Nieding, & Schneider, 2008).

Although training programmes targeting number sense generally show positive effects on achievement, the trained tasks incorporated in these programmes often target a variety of skills, such as counting and number line estimation (Jordan et al., 2012; Toll & Van Luit, 2013b). It is therefore unclear which aspects of training programmes can be associated with gains in various aspects of number sense. A notable exception are the number line trainings that use linear numerical board games to advance number line placements of young children (Ramani & Siegler, 2011; Siegler & Ramani, 2008, 2009), but these programmes have not yet been compared to other types of interventions. Also, it is debated whether training programmes targeting domain-general skills such as working memory could lead to the same results (Kroesbergen, Van ‘t Noordende, & Kolkman, 2014; Toll & Van Luit, 2012). These issues are addressed in quasi-experimental studies in this dissertation.

Relations between working memory and number-skills

Another factor that is thought to play a central role in the development of number skills (mathematical skill as well as number sense) is the capacity and functioning of working memory and its executive functions: inhibition, shifting, and updating (Bull & Lee, 2014; Bull & Scerif, 2001; Geary et al., 2004; Kolkman, Hoijtink, Kroesbergen, & Leseman, 2013; Passolunghi, Mammarella, & Altoè, 2008). Working memory functioning has been found to predict mathematical achievement across time (Gathercole, Tiffany, Briscoe, & Thorn, 2005; Mazzocco & Kover, 2007; Toll, Van der Ven, Kroesbergen, & Van Luit, 2011) over and above the relation with intelligence (Alloway & Alloway, 2010).

(14)

14

Definitions of working memory

The most frequently used model of working memory is the tripartite model of Baddeley and Hitch (1974). This model comprises three subcomponents, each having a distinct function in information storage and processing. Two storage systems, the visuospatial sketch pad and the phonological loop, form temporary storage units for visual and spatial information, and phonological and auditory information, respectively. These storage systems are also known as slave systems. Their capacity is typically measured through simple span tasks, in which strings of information, increasing in length, must be replicated (e.g., a dot appearing in different consecutive locations for the visuospatial sketch pad and word lists for the phonological loop). A third component, the central executive, is responsible for the coordination of information stored within the slave systems. Central executive capacity is usually measured using complex span tasks, in which information must be replicated as well as manipulated. Thus, the slave systems are responsible for information storage, while the central executive is responsible for both storage and processing of information (Oberauer, Süß, Wilhelm, & Wittman, 2003).

The coordinating role of the central executive can be further differentiated into a number of sub-processes (Baddeley, 1996). A framework based on both Baddeley (1996) and the executive function literature in which the central executive is subdivided into inhibition, shifting and updating (Miyake et al., 2000) is commonly used. Inhibition is the ability to suppress a dominant response in favour of another or no response, shifting is the ability to switch between response sets, and updating is the ability to monitor information in working memory. Executive functions (inhibition, shifting and updating) are seen as functionally detailed components of the central executive in this dissertation. The distinction between these three executive functions has been confirmed in some factor analytical studies targeting children (e.g., Hughes, 1998, Lehto, Juujärvi, Kooistra, & Pulkkinen, 2003; Rose, Feldman, & Jankowski, 2011) but not in all (e.g., Van der Sluis, De Jong, & Van der Leij, 2007; Van der Ven, Kroesbergen, Boom, & Leseman, 2012). Nevertheless, many studies have employed this distinction in their research on the relations between executive functioning and mathematical performance, and this distinction is maintained in this dissertation.

Relations between working memory and mathematics

The relations between working memory and various measures of mathematical skill have been studied extensively, and there is a general consensus that mathematical achievement is

(15)

15 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

associated with working memory performance (Bull & Scerif, 2001; Geary et al., 2004; Passolunghi et al., 2008; St Clair-Thompson & Gathercole, 2006). Working memory has been hypothesised to be involved in the execution of mathematical problems, for example, when children need to remember partial answers to a problem, and in growth in mathematical skill, for example, when instructions need to be remembered to practice sets of problems later on. Yet, it is unclear which components of working memory can predict mathematical performance, and which cannot, because results of various studies contradict one another. For example, relations between mathematical skill and the executive function inhibition have been found in some studies (Bull & Scerif, 2001; Espy et al., 2004; St Clair-Thompson & Gathercole, 2006), but not in others (Andersson, 2008; Monette, Bigras, & Guay, 2011). Similarly, studies regarding associations between phonological loop performance and mathematics achievement have reported both positive relations (Panaoura & Philippou, 2007; Passolunghi et al., 2008) and null-results (Jarvis & Gathercole, 2003; Passolunghi et al., 2007). Such contradictions in outcomes can be found for all other components of working memory.

