• No results found

VU Research Portal

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "VU Research Portal"

Copied!
45
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Cognitive decline in multiple sclerosis

van Geest, Q.

2018

document version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Link to publication in VU Research Portal

citation for published version (APA)

van Geest, Q. (2018). Cognitive decline in multiple sclerosis: A multifaceted dynamic approach.

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ?

Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

E-mail address:

(2)
(3)
(4)

Chapter 3.1

Is impaired information

processing speed a matter

of structural or functional

damage in multiple sclerosis?

NeuroImage: Clinical (submitted)

(5)

Abstract

Objective Cognitive deficits, especially those of information processing speed (IPS), are common in multiple sclerosis (MS), however, the underlying neurobiological mechanisms remain poorly understood. In this study, we examined structural and functional brain changes separately, but also in an integrative manner, in relation to IPS performance.

Methods IPS was measured using the symbol digit modalities test (SDMT) in 330 MS patients and 96 controls. Patients with IPS impairment (IPS-I, Z-score < -1.5) were compared to patients with preserved IPS performance (IPS-P) on volumetric measures, white matter integrity loss (using diffusion tensor imaging) and the severity of functional connectivity changes (using resting-state fMRI). Significant predictors of IPS performance were used to create groups of mild or severe structural and/or functional damage to determine the relative effect of structural and/or functional changes on IPS.

Results IPS-I patients, compared to IPS-P patients, showed lower deep gray matter volume and lower WM integrity, but stronger increases in functional connectivity. Patients with predominantly structural damage had worse IPS (Z-score = -1.49) than patients with predominantly functional changes (Z-score = -0.84), although both structural and functional measures remained significant in a regression model. Patients with severe structural and functional changes had worst IPS (Z-score = -1.95).

(6)

91

Introduction

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a progressive, inflammatory, and neurodegenerative disease of the central nervous system characterized by demyelination and

neuronal loss.1 In addition to physical disabilities, cognitive deficits are common,

affecting approximately 40-70% of the MS patients.2,3 Among cognitive deficits,

problems with information processing speed (IPS) are frequently seen and already

present early in the disease.2,4–6

In MS, previous imaging studies have shown that IPS deficits are related to

structural or functional brain abnormalities,5,7–13 including deep gray matter

(DGM) atrophy14 and white matter (WM) integrity loss,11 as well as changes in

functional connectivity.12,13 Unfortunately, studies that have integrated structural

and functional measures to explain IPS deficits are currently lacking. Although structural and functional brain characteristics are intertwined to a certain extent,

there is no one-to-one relation between these two.15 Therefore, it might be that

structural damage may occur in the presence of minor functional changes but may also involve severe functional changes. As complex cognitive functions such as IPS arise from an efficient interplay between the brain's functional and structural architecture, varying levels of structural and/or functional damage may

also result in different levels of IPS impairment in MS.16 This emphasizes the need

to consider both structural and functional measures simultaneously to be able to better understand IPS deficits.

We hypothesize that to increase our understanding of the underlying neurobiology of IPS deficits an integrated measure of functional and structural brain changes

is essential, instead of studying either one or the other.17 Therefore, we integrated

advanced functional and structural MRI measures to examine the relative and joint impact of functional and structural brain changes in explaining IPS performance.

Methods

Participants

All participants with complete functional and structural imaging protocols (see

below) of the Amsterdam MS Cohort18,19 were included, resulting in 330 MS

(7)

Neuropsychological testing

All subjects underwent neuropsychological evaluation using an expanded Brief

Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological tests, as previously described.20 Of

these assessments, we formed groups based on IPS only, as measured with the

symbol digit modalities test (SDMT),21 which was corrected for effects of sex, age

and education.3 These scores were subsequently converted to Z-scores based

on the mean and standard deviation (SD) of the HC and used to categorize MS patients into either IPS impaired (IPS-I, Z-score ≤ -1.5 on SDMT) or IPS preserved (IPS-P, Z-score > -1.5 on SDMT). For descriptive purposes, similar cut-off scores were applied to the remaining cognitive domains.

MR imaging

MR imaging was performed on a 3T scanner (GE Signa HDxt, Milwaukee, WI, USA) using an eight-channel head-coil. The structural imaging protocol included a 3D T1-weighted inversion-prepared fast spoiled gradient recall sequence (FSPGR, TR

7.8 ms, TE 3 ms, TI 450ms, FA 12º, sagittal 1.0 mm sections, 0.94 × 0.94 mm2

in-plane resolution) for volumetric measurements, a 3D fluid-attenuated inversion-recovery sequence (FLAIR, TR 8000 ms, TE 125 ms, TI 2350 ms, sagittal 1.2 mm slices,

0.98 × 0.98 mm2 in-plane resolution) for lesion detection and a diffusion tensor

imaging (DTI) sequence covering the entire brain using five volumes without directional weighting (i.e. b0) and 30 volumes with non-collinear diffusion gradients

(EPI, b = 1000 s/mm2, TR 13000 ms, TE 91 ms, FA 90º, 2.4 mm contiguous axial slices,

2 × 2 mm2 in-plane resolution). Brain function was assessed using resting-state

functional MRI with whole-brain coverage using 202 volumes, of which the first two were discarded (EPI, TR 2200 ms, TE 35 ms, FA 20º, 3 mm contiguous axial

slices, 3.3 × 3.3 mm2 in-plane resolution).

Volumetric measures

Hyperintense lesions were automatically segmented on the FLAIR images22 and

filled on the T1 using LEAP23 to minimize the impact of lesions on volumetric

measures and registration algorithms. Normalized gray matter (NGMV) and WM (NWMV) volumes were calculated with SIENAX (part of FSL 5). FIRST was used to segment deep gray matter (DGM) structures and the volume of these structures were computed, summed and normalized for head size, resulting in normalized DGM volume (NDGMV). Normalized cortical volumes (NCGMV) were computed by subtracting FIRST segmentations from the SIENAX-based GM

(8)

93

A

B

C

Lesion load White matter

segmentation matter segmentationCortical gray Deep gray mattersegmentation Mean FA

Power atlas Individual

FC matrix control FC matrixAverage healthy deviation matrixIndividual FC

ROI RO I 0 0 0.23 0.23 -0.08 -0.08 0 0.17 0.17 0 0.12 0.12 -0.15 -0.15 0.47 0.47 0 0 0.02 0.02 -0.38 -0.38 0 0.05 0.05 0 0.18 0.18 0.07 0.07 0.16 0.16 0 0 0.21 0.21 0.30 0.30 0 0.12 0.12 0 -0.06 -0.06 -0.22 -0.22 0.34 0.34

-

=

Structural damage Functional damage median severe mild mild severe Increased functional

connectivity Decreased functionalconnectivity

S F

S F

S F

S F

Figure 3.1.1 Data analysis fl owchart

(9)

Severity of fractional anisotropy-based damage

DTI data were pre-processed using FSL 5, including motion- and eddy current correction on images and gradient vectors, followed by diffusion tensor fitting. Since fractional anisotropy (FA) is the most commonly examined diffusion measure in MS, we focused on whole-brain FA as a measure of WM integrity, also to limit the number of dependent variables. To obtain skeletonised FA maps,

the default tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) pipeline was used.24 Mean FA scores

of the WM skeleton were extracted for each subject as a measure of whole-brain WM integrity.

