• No results found

University of Groningen The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors across adult life span van Eersel, Maria Elisabeth Adriana

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "University of Groningen The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors across adult life span van Eersel, Maria Elisabeth Adriana"

Copied!
13
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors across adult life span

van Eersel, Maria Elisabeth Adriana

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date: 2018

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

van Eersel, M. E. A. (2018). The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors across adult life span. Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

7

(3)

SUMMARY AND GENERAL DISCUSSION

Worldwide, the prevalence of dementia is expected to increase to 75 million persons in 2030.1 Because, up till now, no curative treatment is available, the World Health

Organisation (WHO) recommends to conduct more research on prevention strategies for dementia.2 Cardiovascular diseases and dementia share similar pathogenetic processes,

such as atherosclerosis, causing by common vascular risk factors like hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus and smoking.3 Therefore, it is generally assumed

that early treatment of vascular risk factors might be an effective strategy to prevent dementia.

However, various randomized-controlled trials have found inconsistent results about the effect of treatment of vascular risk factors on cognitive performance.4 A possible

explanation for these results is that the treatment was initiated too late, as it was started at the age of 60 years or older. It could be argued that starting treatment at this age may be too late for effective prevention of cognitive impairment, as vascular risk factors may contribute to the onset of neurodegenerative changes several decades prior to the clinical expression of cognitive impairment.5,6 Therefore, in line with the recommendation of the WHO, the

general aim of this thesis was to explore the association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors and its treatment during the whole adult life span. We explored this association in the prospective observational cohort of the Prevention of REnal and Vascular ENd-stage Disease (PREVEND) study comprising persons aged 35 to 82 years old.7,8

We encountered several challenges in exploring cognitive performance and vascular risk factors in young and middle-aged persons. It is generally felt that previous studies exploring this association in this age group were limited by methods that were not sensitive enough to measure the changes in cognitive performance and vascular burden in young and middle-aged persons.9,10 Therefore, we searched for various solutions to these

methodological issues. First, we chose to measure cognitive performance with the Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) in addition to a memory test, because it is sensitive to the earliest changes in cognitive performance in both young and old persons.11,12 Second, by

using a well-validated vascular risk score like the Framingham Risk Score for Cardiovascular Disease, we explored the synergistic effects of vascular risk factors instead of focusing on a single risk factor.13 Moreover, vascular risk scores are particularly valuable to identify the

vascular burden in young and middle-aged persons because in this age group vascular risk factors often are only marginally elevated if considered separately but result in a clearly vascular burden if considered together.14 However, although age is a major vascular risk

factor, it is not amenable to treatment or prevention strategy. Therefore, we decided to

(4)

also explore the association of cognitive performance with treatable vascular risk factors independent of age. To this end, we used a treatable vascular risk score based on the treatable components of the Framingham Risk Score for Cardiovascular Disease.13 In the

next section, we describe the main findings of the thesis. Summary of the main findings

We explored the association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors and its treatment stepwise. First, we examined the association in both young and old persons cross-sectionally. Chapter 2 showed that a worse overall vascular risk was associated with poor cognitive performance. This was not only found in the older age groups, but also in the young age groups of 35-54 years old. In Chapter 3, we explored whether this association was different for young persons compared to old persons. We evaluated this research question with the single vascular risk factor type 2 diabetes mellitus as we found in Chapter 2 that diabetes mellitus was one of the main influencing components of vascular risk. Chapter

3 showed that persons with type 2 diabetes mellitus had poorer cognitive performance than

persons without diabetes mellitus. This confirmed the results of Chapter 2 that cognitive performance is negatively associated with vascular risk factors. Interestingly, Chapter 3 showed that the difference in cognitive performance between persons with and without diabetes mellitus was largest at age 35-44 years and gradually decreased with increasing age. Thus, the association of cognitive performance with diabetes mellitus was strongest in young persons.

