• No results found

University of Groningen The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors across adult life span van Eersel, Maria Elisabeth Adriana

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "University of Groningen The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors across adult life span van Eersel, Maria Elisabeth Adriana"

Copied!
21
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors across adult life span

van Eersel, Maria Elisabeth Adriana

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date: 2018

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

van Eersel, M. E. A. (2018). The association of cognitive performance with vascular risk factors across adult life span. Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

6

Treatment of Vascular Risk Factors and

Cognitive Performance in Persons Aged

35 Years or Older: Longitudinal Study

Marlise E.A. van Eersel

Sipke T. Visser

Hanneke Joosten

Ron T. Gansevoort

Joris P.J. Slaets

Gerbrand J. Izaks

(3)

ABSTRACT

Background: Lowering vascular risk is associated with a decrease in the prevalence of

cardiovascular disease and dementia. However, it is still unknown whether lowering of vascular risk protects cognitive performance. Therefore, we compare the change in cognitive performance in persons with and without treatment of vascular risk factors.

Methods: In this longitudinal study, 256 persons (mean age, 58 years) had treatment for

vascular risk factors during the whole follow-up (treatment group) and 1,678 persons (mean age, 50 years) had no treatment (control group). Cognitive performance was three times measured during follow-up of 5.5 years with Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) and Visual Association Test (VAT), and calculated as average of standardized RFFT and VAT score per participant. Because treatment allocation was nonrandomized, additional analyses were performed in demographic and vascular risk-matched samples and adjusted for propensity scores.

Results: In treatment group mean (SD) cognitive performance changed from -0.30 (0.80)

to -0.23 (0.80) to 0.02 (0.87), and in control group from 0.08 (0.77) to 0.24 (0.79) to 0.49 (0.74) at first, second and third measurement, respectively (Ptrend <.001). After adjustment for demographics and vascular risk, the change in cognitive performance during follow-up was not statistically significantly different between the treatment and control group: mean estimated difference, -0.10 (95%CI, -0.21 to 0.01; P = 0.08). Similar results were found in matched samples and after adjustment for propensity score.

Conclusion: Change in cognitive performance during follow-up was similar in treated and

untreated persons. This suggests that lowering vascular risk does not improve nor worsen

(4)

INTRODUCTION

Worldwide, the prevalence of dementia is expected to reach 66 million persons in 2030.1

Because, up till now, no curative treatment is available, there is an increasing urge to prevent dementia in its earliest stages.2 Cardiovascular diseases and dementia share

similar pathogenetic processes, such as atherosclerosis, activating by common vascular risk factors like hypertension and hypercholesterolemia.3 Therefore, it is generally assumed

that treatment of vascular risk factors could be an effective strategy to prevent dementia. The effect of treatment of vascular risk factors on cognitive performance was investigated in various randomized-controlled trials (RCTs) but, unfortunately, no positive effect was found.4-7 A possible explanation for this lack of effect might be that the intervention in these

RCTs was focused on only one vascular risk factor. This limitation was acknowledged in the FINGER and preDIVA trials that investigated the effect of a multidomain vascular intervention.8-9In the FINGER trial, this intervention was associated with better cognitive

performance. However, the intervention also included cognitive training which could have influenced cognitive performance positively.8 In the preDIVA trial, on the other hand, there

was no positive effect of the multidomain vascular intervention on cognitive performance, possibly because there was a similar reduction in vascular risk in the intervention and control group.9 Therefore, it is still unknown whether treatment of vascular risk factors is

positively associated with cognitive performance.

The aim of this longitudinal observational study was to investigate the association of treatment of vascular risk factors with cognitive performance by comparing the change in cognitive performance of persons with and without treatment of vascular risk factors over a follow-up period of nearly six years. The study included 1,934 persons aged 35-82 years, who completed two to three measurements of cognitive performance and vascular risk.

6

(5)

METHODS

Study design

This study was part of the Prevention of REnal and Vascular ENd-stage Disease (PREVEND) study, a prospective observational study in the city of Groningen, the Netherlands. The data were collected in the third, fourth and fifth survey of PREVEND that included assessment of demographic and vascular risk factors, measurement of hematological and biochemical parameters, and tests of cognitive function.10-12 The change in cognitive performance during

follow-up was compared between the treatment and control group.

The PREVEND study was approved by the medical ethics committee (METc) of University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands, and conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Helsinki declaration. All participants gave written informed consent.

