• No results found

Pretending to be someone else Does acting experience diminish the cognitive cost of lying?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Pretending to be someone else Does acting experience diminish the cognitive cost of lying?"

Copied!
30
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Pretending  to  be  someone  else  

Does  acting  experience  diminish  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying?  

                                                                     

Number  of  EC:     21  

Date:           March  25,  2013  until  March  12,  2014  

Name:         Chantal  Keijlard  

Student  ID:         5960428  

Supervisor:         Bruno  J.  Verschuere  PhD.   Co-­‐assessor:         prof.  dr.  Denny  Borsboom   UvA  Representative:     prof.  dr.  Denny  Borsboom    

University  of  Amsterdam,  Faculty  of  Social  and  Behavioral  Sciences,  Clinical  Psychology    

MSc  in  Brain  and  Cognitive  Sciences,  University  of  Amsterdam     Cognitive  Neuroscience  track  

(2)

Abstract  

Lying  typically  comes  with  a  cognitive  cost  as  opposed  to  telling  the  truth.  For  instance,   lying   usually   takes   longer   than   truth   telling.   The   boundary   conditions   of   this   effect   however  are  unknown.  Whereas  some  have  argued  that  this  cognitive  cost  is  hardwired,   recent   research   has   suggested   that   the   cognitive   cost   of   lying   is   malleable   through   practice.   Here,   we   examined   whether   individual   differences   in   experience   with   deception  modulate  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying.  In  this  study  we  tested  individuals  who   are  trained  in  one  specific  form  of  deception:  Pretending  to  be  someone  else.  Hence  we   examined  the  differences  in  cognitive  cost  of  lying  regarding  denying  autobiographical   information   between   actors   and   non-­‐actors   with   three   different   reaction   time   based   deception  tests.  There  was  no  effect  of  acting  experience  for  any  of  the  deception  tests,   hence  the  results  pointed  in  the  direction  of  a  fixed  cognitive  cost  of  lying.  However,  two   of  the  three  deception  tests  failed  to  indicate  a  cognitive  cost  of  lying  altogether.  Due  to   the   small   sample   size   and   the   lack   of   significant   results,   conclusions   about   the   malleability  of  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying  cannot  be  made  with  certainty.  Even  though  the   current  study  failed  to  provide  evidence  for  the  malleability  of  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying,   certain  methodological  adjustments  could  be  made  that  might  yield  a  different  outcome.   Implications  of  a  variety  of  outcomes  will  be  discussed  in  this  paper.      

 

(3)

Introduction  

Deceiving   others   is   said   to   be   more   cognitively   constraining   than   truth   telling.   Apart   from   constructing   the   lie,   one   has   to   keep   the   truth   in   mind,   while   at   the   same   time   inhibiting   it   (Walczyk,   Roper,   Seemann,   &   Humphrey,   2003;   Spence   et   al.,   2004).   Previous  research  indicated  that  lying  takes  longer,  and  is  more  error  prone  than  telling   the  truth  (Furedy,  Davis,  &  Gurevich,  1988;  Seymour,  Seifert,  Shafto,  &  Mosmann,  2000;   Spence  et  al.,  2001;  Farrow  et  al.,  2003;  Walczyk  et  al.,  2003;  Sartori,  Agosta,  Zogmaister,   Ferrara,   &   Castiello,   2008;   Seymour   &   Kerlin,   2008;   Osman,   Channon,   &   Fitzpatrick,   2009;   Walczyk,   Mahoney,   Doverspike,   &   Griffith-­‐Ross,   2009;   Hu,   Wu,   &   Fu,   2011;   Verschuere,  Spruyt,  Meijer,  &  Otgaar,  2011;  G.  Visu-­‐Petra,  Miclea,  &  L.  Visu-­‐Petra,    2012;   Williams,   Bott,   Patrick,   &   Lewis,   2013).   According   to   R.   Johnson,   Barnhardt,   &   Zhu   (2005)  this  “cognitive  cost  of  lying”  is  resistant  to  practice,  as  it  is  caused  by  inherent   limitations   of   human   information   processing.   However,   recent   research   has   provided   initial  evidence  against  this  statement.  Verschuere  et  al.  (2011)  found  that  changing  the   proportion  of  the  truth  and  lie  responses  in  a  lie  detection  test  affected  the  ease  of  lying.   When  lie  trials  outnumbered  truth  trials  lying  became  easier,  whereas  when  truth  trials   outnumbered  lie  trials  lying  became  more  difficult.  These  findings  provide  evidence  for   the  claim  that  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying  is  not  fixed.  In  fact  it  may  be  malleable  through   training.   Additional   research   replicated   this   training   effect,   while   also   showing   that   it   was  specific  to  practiced  questions  only  and  did  not  generalize  to  untrained  questions   (Van  Bockstaele  et  al.,  2012).  The  training  however  took  place  in  a  laboratory,  making  it   difficult   to   generalize   the   results   outside   of   a   laboratory   setting.   Additionally,   the   training  was  quite  short  (single  training  of  540  trials).  It  has  been  argued  that  one  needs   around  10,000  hours  of  practice  in  order  to  reach  an  expert  level  of  performance  with   domain-­‐specific   learning   (Ericsson,   Krampe,   &   Tesch-­‐Römer,   1993;   Ericsson   &   Lehmann,  1996).  Since  it  is  unrealistic  to  train  people  in  a  laboratory  for  that  amount  of   time,  we  suggested  a  different  research  approach  for  examining  the  malleability  of  the   cognitive   cost   of   lying.   Instead   of   training   individuals   extensively   at   lying,   it   is   more   efficient  to  test  certain  groups  of  individuals  who  are  trained  at  deception  already  (e.g.   actors).   Such   cross-­‐sectional   designs   allow   us   to   examine   individuals   with   different   levels  of  experience  with  lying.    