Second, we further explored the association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors longitudinally. Before evaluating this, it was necessary to evaluate the longitudinal performance on the Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) because, up till now, it was unknown how the RFFT score after repeated measurements could be interpreted. In the PREVEND study, the performance on the RFFT was measured three times over an average follow-up period of six years. Chapter 4 showed that the performance on the RFFT increased by repeated testing. This is in contrast with the expectation that cognitive performance declines with increasing age. So, repeating the RFFT causes a practice effect. In addition, the increase in performance on the RFFT was largest in young persons and gradually decreased with increasing age. The increase in performance on the RFFT was not dependent on educational level. Thus, in longitudinal designs we have to adjust the change in cognitive performance measured by the RFFT for a practice effect.

Accordingly, it could be investigated whether the change in cognitive performance was influenced by vascular risk as measured with the treatable vascular risk score. Chapter 5 showed that the change in cognitive performance was negatively associated with treatable

(5)

vascular risk in both young and old persons. This supported the hypothesis that treatment of vascular risk factors might be effective as prevention of cognitive performance.

Finally, in Chapter 6, we explored whether treatment of vascular risk factors could be useful in preventing cognitive impairment. We evaluated this by comparing the change in cognitive performance in persons with and without treatment of vascular risk factors during a follow-up period of six years. Treatment of vascular risk factors included pharmacological treatment of hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus and prevention of arterial thrombotic events. The change in cognitive performance did not differ between the persons with and without treatment of vascular risk factors despite the fact that at baseline the persons with treatment were older, had high treatable vascular risk and worse cognitive performance. In other words, we found no evidence that pharmacological treatment of vascular risk factors did improve the cognitive performance of persons with a high treatable vascular risk.

To summarize, this thesis described that vascular risk factors like hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus and smoking are associated with cognitive performance during the whole adult life span. The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors was strongest at young age. Therefore, it could be hypothesized that starting treatment of vascular risk factors in as early stage as possible might be effective as prevention of cognitive impairment. However, in this thesis we could not confirm this hypothesis as treatment of vascular risk factors did not improve nor worsen cognitive performance of young and old persons with a high treatable vascular risk during a period of six years.

General Discussion

This thesis underlines two important considerations for further research on prevention strategies for cognitive impairment. First, it should be considered to perform this research during the whole adult life span as vascular risk factors are associated with cognitive performance at a young age. However, this implicates several methodological issues in exploring the effect of a prevention strategy on cognitive performance. Second, it should be considered to start the prevention of cognitive impairment as early as possible and, therefore, in young persons. However, young persons are then exposed to longer duration of prevention and to possible adverse events. These two important considerations are discussed in the next sections.

(6)

Prevention research during the whole adult life span

In this thesis, we found that the changes in cognitive performance caused by vascular risk factors start at a relatively young age (Chapter 2 and 5). This confirmed the hypothesis of Muller et al. that cognitive performance gradually declines from the third decade of life and that vascular risk factors contribute to this process.15 As well, it supported the observation

from the Whitehall study that cognitive impairment is already present at a relatively young age.16 Consequently, the prevalence of cognitive impairment increases from the fourth

decade of life in the same way as the prevalence of other chronic diseases such as osteoarthritis, cardiovascular disease, diabetes mellitus and cancer.17,18 It is likely that the

biological processes responsible for all these chronic diseases start several years prior to the clinical expression of the disease.19,20 Subsequently, it could be hypothesized that

starting treatment to stop or alter these biological processes in as early stage as possible might be effective as prevention. Therefore, research on prevention strategies for chronic diseases like cognitive impairment should not only be done in old persons, but primarly in young persons.