This variety in outcomes, which is addressed in the current dissertation, may be due to the complexity and variety of the tasks employed to measure working memory and the range of aspects that comprise the working memory model (Baddeley, 2007). Furthermore, inconsistencies may be due to characteristics of tasks and constructs specific to the working memory component (for an overview, see Raghubar, Barnes, & Hecht, 2010). Best, Miller, and Naglieri (2011), on the other hand, proposed that varieties in the mathematical task caused the inconsistencies in outcomes: they postulated that mathematical problem solving is strongly dependent on strategy formulation and implementation, and self-monitoring, requiring high working memory involvement. Calculation, on the other hand, was suggested to be more related to fact retrieval, which requires less executive control (Best et al., 2011). Findings reported by Fuchs et al. (2005) suggested the same (see also: Raghubar et al., 2010). Furthermore, it has been proposed that mathematics performance may be related to executive functioning to a different extent in various developmental stages (Best et al., 2011). Age has been reported to moderate the relationship in several studies (Imbo & Vandierendonck, 2007; McKenzie, Bull, & Gray, 2003; Rasmussen & Bisanz, 2005; Van der Ven, Van der Maas, Straatemeier, & Jansen, 2013; Van de Weijer-Bergsma, Kroesbergen, Prast, & Van Luit, 2014), yielding the primary conclusion that older children rely less on working memory in mathematical problem solving than younger children do, suggesting that part of the inconsistencies between studies may be explained by the age of the participants.

(16)

16

Investigating the relations between mathematics achievement and working memory, as is one of the aims of the current dissertation, is of both theoretical and practical relevance. Research findings concerning this relation may contribute to our understanding of the specific cognitive abilities involved in solving mathematical problems, and how these cognitive abilities play a role in both proficiency and deficiency in mathematical skill. On a more practical level, such findings may guide decisions in development of interventions, but knowledge of working memory involvement in mathematical problem solving may also help practitioners monitor the extent to which various mathematics practice activities impose on relevant working memory components, and help them formulate curriculum plans in which working memory imposition is adequately managed. Taking into account working memory involvement in mathematical problem solving may, through these procedures, both benefit children with a delay in mathematics achievement, and pose challenges to children with high proficiency in the domain of mathematics problem solving.

Relations between working memory and number sense

The relations between mathematics achievement and working memory functioning have been hotly debated and extensively investigated. More recently, research trends started to centre around number sense, generally considered to be a precursor of mathematical skill, and therefore hypothesised to be associated with working memory as well. Children are expected to employ working memory capacity when practising number sense tasks, for example when evaluating which items in a set have already been counted in order to reach a total number of items. There is a limited body of literature available targeting the associations between working memory and its executive functions and number sense (e.g., Bull, Espy, & Wiebe, 2008; Bull & Scerif, 2001; Clark, Pritchard, & Woodward, 2010; Kroesbergen et al., 2009). Differences in numerical skill can be explained using children’s executive functions (Bull & Scerif, 2001; Kroesbergen et al., 2009; Navarro et al., 2011). However, there is no consensus regarding the role that each specific executive function plays in number sense. Several authors have reported that inhibition plays a role in numerical cognition (Kolkman, Hoijtink et al., 2013; Kroesbergen et al., 2009), but this predictive role could not be confirmed in other studies (e.g., Lee et al., 2012; Navarro et al., 2011). Updating is usually seen as the most important predictor of number sense (Kroesbergen et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2012), but despite these claims, research targeting the relations between number sense and executive functions is limited. More information with regard to the associations between working memory and its

(17)

17 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

executive functions on the one hand, and number sense on the other hand, is needed. This need is addressed in the current dissertation. Findings regarding the association between number sense and working memory functioning may provide insight into the cognitive abilities needed to support domain-specific knowledge acquisition, which in turn may help formulate curriculum plans and intervention plans. Children with working memory deficits have been found to be able to improve in number sense after a number sense directed intervention, but not to reach the same level of performance as their peers with typical working memory functioning after intervention (Toll & Van Luit, 2013a). Insight into working memory involvement in various number sense activities may guide intervention strategies that bring children with working memory deficiencies to the same level as their typicall developing peers.