Processing of functional images

Pre-processing of fMRI data was carried out using the default pipeline of MELODIC, consisting of motion correction, removal of non-brain tissue, spatial smoothing using a 5 mm full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian kernel and high-pass temporal filtering to cut off frequencies below 0.01 Hz. All resting-state fMRI scans were checked for artifacts, excessive motion and registration errors. The individual level of motion was calculated based on the average frame-to-frame motion. The amount of motion was not different between HC and MS (p = 0.34) and no subject moved more than 0.3 mm. To remove signal originating from residual non-brain tissue as well as from voxels sensitive to EPI-distortions, voxels with a signal intensity in the lowest quartile of the robust range were

excluded.20,25 For this study we used the power atlas,26 which was specifically

designed to study brain networks and consists of 264 GM regions. This atlas was registered to 3DT1 space with inverted non-linear registration parameters, using nearest neighbour interpolation. The atlas was then multiplied with the SIENAX and FIRST segmentations to include GM only. Subsequently, inverted boundary-based registration matrices were used to register the atlas to each individual fMRI scan using nearest neighbour interpolation. Regions of interest (ROIs) were excluded if these contained missing values in more than 10% of the subjects after the final registration to fMRI, leaving 238 GM regions in the final atlas for which mean time series were calculated. Functional connectivity matrices were formed by calculating Pearson correlations between the time series of all these pairs of nodes. Since the controversial nature of negative connectivity values, only positive

correlation coefficients were considered.27,28 Since the average level of functional

connectivity is known to be highly variable, which could hamper between-group

comparisons,29 these raw correlation coefficients were converted to relative

connectivity Z-scores by subtracting each individual's mean connectivity and

(10)

95

Severity of functional network changes

While global structural measures (e.g. NGMV, NWMV and whole-brain FA) are commonly computed, there is no such equivalent with regard to functional measures. Studies usually address regional changes in functional connectivity. After computing global structural measures, however, we subsequently aimed to design a whole-brain functional network measure representing the severity of functional connectivity changes in each individual. In other words, we needed an individual quantification of the amount of deviation from normal in functional connectivity levels. To determine "normal" connectivity levels for each link, we constructed an average normalized HC matrix (based on all 96 HC). Subsequently, we subtracted each individual normalized connectivity matrix from the aforementioned average HC matrix, resulting in a deviation score per element of the connectivity matrix. The values of these deviation matrices could thus either be positive, reflecting an increase in functional connectivity, or negative, reflecting

a decrease in functional connectivity (Figure 3.1.1B). For each individual matrix,

the average connectivity level of increased and decreased links was used as two measures of the severity of functional network changes for subsequent analyses.

Function versus structure

Together, this pipeline resulted in four measures of structural damage (i.e. NDGMV, NCGMV, lesion volume, and severity of WM integrity loss) and two measures of functional damage (i.e. the severity of increased and decreased functional connectivity changes). First, we compared these measures between IPS-I and IPS-P patients. Subsequently, in the entire MS group, a backward regression model was conducted to determine which of these significant functional and/or structural variables were the main independent predictors of IPS performance. Finally, to be able to integrate structural and functional measures, structural predictors were paired with functional predictors to determine the combined amount of damage for each patient individually. Four groups of patients, classified based on median splits of functional and structural damage, were examined in relation to IPS performance, namely:

1. Patients with mild functional damage and mild structural damage

2. Patients with severe functional damage but only mild structural damage, from here on referred to as "predominantly functional damage"

3. Patients with mild functional damage but severe structural damage, from here on referred to as "predominantly structural damage"

(11)

Mild damage was defined as scores lower than the median score (based on the MS group), whereas severe damage was defined as scores higher than the median score. In all groups the relation between the severity of structural and functional damage with IPS performance was investigated. To limit the number of comparisons, only consecutive groups were compared on IPS performance.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS version 22.0 (Chicago, IL, USA). All variables were checked for normal distributions by histogram inspection and normality tests. Before conducting statistical analyses, it was checked whether the required assumptions were met. General linear models were used to compare measures of interest between groups including age, sex, and education as covariates. The linear regression model used to determine independent predictors of IPS followed a backward selection procedure. To examine how functional and structural measures were interrelated, Pearson correlations were calculated for normally distributed variables, or Spearman's Rank-Order correlations for non-normally distributed variables. Test statistics were considered significant with

p-values < 0.05 (Bonferroni corrected).

Results

Demographics and cognitive profiles: IPS-I versus IPS-P

Of all MS patients, 130 (39%) were defined as IPS-I, and 200 (61%) as IPS-P (see

Table 3.1.1). No difference in sex was found for the two groups. However, IPS-I patients were older and had a lower educational level compared to IPS-P patients. The IPS-I group consisted of a higher percentage of progressive patients (36% versus 20%, p = 0.002) and had higher EDSS scores (EDSS: 3.5 versus EDSS: 3.0,

p = 0.01) compared to IPS-P patients. The disease duration was longer in IPS-I

patients compared to IPS-P patients. Of the IPS-I patients, 33% showed impairment on one additional cognitive domain, 15% on two cognitive domains, 14% on three cognitive domains and 14% on more than three cognitive domains. Of the IPS-P patients, 24% showed impairment on one cognitive domain, 14% on two cognitive domains, 6% on three cognitive domains and 6% on more than three cognitive domains.

Functional and structural damage: IPS-I versus IPS-P

(12)

97

Table 3.1.1 Demographics IPS impaired patients, IPS preserved patients and healthy controls

IPS impaired IPS preserved HC p

n 130 200 96 –

Age, years 50.01 (11.33) 46.93 (10.74) 45.87 (10.45) 0.01a,c

Female/male 85/45 140/60 56/40 0.14 Educational level, years* 4.00 (3.00 – 6.00) 5.00 (4.00 – 6.00) 6.00 (4.00 – 7.00) 0.002a,b

RRMS/SPMS/PPMS 83/31/16 160/20/20 – 0.002 Symptom duration, years* 15.82 (7.8 – 21.59) 9.80 (6.63 – 20.32) – 0.01 EDSS* 4.0 (3.0 – 6.0) 3.0 (2.00 – 4.00) – 0.04 SDMT 37 (8.6) 58 (8.9) 61 (9.81) 0.001a,b,c

EDSS: expanded disability status scale; HC: healthy controls; IPS: information processing speed; RRMS: relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis; PPMS: primary progressive multiple sclerosis; SDMT: symbol digit modalities test; SPMS: secondary progressive multiple sclerosis

* Not normally distributed data for which median (interquartile range) are provided

a Significant difference between IPS impaired and HC b Significant difference between IPS preserved and HC c Significant difference between IPS impaired and IPS preserved

Table 3.1.2 Structural and functional MRI characteristics of IPS impaired patients, IPS preserved patients and healthy controls

IPS impaired IPS preserved HC p

n 130 200 96 – NGMV (ml) 758.81 (60.42) 799.98 (59.04) 818.54 (53.13) < 0.001a,b,c NWMV (ml) 657.55 (35.95) 675.98 (32.87) 697.09 (31.29) < 0.001a,b,c NDGMV (ml) 52.76 (7.49) 58.39 (5.28) 62.91 (37.35) < 0.001a,b,c NCGMV (ml) 726.33 (56.77) 763.64 (47.06) 779.41 (52.27) < 0.001a,b,c Normalized TLL (ml)* 21.67 (10.71 – 35.59) 10.51 (5.62 – 17.83) – < 0.001 FA whole brain 0.39 (0.03) 0.40 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) < 0.001a,b,c

Absolute FC 0.93 (1.02) 0.53 (1.03) 0.00 (1.00) < 0.001a,b,c

Decreased FC -0.70 (0.95) -0.42 (0.97) 0.00 (1.00) < 0.001a,b

Increased FC 0.95 (1.13) 0.52 (1.12) 0.00 (1.00) < 0.001a,b,c

For descriptive purposes, functional connectivity values were converted to Z-scores based on the mean and SD of the HC

FA: fractional anisotropy; FC: functional connectivity; HC: healthy controls; IPS: information processing speed; NCGMV: normalized cortical gray matter volume; NDGMV: normalized deep gray matter volume; NGMV: normalized gray matter volume; NWMV: normalized white matter volume; TLL: total lesion load * Not normally distributed data for which median (interquartile range) are provided

(13)

IPS-P patients (p = 0.01; Figure 3.1.2A). In the entire MS group, a stronger increase in functional connectivity was associated with lower NCGMV and NDGMV (r = -0.218 and r = -0.179, p < 0.001), as well as higher lesion load and loss of WM integrity (ρ = 0.180 and r = -0.231, p < 0.001).

Which measure is the best predictor of IPS deficits in MS?