Most interestingly, the association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors was strongest in young persons (Chapter 3). There are several explanations for this finding. First, the onset of vascular risk factors may be years before the clinical diagnosis.19-21 It

is likely that in this preclinical period, untreated chronic hyperglycemia or hypertension already causes important and irreversible changes in the brain. For example, the vascular risk factors cause cerebrovascular lesions and atherosclerosis inducing cerebral hypoperfusion.5,22 Second, both people with and without vascular risk factors undergo

age-related neurodegenerative changes that are caused by other mechanisms such as, for example, oxidative stress, mitochondrial dysfunction, protein misfolding, alterations in synapse integrity and dendritic spine remodeling.23,24 In addition, cerebral atherosclerosis is

also associated with aging.25 Therefore, it can be suggested that the association between

cognitive performance and vascular risk factors is weaker at old age by the competitive accumulation of different age-related cerebrovascular and neurodegenerative changes. The hypothesis is further supported by the finding that vascular risk factors may have a different predictive value in young and old persons. Several studies found, for example, that the presence of hypertension in midlife is associated with elevated risk on dementia, but this association is less certain when hypertension occurs in late-life.26,27 Accordingly,

the effect of vascular risk factors on brain structure and cognitive performance may change across the adult life span.

Therefore, it needs to be considered to investigate prevention strategies for cognitive impairment during the whole adult life span. However, this leads to methodological issues

(7)

in exploring the effect of treatment of vascular risk factors on cognitive performance as prevention strategy. The most important issues are the choice of the primary outcome and the sensitivity of common risk scores to measure vascular burden in young and middle-aged persons.

Methodological issue: primary outcome

An issue in the research on prevention strategies for dementia is the choice of the primary outcome. Prevention or delaying the onset of dementia is generally preferred as primary outcome in current dementia prevention trials because of its apparently unambiguous definition according to existing guidelines.28 However, it should be considered to change

the aim of these trials in prevention of cognitive impairment. The decline in cognitive performance before the clinical diagnosis of dementia may already have a large impact on quality of life for both patient and caregiver. Therefore, it is clinically more relevant to prevent cognitive impairment. Moreover, by using dementia as a primary outcome, the follow-up period has to be long and the sample size large to detect differences in incident dementia when the prevention starts at a relatively young age.28,29 For example, Richard

et al. calculated a required sample size of 15,000 subjects for a dementia prevention trial

when starting an intervention at the age of 60-70 years.28 This does not appear to be

feasible. Therefore, we recommend using the changes in cognitive performance as primary outcome in the research on prevention strategies for cognitive impairment to make these trials feasible and clinically relevant.

The changes in cognitive performance could be measured with cognitive function tests. However, these tests have to meet a number of criteria to measure the small changes in cognitive performance over time in young persons. They need to be sensitive enough to the earliest changes in cognitive performance in young persons. As well, repeating the cognitive function test might cause a practice effect resulting in an increase in test score. Therefore, the change in cognitive performance over time should be well interpreted.30 In this thesis,

we used the figure fluency test RFFT because previous studies showed its sensitivity to the earliest changes in cognitive performance in both young and old persons.11,12 As a result,

we found that cognitive performance declines at a relatively young age (Chapter 2, 3 and 5). To monitor the change in cognitive performance over time, it is common practice to repeat the RFFT. Repeating the RFFT leads to an increase in test score possibly due to a practice effect (Chapter 4). Practice effects can be ascribed to different factors such as familiarity with the test, memory of specific test items and learning test strategies.31,32 Interestingly,

the practice effect of the RFFT persisted three to six years after the first measurement of

(8)

the RFFT (Chapter 4). This longstanding practice effect was also found in other cognitive function tests such as the Verbal Learning Test (VLT) and the Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT).33 They were even detectable up to seven years after the first measurement of

cognitive performance.34 Therefore, practice effects in cognitive function tests are most

likely not avoidable and have to be accounted for in longitudinal designs.

Practice effects, or in other words learning, may be a reflection of cognitive performance. A frequent early sign of dementia is impairment of the cognitive domain memory and learning.35 Subsequently, an improvement in performance on a cognitive function test at

repeated testing may be the result of learning and represent a stable course of cognitive performance. Similarly, a stable performance on a cognitive function test at repeated testing does most likely not reflect a stable course of cognitive performance but decline. Therefore, we think that practice effects are a valuable outcome to evaluate the changes in cognitive performance over time. However, in order to have an estimate of the variance in performance due to practice effects in a repeated measurement design we need more longitudinal data in healthy persons.