Training of working memory

It has been suggested that working memory capacity can be trained in a similar way to the training of domain-specific skills: through ability-guided practice and repetition (Klingberg et al., 2005; Klingberg, Forrsberg, & Westerberg, 2002; Thorell, Lindqvist, Bergman Nutley, Bohlin, & Klingberg, 2009). Improvements in working memory capacity after working memory training have been found in children as young as four or five years old (Diamond, Barnett, Thomas, & Munro, 2007; Röthlisberger, Neuenschwander, Cimeli, Michel, & Roebers, 2012). Especially working memory trainings that are adaptive to the level of the child can facilitate working memory, and mathematical achievement, associated with working memory performance, has been found to improve after working memory training as well (Holmes & Gathercole, 2013; Holmes, Gathercole, & Dunning, 2009), suggesting that gains in working memory can not only lead to better information processing, but also that there is a causal relationship between mathematics performance and working memory functioning. It must be noted though that the general effectiveness of working memory training is disputed, and that transfer to other skills such as numerical skills is, on the average, weak (Melby-Lervåg & Hulme, 2012; Rapport, Orban, Kofler, & Friedman, 2013).

No study to date has yet compared the gains after a number sense training directly with the gains after a working memory without a numerical component, in number sense skills or in working memory performance. A training study in which number sense and working memory trainings were compared to one another contained a strong number component in the working memory training (Kroesbergen et al., 2014). This issue, therefore,

(18)

18

is addressed in this dissertation. Also, although effects of working memory training in kindergartners have been investigated, and transfer towards academic domains has been studied previously (Diamond et al., 2007; Holmes & Gathercole, 2013; Holmes et al., 2009; Röthlisberger et al., 2012), only a limited number of studies has yet investigated transfer of working memory training in children as young as kindergartners. Addressing training possibilities for working memory is not only of practical relevance, providing evidence for the intervention strategies applicable in scholastic settings, but can also contribute to our theoretical understanding of the relation between working memory and number sense: if a training targeting working memory indeed shows the capacity to improve working memory functioning, and if these gains in working memory transfer to number sense, this can serve as experimental support for the causal relationship between working memory and number sense. However. This can only be interpreted as such if variance in the gains in working memory can be related to variance in gains in number sense, and if gains in number sense do not exceed the gains in working memory performance. Mean gains in a trained group in number sense that do not relate directly to gains in working memory, or exceed them, would be indicative of the involvement of other processes that are both facilitated by working memory training and related to number sense.

Outline of this dissertation

In summary, current research trends centre around critical investigation of number sense, its components, and relation to working memory. Also, number sense and working memory are considered important predictors of mathematics achievement, both concurrently and longitudinally, but which specific assets of these predictors make up key-determinants of success or failure in mathematics remains unclear. Predictive associations hypothesised in current literature between mathematics achievement, number sense, and working memory are summarised in Figure 1.1: working memory is considered a predictor of both mathematical skill and number sense, and number sense is considered a predictor of mathematics achievement. These are the relations under investigation in the current dissertation. This dissertation contributes to the growing body of knowledge regarding the relations between these constructs by summarising, replicating, reformulating, and expanding upon the model presented in Figure 1.1.

Relations between number sense, working memory, and mathematics achievement are investigated using various approaches: 1) meta-analytical studies targeting working memory

(19)

19 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

as a predictor of mathematics achievement and number sense, 2) relations between working memory and number sense, measured concurrently, 3) predictive value of working memory for number sense, and of number sense for mathematics achievement, and 4) facilitation of number sense and working memory through training studies. These approaches are embedded in the longitudinal MathChild study; a three-year longitudinal research project consisting of three part-projects, and with the general aim to investigate the predictive relationships between non-symbolic and symbolic number sense, and mathematics achievement in children from kindergarten to the early primary school years. This dissertation is a product of one of these part-projects, originally aiming only to investigate facilitation of number sense and working memory through training, but which has expanded over the course of the study to address the relations between these constructs in a more complete fashion.

Figure 10.1. Hypothesised relations between working memory performance, number sense, and mathematics achievement.

Section 1: Meta-analyses

Chapters 2 and 3 focus on working memory as a predictor of mathematics achievement (chapter 2) and number sense (chapter 3). In these chapters, reported associations are aggregated using meta-analysis, and variance around the mean effect size is explained using predictor variables that described differences between measures, samples, designs, and studies.