The final model (R2 = 0.454, p < 0.001) contained the following predictors for

worse IPS performance: lower NDGMV (β = 0.374, p < 0.001), older age (β = -0.206,

p < 0.001), lower education (β = 0.180, p < 0.001), loss of WM integrity (β = 0.152, p = 0.012), male sex (β = -0.116, p = 0.010), and increased functional connectivity

(β= -0.102, p = 0.021). Based on this regression model, NDGMV, loss of WM integrity, and increased functional connectivity were selected as structural and functional measures of interest. To obtain the individual effect sizes of these measures, separate models were conducted containing demographic variables and the measure of interest. As a result, NDGMV volume explained 42% of the variance in IPS (p < 0.001), WM integrity 37% (p < 0.001), and increased functional connectivity 24% (p < 0.001).

Impact of different severities of functional and structural damage

After identifying NDGMV, loss of WM integrity, and functional connectivity as independent predictors of IPS, four groups were created based on the severity

of functional and structural damage (Supplementary Table 3.1.1): mild structural

and functional damage (group 1), predominantly functional damage (group 2), predominantly structural damage (group 3), and functional as well as structural damage (group 4). Since two structural measures appeared to be predictors of IPS, NDGMV and WM integrity were combined in one composite score by first creating

Z-scores relative to the healthy controls for both measures separately and then

add these Z-scores. In this way, NDGMV and loss of WM integrity were averaged

into one structural damage composite score (Figure 3.1.2B). As expected, group

4 had worst IPS performance compared to all other groups (Z-score: -1.95 ± 1.41; group 4 vs 3: p = 0.02). Group 1 had best IPS performance (Z-score: -0.40 ± 1.10; group 1 vs 2: p = 0.02). Additionally, group 3 had worse IPS performance (Z-score: -1.49 ± 1.12) than group 2 (Z-score: -0.84 ± 1.10; p = 0.002). Groups 2 and 4 did not differ on the severity of functional connectivity, while groups 1 and 3 did not differ on the level of structural damage.

Impact of DGM atrophy and loss of WM integrity separately

(14)

99

in IPS performance was observed between patients with predominantly WM integrity loss and those with predominantly functional damage (p > 0.05;

Figure 3.1.2D).

Figure 3.1.2 IPS status is reflected by different severities of functional and structural changes

IPS impaired (IPS-I) patients showed more severe increases in functional connectivity than IPS preserved (IPS-P) patients (A). A similar stepwise deterioration in IPS performance was observed for functional changes combined with a composite score of structural damage (B), functional changes combined with DGM volume loss (C) and functional changes combined with WM integrity loss (D). In general, mild functional and mild structural changes were associated with the best IPS performance. Severe functional changes were associated with better IPS than severe structural changes, whereas severe functional changes combined with severe structural changes were associated with the worst IPS.

(15)

Discussion

Using innovative and integrated measures for functional and structural damage, we were able to demonstrate that different severities of functional and structural damage reflect stepwise worsening of IPS. MS patients with mild functional and mild structural damage had the best, although lower than HC, IPS. A further decline in IPS was found in MS patients with predominantly functional damage. MS patients with predominantly structural damage had worse IPS than the previous two groups, whereas MS patients with both severe functional and severe structural damage were worst off. The severity of functional network changes seemed to have an additive effect on IPS performance, as a similar degree of structural damage can be accompanied with either mild or severe functional network changes, resulting in different levels of IPS.

Until now, functional connectivity studies mostly focused on a few selected

regions.19,30,31 In this study, we defined an innovative whole-brain functional

connectivity measure to assess the severity of connectivity changes in one single measure, which allows to map widespread functional connectivity changes. Both increased and decreased functional connectivity were observed in MS patients compared to HC, but only increased functional connectivity changes discriminated IPS-P from IPS-I patients. Several studies have reported increased levels of functional connectivity during rest as a correlate of cognitive

deficits.19,20,32,33 This increase in functional connectivity could indicate altered

functional network activities, including increased levels of network synchrony

and abnormal oscillatory rhythm.34 In the current study, changes in whole-brain

measures were related to IPS performance. The rationale for investigating IPS was based on its high frequency and early presence in the disease. In addition, this domain is likely to depend on the interaction across many distant brain regions and cannot be assigned to one single brain region, and therefore differences in performance might reflect changes in whole-brain structural and functional measures. However, since IPS scores might influence and might be influenced by deficits in other cognitive domains, it is unfortunately not feasible to examine

IPS deficits in isolation.35,36 This is also shown by our own data since more cognitive

deficits were detected in the IPS-I group.

(16)

101

predominantly functional damage (Z-score: -0.84). Larger cognitive consequences of structural damage were also shown by the regression models, that is, structural measures possessed a stronger predictive value. The strong influence of structural measures on IPS performance was previously reported as well. Not only loss of

WM integrity,9,11 but also loss of DGM volume8,14 was associated with worse IPS.

This might be explained by the relatively rigid nature of the brain's structural architecture, whereas the functional network might be better able to circumvent

damage due to its dynamic and more flexible properties.16

Among the structural measures, it seemed that DGM volume had a stronger effect than WM integrity loss. Contrary to what we observed for DGM volume, there was no significant difference in IPS performance between patients with predominantly WM integrity loss and those with predominantly functional damage. Like fMRI changes, WM integrity loss is likely to reflect more subtle damage, especially in

the so-called normal appearing WM,37,38 while a measure like DGM atrophy is

an MRI marker for (substantial) neurodegeneration,39,40 possibly explaining the

larger impact of the latter on IPS. It is important, however, to note that we focused on whole-brain measures, while certain focal structural and functional changes might influence the results differently.

Although structural damage was a strong predictor for IPS performance, investigating the joint impact of structural and functional measures showed that some subgroups with a similar degree of structural damage (i.e. both group 1 and group 2, as well as group 3 and group 4) had different degrees (i.e. mild or severe) of functional changes. For example, in some patients, severe structural damage occurred simultaneously with severe functional changes (group 4), associated with worse IPS scores than when accompanied with mild functional changes (group 3). One could hypothesize that in this subgroup the functional network "suffers" from the structural changes. The absence of a strict one-to-one relation between the level of structural and functional damage, emphasizes the value of integrating both measures. Our findings showed that adding information about the severity of functional changes is needed to distinguish between different levels of IPS performance in patients with similar degrees of structural damage. This might indicate that the functional network acts as mediating factor between the level of structural damage and IPS performance. Additionally, the resilience of the functional brain network might also limit cognitive consequences, as observed in patients with severe structural damage and only mild functional damage. More resilient networks might theoretically be able to cope with a larger amount

(17)
(18)

103

References

1 Stys PK, Zamponi GW, van Minnen J, et al. Will the real multiple sclerosis please stand up? Nat Rev Neurosci 2012;13:507-14.

2 Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet 2008;7:1139-51.

3 Amato MP, Portaccio E, Goretti B, et al. The Rao ’ s Brief Repeatable Battery and Stroop test : normative values with age, education and gender corrections in an Italian population. Mult Scler J 2006;12:787-93.

4 Deloire MSA. Cognitive impairment as marker of diffuse brain abnormalities in early relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:519-26.

5 Khalil M, Enzinger C, Langkammer C, et al. Cognitive impairment in relation to MRI metrics in patients with clinically isolated syndrome. Mult Scler J 2011;17:173-80.

6 Archibald CJ, Fisk JD. Information processing efficiency in patients with multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2000;22:686-701.

7 Moroso A, Ruet A, Lamargue-Hamel D, et al. Posterior lobules of the cerebellum and information processing speed at various stages of multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2017;88:146-51.

8 Bergsland N, Zivadinov R, Dwyer MG, et al. Localized atrophy of the thalamus and slowed cognitive processing speed in MS patients. Mult Scler J 2016;22:1327-36.

9 Schoonheim MM, Vigeveno RM, Rueda Lopes FC, et al. Sex-specific extent and severity of white matter damage in multiple sclerosis: implications for cognitive decline. Hum Brain Mapp 2014;35:2348-58.

10 Benedict RHB, Zivadinov R, Carone DA, et al. Regional lobar atrophy predicts memory impairment in multiple sclerosis. Am J Neuroradiol 2005;26:1824-31.