Allocation of treatment groups

Allocation was nonrandomized. Participants were allocated to the treatment group if they had pharmacological treatment of vascular risk factors for the first time ≤100 days before the first measurement of cognitive performance and continued treatment during follow-up. Participants were allocated to the control group if they did not have any pharmacological treatment of vascular risk factors during the whole study period. Treatment of vascular risk factors included pharmacological treatment of hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus and prevention of arterial thrombotic events. Pharmacological data were obtained from the IADB.nl prescription database.13

Cognitive performance

Cognitive performance was measured as a composite score of two tests: the Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) and the Visual Association Test (VAT). The RFFT is a measure of executive function and provides information regarding planning, divergent thinking and the ability to shift between different cognitive tasks. The lowest (worst) score is 0 points, the highest (best) score is 175 points.14,15 The RFFT is sensitive to changes in cognitive

performance in young and old persons.14,16 The VAT is a brief learning task that is designed

to detect memory impairment including anterograde amnesia. The lowest (worst) score is 0 points, the highest (best) score is 12 points.17

(6)

To create a composite cognitive score, the raw RFFT and VAT scores at each measurement were standardized to z-scores (based on the mean and standard deviation of each test at the first measurement) and subsequently averaged.

Covariates

Data on age, gender and educational level were obtained from a questionnaire. Educational level was divided into two groups: low level (0-12 years of education) and high level (≥13 years of education).18 A history of cardiovascular events was defined as a prior cardiac,

cerebrovascular or peripheral vascular event requiring hospitalization and was derived from the Dutch national registry of hospital discharge diagnoses during follow-up.

Treatable vascular risk was based on the components of the Framingham Risk Score for Cardiovascular Disease (FRS-CD) that are amenable to treatment: diabetes mellitus (yes/ no), current smoker status (yes/no), systolic blood pressure (mmHg), total cholesterol (mmol/l), HDL cholesterol (mmol/l) and use of blood pressure lowering drugs (yes/no). The FRS-CD is 11designed to predict the risk of a new cardiovascular event within the next ten

years.19 Details on the measurements of the separate risk score components are described

previously.20

Propensity score

A propensity score balances covariates in observational studies associated with the prescription of medication and is used to reduce bias by indication in non-randomized studies.21 In this study, the propensity score for treatment of vascular risk factors was

calculated by a logistic regression model. The dependent variable was treatment of vascular risk factors (yes, no). The independent variables were demographic factors, vascular risk factors and (family-) history of cardiovascular disease as described in Table 1. These variables were selected because in other studies, it was found that they are (potentially) associated with the prescription of treatment of vascular risk factors whereas they may also be associated with cognitive function.22,23 Because the focus of the regression model was

on optimal prediction, every initial variable was left in the model, regardless of the level of statistical significance of its coefficient. The R square of the full model was 0.37.

(7)

Group

Demographic factors

Vascular risk factors

Medical history

Variables Age (years)

Gender (men, women) Educational level (low, high) Race (Western-descent, other)

Social situation (live alone, live with partner without children, live with partner and children, live without partner and with children),

Work situation (job, unemployed, unable to work, retired) Net income per month (<€1,200, €1,200-1,799, €1,800-2,199, €2,200-2,799, €2,800-3,799, €3,800-5,800, >€5,800)

Current smoker status (yes, no) Presence of diabetes mellitus (yes, no) Cholesterol (mmol/L)

Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) Body mass index (kg/m2)

Waist circumference (cm) Use of alcohol (yes, no)

Regular physical exercise (yes, no)

Presence of left ventricular hypertrophy on ECG (yes, no) Presence of albuminuria ≥30mg/24 hours (yes, no) Presence of history of cardiovascular disease (yes, no) Presence of family history of cardiovascular disease (yes, no)

Table 1. The independent variables that were included in logistic regression model to calculate the

estimated propensity score for treatment of vascular risk factors.

(8)

Matching

A matched subsample of participants without and with treatment of vascular risk factors was created by one-to-one matching on age, gender, educational level and treatable vascular risk.

Statistical analysis

Parametric data are presented as mean and standard deviation (SD) and nonparametric data as median and interquartile range (IQR). Differences were tested by the independent-samples t test or, if appropriate, Mann-Whitney U test. Differences between paired observations were tested by the paired-samples t test or, if appropriate, Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Differences in proportion were tested by Chi-Square test. Trends across measurements were analyzed by ANOVA for parametric data and by the Kruskal-Wallis H test for nonparametric data.