Before   we   delve   into   the   current   experiment   we   will   first   elaborate   on   the   cognitive   cost   of   lying   by   describing   several   theories   on   the   topic.   Secondly,   multiple   deception   paradigms   will   be   discussed   after   which   research   attempting   to   alter   the   cognitive  cost  of  lying  is  described  as  well  as  individual  differences  regarding  this  cost.   Thirdly,  an  extensive  description  of  the  current  experiment  will  be  provided  after  which   the  implications  of  the  findings  will  be  discussed.  Finally,  a  proposal  for  future  research   will  be  provided.    

 

The  cognitive  cost  of  lying  

There  are  several  theories  about  the  cognition  of  deception.  In  this  section  some  of  these   theories  will  be  described  in  order  to  provide  an  overview  of  the  opinions  on  how  the   enhanced  difficulty  for  lying  is  expressed.  The  underlying  cognitive  tasks  these  theories   propose  are  involved  will  also  be  discussed.    

According  to  the  Activation-­‐Decision-­‐Construction  Model  of  Lying  (ADCM)  there   are   three   cognitive   events   (divided   into   sub-­‐steps   a   to   g)   that   occur   during   the   act   of   deception   (activation,   decision,   and   construction   component,   Walczyk   et   al.,   2003).   When   asked   a   question,   relevant   information   from   semantic   and   episodic   memory   is   activated  (step  a;  Kintsch,  1998,  cited  in  Walczyk  et  al.,  2003).  The  question  itself  will  

(4)

occupy   the   articulatory   loop   in   working   memory   (WM;   step   b).   Consequently,   information  in  long-­‐term  memory  (LTM),  which  is  linked  to  the  activated  information,   will  become  active,  allowing  the  deceiver  to  use  this  additional  information  to  construct   a  lie  (step  c;  Ericsson  &  Kintsch,  1995;  Walczyk  et  al.,  2003).  Based  on  this  information  a   decision  is  being  made  about  the  favorable  response  (i.e.  whether  lying  would  be  more   beneficial  than  telling  the  truth  or  vice  versa,  step  d).  If  a  decision  to  lie  has  been  made,   an   inhibitory   link   is   established,   which   prevents   the   individual   from   telling   the   truth   (step  e;  Walczyk  et  al.,  2003;  Williams  et  al.,  2013).  Additionally,  implausible  lies  or  lies   that   may   otherwise   jeopardize   the   believability   of   the   liar   are   inhibited.   After   this   selection  process  the  lie  that  is  most  active  will  now  become  fully  active  in  WM  (step  f)   after  which  it  is  uttered  (step  g).  The  ADCM  proposes  that  the  activation  component  is   automatic,   whereas   the   decision   to   lie   or   tell   the   truth   is   intentional   as   is   the   construction  of  a  lie  itself  (Walczyk  et  al.,  2003).  In  order  to  find  evidence  for  the  ADCM,   Walczyk   et   al.   (2003)   examined   each   of   the   cognitive   components   of   this   model.   They   found  that  a  question  indeed  made  the  truth  become  active,  even  when  the  individual   had  the  intention  to  lie  to  the  question.  When  open-­‐ended  questions  were  asked,  it  took   participants   on   average   166   ms   to   make   the   decision   to   lie.   Walczyk   et   al.   (2005)   replicated   this   effect.   Furthermore,   the   construction   of   a   lie   made   participants   even   slower  to  respond  to  the  questions  as  was  also  found  earlier  by  Spence  et  al.  (2001).    