11 Dineen RA, Vilisaar J, Hlinka J, et al. Disconnection as a mechanism for cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Brain a J Neurol 2009;132:239-49.

12 Schoonheim MM, Geurts JJG, Landi D, et al. Functional connectivity changes in multiple sclerosis patients: A graph analytical study of MEG resting state data. Hum Brain Mapp 2013;34:52-61.

13 Leavitt VM, Wylie G, Genova HM, et al. Altered effective connectivity during performance of an information processing speed task in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2012;18:409-17.

14 Batista S, Zivadinov R, Hoogs M, et al. Basal ganglia, thalamus and neocortical atrophy predicting slowed cognitive processing in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol 2012;259:139-46.

15 Hillary FG, Grafman JH. Injured Brains and Adaptive Networks: The Benefits and Costs of Hyperconnectivity. Trends Cogn Sci 2017;21:385-401.

16 Park H-J, Friston K. Structural and Functional Brain Networks: From Connections to Cognition. Science (80- ) 2013;342:1238411-1238411.

17 Chard D, Trip S. Resolving the clinico-radiological paradox in multiple sclerosis. F1000Res 2017;6:1828–37.

18 Daams M, Steenwijk MD, Wattjes MP, et al. Unraveling the neuroimaging predictors for motor dysfunction in long-standing multiple sclerosis. Neurology 2015;85:248-55.

19 Schoonheim MM, Hulst HE, Brandt R, et al. Thalamus structure and function determines severity of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 2015;84:776-83.

20 Meijer K, Eijlers AJC, Douw L, et al. Increased connectivity of hub networks and cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 2017;In press.

21 Benedict RH, DeLuca J, Phillips G, et al. Validity of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test as a cognition performance outcome measure for multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 2017;:1352458517690821.

(19)

23 Chard DT, Jackson JS, Miller DH, et al. Reducing the impact of white matter lesions on automated measures of brain gray and white matter volumes. J Magn Reson Imaging 2010;32:223-8.

24 Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 2006;31:1487-505.

25 Eijlers AJC, Meijer KA, Wassenaar TM, et al. Increased default-mode network centrality in cognitively impaired multiple sclerosis patients. Neurology 2017;88:1-9.

26 Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, et al. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 2011;72:665–78. 27 Chai XJ, Castañón AN, Öngür D, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal

regression. NeuroImage 2012;59(2):1420-28;:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.048.

28 Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, et al. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005;102:9673-78.

29 Finn ES, Shen X, Scheinost D, et al. Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat Neurosci 2015;18:1-11.

30 Rocca MA, Valsasina P, Leavitt VM, et al. Functional network connectivity abnormalities in multiple sclerosis: Correlations with disability and cognitive impairment. Mult Scler 2017;:1352458517699875.

31 Hulst HE, Schoonheim MM, Roosendaal SD, et al. Functional adaptive changes within the hippocampal memory system of patients with multiple sclerosis. Hum Brain Mapp 2012;33:2268-80.

32 Hawellek DJ, Hipp JF, Lewis CM, et al. Increased functional connectivity indicates the severity of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011;108:19066-71.

33 Leavitt VM, Wylie GR, Girgis PA, et al. Increased functional connectivity within memory networks following memory rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis. Brain Imaging Behav 2014;8:394-402.

34 Denève S, Machens CK. Efficient codes and balanced networks. Nat Neurosci 2016;19:375-82.

35 Forn C, Belenguer A, Parcet-Ibars MA, et al. Information-processing speed is the primary deficit underlying the poor performance of multiple sclerosis patients in the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT). J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2008;30:789-96.

36 DeLuca J, Chelune GJ, Tulsky DS, et al. Is Speed of Processing or Working Memory the Primary Information Processing Deficit in Multiple Sclerosis? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004;26:550-62.

37 Moll NM, Rietsch AM, Thomas S, et al. Multiple sclerosis normal-appearing white matter: Pathology-imaging correlations. Ann Neurol 2011;70:764-73.

38 Miller DH, Thompson AJ, Filippi M. Magnetic resonance studies of abnormalities in the normal appearing white matter and grey matter in multiple sclerosis. J. Neurol. 2003;250:1407–19.

39 Popescu V, Klaver R, Voorn P, et al. What drives MRI-measured cortical atrophy in multiple sclerosis? Mult Scler 2015;1-11. 40 Rocca MA, Battaglini M, Benedict RHB, et al. Brain MRI atrophy quantification in MS. Neurology 2017;88:403-13. 41 Aerts H, Fias W, Caeyenberghs K, et al. Brain networks under attack: Robustness properties and the impact of lesions.

Brain. 2016;139:3063-83.

(20)

105

Supplementary Table 3.1.1 Demographics and MRI characteristics of the groups with mild and severe structural and/or functional changes

Mild S & F Mild S & severe F Severe F & mild S Severe F & S p

Functional & WM changes

n

Age

Sex (female/male) Education

Disease duration, years FA NDGMV Increased FC 97 43.09 (8.46)b,c,d 52/45b,c,d 6 (4 – 6) 10.48 (5.91)c,d 0.41 (0.01)c,d 59.54 (4.58)c,d 0.65 (0.03)b,d 68 46.15 (11.91)a,c,d 47/21 4 (3 – 6) 12.00 (7.24)c,d 0.41 (0.01)c,d 59.25 (4.72)c,d 0.76 (0.03)a,c 67 49.70 (9.41)a,b,d 48/19 5 (4 – 6) 16.74 (8.09)a,b,d 0.38 (0.02)a,b 52.86 (6.90)a,b 0.66 (0.03)b,d 96 54.13 (0.59)a,b,c 76/20 5 (3 – 6) 19.35 (8.97)a,b,c 0.37 (0.02)a,b 52.43 (7.19)a,b 0.76 (0.03)a,c < 0.001 0.002 n.s. < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001

Functional & GM changes

n

Age

Sex (female/male) Education

Disease duration, years FA NDGMV Increased FC 91 42.40 (8.65)a,b,c 56/35 6 (4 – 6) 10.75 (6.06)c,d 0.41 (0.02)b,c,d 61.42 (3.30)c,d 0.65 (0.03)b,d 75 47.59 (12.20)a,d 64/11a,c,d 3 (4 – 6) 12.71 (6.96)c,d 0.40 (0.02)a,c,d 61.21 (2.85)c,d 0.76 (0.03)a,c 74 49.82 (8.71)a,d 45/29 5 (4 – 6) 15.76 (8.19)a,b,d 0.39 (0.02)a,b,d 51.35 (4.98)a,b 0.67 (0.03)b,d 90 53.55 (10.79)a,b,c 60/30 5 (3 – 6) 19.33 (9.50)a,b,c 0.38 (0.02)a,b,c 50.24 (15.59)a,b 0.76 (0.06)a,c < 0.001 0.003 n.s. < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001

Functional & combined structural changes

n

Age

Sex (female/male) Education

Disease duration, years FA NDGMV Increased FC 100 41.98 (8.96)b,c,d 58/42b,d 5 (4 – 6) 10.32 (5.51)c,d 0.41 (0.01)c,d 60.55 (4.05)c,d 0.65 (0.03)b,d 67 45.50 (12.17)a,c,d 54/13 3 (5 – 6) 11.73 (6.77)c,d 0.41 (0.01)c,d 61.03 (3.44)c,d 0.75 (0.03)a,c 69 50.58 (8.03)a,b,d 46/23 5 (4 – 6) 16.78 (8.21)a,b,d 0.38 (0.02)a,b 51.81 (5.79)a,b 0.66 (0.02)b,d 94 54.38 (10.17)a,b,c 67/27 5 (3 – 6) 19.50 (9.17)a,b,c 0.37 (0.03)a,b 51.06 (6.09)a,b 0.76 (0.03)a,c < 0.001 0.0200 n.s. < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 F: functional; FA: fractional anisotropy; FC: functional connectivity; GM: gray matter NDGMV: normalized deep gray matter; S: structural

(21)
(22)
(23)

Abstract

Objective To explore the added value of dynamic functional connectivity (dFC) of the default mode network (DMN) during resting-state (RS), during an information processing speed (IPS) task, and the within-subject difference between these conditions, on top of conventional brain measures in explaining IPS in people with multiple sclerosis (pwMS).