The longitudinal association of cognitive performance with treatment of vascular risk factors was investigated by linear multilevel analysis (linear mixed model analysis). Cognitive performance was the dependent variable. Treatment of vascular risk factors (yes, no) was the independent variable. The analysis included the data of all participants who completed the cognitive tests on at least two measurements. Consecutive measurement (1,2,3) was the lowest level and participant the highest level. In this model, a significant main effect for treatment indicates an overall treatment effect over all three measurements. The interaction term treatment x consecutive measurement number was added to assess the treatment effect at the different measurements (1,2,3). First, adjustment was made for consecutive measurement number, age, interaction age x consecutive measurement number, educational level and treatable vascular risk. Second, adjustment was made for consecutive measurement number and propensity score. In all models, the variables cognitive performance, age (years) and treatable vascular risk (points) were entered as continuous variables. Treatment of vascular risk factors (yes, no), consecutive measurement number (1,2,3) and educational level were entered as categorical variables. The level of statistical significance was set at 0.05. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 22.0 (IBM, Amonk, NY).

(9)

RESULTS

Study population

A total of 3,601 persons completed the cognitive tests at multiple measurements: 2,431 (68%) persons at three measurements and 1,170 (32%) persons at two measurements. Of those, 21 persons (0.6%) were excluded because of incomplete demographic data, 8 persons (0.2%) because of missing data on treatable vascular risk, 484 (13%) persons because of missing data on pharmacological treatment and 1,154 (32%) persons because of treatment of vascular risk factors before the first measurement or started during follow-up (Figure 1). Thus, the total study population included 1,934 persons. The mean age (SD) was 51 (10) years, 47% was men and 96% was of Western-European descent (Table 2).

Two hundred fifty-six persons (12%) had pharmacological treatment of vascular risk factors for the first time at the first measurement of cognitive performance and continued during follow-up. Persons in the treatment group were older and had a lower educational level compared to persons of the control group. The prevalence of cardiovascular history was higher in the treatment group. Also, persons of the treatment group had a higher treatable vascular risk than persons of the control group (Table 2). In addition, the treatable vascular risk of the treatment group did not change statistically significantly during follow-up despite pharmacologically treatment of vascular risk factors (P = 0.41).

(10)

Figure 1. Flowchart of the selection of the study population.

a Persons with treatment of vascular risk factors before the first measurement are persons with pharmacological

treatment of hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus and prevention of arterial thrombotic events ≥100 days for the first measurement of cognitive performance.

b Persons with treatment of vascular risk factors between the first and third measurement are persons who started

with pharmacological treatment of hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus and prevention of arterial thrombotic events after the first measurement of cognitive performance.

c Persons with treatment of vascular risk factors at first measurement are persons with for the first time

phar-macological treatment of hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, diabetes mellitus and prevention of arterial

6

(11)

n (%)

Age (years), mean (SD) Age groups, n (%) 35 to 44 years 45 to 54 years 55 to 64 years 65 to 74 years ≥75 years Gender, n (%) Men Women Educational level, n (%) Low (≤12 years) High (≥13 years) Race, n (%) Western-European Other Cardiovascular historyc, n (%)

Treatable vascular risk (points)d, mean (SD) Cognitive performance

RFFT (points), mean (SD) VAT (points), mean (SD) Composite z-scoree, mean (SD)

All 1.934 (100) 51 (10) 542 (28) 713 (37) 449 (23) 190 (10) 40 (2) 916 (47) 1018 (53) 624 (32) 1310 (68) 1849 (96) 72 (4) 19 (1) 1 (3) 74 (26) 10 (2) 0.03 (0.78) Control 1.678 (100) 50 (10) 520 (31) 641 (38) 357 (21) 133 (8) 27 (2) 760 (45) 918 (55) 498 (30) 1180 (70) 1604 (96) 61 (4) 4 (<1) 1 (3) 76 (25) 10 (2) 0.08 (0.77) Treatmenta 256 (100) 58 (10) 22 (9) 72 (28) 92 (36) 57 (22) 13 (5) 156 (61) 100 (39) 126 (49) 130 (51) 245 (96) 11 (4) 15 (6) 4 (3) 64 (25) 9 (2) -0.30 (0.80) Pb N/A <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 0.62 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001

Table 2. Characteristics of the study population at the first measurement (baseline).