The   Parallel   Task   Set   Model   (Seymour,   2001,   cited   in   Walczyk,   Igou,   Dixon,   &   Tcholakian,  2013)  posits  that  during  deceitful  answering  to  multiple  choice  questions  a   liar  has  to  remember  the  correct  answer,  select  the  (previously  chosen)  incorrect  (and   thus  deceitful)  alternative  from  the  available  answers,  prepare  to  give  the  response,  and   finally  execute  the  response.  Recognizing  the  correct  response  occurs  automatically  and   fast.   Recollection   of   the   deceitful   response   however,   occurs   slower   and   is   under   conscious  control.  When  hiding  Guilty  knowledge  (e.g.  the  murder  weapon  from  a  crime   scene),  the  liar  has  to  inhibit  the  response  for  the  correct  alternative,  and  execute  the   previously   chosen   deceitful   response,   causing   a   response   conflict,   which   then   in   turn   results  in  longer  reaction  times.    

The  Interpersonal  Deception  Theory  (Buller  &  Burgoon,  1996;  Burgoon  &  Buller,   2008,   cited   in   Walczyk   et   al.,   2013)   focuses   more   on   the   interaction   between   the   deceiver  and  his  or  her  conversation  partner.  According  to  this  theory  the  cognitive  load   that   comes   with   deception   comprises   several   factors.   Deceivers   have   to   make   up   a   believable  story,  while  at  the  same  time  monitoring  the  responses  of  their  conversation   partner,  as  well  as  keep  track  of  the  lies  they  created  during  the  conversation.  Because   this  is  already  vexing  enough  as  it  is,  additional  tasks  such  as  controlling  for  behaviors   that  cue  deception  will  lead  to  a  cognitive  overload.  This  cognitive  overload  will  result  in   the  deceiver  expressing  behavioral  signs  of  deception  (e.g.  vagueness  in  the  story  being   told,   frequent   pausing),   risking   exposure   (Walczyk   et   al.,   2003).   Skilled   liars   however,   may  have  better  working  memory,  allowing  them  to  process  all  the  relevant  information   without  causing  a  cognitive  overload.  These  liars  should  then  be  more  difficult  to  expose   with  the  conventional  lie  detection  techniques.    

Generally,   the   theories   described   above   recognize   the   claim   that   lying   is   more   difficult   than   telling   the   truth.   However,   there   is   no   clear   consensus   on   how   this   enhanced  difficulty  is  expressed.  Several  deception  tests  have  been  developed  that  aim   to   pinpoint   deception.   In   the   next   section   some   deception   paradigms   and   their   applications  will  be  discussed.  

   

(5)

