• No results found

Analysis of the driving task: System-analytical points of view

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Analysis of the driving task: System-analytical points of view"

Copied!
8
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

R-72-4

. A.,.'IALYSIS

OF

TllE DRIVING TASK: SYSTEM ANALYTICAL POINT$ OF Vlmv

D.J.

Griep, research psychoIogist

Institute

foi Road Safety Research SWOV, Voorburg, The Netherlands

...J

\

to be presented at OECD Symposium on rond user perception and

decision making,

nom~,

November

1972~

(2)

1. SYSTI0{-A.!.~ALYTICAL VIE'''POINTS

1.1. Introduction

Much road safety research so far has been done from a differential point of view'. It ''laS mainly concerned wi th demonstrating the , relevanee for accident liability of general and permanent differ-ences between 'drivers (such as personality, character, and

physiological, biologicalor anatomical characteristics). This research has little demonstrable relevanee for countermeasures, espccially for driver selection. A main reason is the low stability of accident liability as a driver characteristic.

Replacing driver selection by measures for influencing d~iving

,

behaviour by means of safety promotion, education, etc., differen-tial reseaich was aimed a~ more specific and time dependent

variables, like alcohol, age, driving experience and their effect on accident liability.

In the field of general driving behaviour research, at this tBDe, at least three directions can be distinghuised:

1. A cmnpilation of the sensori-mot~r, perceptive and cognitive functions relevant to driving ability.

2. Development of driving sBilul,.tors and instrumented vehicles.

3.

System-analytical research into the driving task, in ,.,hich driver, road, vehicle and traffic components are represented.

1.2. ITiernrchicnl relations in the driving task

The driving task can be'analysed at four different levels: 1. Choice of destination and means of transportation.

2. Choice of route (once destination and vehicle are decidcd).

3.

Choice of manoeuvre, related to vehicle guidance along the road, and relntively to other road users (given the route).

4.

Vehicle operation (acting out the manoeuvre).

(3)

- 2

-The level of analysis indicates the extent of thc loop (see Figure

1.1.).

Some remarks are:

Every level of analysis involves specific tasks (positioning thc destination on a map, the route in a network, the road in a route, the vcllicle in a track) ,';hich do imply specific activ-ities. This also ilnplies the possibility of overlap of different activities (for instance overtaking another vehicle and turning off at an intèrsection).

A more extensive loop is a higher hierarchy of control. Posi-tioning"the route incorrectly in a network

(9)

necessitates a

fresh choise of route (2), choise of manoeuvres

(3)

and so on

(9).

The more extensive the loop, the less specific and time-depen-dent the information will beo Concerning the most extended loop, information can be obtained by consul ting maps~ The relation bet'veen vehicle operation and response, ho'"ever, is detennined by characteristics that are specific (to make and type) and is also time-dependent (sensitive to vehicle load, ,vind force, etc.). Consequently in the case of higher ordered control the informa-tion can be based on more abstract codes (for exrunple a road number

(9)

agains t the distanc eper ceived bet'veen the vehi cle and the roadedge

(7).

1.3.

Criterion problems

The purpose of using the road can be formulated in terms of reaching a destination or providing activity. In the former case costs, time and effort of covering the distance betveen origin and destination can be evaluated negatively as they exceed those of alternative means of travel (public transport). In the latter case the activity inherent in travel (as driver or as passenger) can provide the opportunity for a positively rated effort and pastilne and is a means functionally unrelated to the objective of reaching a destination. The two objectives, hove~er, are not mutually exclusive.

In the following decisions regarding travel, choise of desti-natiol1 and means of transportation 'viII be disrcgapdcd. Their

(4)

4

-treatment is assmned to require a decisional-oriented

sociolo-gical approach.

There are a number of distinct aspects to reaching a

destina-tion:

1.

Routes can differ in distance (D) bet",een origin and

desti-nation.

2.

There may

pe

a difference in average speed

(V

per route.

j

ourney

time)

3.

Given the average speed for the route, there may be

a

varia-tion in speed during the route.(sd ).

v

4.

For each change in speed, a difference in av!rage

accelera-tion or deceleraaccelera-tion (a) is possible.

5.

Givcn the average acceleration or deceleration for change in

speed, there may be a variation in acceleration or deceleration

with su eh change in speed (sd ).

a

6.

And

so on.

The succession of aspects reflects an increase in

differentia-tion (distance, speed, acceleradifferentia-tion/deceleradifferentia-tion, etc.) and

grea ter susceptibil i ty to time-dependellt

var~iation.

Figure. 1.2.

illustrated this. Some remarks are:

Traffic flow stability increases if variation in movemer..ts of

the component elements can be described

,vi

th the help of 101,..er

derivatives.

The consequence of this is that manoeuvres for guiding the

vehi-cle, along the road and relatively to other road users, decrease

in number and complexity, Le. they can be selected by reference

to fe,",er and simplel' posi tion :r;.,eferences·.

Hanoeuvring leads to a change in the vehic'le' s lateral posi tion

and/or i ts speed. 'J.1hese changes can be described

,vi

th greater or

less differentiation (direction, distance, speed, etc.).

~iven

thc control dynmllics of the vellicle (see

F~gure

1.3.),

tbe

mov~­

~ents

folIo,", from the operation of the vehicle. (The relation

between vehicle operatioll and response will be disregarded as

(5)

i i

, I

;

\.

dcstination

1.

distance

I

(D)

1

"

2.

average

speed

(V)l

3.

variution

in

speed

~.

average

acceleration

or

deceleration

5.

vuriation

in

acceleration

or

deceleration

rigure

1.