(20)

20

In chapters 4 and 5, relations between number sense and working memory are further investigated using original data. In chapter 4, the predictive role of executive functions for a number-to-position task is examined in children from kindergarten to first year of primary school. In chapter 5, the factor structure of number sense is being investigated with a diverse battery of number sense tasks, and resulting factors are predicted using working memory variables measured concurrently.

Section 3: Longitudinal studies

In chapters 6 and 7, longitudinal studies of number sense are reported, with a focus on performance on the number line estimation task. In chapter 6, latent growth modelling is applied, and latent class analysis is used to identify trajectories of development in performance on this task. Growth variables and class membership are predicted using both the factors reported on in chapter 5, and working memory variables. Mathematics performance is used as an outcome variable. In chapter 7, performance on the Number-to-Position task is investigated using various models of number line placement, each referring to the use of certain reference points in making these placements. Moreover, longitudinal associations with mathematics achievement are investigated using cross-lagged panel analyses.

Section 4: Training studies

In chapters 8 and 9, the possibilities to ameliorate number sense are investigated using various training programmes aimed to improve kindergartners’ number sense. In chapter 8, a training in which number sense skills are targeted directly is compared to a training study in which number sense skills are targeted indirectly, through the training of working memory. In chapter 9, two trainings are compared that both focus on number sense: a counting training and a number line training.

Finally, in chapter 10, which consists of a general discussion, the findings of the eight studies included in this dissertation are summarised and discussed, and conclusions regarding the role of number sense and working memory skills in the development of mathematics achievement are drawn.

References

Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in

academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 20-29. doi:

(21)

21 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. -M. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 85-106. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2003.10.002

Andersson, U. (2008). Working memory as a predictor of written arithmetic skills in children: The importance of executive functions. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 181-203. doi: 10.1348/000709907X209854

Ashcraft, M. H., & Moore, A. M. (2012). Cognitive processes of numerical estimation in young children.

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 246-267. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.005

Aunio, P., Hautamäki, J., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2005). Mathematical thinking intervention programmes for preschool children with normal and low number sense. European Journal of Special Needs Education,

20, 131-146. doi: 10.1080/08856250500055578

Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5-28.

Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning

and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). New York: Academic Press.

Baroody, A. J., Eiland, M. D., Purpura, D. J., & Reid, E. E. (2012). Fostering at-risk kindergarten children’s number sense. Cognition and Instruction, 30, 435-470. doi: 10.1080/07370008.2012.720152

Baroody, A. J., Eiland, M., & Thompson, B. (2009). Fostering at-risk preschoolers’ number sense. Early

Education and Development, 20, 80-128. doi: 10.1080/10409280802206619

Barth, H. C., & Paladino, A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence against a representational shift. Developmental Science, 14, 125-135. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00962.x Berch, D. B. (2005). Making sense of number sense: Implications for children with mathematical disabilities.

Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 333-339. doi: 10.1177/00222194050380040901

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual

Differences, 21, 327-336. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007

Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in prescoolers: longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental

Neuropsychology, 33, 205-228. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982312

Bull, R., & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development

Perspectives, 8, 36-41. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12059

Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive function as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition,

switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 273-293. doi:

10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3

Chu, F. W., VanMarle, K., & Geary, D. C. (2013). Quantitative deficits of preschool children at risk for mathematical learning disability. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 195. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00195 Cirino, P. T. (2011). The interrelationships of mathematical precursors in kindergarten. Journal of Experimental

(22)

22

Clark, C. A. C., Pritchard, V. E., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1176-1191. doi: 10.1037/a0019672 Cowan, R., & Powell, D. (2014). The contributions of domain-general and numerical factors to third-grade

arithmetic skills and mathematical learning disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 214-229. doi: 10.1037/a0034097

Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1-42. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-N Dehaene, S. (2001). Précis of the number sense. Mind & Language, 16, 16-36. doi: 10.1111/1468-0017.00154 Dehaene, S. (2003). The neural basis of the Weber-Fechner law: A logarithmic mental number line. Trends in

Cognitive Sciences, 7, 145-147. doi : 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00055-X

Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing.

Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83-120.