Deception  Paradigms  

The  most  frequently  used  procedure  for  the  polygraph  test  is  the  Control  Question  Test   (CQT;   e.g.   Ben-­‐Shakhar,   Bar-­‐Hillel,   &   Kremnitzer,   2002;   Walczyk   et   al.,   2013),   which   consists  of  critical  questions  (e.g.  relevant  to  a  committed  crime),  and  control  questions   (usually   related   to   the   respondent’s   past).   If   the   physiological   responses   (heart   rate,   respiration,   blood   pressure,   skin   conductance   response)   to   critical   items   are   significantly   higher   than   the   responses   to   control   items   this   indicates   a   positive   test   outcome  (i.e.  indicating  that  the  respondent  is  lying),  if  the  physiological  responses  are   lower   for   critical   items   this   indicates   a   negative   test   outcome   (i.e.   indicating   that   the   respondent  is  telling  the  truth),  and  if  the  physiological  responses  between  item  types   do  not  differ  the  outcome  of  the  polygraph  is  undecided  (e.g.  Fiedler,  Schmid,  &  Stahl,   2002).  According  to  several  researchers  the  critical  questions  of  the  CQT  are  emotionally   significant   to   anyone,   eliciting   a   relatively   strong   response   in   guilty   and   innocent   interviewees  alike.    This  in  turn  elevates  the  risk  of  a  false  positive  outcome  (an  innocent   individual   obtaining   a   guilty   test   score,   e.g.   Fiedler   et   al.,   2002;   Ben-­‐Shakhar   &   Elaad,   2003).   While   the   validity   of   the   CQT   is   heavily   contested,   there   are   at   least   three   paradigms   that   show   promise:   the   Concealed   Information   Test   (CIT,   Lykken,   1959;   Lykken,   1974)1,   the   Sheffield   Lie   Test   (SLT),   and   the   autobiographical   Implicit   Association  Test  (aIAT).  

The  CIT  aims  to  detect  guilty  knowledge,  rather  than  guilt  itself  (Ben-­‐Shakhar  et   al.,  2002;  Verschuere  et  al.,  2011).    The  test  consists  of  questions  that  focus  on  a  detail  of   an  event,  which  can  only  be  known  to  individuals  with  detailed  knowledge  of  said  event   (e.g.  Your  getaway  car  was  a  …).  After  each  question,  the  examinee  is  presented  with  a   succession  of  several  possible  alternatives  (e.g.  Nissan,  Honda,  Toyota,  Ford,  Chrysler).   The   examinee   is   typically   instructed   to   say   “no”   to   all   alternatives.   To   an   innocent   examinee   the   correct   alternative   (the   probe,   e.g.   Ford)   is   indistinguishable   from   the   irrelevant   alternatives   (e.g.   Nissan,   Honda,   Toyota,   Chrysler),   whereas   to   an   examinee   with   guilty   knowledge   this   alternative   stands   out   from   the   others.   In   the   polygraph   version  of  the  CIT  the  physiological  response  to  the  correct  alternative  is  then  compared   with   the   physiological   responses   to   the   irrelevant   alternatives.   Typically,   individuals   with   guilty   knowledge   show   a   larger   skin   conductance   response   (SCR)   to   the   critical   information   as   compared   to   the   irrelevant   information   (e.g.   Gronau,   Ben-­‐Shakhar,   &   Cohen,   2005;   Verschuere   &   Crombez,   2008;   Verschuere,   Crombez,   Smolders,   &   Clercq,   2009).  Other  versions  of  the  CIT  have  also  proven  their  use  in  detecting  knowledge  of   critical  information.  For  example,  the  Event  Related  Potential  (ERP)  based  CIT  (Farwell   &   Donchin   1991)   detects   guilty   knowledge   by   comparing   the   ERPs   to   critical   information   with   the   ERPs   to   irrelevant   information.   Evidence   for   reaction   time   differences   between   critical   and   irrelevant   information   was   found   in   ERP   studies   (e.g.  

Rosenfeld,   Biroschak,   &   Furedy,   2006).   Additionally,   reaction   times   were   found   to   be   highly   valid   as   a   separate   measure   of   guilty   knowledge   (Seymour,   Seifert,   Shafto,   &   Mosmann,  2000;  Seymour  &  Kerlin,  2008;  Verschuere,  Crombez,  Degrootte,  &  Rosseel,   2010).   Using   reaction   times   as   a   measure   of   guilty   knowledge   is   appealing   in   part   because   its   application   is   less   complex   and   less   costly   than   any   type   of   physiological   measure   (Seymour   et   al.,   2000;   Verschuere,   Crombez,   Degrootte,   &   Rosseel,   2010).   However,  reliable  measures  of  both  the  ERP-­‐based  CIT  and  the  RT-­‐based  CIT  require  an   increased   amount   of   trials   as   compared   to   the   polygraph-­‐based   CIT   (Noordraven   &   Verschuere,  2013).  Furthermore,  the  stimuli  are  rapidly  presented  in  succession,  which                                                                                                                  