Methods In 29 pwMS and 18 healthy controls, IPS was assessed with the Letter Digit Substitution Test and Stroop Card 1 and combined into an IPS-composite score. White matter (WM), grey matter (GM), and lesion volume were measured using 3T MRI. WM integrity was assessed with diffusion tensor imaging. During RS and task-state fMRI (i.e. symbol digit modalities task, IPS), stationary functional connectivity (sFC; average connectivity over the entire time series) and dFC (variation in connectivity using a sliding window approach) of the DMN was calculated, as well as the difference between both conditions (i.e. task-state minus RS; ΔsFC-DMN and ΔdFC-DMN). Regression analysis was performed to determine the most important predictors for IPS.

Results Compared to controls, pwMS performed worse on IPS-composite (p = 0.022), had lower GM volume (p < 0.05) and WM integrity (p < 0.001), but no alterations in sFC and dFC at the group level. In pwMS, 52% of variance in IPS-composite could be predicted by cortical volume (β = 0.49, p = 0.01) and ΔdFC-DMN (β = 0.52, p < 0.01). After adding dFC of the DMN to the model, the explained variance in IPS increased with 26% (p < 0.01).

(24)

109

Introduction

Up to 50% of people with multiple sclerosis (pwMS) suffer from problems with information processing speed (IPS), also known as "cognitive slowing". Deficits in IPS are among the first cognitive symptoms in pwMS and related to reduced

quality of life.1–4 The search for neural correlates of IPS deficits resulted in several

structural and functional brain measures. These include white and grey matter

damage (e.g. lesions, atrophy, and reduced tissue integrity),5–7 but also changes

in activation and functional connectivity (FC) during an IPS task or during

resting-state (RS).8–10 Although these measures do explain IPS to a certain extent,

there is still room to improve the relationship between brain measures and IPS. For example, intuitively IPS depends on the ability of the brain to rapidly transfer information within its functional network. As FC measures have previously been averaged over the entire scanning session (i.e. time series), from here on referred to as stationary FC (sFC), the variability in FC over time has not been taken into account. With this latter measure, the changes in connectivity strength during a time series are obtained, from here on referred to as dynamic FC (dFC). As dFC

seems to be behaviorally relevant with respect to cognition in healthy subjects11–13

or symptoms in neurological disorders,14–16 we argue that dFC could also be of

importance for maintained IPS in pwMS, as it could reflect the fast-changing

connectivity patterns within the brain that cannot be captured with sFC.11

Brain networks

The brain's functional network has an intrinsic organization, namely various (interconnected) RS networks that can be identified with RS functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). This intrinsic organization of the brain has been linked to cognitive functioning, as it is thought enable the flow of activity (i.e. information)

during task performance.17,18 An important brain network related to cognitive

(dys)functioning is the default mode network (DMN).19 This network consists of

several core regions, including the medial temporal lobe, medial prefrontal cortex,

posterior cingulate cortex, and inferior parietal cortex.19 Recent studies have

shown task-related 'responsivity' in sFC of the DMN, that is, the ability to change the connection strength upon task demands, to enable information integration

throughout the brain.20,21

Dynamics of the DMN

Previous studies have linked DMN dynamics during RS or task-state to cognitive functioning, such as executive functioning, cognitive flexibility, concept formation, and (working) memory, in healthy subjects and individuals with neurological

disorders (e.g. temporal lobe epilepsy and MS).16,22–28 Furthermore, one study in

(25)

frontoparietal network during task-state relative to RS was related to better

cognitive flexibility outside the scanner (i.e. Stroop task).23 Together with studies

showing differences in dFC between RS and task-state, the change in dFC of the DMN between RS and task-state might reflect the ability of the brain to adapt as task demands change, in order to optimally execute the task at hand (i.e. increased

information processing throughout the brain).11,29–31

To investigate whether dFC of the DMN is indeed a neural correlate of IPS in MS, and relevant next to previously identified correlates, we explored its incremental value when explaining IPS variance on top of conventional measures of brain abnormalities (defined as: brain atrophy, lesions, white matter integrity, and sFC of the DMN). We hypothesized that dFC of the DMN, and especially the difference in dFC between RS and task-state, that is, the ability of the brain to adapt upon task demands, would explain unique variance in IPS.

Materials and methods

Subjects and study design

In this prospective observational study, all pwMS (n = 33) and healthy controls (HCs; n = 19) met the following inclusion criteria: (1) aged 18–65 years, (2) no contra-indications for MRI, (3) no psychiatric or neurological disease (for pwMS: other than MS). For pwMS, additional inclusion criteria were: (4) a diagnosis of relapsing-remitting MS, and (5) without relapse or steroid treatment for at least four weeks prior to study measurements. Subjects performing below chance level (< 50% correct, n = 3 pwMS) on the fMRI paradigm were excluded from the entire study, as well as subjects with many frame-to-frame head displacements (> 0.5 mm for > 20% of frames, n = 1 pwMS and n = 1 HC) during fMRI to minimize

motion effects on dFC measures.32 The study was approved by the local institutional

(26)

111

Clinical measures

All subjects underwent neuropsychological testing, including, among others, the Letter Digit Substitution Task (LDST; oral version, 90 seconds), which is an

equivalent of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT),33 and the Stroop Test (for all

tests see Supplementary Methods).34 Scores on all neuropsychological tests were

converted into a Z-score relative to HCs. Scores on the LDST and Stroop Card 1 were averaged into one IPS composite Z-score. Anxiety and depression levels were

assessed with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.35 Fatigue was measured

using the Checklist of Individual Strength.36 Additionally, physical disability was

assessed by a trained physician using the Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS).37

MRI acquisition

All subjects were examined using a 3T whole-body MRI scanner (GE Signa-HDxt, Milwaukee, WI, USA) with a 32-channel head coil. The protocol included the following sequences: three-dimensional (3D) T1-weighted fast spoiled gradient echo for volume measurements (repetition time (TR): 8.22 ms, echo time (TE): 3.22 ms, inversion time (TI): 450 ms, flip angle 12 degrees, 1.0 mm sagittal slices,

0.94 × 0.94 mm2 in-plane resolution); 3D fluid-attenuated inversion recovery

(FLAIR; TR: 8000 ms, TE: 128 ms, TI: 2343 ms, 1.2 mm sagittal slices, 0.98 × 0.98

mm2 in-plane resolution) for white matter (WM) lesion detection; and diffusion

tensor imaging (DTI; TR: 7200 ms, TE: 83 ms, flip angle 90 degrees, 57 axial slices with an isotropic 2.0 mm resolution) with 5 volumes without directional weighting

and 30 volumes with non-collinear diffusion gradients (b-value: 1000 s/mm2) to

assess WM integrity. To correct for echo planar imaging (EPI) induced artifacts, two scans with reversed phase-encode blips were acquired for DTI. Furthermore, RS fMRI (eyes closed; EPI, 202 volumes, TR: 2200 ms, TE: 35 ms, flip angle 80 degrees,

3 mm contiguous axial slices, 3.3 × 3.3 mm2 in-plane resolution) and task-related

(i.e. task-state) fMRI (IPS paradigm; EPI, 460 volumes, TR: 2000 ms, TE: 30 ms, flip

angle 80 degrees, 4 mm contiguous axial slices, 3.3 × 3.3 mm2 in-plane resolution)

were performed to measure sFC and dFC.