De Smedt, B., & Boets, B. (2010). Phonological processing and arithmetic fact retrieval: Evidence from developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3973-3981. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia. 2010.10.018

De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L.,& Ghesquière, P. (2009). The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child

Psychology, 103, 469-479. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.010

Desoete, A., Ceulemans, A., De Weerdt, F., & Pieters, S. (2012). Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic and non-symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 64-81. doi : 10.1348/2044-8279.002002

Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). The early years: Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387-1388. doi: 10.1126/science.1151148

Ebersbach, M., Luwel, K., Frick, A., Onghena, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2008) The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year old children: Evidence for a segmented linear model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 1-17. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.08.006

Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M. M. Hamby, A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children. Developmental

Neuropsychology, 26, 465-486. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2601_6

Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Paulsen, K., Bryant, J. D., & Hamlett, C. L. (2005). The prevention, identification, and cognitive determinants of math difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 493-513. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.493

Gathercole, S. E., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., & Thorn, A. (2005). Developmental consequences of poor phonological short-term memory function in childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 598-611. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00379.x

Geary, D. C. (2003). Learning disabilities in arithmetic: Problem solving differences and cognitive deficits. In H. L. Swanson, K. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 199-212). New York: Guilford Press.

(23)

23 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

Geary, D. C. (2010). Mathematical disabilities: Reflections on cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 130-133. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.008 Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study.

Developmental Psychology, 47, 1539-1552. doi: 10.1037/a0025510

Geary, D. C. (2013). Early foundations for mathematics learning and their relations to learning disabilities.

Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 23-27. doi: 10.1177/0963721412469398

Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., & DeSoto, M. C. (2004). Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 121-151. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.03.002 Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2008). Development of number line representations

in children with mathematical learning disability. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 277-299. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982361

Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2011). The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 141, 642-648. doi: 10.1037/a0026218

Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. (1978) The child's understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Gersten, R., & Chard, D. J. (1999). Number sense: Rethinking arithmetic instruction for students with

mathematical disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 33, 18-28. doi:

10.1177/002246699903300102

Gilmore, C., Attridge, N., Clayton, S., Cragg, L., Johnson, S., Marlow, N., … Inglis, M. (2013). Individual differences in inhibitory control, not non-verbal number acuity, correlate with mathematics achievement. PLoS ONE, 8, e67374. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067374

Göbel, S. M., Watson, S. E., Lervåg, A., & Hulme, C. (2014). Children's arithmetic development: It is number knowledge, not the approximate number sense, that counts. Psychological Science, 25, 789-798. doi: 10.1177/0956797613516471

Halberda, J., Mazzocco, M. M. M., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Individual differences in nonverbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement. Nature, 455, 665-668. doi: 10.1038/nature07246

Hollands, J. G., & Dyre, B. P. (2000). Bias in proportion judgements: The cyclical power model. Psychological

Review, 107, 500-524. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.500

Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2008). Domain-specific and domain-general changes in children’s development of number comparison. Developmental Science, 11, 644-649. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00712.x Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical distance

effect and individual differences in achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 17-29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001

Holmes, J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2014). Taking working memory training from the laboratory into schools.

Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 34,

440-450. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2013.797338

Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12, F9-F15. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00848.x

(24)

24

Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British

Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233-253. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00921.x

Imbo, I., & Vandierendonck, A. (2007). The development of strategy use in elementary school children: Working memory and individual differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 284-309. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.09.001

Jarvis, H. E., & Gathercole, S. E. (2003). Verbal and non-verbal working memory and achievements on national curriculum tests at 11 and 14 years of age. Educational and Child Psychology, 20, 123-140.

Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., Dyson, N. I., Hassinger-Das, B., & Irwin, C. (2012). Building kindergartners’ number sense: A randomized controlled study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 647-660. doi: 10.1037/a0029018

Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., & Ramineni, C. (2008). A number sense assessment tool for identifying children at risk for mathematical difficulties. In A. Dowker (Ed.), Mathematical difficulties: Psychology and

intervention (pp. 45-58). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Jordan, N. C., Hansen, N., Fuchs, L. N., Siegler, R. S., Gersten, R., & Micklos, D. (2013). Developmental predictors of fraction concepts and procedures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 45-58. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.02.001

Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Oláh, L. N., & Locuniak, M. (2006). Number sense growth in kindergarten: A longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child Development, 77, 153-175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00862.x

Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., … Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD – A randomized, controlled trial.

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177-186. doi:

10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010

Klingberg, T., Forrsberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in children with ADHD.