Abbreviations: RFFT, Ruff Figural Fluency Test; VAT, Visual Association Test; SD, standard deviation; N/A, not applicable.

a Treatment group included persons who had treatment of vascular risk factors for the first time at the first

measurement of cognitive function.

b P-values refer to comparisons between persons with and without treatment of vascular risk factors.

c All nineteen persons with a cardiovascular history had a cardiac event. There were no cerebrovascular of

peripheral vascular event.

d Treatable vascular risk is based on the components of Framingham Risk Score for Cardiovascular

Disease that are amenable to treatment and included diabetes mellitus, current smoker status, total cholesterol, HDL-cholesterol, systolic blood pressure and use of blood pressure lowering medication.19

e Cognitive performance was measured as a composite score of two tests (z-score): the Ruff Figural Fluency Test

(RFFT) and the Visual Association Test (VAT).15,17

(12)

Longitudinal change in cognitive performance

The mean (SD) total follow-up time was 5.5 (0.7) years. As reported previously12, in the total

study population the mean (SD) cognitive performance increased between consecutive measurements from 0.03 (0.78) at first measurement to 0.18 (0.81) at second measurement and to 0.44 (0.77) at third measurement (Ptrend <.001).

The mean (SD) cognitive performance in the treatment group was lower than in the control group. In the treatment group, the mean (SD) cognitive performance changed from -0.30 (0.80) to -0.23 (0.80) to 0.02 (0.87) and in the control group, from 0.08 (0.77) to 0.24 (0.79) to 0.49 (0.74) at first, second, and third measurement, respectively (Ptrend <.001) (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Mean cognitive performance during follow-up per control and treatment group.

Figure A: unadjusted means. Figure B: covariate-adjusted estimated means from linear mixed models adjusted for age, educational level, interaction age x measurement and treatable vascular risk.

Cognitive performance was measured as a composite score of two tests (z-score): the Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) and the Visual Association Test (VAT).15,17 Bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

(13)

Adjustment for demographic factors and vascular risk

If the change in cognitive performance was adjusted for demographic factors and vascular risk, the difference in cognitive performance between the two groups was smaller (Figure 2). The covariate-adjusted linear mixed model analysis did not show a statistically significant overall treatment effect: the mean difference between the treatment and control group was -0.07 (95%CI, -0.16 to 0.01; P = 0.08). Moreover, the estimated mean differences per measurement between the treatment and control group was only statistically significant after adjustment for demographic factors but it was not significant after adjustment for the demographic factors and treatable vascular risk (Table 3).

Adjustment for propensity score

For 1,685 (87%) persons, a propensity score for pharmacological treatment of vascular risk factors could be calculated. If the change in cognitive performance was adjusted for propensity score, the covariate-adjusted linear mixed model analysis also neither showed a statistically significant overall treatment effect: the mean difference between treatment and control group was -0.06 (95%CI, -0.18 to 0.06; P = 0.32). Moreover, the estimated mean difference between the treatment and control group was not statistically significant at any measurement (Table 4).

Matched samples

Overall, 239 persons from the treatment group could be matched one-to-one to the control group. There were no statistically significant differences between the matched samples in age, gender, educational level or treatable vascular risk (P >0.58). On average, the treatment sample had a slightly lower cognitive performance than the control sample at all measurements. In the treatment sample, the mean (SD) cognitive performance changed from -0.27 (0.80) to -0.20 (0.80) to 0.05 (0.86) and in the control sample, from -0.23 (0.79) to -0.13 (0.91) to 0.17 (0.79), at first, second and third measurement, respectively (Ptrend <0.001). The overall treatment effect was not statistically significant in linear mixed model analysis: the mean difference between the matched treatment and control sample was -0.07 (95%CI, -0.21 to 0.07; P = 0.31). Moreover, the estimated mean difference between the matched samples was not statistically significant at any measurement (Table 4).

(14)

DISCUSSION

In this large community-based observational study of middle-aged and older persons, the mean change in cognitive performance in the treatment and control group was similar despite the fact that at baseline the treatment group was older, had high treatable vascular risk and worse cognitive performance. This suggests that treatment of vascular risk factors does not improve nor worsen cognitive performance.