2.

Hierarchy

of

movements

.I !

-l-ï

,--,

f

:

, , I t _______ _

(8:)2

(sd

)1

a \"JI I

I

I

l-ï

, -I I , I ( I

,---(ä)n

--

--'---• r

(sd'

)

2 a I

!

I _____ J

(sdu)p

(6)

'.

6

-vehicle

'control

handling

dynamics

vehicle

steerlug ",heel

\'i'heel

orientation

position

. ,

position

.

fuure

1.3.

Given the route, a shorter journey time irnplies a biggel' average

numbcr of decisions per unit of time and hence a biggel' nUllJber of

'irong decisions per unit of time. Thc ratio with the total

nurnber of decis ions, hO'iever, is determined by the di

scriminabi-litY,and the perceptibility of possible manoeuvres (both of them

depending on the structure of the driving situation).

\vrong decisions, unless corrected, resul t in collisions • 'vrong

decisions can be corrected if there is enough spare space

avail-able (for instance reservations) and/or spare response capacity

(of driver and vehicle) (for instance in acceleration). The

cor-rection of wrong decisions requires additional driver effort and

results in an additional wovement variation (acceleration or

de-ccleration laterally and/or longitudinally).

1.4. Journey

t~le,

driving effort and control level

The higher the turnover rate, the shorter the time wi thin 1"hich

feedback infonnation should be available. This may lead to:

1.

Greater perceptive-motor effort (gover:i:lcd by the driver

I

s

perceptual and response capacity and by the control

characteris-tics of the vehicle).

2.

Stronger anticipati'on of the input (preview control) (limited

by the irregularity of the input, for instance wind

gllS

ts , track

of road) •

(7)

7

-3.

More efficient selection and coding of information (limited by tbe

vay in vbicb tbe information is presented to and processed by tbe driver)~ 4. A lover-order control system, by selection of a less differentiated input (for instance a cbange of distance relative to tbe roadedge and/or relative to otber vebicles, instead of speed and/or acceleration of tbe relative cbange in position; tbis results in lower manoeuvring accur~cy). So far researcb bas been concentrated on manoeuvring bebaviour related to guiding tbe vebicle along tbe road and relative to otber vebicles. In analising driver's manoeuvring bebaviour, i t must be. borne in mind tbat information is not always coded in signals but must be obtained by tbe driver from direct perception of stimuli, sucb as road markings, ve-bicle lights. Rence, variation is possible in tbe selection and coding of

(input) information ,{hicb forms a complication in a cybernetic closed loop . approach.

1.5. Analysis of the driving task: summary

The "Analysis of the driving task" can be regarded as a link between general theories or human functioning and the design of specific vehicle, road and traffic charactcristics.

For road safety measures the part dealing vith manoeuvring seems to be of most direct importance.

Even vith this limitation the relevant theories cover almost the entire range of human functioning, while knovledge is also required of road, traf-fic and vehicle engineering aspects.

Hefore arriving at usefull models that take into account perception, infor-mation and decision processes at the one hand and vehicle operation and con-trol at the other, much future analytical and empirical vork appears to be done. In the meantime intermediate criteria have to be chosen. This applies also to the evaluation of driving aids and other improvements of driving conditionso

Research going on in the Netherlands is aimed at perceptual and information processing aspects of tbe drivers task in manoeuvering his vebicle along the road and relative to other vehicles.

Some of the results of this cooperative project (Institute for Perception and the Institute for Road Safety Research), vhich bears significance for the design of vehicle and road lighting, signalling and marking systems, viII be presented at this s~posium.

(8)

8

-REFERENCES

Griep, D.J. (1970). Propaganda and alternative countermeasures for road

safety. Accid. Anal.

&

Prev. 2 (1970) 2 : 127

tlm

140.

Kelley, Ch.R. (1968). Theory on manual and automatic control. New York,

Wiley.

Michael s, R.H.

&

Stephens , B.

Wo.

(1967). Hethods of human engineering.

SAE paper

No.

670719. N.Y. Society of Automotive Engineers.

OECD (Organisation for Economie Co-operation and Development) (1970).

Driver Behaviour. Prepared by the OECD Research Group on Driver

Beha-viour. Paris, OECD

o

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

The solution to the increased transdermal delivery of lipophilc drugs does not simply lie in producing a derivative with a higher aqueous solubiliiy and more ideal

Echter hebben Damerell, Howe en Milner-Gulland (2013) in hun onderzoek naar 161 kinderen in de leeftijd 7 tot 15 jaar voor het eerst aangetoond dat wel een relatie is tussen de

Concluding, by executing three steps: transcription of audio to text by means of ASR, filtering the transcriptions using NLP techniques to obtain the content of the discussions,

De bevindingen komen in redelijke mate overeen met het beleidsbroodje van Lipsky (2010, p.13 &159). De respondenten hebben een interne drive om cliënten kwalitatief goede zorg

classificatie voldaan, bepaalde gedragskenmerken zijn niet exclusief voor Autisme en bij ODD, ADHD en Angststoornissen komen nog andere gedragskenmerken voor die niet bij

Vanaf januari 2001 financiert PZ een deelproject in Koeien & Kansen dat antwoord moet geven op de vraag welke invloed een bedrijfsvoering, waarin scherper gevoerd en bemest

Eventuele verschillen in uitspoeling van bestrijdingsmiddelen zijn hierdoor niet te verklaren door het verschil in watergift.. Zowel in het fertigatievak als in het traditionele

In summary, the non-empirical, qualitative research design of a qualified systematic literature review is used to answer the research objectives.. The research design of this