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 781-791. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.6.781.8395

Kolkman, M. E., Hoijtink, H. J. A., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2013). The role of executive functions in numerical magnitude skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 24, 145-151. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.004

Kolkman, M. E., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2013). Early numerical development and the role of

non-symbolic and symbolic skills. Learning and Instruction, 25, 95-103. doi:

10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.12.001

Krajewski, K., Nieding, G., & Schneider, W. (2008). Kurz- und langfristige Effekte mathematischer Frühförderung im Kindergarten durch das Programm “Mengen, zählen, Zahlen” [Short-term and long-term effects of early math education in kindergarten]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und

Pädagogische Psychologie, 40, 135-146. doi: 10.1026/0049-8637.40.3.135

Kroesbergen, E. H., Van Luit, J. E. H., Van Lieshout, E. C. D. M., Van Loosbroek, E., & Van de Rijt, B. A. M. (2009). Individual differences in early numeracy: The role of executive functions and subitizing.

(25)

25 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

Kroesbergen, E. H., Van ’t Noordende, J. E., & Kolkman, M. E. (2014). Training working memory in kindergarten children: Effects on working memory and early numeracy. Child Neuropsychology, 20, 23-37. doi:10.1080/09297049.2012.736483

Lago, R. M., & DiPerna, J. C. (2010). Number sense in kindergarten: A factor-analytic study of the construct.

School Psychology Review, 39, 164-180.

Landerl, K. (2013). Development of numerical processing in children with typical and dyscalculic arithmetic skills – A longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 459. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00459

Laski, E. V., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child

Development, 78, 1723-1743. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01087.x

Lee, K., Ng, S. F., Pe, M. L., Ang, S. Y., Hasshim, M. N. A. M., & Bull, R. (2012). The cognitive underpinnings of emerging mathematical skills: Executive functioning, patterns, numeracy, and arithmetic. British

Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 82-99. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02016.x

LeFevre, J. -A., Jimenez Lira, C., Sowinski, C., Cankaya, O., Kamawar, D., & Skwarchuk, S. -L. (2013). Charting the role of the number line in mathematical development. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 641. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00641

Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence

from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 59-80. doi:

10.1348/026151003321164627

Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. Developmental Science, 14, 1292-1300. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x

Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Origins of number sense: Large number discrimination in human infants.

Psychological Science, 14, 396-401. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01453

Mazzocco M. M. M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011) Preschoolers’ precision of the approximate number system predicts later school mathematics performance. PLoS ONE, 6, e23749. doi:10.1371/ journal.pone.0023749

Mazzocco, M. M. M., & Kover, S. T. (2007). A longitudinal assessment of executive functioning skills and their association with math performance. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal

Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 13, 18-45. doi: 10.1080/09297040600611346

McKenzie, B., Bull, R., & Gray, C. (2003). The effects of phonological and visual spatial interference on children's arithmetical performance. Educational and Child Psychology, 20, 93-108.

Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2012). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review.

Developmental Psychology, 49, 270-291. doi: 10.1037/a0028228

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Moll, K., Göbel, S. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2014). Basic number processing in children with specific learning disorders: Comorbidity of reading and mathematics disorders. Child Neuropsychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.899570

(26)

26

Monette, S., Bigras, M., & Guay M. –C. (2011). The role of executive functions in school achievement at the

end of Grade 1. Journal of Experimental Chidl Psychology, 109, 158-173. doi:

10.1016/j.jecp.2011.01.008

Mononen, R., Aunio, P., & Koponen, T. (2014). A pilot study of the effects of RightStart instruction on early numeracy skills of children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 35, 999-1014. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.02.004

Mundy, E., & Gilmore, C. K. (2009). children's mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 490-502. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.003 Navarro, J. I., Aguilar, M., Alcalde, C., Ruiz, G., Marchena, E., & Menacho, I. (2011). Inhibitory processes,

working memory, phonological awareness, naming speed, and early arithmetic achievement. Spanish

Journal of Psychology, 14, 580-588. doi: 10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.6

Noël, M. -P, & Rousselle, L. (2011). Developmental changes in the profiles of dyscalculia: An explanation based on a double exact – and – approximate number representation model. Frontiers in Human

Neuroscience, 5, 165. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00165

Oberauer, K., Süß, H. -M., Wilhelm, O., & Wittman, W. W. (2003). The multiple faces of working memory: Storage, processing, supervision, and coordination. Intelligence, 31, 167-193. doi : 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00115-0

Opfer, J. E., Siegler, R. S., & Young, C. J. (2011). The powers of noise fitting: Reply to Barth and Paladino.