Methodological issue: measurement of vascular risk factors

An important issue in exploring the effect of treatment of vascular risk factors on cognitive performance is the measurement of vascular risk factors across the adult life span. Vascular burden in young persons can be underestimated because single vascular risk factors often are only marginally elevated. However, vascular risk factors often occur together and act via shared biological pathways that may finally result in a clearly elevated vascular burden.14 In

this thesis, we found by using a vascular risk score that young persons may have the same vascular burden as older persons (Chapter 2). Therefore, by considering the synergistic effect of vascular risk factors in a vascular risk score,13 it may reflect more accurately the

vascular burden in both young and old persons.

Importantly, we have to realize that vascular risk scores are largely based on age.13,36

Although age is a major vascular risk factor, age is not amenable to treatment or prevention strategy. Therefore, we decided to explore the association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors by using a treatable vascular risk. The treatable vascular risk was based on the treatable risk factors hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus and smoking, independently of age. In this thesis, we found that the change in cognitive performance was associated with treatable vascular risk (Chapter 5). This suggests that treatment of vascular risk factors might be effective as prevention of cognitive impairment. But this is still not at all of proof of efficacy.

(9)

Pharmacological treatment of vascular risk factors as prevention

So far, in this thesis, we found that vascular risk factors are associated with cognitive performance at a relatively young age and, therefore, treatment of vascular risk factors may have the most effect on cognitive performance if it is started at a young age (Chapter

2, 3 and 5). So, we decided to investigate in a large community-based cohort whether

treatment of vascular risk factors could be effective as prevention for cognitive impairment. Interestingly, we found that during a follow-up period of six years the changes in cognitive performance was similar between the persons with and without treatment of vascular risk factors despite the fact that at baseline persons with treatment were older, had higher treatable vascular risk and worse cognitive performance (Chapter 6). This suggests that lowering the vascular risk with treatment of vascular risk factors does not improve the cognitive performance. However, we found also that treatment of vascular risk factors does not worsen cognitive performance in people with a high treatable vascular risk. This is in contrast with other observational studies that suggest, for example, that lowering blood pressure in older people might lead to cerebral hypoperfusion and thus to poorer cognitive performance.37,38 Therefore, our findings suggest that treatment of vascular risk factors does

not improve cognitive performance, but it does also not worsen the cognitive performance. It is generally acknowledged that observational studies may overestimate treatment effect as compared to (subsequent) RCTs on the same questions.39 When exploring the

effect of treatment of vascular risk factors on cognitive performance with an RCT, there are several challenges to overcome. As described previously, such a RCT requires a large sample and long follow-up to detect significant differences in cognitive performance between the treatment and the control group when the treatment starts at the age of 30 years.28,29

Additionally, the importance of vascular risk management to prevent cardiovascular disease is undisputed and, therefore, withholding or withdrawing treatment in control subjects for a long period would be unethical.28,40 Consequently, it is in fact infeasible to perform an RCT

exploring treatment of vascular risk factors as prevention for cognitive impairment in young persons. Therefore, we think that our large observational cohort study comprising young, middle-aged and old persons adds valuable insights to what is known from recent RCTs in old persons.

Furthermore, it is unclear whether pharmacological treatment of vascular risk factors is cost effective as prevention strategy for cognitive impairment. An intervention that delays cognitive impairment could reduce the impact of this disease on quality of life for patient and caregiver. However, the question is whether pharmacological treatment of vascular risk

(10)

factors is a good intervention to reduce the burden of cognitive impairment. Prevention of cognitive impairment with treatment of vascular risk factors is not only accompanied with high costs of drugs and doctor visits,41 but also with the daily effort of an individual to take

drugs possibly from his third decade of life. In addition, the individuals are exposed for a long period of time to possible adverse effects of these drugs which may result in other chronic diseases such as, for example, hepatitis or pancreatitis.41 So, there are also disadvantages

to the treatment of vascular risk factors as prevention of cognitive impairment. Therefore, future research on the underlying mechanisms of cognitive impairment is necessary to know whether there are other opportunities to prevent cognitive impairment.