Structural MRI measures

Whole-brain and lesion volume

Lesions were automatically segmented on FLAIR images and filled on the 3DT1

images using LEAP.38,39 WM and grey matter (GM) volumes were measured using

SIENAX.40 Volumes of deep GM structures were measured using FIRST (FSL v5.0.9,

fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Cortical GM volume was measured by subtracting the FIRST

(27)

Severity and extent of WM damage

The susceptibility-induced off-resonance field was estimated for the diffusion weighted sequence using a method described previously, and the two images

were combined into a single corrected one (and used for further processing).41

Motion- and eddy-current correction was performed, followed by diffusion tensor fitting (FMRIB's Diffusion Toolbox, FSL). Tract-based spatial statistics with

default settings was used to obtain skeletonized fractional anisotropy (FA) maps.42

In order to obtain individual measures of whole-brain WM integrity damage, we quantified the severity and extent of WM damage using a method that has

been described previously.43 In short, each subject's skeleton was voxelwise

expressed as a Z-score relative to HCs. The severity of WM damage in a subject was subsequently calculated by computing average normalized FA score within the skeleton. The extent of WM damage was calculated by counting the number

of voxels exceeding the threshold of Z < -3.1 (p < 0.001) in each subject.43 Finally,

the whole-brain average FA of the white matter was obtained for each subject.

Functional MRI

Task-state fMRI paradigm

Task-state fMRI was obtained by examining IPS using the modified version of the

SDMT (mSDMT) inside the scanner.8 Briefly, in the stimulus condition a panel of

nine paired stimulus boxes were presented. The boxes in the upper row contained a symbol, while the boxes in the lower row contained a digit (1 to 9). Below this panel, a symbol-digit pair was presented, and the subject had to indicate via a button box whether or not this symbol-digit pair matched one of the pairs in the upper panel. The timing of the stimuli was pre-specified. Accuracy was used for the analyses of behavioral task data. Reaction time for correct trials was not included, as this can be influenced by motor problems in pwMS. The paradigm lasted approximately 15 minutes.

Preprocessing of fMRI data

(28)

113

Atlas construction

In this study, we used the Brainnetome atlas44 (210 regions) to parcellate the brain.

By overlaying the Yeo7 RS network atlas45 on the Brainnetome atlas, we were

able to identify which Brainnetome regions belonged to the DMN based on two criteria: 1) > 50% of voxels of a Brainnetome region had to overlap with the DMN from the Yeo7 atlas, and 2) the overlap with the DMN should be the highest and the differences in overlap with the second highest RS network should be > 15%. In total, 38 regions of the Brainnetome atlas were considered part of the DMN. After preprocessing of RS and task-state images, the Brainnetome atlas in standard space was non-linearly registered to each subject's 3D T1 image, and subsequently masked for GM (binarized SIENAX segmentation). Next, the subcortical regions derived by FIRST were added, resulting in an atlas containing 224 regions. This novel atlas was then linearly registered to RS and task-state fMRI space, where areas known to be prone to artifacts were removed (e.g. orbitofrontal cortex), by excluding voxels with a signal intensity in the lowest quartile of the robust intensity range (i.e. the minimum and maximum if the outer tails of the intensity distribution are ignored). Finally, the average time series for each brain region was obtained during RS and task-state fMRI, and imported into Matlab R2012a (Natick, Massachusetts, USA) for further analysis.

Stationary FC

To obtain sFC, Pearson correlation coefficients between all brain regions (i.e. region of interest-wise analysis) over the entire time series (absolute values) were

calculated for RS and task-state fMRI separately (see Figure 3.2.1A).

Dynamic FC

For dFC, a sliding-window approach was used with settings that were selected

based on previous studies (see Figure 3.2.1A), resulting in 35 and 86 sliding windows

for RS and task-state time series, respectively.28,46 For RS time series the window

length was 27 volumes (59.4 s) with a shift of 5 volumes (11 s). For task-state time series, the window length was 30 volumes (60 s) with a shift of 5 volumes (10 s). Note that the differences in window length and shift were caused by differences in TR. Next, absolute Pearson correlation coefficient was calculated between all 224 regions for each window. Finally, the absolute difference in FC was calculated between each consecutive window and subsequently summed per matrix cell, resulting in a 224 by 224 dFC matrix.

DMN connections

(29)

and dFC measure was divided by its corresponding whole-brain average sFC and dFC. Furthermore, we calculated the difference in sFC of the DMN (ΔsFC-DMN) between RS and task-state as follows: task-state sFC DMN minus RS sFC DMN. The differences in dFC of the DMN (ΔdFC-DMN) was calculated similarly: task-state dFC DMN minus RS dFC DMN. Positive values indicate an increase in sFC or dFC in

task-state relative to RS. See Figure 3.2.1B and Figure 3.2.1C for a schematic overview of

functional measures.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed in SPSS version 22 (Armonk, NY, USA). Normality of variables was investigated using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and visual inspection of histograms. NLV and the extent of WM damage were log-transformed to obtain normally distributed data. EDSS, educational level, questionnaires for anxiety and depression, and mSDMT accuracy were not transformed and tested with Mann-Whitney U tests. Univariate and multivariate general linear models were constructed to assess group differences in behavioral measures (IPS, questionnaires, and task performance), structural MRI (atrophy, extent and severity of white matter damage), sFC, and dFC (with average head motion as covariate to further limit its effect on the analyses). We performed a two-tailed one-sample t-test in each group to test whether ΔsFC-DMN and ΔdFC-DMN differed from zero (indicating a change). The differences in ΔsFC-DMN and ΔdFC-DMN between groups were analyzed with a univariate general linear model. Additionally, we explored the relationship between dFC and conventional brain measures using Pearson correlation coefficients. Because of multiple testing regarding brain measures and the correlation analyses, Benjamini-Hochberg

false discovery rate corrected p-values (corr. p) are reported for these analyses.47

(30)

115

Post hoc specifi city analysis

To investigate the specificity of the relationship between dynamic brain measures that were significantly related to IPS, we performed a correlation analysis (Spearman's rank correlation) with other outcome variables, including EDSS score, fatigue, depression, and all neuropsychological tests.

Time Br ain a cti va tion Time Br ain a cti va tion sFC Timeseries FC FC FC FC

window 1window 2window 3

window 4 etc.

Resting-state Task-state (IPS paradigm)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 4

|ΔFC| |ΔFC| |ΔFC|

dFC =

Stationary functional connectivity Dynamic functional connectivity A B C Timeseries RS sFC RS dFC task-state sFC task-state dFC ΔdFC-DMN (task-state – RS) ΔsFC-DMN (task-state – RS)

Figure 3.2.1 Schematic overview of functional measures

Stationary (s) functional connectivity (FC) was calculated with Pearson correlation coeffi cients over the entire time series, whereas for dynamic (d) FC the time series were divided into sliding windows (A). For each sliding window, FC was calculated and subsequently the absolute diff erence between each consecutive window was calculated and summed as a measure of dFC. For both resting-state (RS) and task-state fMRI, sFC and dFC of the default mode network was obtained (B). Additionally, the diff erence in sFC and dFC between task-state and RS was calculated (C).

(31)

Results

Demographics and clinical measures

The final sample size included 29 pwMS (18 women, mean age: 41.3 ± 9.3 years, mean disease duration: 11.1 ± 7.1 years) and 18 HCs (11 women, mean age: 40.7 ±

13.3 years; see Table 3.2.1). No differences between groups were found for age,

sex, and educational level. Anxiety and depression scores were similar between groups, but pwMS reported more fatigue than controls (pwMS: 72.0 ± 33.6, HCs: 47.1 ± 18.3, p = 0.007). Compared to HCs, pwMS performed worse on the LDST (Z-score: -1.1 ± 1.4, p = 0.005), but not on the Stroop Card 1 (Z-score: -0.5 ± 1.4, p = 0.186). The IPS composite Z-score was lower in pwMS (Z-score: -0.8 ± 1.3) compared to HCs (Z-score: 0.0 ± 0.8, p = 0.022). Furthermore, compared to HCs, pwMS performed worse on tests for visuospatial memory, executive

functioning, working memory, and verbal fluency (see Supplementary Table

3.2.1). In total, 6 pwMS met the criteria for cognitive impairment (scoring at least 2 SD below that of HCs on at least 2 tests). Accuracy on the IPS task inside the scanner did not differ between groups.