(6)

may  lead  to  items  not  being  processed  by  the  participant.  For  this  reason,  inclusion  of  a   third   item   type   (target   items)   is   desirable   (Seymour   et   al.,   2000;   Noordraven   &   Verschuere,  2013).  Before  testing,  the  examinee  is  presented  with  the  target  items  (e.g.   Mazda)  and  is  instructed  to  learn  these  by  heart.  During  the  test,  the  examinee  indicates   as   quickly   as   possible   whether   the   presented   stimulus   is   a   target,   or   not.   Thus,   participants  will  still  answer  “no”  to  both  probe  items  and  irrelevant  items  and  “yes”  to   target   items.   Although   the   CIT   comes   in   many   forms   (e.g.   mock   crime   CIT,   autobiographical  CIT)  a  consistent  finding  is  that  probe  items  elicit  longer  reaction  times   and   higher   error   rates   than   irrelevant   items   for   participants   who   possess   guilty   knowledge   (Seymour   et   al.,   2000;   Verschuere   et   al.,   2010;   Visu-­‐Petra   et   al.,   2012;   Noordraven  &  Verschuere,  2013;  G.  Visu-­‐Petra,  Miclea,  Bus,  &  L.  Visu-­‐Petra,  2014).    

Another  frequently  used  reaction  time  based  deception  test  is  the  SLT  (Spence  et   al.,  2001).2  The  visual  SLT  consists  of  36  questions  starting  with  the  stem  “In  the  course   of  today  have  you  done  any  of  the  following?”  after  which  certain  common  daily  actions   appear  on  the  screen  in  succession  (e.g.  Made  your  bed).  Participants  are  instructed  to   answer  with  either  “yes”  or  “no”  by  pressing  the  corresponding  key  on  a  keyboard.  The   words  yes  and  no  appear  at  the  bottom  of  the  screen  below  each  question  as  a  reminder   of  which  buttons  to  use  for  which  response.  They  are  presented  either  in  red  or  green   indicating  whether  the  participant  is  to  lie  or  tell  the  truth  about  the  question.  After  an   initial   template   round   (in   which   participants   respond   truthfully   to   all   items)   each   question  is  presented  twice;  once  requiring  a  lie  response  and  once  requiring  a  truthful   response.   Reaction   times   and   accuracy   scores   are   recorded   (Spence   et   al.,   2001).  The   comparison  of  interest  is  between  the  Lie  and  Truth  trials  (Lie-­‐Truth  difference,  the  lie   effect).  Typically,  participants  show  an  increased  SCR  (Furedy  et  al.,  1988)  and  longer   reaction  times    (Spence  et  al.,  2001)  to  Lie  trials  as  compared  to  Truth  trials.    

Most   recently,   the   aIAT   (Sartori   et   al.,   2008)   was   introduced.   The   standard   IAT   measures   the   strength   of   associations   through   reaction   times   on   classification   tasks   (Greenwald,   Nosek,   &   Banaji,   2003).   The   aIAT   can   be   used   to   assess   the   truth   about   autobiographical   information   (Sartori   et   al.,   2008;   Verschuere,   Prati,   &   De   Houwer,   2009).  The  test  consists  of  four  categories:  True  sentences  (with  known  ground  truth),   False   sentences   (sentences   of   clear   falsities),   Guilty   sentences   (sentences   about   real   autobiographical   events),   and   Innocent   sentences   (sentences   about   fictitious   autobiographical  events).  The  categories  are  combined  in  such  a  way  that  there  is  a  clear   distinction  between  the  response  patterns  for  true  and  false  autobiographical  sentences.   In  the  congruent  block  guilty  sentences  are  combined  with  true  sentences  and  innocent   sentences  are  combined  with  false  sentences.  In  the  incongruent  block  guilty  sentences   are   combined   with   false   sentences   and   innocent   sentences   are   combined   with   true   sentences.   Generally,   participants   are   slower   to   respond   in   the   incongruent   block   (as   compared  to  the  congruent  block),  because  this  block  is  incompatible  with  their  implicit   (congruent)  association  (Sartori  et  al.,  2008;  Frost  et  al.,  2010).  The  accuracy  of  the  aIAT   was   assessed   in   a   meta-­‐analysis   conducted   by   Agosta   &   Sartori   (2013).   They   found   a   high   overall   accuracy.   Repetition   of   the   aIAT   did   not   seem   to   decrease   the   overall   accuracy  of  the  test.  However,  the  overall  effect  size  of  the  aIAT  was  moderate.    