Future perspectives

Although the WHO recommends to conduct more research on prevention strategies for cognitive impairment, we need to ask ourselves to what extent cognitive impairment can be prevented. The neurodegenerative changes in the brain related to cognitive impairment start at a relatively young age.6,16 Therefore, it is likely that prevention may have the most

effect on cognitive performance if it is started at a young age. However, young persons are then exposed to longer duration of prevention and to possible adverse effects.42 In

this thesis, we found that treatment of vascular risk factors did not improve nor worsen the cognitive performance of persons with a high vascular risk. If we weigh this against the costs and possible adverse events, it seems likely that pharmacological treatment of vascular risk factors is the first appropriate prevention strategy for cognitive impairment. Therefore, more research is needed to explore whether other risk factors of cognitive impairment can be useful as prevention target for cognitive impairment.

Other risk factors are increasing age and genetic factors that may contribute to the neuropathological changes related to cognitive impairment. Unfortunately, genetic factors are yet not modifiable factors and, therefore, not useful as prevention target for cognitive impairment. Consequently, age is maybe the only risk factor that can be used as prevention target for cognitive impairment. However, up till now, several age-related biological changes in the brain associated with cognitive impairment are not yet fully understood, such as, for example, shrinkage in brain volume, alteration in synapse integrity and dendritic remodeling.24,43,44 Therefore, future research on the underlying mechanisms of age-related

cognitive impairment is necessary to know whether there are opportunities to prevent cognitive impairment.

(11)

References

1. Alzheimer’s Disease International. World Alzheimer Report 2015: The Global Impact of Dementia: An analysis of prevalence, incidence, cost and trends. London, 2015:1-84.

2. World Health Organization. Dementia: a public health priority. 2012: http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstre am/10665/75263/1/9789241564458_eng.pdf?ua=1. Accessed on 8 November, 2017.

3. Qiu C, Fratiglioni L. A major role for cardiovascular burden in age-related cognitive decline. Nat Rev Cardiol 2015 May;12(5):267-277.

4. Ligthart SA, Moll van Charante EP, Van Gool WA, Richard E. Treatment of cardiovascular risk factors to prevent cognitive decline and dementia: a systematic review. Vasc Health Risk Manag 2010 Sep 7;6:775-785.

5. Rodrigue KM, Rieck JR, Kennedy KM, Devous MD S, Diaz-Arrastia R, Park DC. Risk factors for beta-amyloid deposition in healthy aging: vascular and genetic effects. JAMA Neurol 2013 May;70(5):600-606. 6. Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Craft S, Fagan AM, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011 May;7(3):280-292.

7. Mahmoodi BK, Gansevoort RT, Veeger NJ, Matthews AG, Navis G, Hillege HL, et al. Microalbuminuria and risk of venous thromboembolism. JAMA 2009 May 6;301(17):1790-1797.

8. Lambers Heerspink HJ, Brantsma AH, de Zeeuw D, Bakker SJ, de Jong PE, Gansevoort RT, et al. Albuminuria assessed from first-morning-void urine samples versus 24-hour urine collections as a predictor of cardiovascular morbidity and mortality. Am J Epidemiol 2008 Oct 15;168(8):897-905. 9. Yaffe K, Vittinghoff E, Pletcher MJ, Hoang TD, Launer LJ, Whitmer R, et al. Early adult to midlife

cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function. Circulation 2014 Apr 15;129(15):1560-1567. 10. Rovio SP, Pahkala K, Nevalainen J, Juonala M, Salo P, Kahonen M, et al. Cardiovascular Risk Factors

From Childhood and Midlife Cognitive Performance: The Young Finns Study. J Am Coll Cardiol 2017 May 9;69(18):2279-2289.