Developmental Science, 14, 1194-1204. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01070.x

Panaoura, A., & Philippou, G. (2007). The developmental change of young pupils' metacognitive ability in mathematics in relation to their cognitive abilities. Cognitive Development, 22, 149-164. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.08.004

Passolunghi, M. -C. Mammarella, I. C., & Altoè, G. (2008). Cognitive abilities as precursors of the early acquisition of mathematical skills during first through second grades. Developmental Neuropsychology,

33, 229-250. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982320

Passolunghi, M. -C., Vercelloni, B., & Schadee, H. (2007). The precursors of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological ability and numerical competence. Cognitive Development, 22, 165-184. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.09.001

Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., & Hecht, S. A. (2010). Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learning and Individual Differences,

20, 110-122. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.005

Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S., (2011). Reducing the gap in numerical knowledge between low- and middle-income pre-schoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32, 146-159. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2011.02.005

Rapport, M. D., Orban, S. A., Kofler, M. J., & Friedman, L. M. (2013). Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 1237-1252. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.005

Rasmussen, C., & Bisanz , J. (2005). Representation and working memory in early arithmetic. Journal of

(27)

27 1 | Gen eral In tr o d u cti o n

1

Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2011). Modeling a cascade of effects: the role of speed and executive functioning in preterm/full-term differences in academic achievement. Developmental

Science, 14, 1161-1175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01068.x

Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Michel, E., & Roebers, C. M. (2012). Improving executive functions in 5- and 6-year-olds: Evaluation of a small group intervention in prekindergarten and kindergarten children. Infant and Child Development, 21, 411-429. doi: 10.1002/icd.752

Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., Defever, E., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 344-357. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02048.x

Sasanguie, D., Göbel, S. M., Moll, K., Smets, K., & Reynvoet, B. (2013). Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: What underlies mathematics achievement? Journal of

Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 418-431. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.012

Sasanguie, D., Van den Bussche, E., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). Predictors for mathematics achievement? Evidence from a longitudinal study. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6, 119-128. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2012.01147.x

Siegler R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child

Development, 75, 428-444. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00684.x

Siegler R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14, 237-243. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.02438

Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2008). Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children’s numerical development. Developmental Science, 11, 655-661. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00714.x Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games – but not circular ones – improves

low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 545-560. doi: 10.1037/a0014239

Slusser, E. B., Santiago, R. T., & Barth, H. C. (2013). Developmental change in numerical estimation. Journal

of Experimental Psychology, 142, 193-208. doi: 10.1037/a0028560

Starr, A., Libertus, M. E., & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 18116-18120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302751110

St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 59, 745-759. doi: 10.1080/17470210500162854

Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Bergman Nutley, S., Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12, 106-113. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00745.x

Toll, S. W. M., Van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2011). Executive functions as predictors of math learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 521-532. doi: 10.1177/0022219410387302

(28)

28

Toll, S. W. M. & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2012). Early numeracy intervention for low-performing kindergartners.

Journal of Early Intervention, 34, 243-264. doi: 10.1177/1053815113477205

Toll, S. W. M., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2013a). Accelerating the early numeracy development of kindergartners with limited working memory skills through remedial education. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 34, 745-755. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.09.003

Toll, S. W. M., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2013b). The development of early numeracy ability in kindergartners with limited working memory skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 45-54. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.03.006

Van der Sluis, S., De Jong, P. F., & Van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427-449. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.001 Van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., Boom, J., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2012). The development of

executive functions and early mathematics: A dynamic relationship. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 100-119. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02035.x

Van der Ven, S. H. G., Van der Maas, H. L. J., Straatemeier, M., & Jansen, B. R. J. (2013). Visuospatial working memory and mathematical ability at different ages throughout primary school. Learning and

Individual Differences, 27, 182-192. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.09.003

Van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Kroesbergen, E. H., Prast, E. J., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2014). Validity and reliability of an online visual–spatial working memory task for self-reliant administration in school-aged children.