The   deception   tests   mentioned   above   consistently   and   repeatedly   showed   that   lying   took   longer   than   telling   the   truth.   These   findings   point   out   the   effectiveness   of   reaction  time  based  deception  tests  (e.g.  Verschuere  et  al.,  2010).  Although  it  has  been                                                                                                                  

2  The  predecessor  of  the  SLT  (the  differentiation  of  deception  paradigm,  DDP)  was  proposed  by  Furedy  et   al.  (1988).    

(7)

argued  that  reaction  time  measures  are  easily  deliberately  altered  by  the  examinee  (e.g.   Farwell  &  Donchin,  1991)  research  has  suggested  that  fast  responses  (of  800  ms  or  less)   are  very  difficult  if  not  impossible  to  intentionally  manipulate  (e.g.  Seymour  et  al.,  2000).    

 

Altering  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying  

Earlier  research  has  provided  evidence  indicating  that  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying  can  be   deliberately   altered   so   as   to   either   enhance   or   impair   lie   detection.   By   enhancing   the   cognitive   cost   of   lying,   the   behavioral   differences   between   lying   and   telling   the   truth   within  an  individual  will  be  magnified,  making  it  easier  to  distinguish  between  the  two   (Vrij  et  al.,  2008)  So  far,  many  attempts  have  been  made  at  altering  the  cognitive  cost  of   lying.  In  this  section  we  will  describe  some  of  the  methods  that  were  used.    

In  order  to  increase  the  difficulty  of  lying  Vrij  et  al.  (2008)  instructed  participants   to   recall   a   story   in   reversed   order.   The   participants   were   divided   into   two   groups:   innocent   or   guilty.   The   “innocent”   participants   participated   in   a   staged   event   during   which   they   encountered   a   person   who   came   looking   for   their   wallet.   After   finding   the   wallet  in  the  room  the  person  in  question  claimed  that  a  £10  note  was  missing  from  the   wallet.   The   participant   was   informed   that   he/she   would   be   questioned   about   the   missing  money.  The  “guilty”  participants  committed  a  mock  crime  (stealing  £10  from  the   aforementioned   wallet).   During   questioning   these   participants   had   to   deny   the   mock   crime  by  using  the  innocent  scenario  as  their  alibi.  To  increase  the  cognitive  load  of  this   task  all  participants  were  instructed  to  recall  the  story  in  reversed  order.  Since  the  guilty   participants  did  not  really  carry  out  the  innocent  scenario,  telling  the  story  in  reversed   order   should   be   particularly   cognitively   constraining   for   this   group.   Deceitful   parts   of   the  stories  were  much  easier  to  detect  when  the  story  was  told  backwards  instead  of  in   chronological  order.  This  effect  was  expressed  by  several  deception  cues  apparent  in  the   guilty  participants  (i.e.  more  speech  hesitations,  slower  speech  rate,  more  fidgeting  with   hands  and  feet,  more  speech  errors,  increased  eye  blinking)  as  compared  to  the  innocent   participants.   These   deception   cues   were   less   apparent   when   the   stories   were   told   in   chronological  order.  The  researchers  concluded  that  recalling  an  alibi  in  reversed  order   would   be   particularly   difficult   for   liars   since   they   already   experience   an   increased   cognitive   load   due   to   the   construction   of   the   lie   (alibi)   itself.   These   results   show   that   increasing  cognitive  load  will  indeed  impair  the  ability  to  lie.  Vrij  et  al.  (2012)  replicated   these  results  using  similar  methods.    Similar  results  were  found  using  different  methods.   For  instance,  it  has  been  shown  that  asking  unanticipated  questions  can  also  amplify  the   differences   between   liars   (who   rehearsed   their   story)   and   truth   tellers   (who   give   an   honest  account  of  the  events  when  asked  about  them)  (Vrij,  Mann,  Leal,  &  Fisher,  2009).   Furthermore,   the   instruction   of   examinees   to   keep   eye   contact   with   their   interrogator   can  add  significantly  to  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying  (Vrij,  Leal,  Mann,  &  Fisher,  2010).        