11. Ruff R, Light R, Evans R. The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: A normative study with adults. Dev Neuropsychol 1987;3:37-51.

12. Izaks GJ, Joosten H, Koerts J, Gansevoort RT, Slaets JP. Reference data for the Ruff Figural Fluency Test stratified by age and educational level. PLoS One 2011 Feb 10;6(2):e17045.

13. D’Agostino RB S, Vasan RS, Pencina MJ, Wolf PA, Cobain M, Massaro JM, et al. General cardiovascular risk profile for use in primary care: the Framingham Heart Study. Circulation 2008 Feb 12;117(6):743-753.

14. Lloyd-Jones DM, Leip EP, Larson MG, D’Agostino RB, Beiser A, Wilson PW, et al. Prediction of lifetime risk for cardiovascular disease by risk factor burden at 50 years of age. Circulation 2006 Feb 14;113(6):791-798.

15. Muller M, Sigurdsson S, Kjartansson O, Jonsson PV, Garcia M, von Bonsdorff MB, et al. Birth size and brain function 75 years later. Pediatrics 2014 Oct;134(4):761-770.

16. Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, Ebmeier KP, et al. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ 2012 Jan 5;344:d7622. 17. Yach D, Hawkes C, Gould CL, Hofman KJ. The global burden of chronic diseases: overcoming

impediments to prevention and control. JAMA 2004 Jun 2;291(21):2616-2622.

18. Denton FT, Spencer BG. Chronic health conditions: changing prevalence in an aging population and some implications for the delivery of health care services. Can J Aging 2010 Mar;29(1):11-21. 19. Rader DJ. Effect of insulin resistance, dyslipidemia, and intra-abdominal adiposity on the development

of cardiovascular disease and diabetes mellitus. Am J Med 2007 Mar;120(3 Suppl 1):S12-8.

20. Harris MI, Klein R, Welborn TA, Knuiman MW. Onset of NIDDM occurs at least 4-7 yr before clinical diagnosis. Diabetes Care 1992 Jul;15(7):815-819.

21. Beckman JA, Creager MA, Libby P. Diabetes and atherosclerosis: epidemiology, pathophysiology, and management. JAMA 2002 May 15;287(19):2570-2581.

22. Gispen WH, Biessels GJ. Cognition and synaptic plasticity in diabetes mellitus. Trends Neurosci 2000 Nov;23(11):542-549.

(12)

23. Moreira PI, Duarte AI, Santos MS, Rego AC, Oliveira CR. An integrative view of the role of oxidative stress, mitochondria and insulin in Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2009;16(4):741-761. 24. Dickstein DL, Kabaso D, Rocher AB, Luebke JI, Wearne SL, Hof PR. Changes in the structural

complexity of the aged brain. Aging Cell 2007 Jun;6(3):275-284.

25. Pantoni L. Cerebral small vessel disease: from pathogenesis and clinical characteristics to therapeutic challenges. Lancet Neurol 2010 Jul;9(7):689-701.

26. Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol 2005 Aug;4(8):487-499.

27. Tolppanen AM, Solomon A, Soininen H, Kivipelto M. Midlife vascular risk factors and Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from epidemiological studies. J Alzheimers Dis 2012;32(3):531-540.

28. Richard E, Andrieu S, Solomon A, Mangialasche F, Ahtiluoto S, Moll van Charante EP, et al. Methodological challenges in designing dementia prevention trials - the European Dementia Prevention Initiative (EDPI). J Neurol Sci 2012 Nov 15;322(1-2):64-70.

29. Andrieu S, Coley N, Aisen P, Carrillo MC, DeKosky S, Durga J, et al. Methodological issues in primary prevention trials for neurodegenerative dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2009;16(2):235-270.

30. Salthouse TA. Influence of age on practice effects in longitudinal neurocognitive change. Neuropsychology 2010 Sep;24(5):563-572.