Structural brain changes

NWMV was similar between groups (see Table 3.2.2), while pwMS displayed

lower NCGMV (corr. p = 0.009) and NDGMV (corr. p = 0.002). Additionally, the severity and extent of WM damage was worse in pwMS compared to HCs (corr.

p < 0.001 for both). Furthermore, whole brain FA was lower in pwMS than in HCs

(corr. p < 0.001).

Stationary and dynamic FC

No group differences were observed for RS and task-state sFC and dFC of the DMN

(see Table 3.2.3). Additionally, in both groups, ΔsFC-DMN did not differ significantly

from zero (mean ΔsFC-DMN pwMS: -0.02 ± 0.06, t(28) = 1.70, corr. p = 0.133; mean ΔsFC-DMN HCs: -0.02 ± 0.07, t(17) = 1.06, corr. p = 0.304), suggesting no difference in sFC between RS and task-state. Furthermore, ΔsFC-DMN did not differ between pwMS and HCs.

In pwMS and HCs, ΔdFC-DMN differed significantly from zero (mean ΔdFC-DMN pwMS: 0.02 ± 0.02, t(28) = 3.63, corr. p = 0.004; mean ΔdFC-DMN HCs: 0.03 ± 0.01,

t(17) = 2.81, corr. p = 0.024), suggesting an increase in dFC of the DMN during

(32)

117

Relationship between dFC and conventional brain measures

After correction for multiple testing, none of the correlation coefficients between dynamic and conventional brain measures were statistically significant in pwMS or HCs (corr. p > 0.251).

Table 3.2.1 Demographics and clinical measures

pwMS (n = 29) HCs (n = 18) p Age 41.25 (9.34) 40.68 (13.29) 0.863 Sex (female/male) 18/11 11/7 0.948 Educational levela 6.00 (5.00 – 7.00) 6.00 (5.00 – 7.00) 0.098 Disease duration 11.05 (7.11) – – EDSSa 3.00 (1.00 – 6.00) HADS-Aa 5.50 (0.00 – 12.00)b 4.00 (2.00 – 13.00) 0.170 HADS-Da 3.00 (0.00 – 14.00)b 1.50 (0.00 – 6.00) 0.229 CIS20r 71.97 (33.58)b 47.06 (18.29) 0.007 Z-score LDST -1.11 (1.38) 0.00 (1.00) 0.005

Z-score Stroop Card 1 -0.51 (1.41) 0.00 (1.00) 0.186

IPS composite Z-score -0.81 (1.31) 0.00 (0.78) 0.022

mSDMT performance (inside scanner)

Accuracy (%)a 95.45 (77.27 – 100.00) 96.36 (86.36 – 100.00) 0.307

Displayed data are mean (standard deviation)

CIS20r: Checklist of Individual Strength, revised; EDSS: Expanded Disability Status Scale; HCs: healthy controls; HADS: Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; A: Anxiety; D: Depression; IPS: information processing speed; LDST: Letter Digit Substitution Test; mSDMT: modified Symbol Digit Modalities Test; pwMS: people with multiple sclerosis

(33)

Table 3.2.2 Brain volumes and white matter damage

pwMS (n = 29) HCs (n = 18) Effect size (η2) p p corr.

NWMV (ml) 682.55 (46.91) 699.07 (39.36) 0.033 0.219 0.219 NCGMV (ml) 780.47 (76.20) 839.46 (59.41) 0.148 0.008 0.009 NDGMV (ml) 58.80 (6.88) 65.08 (4.59) 0.207 0.001 0.002 NLV (ml) 22.46 (15.99) – – – –

WM damage

Whole brain average FA

Severity score

Average Z-score skeleton

Extent score

Number of affected voxels (%) 0.33 (0.03)a -0.61 (0.50)a 3430.30 (5115.74)a (2.87%) 0.36 (0.02) 0.00 (0.34) 21.90 (55.86) (0.02%) 0.335 0.321 0.822 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001

Displayed data are mean (standard deviation)

HCs: healthy controls; NCGMV: normalized cortical grey matter volume; NDGMV: normalized deep grey matter volume; NLV: normalized lesion volume; NWMV: normalized white matter volume; WM: white matter; p corr. = false discovery rate corrected p-values; pwMS = people with multiple sclerosis

a n = 27

Predicting IPS

mSDMT accuracy

In pwMS, mSDMT accuracy inside the scanner could not be predicted by

conventional brain measures (block 1 – 4; Table 3.2.4). However, when adding

block 5, the model predicted 23% of the variance in mSDMT accuracy, effectively by ΔdFC-DMN only (β = 0.51, p = 0.006). In HCs, 23% of variance in mSDMT accuracy could be explained by RS dFC of the DMN only (β = 0.53, p = 0.025).

For both pwMS and HCs, Figure 3.2.2A displays the relationship between the final

model and the outcome measure. IPS composite Z-score

In pwMS, confounders and conventional brain measures could explain 26% of variance in IPS composite Z-score (block 1 – 4) with NCGMV (β = 0.47, p = 0.050)

as predictor (Table 3.2.4). When adding dFC of the DMN, the amount of explained

variance increased to 52% (R2 change = 0.25, p = 0.001). The predictors now

included NCGMV (β = 0.49, p = 0.013) and ΔdFC-DMN (β = 0.52, p = 0.001) In HCs, none of the predictors were significantly related to IPS outside the scanner.

(34)

119

Table 3.2.3 Task-state and resting-state stationary and dynamic functional connectivity

pwMS (n = 29) HCs (n = 18) Effect size (η2) p p corr.

Average head motion

Resting-state (mm) Task-state (mm) 0.068 (0.035) 0.091 (0.042) 0.068 (0.028) 0.068 (0.036) < 0.001 0.075 0.989 0.063 0.989 0.167 Resting-state sFC DMN dFC DMN 0.959 (0.051) 1.018 (0.030) 0.952 (0.052) 1.019 (0.024) 0.005 < 0.001 0.647 0.893 0.989 0.989 Task-state sFC DMN dFC DMN 0.940 (0.048) 1.034 (0.024) 0.935 (0.040) 1.049 (0.027) < 0.001 0.035 0.957 0.213 0.989 0.167 Difference

Task-state minus resting-state

ΔsFC-DMN ΔdFC-DMN -0.019 (0.060) 0.016 (0.023) -0.017 (0.068) 0.029 (0.044) < 0.001 0.042 0.917 0.168 0.989 0.167

Displayed data are mean (standard deviation)

dFC: dynamic functional connectivity; DMN: default mode network; HCs: healthy controls; p corr.: false discovery rate corrected p-values; pwMS: people with multiple sclerosis; sFC: stationary functional connectivity

Post hoc specificity analysis

Within both groups, no significant relationship was found between ΔdFC-DMN and EDSS score (pwMS: corr. p = 0.860), fatigue (pwMS: corr. p = 0.768; HC: corr.

p = 0.712), depression (pwMS and HCs: corr. p = 0.712) or any neuropsychological

test (pwMS and HCs corr. p > 0.712).

Post hoc exploration: effect of switching medication

No differences were found between switchers (n = 13) and non-switchers (n = 19) with respect to demographics, disease characteristics, IPS composite Z-score, or

mSDMT accuracy (Supplementary Table 3.2.2). Furthermore, no significant group

differences were found regarding structural MRI, sFC and dFC of the DMN.