Our findings supported the results of the Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER).8 In this RCT, a multidomain

intervention including treatment of vascular risk factors maintained cognitive performance in elderly people during a follow-up period of two years.8 However, our study differs from

the FINGER trial in several aspects. While the FINGER trial included a study population of elderly people with a high risk on dementia, our study included a sample from the general population comprising both middle-aged and older persons. Moreover, duration of follow-up in the FINGER trial was two years, whereas in our study the association of cognitive performance with treatment of vascular risk factors persisted for more than five years. Most importantly, the effect of treatment of vascular risk factors on cognitive performance per se is unclear in the FINGER trial as their multidomain intervention also included diet, physical activity and cognitive training.8 In contrast, our study only compared the change in

cognitive performance between persons with and without treatment of vascular risk factors. Therefore, it probably gives more insight in the effect of multivascular treatment per se in the general population.

Our findings were also in line with the Prevention of Dementia by Intensive Vascular Care (preDIVA) trial of elderly people. In this RCT, intensive treatment of vascular risk factors did not result in a reduced incidence of all-cause dementia in the treatment as compared to the control group.9 This result could possibly be explained by the fact that a

similar vascular risk reduction was achieved in the treatment and control group. However, the primary outcome all-cause dementia in the preDIVA trial was possibly also not sensitive enough to detect a difference in the treatment and control group as dementia is diagnosed relatively late compared to the first cognitive changes that may have been present for a long time. In contrast, our study investigated the change in cognitive performance as outcome. Probably, this is a more sensitive measure that may find relative small differences

(15)

B -0.38 -0.47 -0.53 B -0.10 -0.15 -0.16 B -0.04 -0.09 -0.10 95%CI -0.48 to -0.28 -0.57 to -0.37 -0.65 to -0.41 95%CI -0.20 to -0.01 -0.24 to -0.06 -0.27 to -0.05 95%CI -0.13 to 0.06 -0.18 to 0.01 -0.21 to 0.01 P <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 P 0.03 0.002 0.003 P 0.44 0.08 0.08 Dif

ference in cognitive performance

a between treatment

b group and control group during follow-up: linear mixed model analyses. (z-score): the Ruf

f Figural Fluency

Test (RFFT) and the V

isual

Association

Test (V

ference was calculated as mean cognitive performance of treatment group minus control group.

ement; -2*log likelihood: 9378.05.

Model 1 d Model 2 e Model 3 f

Cognitive performance of treatment group as compared to control group (estimated mean dif

ference)

c

(16)

B -0.35 -0.45 -0.51 B 0.03 -0.07 -0.14 B -0.05 -0.07 -0.09 B -0.05 -0.07 -0.08 95%CI -0.46 to -0.24 -0.56 to -0.34 -0.64 to -0.39 95%CI -0.10 to 0.16 -0.21 to -0.06 -0.28 to 0.00 95%CI -0.20 to 0.10 -0.22 to 0.08 -0.26 to 0.07 95%CI -0.17 to 0.08 -0.20 to 0.06 -0.23 to 0.07 P <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 P 0.68 0.28 0.06 P 0.53 0.36 0.27 a between treatment b group and control group during follow-up: linear mixed model analyses

.

(z-score): the Ruf

f Figural Fluency

Test (RFFT) and the V

isual Association Test (V AT) 15,17 .

ctors; -2*log likelihood: 8752.82. ement, treatable vascular risk; -2*log likelihood: 2319.52.

Model 1 d Model 2 e Model 3 f Model 4 g

Cognitive performance of treatment group as compared to control group (estimated mean dif

ference)

c

Total study population (N=1,685)

Matched sample 1:1 (N=478)

(17)

Limitations and Strengths

Some limitations of this study have to be noted. Most importantly, our study had an observational design whereas it is generally acknowledged that observational studies may overestimate treatment effects in contrast to subsequent RCTs on the same questions,24

and some argue that only RCTs could draw conclusions on the impact of vascular risk management.25 However, RCTs evaluating the effect of treatment of vascular risk factors

on cognitive performance are hindered by important methodological challenges.26 First,

these RCTs require large samples and long follow-up, especially in people aged <70 years. Second, the importance of vascular risk management to prevent cardiovascular disease is undisputed and therefore, withholding or withdrawing treatment in control subjects for a long period would be unethical.25,26 Therefore, we think that large observational cohort

studies comprising middle-aged and old persons add valuable insights to what is known from recent RCTs. To lower the risk of bias in our study due to prescribing treatment by indication we used propensity scores and matching.21 These approaches yielded similar

results.