Behavior Research Methods. Advance online publication. doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0469-8

Van Luit, J. E. H., & Schopman, E. A. M. (2000). Improving early numeracy of young children with special educational needs. Remedial and Special Education, 21, 27-40. doi: 10.1177/074193250002100105 Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2004). Representation of number in animals and humans: A neural model. Journal of

Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1493-1504. doi: 10.1162/0898929042568497

Xenidou-Dervou, I., De Smedt, B., Van der Schoot, M., & Van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2013). Individual differences in kindergarten math achievement: The integrative roles of approximation skills and working memory. Learning and Individual Differences, 28, 119-129. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.09.012

(29)

29 2 | W o rkin g m em o ry and m athe m atics in pri m ary sc h o o l chi ld ren

2

Section 1

Meta-analyses

(30)
(31)

31 2 | W o rkin g m em o ry and m athe m atics in pri m ary sc h o o l chi ld ren

2

Chapter 2

Working memory and mathematics in primary school children:

A meta-analysis

I. Friso-van den Bos S. H. G. van der Ven E. H. Kroesbergen J. E. H. van Luit

Printed as: Friso-van den Bos, I., Van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2013). Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Educational

(32)

32

Abstract

Working memory, including central executive functions (inhibition, shifting and updating) are factors thought to play a central role in mathematical skill development. However, results reported with regard to the associations between mathematics and working memory components are inconsistent. The aim of this meta-analysis is twofold: to investigate the strength of this relation, and to establish whether the variation in the association is caused by tests, sample characteristics and study and other methodological characteristics. Results indicate that all working memory components are associated with mathematical performance, with the highest correlation between mathematics and verbal updating. Variation in the strength of the associations can consistently be explained by the type of mathematics measure used: general tests yield stronger correlations than more specific tests. Furthermore, characteristics of working memory measures, age and sample explain variance in correlations in some analyses. Interpretations of the contribution of moderator variables to various models are discussed.

Introduction

Solving mathematical problems is an important activity in the lives of children. From an early age onwards, learning to count, acquiring number skills, and performing mathematical operations become part of children’s daily activities. These activities remain important throughout a person’s life. Research concerning the underpinnings of success and deficiencies in mathematical skills has expanded during the past two decades. A number of cognitive mechanisms underlying these mathematical skills have been proposed and their contribution to the development of mathematical skill has been investigated. A factor that is thought to play a central role in mathematic skill development is the capacity and the efficiency of working memory and the executive functions: inhibition, shifting and updating (e.g., Bull & Scerif, 2001, Geary et al., 2004; Passolunghi et al., 2008; St Clair-Thompson & Gathercole, 2006). Working memory capacity is frequently used as a predictor of skills in mathematics at a later point in time (see LeFevre, DeStefano, Coleman, & Shanahan, 2005). The number of studies in which the predictive value of working memory and executive functions for mathematical performance is investigated has increased sharply, but the pattern of results is inconsistent: mathematics performance is not consistently predicted by one or all working memory components. Therefore, the present study serves as a meta-analysis of studies in which this relationship was investigated, to investigate whether each working memory component is related to mathematics performance. Moreover, to find an explanation for conflicting results, we investigated the influence of various moderator variables: the type

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

nu als voIgt: vijftien zetels zijn bestemd voor leden van het weten- schappelijk personeel, negen voor leden van het nlet wetenschappelijk personeel en negen

Door naar meerdere indicatoren te kijken wordt een melkveebedrijf niet alleen beoordeeld op basis van één onderwerp (bijvoorbeeld grondwaterkwaliteit).. Melkveehouders op

Note that to reliably determine a complete network path from the viewpoint of the receiver, the Internet would have to be completely symmetric.. End system B End system A AS 1 AS

Het nieuwe artikel 10a van de Advocatenwet biedt nu een (formeel) wettelijk kader voor de vijf kernwaarden van de Nederlandse advocatuur: onafhankelijkheid, partijdigheid,

(Hierdie artikel begin met die bydrae van Vollenhoven en sal met die denke van Dooyeweerd en Stoker opgevolg word.) As inleiding tot die reeks volg vooraf ’n besinning oor dit wat

Once we have selected the appropriate fault tolerance techniques and related archi- tectural tactics, they should be incorporated into the existing software architecture.

The following factors are used to control for cross-sectional asset pricing effect: MKT is the market factor, SMB and HML are the size and book-to-market factor

Koch, Birgitta König-Ries, Volker Markl and Maurice van Keulen Some of the application domains targeted by Trio are data cleaning and integration, information extraction, and