No effect of switching was found on mSDMT accuracy (β = -0.14, p = 0.395). Furthermore, adding the interaction between switching with ΔdFC-DMN to the

model did not increase the amount of explained variance (R2 change = 0.02,

p = 0.463), resulting in a final model explaining 23% of variance in mSDMT by

ΔdFC-DMN (β = 0.52, p = 0.005). Similar findings were obtained for IPS composite

Z-score: no effect of switchers/non-switchers was found on this dependent variable

(β = -0.16, p = 0.263). Additionally, adding the interaction terms between switchers/ non-switchers with ΔdFC-DMN and NCGMV did not improve the model in terms

of explained variance (R2 change = 0.04, p = 0.358). In total, 54% of variance could

(35)

Table 3.2.4 Hierarchical regression models for predicting mSDMT performance and IPS composite Z-score

Adjusted R2 Standardized β Test statistic p

mSDMT accuracy pwMS Full model: block 1 – 4 N/A N/A N/A

Full model: block 1 – 5

ΔdFC-DMN 0.23 0.51 8.83a 2.97b 0.006 0.006

HCs Full model: block 1 – 4 N/A N/A N/A

Full model: block 1 – 5

RS dFC-DMN 0.23 -0.53 6.10a -2.47b 0.025 0.025 IPS composite Z-score

pwMS Full model: block 1 – 4

Age NCGMV 0.26 -0.14 0.47 5.65a -0.60b 2.07b 0.010 0.554 0.050

Full model: block 1 – 5

Age NCGMV ΔdFC-DMN 0.52 < 0.01 0.49 0.52 10.20a < 0.01b 2.69b 3.67b < 0.001 0.998 0.013 0.001

HCs Full model: block 1 – 4 N/A N/A N/A

Full model: block 1 – 5 N/A N/A N/A

dFC: dynamic functional connectivity; DMN: default mode network; HCs: healthy controls; IPS: information processing speed; mSDMT: modified Symbol Digit Modalities Test; N/A: not applicable; pwMS: people with multiple sclerosis; RS: resting-state; sFC: stationary functional connectivity

a F-value b t-value

Discussion

(36)

121

Conventional MRI and IPS

Problems with IPS in MS have typically been explained as a consequence of WM damage, such as lesions and decreased integrity, but also of cortical and deep

GM atrophy.5–7 Although WM damage or deep GM atrophy were not identified as

the most important predictors for IPS in our sample, we did observe that NCGMV could explain up to 26% of variance in IPS outside the scanner. With respect to sFC, we did not observe differences between pwMS and controls, whereas previous studies did show differences in DMN effective and stationary FC at rest and during

an IPS task, both related to IPS.9,10 These contradictory findings might be explained

by methodological differences, such as sample size and operationalization of FC. With respect to the latter, a previous study used a seed-based approach instead

of an atlas-based approach on RS data only.10 The other study investigated the

directionality of FC (i.e. effective FC) during IPS, which provides differential

information than sFC, and is therefore difficult to compare.9 Furthermore, we

normalized FC measures for whole-brain average FC, as this average FC can vary greatly between subjects and potentially drive differences between groups, which

-3 -2 -1 0 1 2 Healthy controls mSDMT accuracy (%) Standardized residual mSDMT accuracy (%) -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 100 95 90 85 80 75

People with multiple sclerosis

Standardized residual 100 97.5 95 92.5 90 87.5 IPS composite Z-score -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3

People with multiple sclerosis

Standardized residual 2 0 -2 -4 A

B Figure 3.2.2 Relationship between the regression

model and outcome measures

For both accuracy on the modified symbol digit modalities test (A) and information processing speed composite Z-score (B), the standardized residuals of the final regression model, including dynamic functional connectivity of the default mode network, is plotted against performance for people with MS and healthy controls separately.

(37)

is not always performed in other studies. Applying this normalization step allowed us to deal with individual differences in FC and can thereby more accurately reflect possible changes in FC between groups.

Dynamics and IPS

Although no differences between pwMS and HCs were observed in dFC of the DMN, adding dFC to the regression model, next to confounding variables and conventional MRI measures, increased the explained variance by the model for IPS inside and outside the scanner. Furthermore, we did not observe a significant relationship between ΔdFC-DMN and EDSS score, fatigue, depression or any other neuropsychological test score, suggesting that our findings are specific for IPS. Changes in RS DMN dynamics have been observed in other neurological disorders, including schizophrenia, autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, depression, and epilepsy. Often, these changes were

related to the severity of symptoms.14–16,48 A large challenge to directly compare

between study results is differences in operationalization of DMN dynamics (e.g. standard deviation of FC, non-overlapping windows, or spectrum analysis) but also variation in types of pathology. Nevertheless, the present measure of dFC was able to pick up individual differences in pwMS and HCs regarding dFC of the DMN and IPS. This is in line with a recent study that was able to identify individuals based on spatial patterns of dynamic characteristics of FC (i.e. 'fingerprinting'), which was a significant predictor for higher order cognitive functions (i.e. fluid

intelligence and executive functions).22

DMN responsivity

In both pwMS and HCs, an increase in dFC of the DMN was observed from RS to task-state, suggesting that this might be a response upon increasing cognitive demands. Interestingly, a larger increase in dFC from RS to task-state in pwMS was the only significant predictor for IPS inside the scanner, as well as a significant predictor for IPS outside the scanner (next to NCGMV). These findings suggest that during IPS, the DMN seems to change its FC pattern more often than during RS,

possibly reflecting increased information flow throughout the network.25 In HCs,

however, lower RS dFC of the DMN was related to better IPS inside the scanner, whereas no predictors were found for IPS outside the scanner (probably explained by limited variation in performance and obtained brain measures). Previous studies in healthy subjects have linked both lower and higher brain dynamics during RS to

better cognitive functioning.12,23,27 These varying results illustrate the complexity

(38)

123

The behavioral relevance of increasing DMN dynamics during task-state relative to RS is in line with a previous study in healthy subjects on cognitive flexibility

(i.e. Stroop task) and dFC of the DMN with the frontoparietal network.23 That is,

higher dFC of the DMN with frontoparietal network during task-state and lower dFC during RS in isolation were related to better cognitive flexibility outside the

scanner.23 However, a larger increase in task-state relative to RS dFC explained even

more variance in cognitive flexibility.23 Another study found that the frontoparietal

network was more dynamic during working memory compared to a control condition, which could be related to better working memory and executive

functions.29 For sFC, the increase in DMN connectivity with respect to increasing

task-load has been described previously in healthy subjects.20,21 In turn, this DMN

responsivity could be related to maintained task performance.20,21 Metaphorically,

this task-evoked responsivity of the brain might be similar as performing a

challenging physical exercise that exposes certain cardiac conditions.13 Hence,

one could speculate that combining RS with task-state functional measures might (partly) capture the ability of a patient's functional network to adapt upon task

demands, which might explain a patient's cognitive abilities.13

Switching medication

The present MS sample consisted of pwMS switching to fingolimod treatment and non-switchers continuing with first-line therapy. The status of pwMS (switchers/ non-switchers) did not relate to IPS or mediated the relationship between ΔdFC-DMN and NCGMV with IPS. These findings suggest that switchers were statistically similar to non-switchers. However, one should keep in mind that more than half of the switchers used first-line therapy prior to fingolimod and changed therapy because of disease activity (n = 7), whereas positivity for John Cunningham virus, and therefore at risk for developing progressive multifocal leukoencephalopathy, was the main reason for pwMS to switch from natalizumab to fingolimod (n = 6) and not so much disease activity. Future studies should explore possible changes in brain dynamics under MS treatment over time in relationship to disease activity and cognitive functioning.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Exploring Variations in Functional Connectivity of the Resting State Default Mode Network in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury.. Preinjury somatization symptoms contribute to clinical

This case study reports on electrical load management that was implemented on the comminution section of a South African gold plant, namely Gold Plant 1. The

Emotionele Empathie Taak als uit de Emotional Contagion Scale naar voren kwam dat mensen met sociale angst juist meer emotionele empathie lijken te hebben als het om negatieve

Although the logical order for a traditional Information Extraction (IE) system is to complete the extraction process before commencing the disambiguation, we start with an

van der Walle, “Velomobiles and the Modelling of Transport Technologies,” in Cycling and Society, edited by Peter Cox, David Horton, and Paul Rosen (Burlington, Vt: Ashgate,

De werkzame beroepsbevolking omvat alle personen van 15 tot 75 jaar die in Nederland wonen en betaald werk hebben, ongeacht in welk land gewerkt wordt... Dat komt neer op

The relationship between cognitive insight and time-varying dy- namics of resting state fMRI was examined with Spearman correlations between self-reflectiveness, self-certainty or

In this subsection, a projected gradient optimization method is presented that reaches an optimal convergence for the class of convex optimization problems with strongly