Another limitation of our study may be the measurement of cognitive performance with two cognitive tests which may not evaluate all cognitive domains. However, the RFFT measures different cognitive abilities such as initiation, planning, divergent reasoning, and the ability to switch between different tasks.14,15 Furthermore, the RFFT was combined with

the VAT as a measure of memory.17 Finally, cognitive performance increased across the

measurements in our study probably due to the repeated exposure to the tests resulting in a practice effect.12 However, a practice effect is dependent on the capacity to learn

and, therefore, the result of different cognitive abilities.27,28 Furthermore, the association of

cognitive performance in our study was adjusted for repeated measurement by entering consecutive measurement number as an independent variable into the regression models.

Finally, our study may be underpowered to detect a statistically significant effect of the treatment of vascular risk factors. However, the estimated difference in cognitive performance between the treatment and control group was 0.08–0.14 z-score (treatment

worse than control). This corresponded to a difference of 2–4 points on the RFFT and

0.2–0.3 points on the VAT. In our opinion, for both tests, these differences are far below the threshold of clinical relevance. Therefore, even if the differences between the treatment and control group would be statistically significant in a (much) larger study, they would still lack a clinical implication.

Despite these limitations, the present study also has several strengths. Our study was

(18)

older persons.8,9 In addition, our study only investigated the association of treatment

of vascular risk factors with cognitive performance and did not include other types of intervention like cognitive training.8

In conclusion, in this large community-based study, the change in cognitive performance during a follow-up period of nearly six years was similar in the treatment and control group. This suggests that treatment of vascular risk factors does not improve nor worsen cognitive performance.

(19)

References

1. Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement 2013 Jan;9(1):63-75.e2.

2. Alzheimer’s Disease International. World Alzheimer Report 2015: The Global Impact of Dementia: An analysis of prevalence, incidence, cost and trends. London, 2015:1-84. https://www.alz.co.uk/ research/WorldAlzheimerReport2015.pdf. Accessed September 20, 2017.

3. Qiu C, Fratiglioni L. A major role for cardiovascular burden in age-related cognitive decline. Nat Rev Cardiol 2015 May;12(5):267-277.

4. Ligthart SA, Moll van Charante EP, Van Gool WA, Richard E. Treatment of cardiovascular risk factors to prevent cognitive decline and dementia: a systematic review. Vasc Health Risk Manag 2010 Sep 7;6:775-785.

5. Peters R, Beckett N, Forette F, Tuomilehto J, Clarke R, Ritchie C, et al. Incident dementia and blood pressure lowering in the Hypertension in the Very Elderly Trial cognitive function assessment (HYVET-COG): a double-blind, placebo controlled trial. Lancet Neurol 2008 Aug;7(8):683-689.

6. ADVANCE Collaborative Group, Patel A, MacMahon S, Chalmers J, Neal B, Billot L, et al. Intensive blood glucose control and vascular outcomes in patients with type 2 diabetes. N Engl J Med 2008 Jun 12;358(24):2560-2572.

7. Shepherd J, Blauw GJ, Murphy MB, Bollen EL, Buckley BM, Cobbe SM, et al. Pravastatin in elderly individuals at risk of vascular disease (PROSPER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2002 Nov 23;360(9346):1623-1630.

8. Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levalahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2015 Jun 6;385(9984):2255-2263.

9. Moll van Charante EP, Richard E, Eurelings LS, van Dalen JW, Ligthart SA, van Bussel EF, et al. Effectiveness of a 6-year multidomain vascular care intervention to prevent dementia (preDIVA): a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2016 Aug 20;388(10046):797-805.

10. Mahmoodi BK, Gansevoort RT, Veeger NJ, Matthews AG, Navis G, Hillege HL, et al. Microalbuminuria and risk of venous thromboembolism. JAMA 2009 May 6;301(17):1790-1797.

11. Lambers Heerspink HJ, Brantsma AH, de Zeeuw D, Bakker SJ, de Jong PE, Gansevoort RT, et al. Albuminuria assessed from first-morning-void urine samples versus 24-hour urine collections as a predictor of cardiovascular morbidity and mortality. Am J Epidemiol 2008 Oct 15;168(8):897-905. 12. van Eersel ME, Joosten H, Koerts J, Gansevoort RT, Slaets JP, Izaks GJ. Longitudinal study of

performance on the Ruff Figural Fluency Test in persons aged 35 years or older. PLoS One 2015 Mar 23;10(3):e0121411.