31. Bartels C, Wegrzyn M, Wiedl A, Ackermann V, Ehrenreich H. Practice effects in healthy adults: a longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing. BMC Neurosci 2010 Sep 16;11:118-2202-11-118.

32. Calamia M, Markon K, Tranel D. Scoring higher the second time around: meta-analyses of practice effects in neuropsychological assessment. Clin Neuropsychol 2012 May;26(4):543-570.

33. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. Detecting the significance of changes in performance on the Stroop Color-Word Test, Rey’s Verbal Learning Test, and the Letter Digit Substitution Test: the regression-based change approach. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008 Jan;14(1):71-80.

34. Salthouse TA, Schroeder DH, Ferrer E. Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age. Dev Psychol 2004 Sep;40(5):813-822. 35. Eslinger PJ, Damasio AR. Preserved motor learning in Alzheimer’s disease: implications for anatomy

and behavior. J Neurosci 1986 Oct;6(10):3006-3009

36. Conroy RM, Pyorala K, Fitzgerald AP, Sans S, Menotti A, De Backer G, et al. Estimation of ten-year risk of fatal cardiovascular disease in Europe: the SCORE project. Eur Heart J 2003 Jun;24(11):987-1003. 37. Euser SM, van Bemmel T, Schram MT, Gussekloo J, Hofman A, Westendorp RGJ, et al. The effect of

age on the association between blood pressure and cognitive function later in life. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009 Jul;57(7):1232-1237.

38. Nilsson SE, Read S, Berg S, Johansson B, Melander A, Lindblad U. Low systolic blood pressure is associated with impaired cognitive function in the oldest old: longitudinal observations in a population-based sample 80 years and older. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2007;19(1):41-47.

39. Hemkens LG, Contopoulos-Ioannidis DG, Ioannidis JP. Agreement of treatment effects for mortality from routinely collected data and subsequent randomized trials: meta-epidemiological survey. BMJ 2016 Feb 8;352:i493.

40. Richard F, Pasquier F. Can the treatment of vascular risk factors slow cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease patients? J Alzheimers Dis 2012;32(3):765-772.

41. Farmacotherapeutisch Kompas. 2017: www.farmacotherapeutischkompas.nl. Accessed on 8 Novem-ber, 2017.

42. Dorresteijn JA, Kaasenbrood L, Cook NR, van Kruijsdijk RC, van der Graaf Y, Visseren FL, et al. How to translate clinical trial results into gain in healthy life expectancy for individual patients. BMJ 2016 Mar 30;352:i1548.

43. Lockhart SN, DeCarli C. Structural imaging measures of brain aging. Neuropsychol Rev 2014 Sep;24(3):271-289.

44. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol 2009;60:173-196.

(13)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

1.. 0.22% per year between the age of 20 and 80 years with accelerated decline with increasing age. 16 Neuropathological changes in the brain like amyloid β and

Abbreviations: FRS, Framingham Risk Score for general cardiovascular disease and includes age, gender, diabetes mellitus, current smoker status, systolic blood pressure, use

Study (DCS) that also found a statistically significant negative association of type 2 diabetes with global cognitive function in young and middle-aged people after adjustment for

The aim of this study was to investigate the longitudinal performance on the RFFT by repeating the test over an average follow-up period of three and six years in a large cohort

Treatable vascular risk is based on the components of Framingham Risk Score for Cardiovascular Disease that are amenable to treatment and included diabetes mellitus, current

If the change in cognitive performance was adjusted for propensity score, the covariate-adjusted linear mixed model analysis also neither showed a statistically significant

In hoofdstuk 5 werd het negatieve verband beschreven, dat bleek te bestaan tussen de verandering van de cognitieve functies gedurende zes jaar follow-up en het behandelbare

Ik wil alle internisten uit Zwolle bedanken voor mijn eerste drie jaar opleiding in de Isala Klinieken te Zwolle: jullie enthousiasme voor het vak Interne heeft mij gevormd tot