13. Visser ST, Schuiling-Veninga CC, Bos JH, de Jong-van den Berg LT, Postma MJ. The population-based prescription database IADB.nl: its development, usefulness in outcomes research and challenges. Expert Rev Pharmacoecon Outcomes Res 2013 Jun;13(3):285-292.

14. Ruff R, Light R, Evans R. The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: A normative study with adults. Dev Neuropsychol 1987;3:37-51.

15. Ruff R. Ruff Figural Fluency Test: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc 1996.

16. Izaks GJ, Joosten H, Koerts J, Gansevoort RT, Slaets JP. Reference data for the Ruff Figural Fluency Test stratified by age and educational level. PLoS One 2011 Feb 10;6(2):e17045.

17. Lindeboom J, Schmand B. Visual Association Test. Manual. PITS B V Leiden, The Netherlands 2003. 18. United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization. International standard classification

of education ISCED 1997. Re-edition 2006. Montreal, Canada: United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization 2006:http://www.uis.unesco.org/Library/Documents/isced97-en.pdf. Accessed September 20, 2017.

(20)

20. Joosten H, van Eersel ME, Gansevoort RT, Bilo HJ, Slaets JP, Izaks GJ. Cardiovascular risk profile and cognitive function in young, middle-aged, and elderly subjects. Stroke 2013 Jun;44(6):1543-1549. 21. Rubin DB. Estimating causal effects from large data sets using propensity scores. Ann Intern Med

1997 Oct 15;127(8 Pt 2):757-763.

22. Schilling C, Mortimer D, Dalziel K, Heeley E, Chalmers J, Clarke P. Using Classification and Regression Trees (CART) to Identify Prescribing Thresholds for Cardiovascular Disease. Pharmacoeconomics 2016 Feb;34(2):195-205.

23. Mohammed MA, El Sayed C, Marshall T. Patient and other factors influencing the prescribing of cardiovascular prevention therapy in the general practice setting with and without nurse assessment. Med Decis Making 2012 May-Jun;32(3):498-506.

24. Hemkens LG, Contopoulos-Ioannidis DG, Ioannidis JP. Agreement of treatment effects for mortality from routinely collected data and subsequent randomized trials: meta-epidemiological survey. BMJ 2016 Feb 8;352:i493.

25. Richard F, Pasquier F. Can the treatment of vascular risk factors slow cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease patients? J Alzheimers Dis 2012;32(3):765-772.

26. Richard E, Andrieu S, Solomon A, Mangialasche F, Ahtiluoto S, Moll van Charante EP, et al. Methodological challenges in designing dementia prevention trials - the European Dementia Prevention Initiative (EDPI). J Neurol Sci 2012 Nov 15;322(1-2):64-70.

27. Bartels C, Wegrzyn M, Wiedl A, Ackermann V, Ehrenreich H. Practice effects in healthy adults: a longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing. BMC Neurosci 2010 Sep 16;11:118-2202-11-118.

28. Calamia M, Markon K, Tranel D. Scoring higher the second time around: meta-analyses of practice effects in neuropsychological assessment. Clin Neuropsychol 2012 May;26(4):543-570.

(21)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Study (DCS) that also found a statistically significant negative association of type 2 diabetes with global cognitive function in young and middle-aged people after adjustment for

The aim of this study was to investigate the longitudinal performance on the RFFT by repeating the test over an average follow-up period of three and six years in a large cohort

Treatable vascular risk is based on the components of Framingham Risk Score for Cardiovascular Disease that are amenable to treatment and included diabetes mellitus, current

Interestingly, we found that during a follow-up period of six years the changes in cognitive performance was similar between the persons with and without treatment of vascular risk

In hoofdstuk 5 werd het negatieve verband beschreven, dat bleek te bestaan tussen de verandering van de cognitieve functies gedurende zes jaar follow-up en het behandelbare

Ik wil alle internisten uit Zwolle bedanken voor mijn eerste drie jaar opleiding in de Isala Klinieken te Zwolle: jullie enthousiasme voor het vak Interne heeft mij gevormd tot

In this large community-based observational study of middle-aged and older persons, the mean change in cognitive performance in the treatment and control group was similar despite

Medicamenteuze behandeling van vasculaire risicofactoren vanaf jong volwassen leeftijd is moeilijk haalbaar als preventieve strategie voor cognitieve achteruitgang.. People will