The   research   described   above   indicates   that   the   cognitive   cost   of   lying   can   be   enhanced.  This  is  an  important  notion  when  fabricating  deception  tests  since  deception   tests  aim  to  pinpoint  differences  between  liars  and  truth  tellers.  However,  it  is  possible   that  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying  can  also  be  diminished.  It  has  been  noted  that  lying  is  not   always  more  difficult  than  telling  the  truth  (McCornack,  1997,  cited  in  Vrij  et  al.,  2008).   In   the   event   that   someone   fakes   a   deception   test   (i.e.   appearing   innocent   when   being   guilty),   the   difference   between   truth   tellers   and   liars   will   diminish   making   it   more   difficult   to   catch   liars.   Deception   paradigms   should   be   able   to   indicate,   if   not   prevent   such  occurrences.  As  mentioned  briefly  in  the  introduction,  replicating  Verschuere  et  al.   (2011),   Van   Bockstaele   et   al.   (2012)   manipulated   the   truth   and   lie   proportions   in   a   Sheffield   lie   test   in   order   to   examine   the   general   malleability   of   the   cognitive   cost   of  

(8)

lying.  When  participants  were  in  the  “frequent  lie  condition”  (75%  lie  trials,  25%  truth   trials),   lying   became   easier   (i.e.   the   difference   in   reaction   times   and   accuracy   scores   between   truth   and   lie   trials   was   smaller),   whereas   when   they   were   in   the   “frequent   truth   condition”   (75%   truth   trials,   25%   lie   trials)   lying   became   more   difficult.   These   results   indicated   that   the   cognitive   cost   of   lying   is   malleable   through   training   of   deception  skills,  albeit  only  for  practiced  items  (Van  Bockstaele  et  al.,  2012).  Although   this   training   effect   did   not   transfer   to   unpracticed   items,   learning   effects   have   been   shown   to   transfer   between   working-­‐memory   tasks,   provided   that   these   tasks   are   mediated  by  the  same  neural  system  (Dahlin,  Neely,  Larsson,  Bäckman,  &  Nyberg,  2008).     According   to   Osman,   Channon,   and   Fitzpatrick   (2009)   deception   tests   require   similar   cognitive  processes  as  the  standard  Stroop  task,  since  both  tests  require  the  participant   to  overcome  interference  of  incongruent  items.  To  test  this  hypothesis,  they  compared   the  Stroop  scores  between  participants  in  a  lie  condition  (100%  lie  trials)  to  the  Stroop   scores   of   participants   in   a   truth   condition   (100%   truth   trials).   Participants   in   the   lie   condition   showed   enhanced   performance   on   the   Stroop   task.   Hence,   the   researchers   concluded  that  deception  experience  could  indeed  transfer  to  performance  on  cognitive   tasks   with   a   similar   neural   basis.   Whether   this   transfer   effect   was   temporary   or   permanent  remains  to  be  seen.  

The  research  described  in  this  section  shows  clear  evidence  for  the  malleability  of   the   cognitive   cost   of   lying.   However,   it   is   not   clear   whether   this   cost   can   be   altered   permanently   through   training.   Furthermore,   individual   differences   in   deception   skills   were  not  taken  into  account  as  of  yet.    

 

Individual  differences  

Not  much  is  known  about  the  individual  differences  in  the  ability  to  successfully  deceive   others.   It   is   possible   that   not   everyone   is   affected   by   the   cognitive   cost   of   lying   to   the   same   extent.   For   instance,   certain   individuals   may   already   be   trained   at   deception   through   their   work   (e.g.   poker   players,   actors)   or   because   they   experience   a   constant   urge   to   lie   in   daily   life   (e.g.   pathological   liars,   individuals   cheating   on   their   spouses).   Moreover,   it   appears   as   though   lying   itself   is   only   more   cognitively   constraining   than   telling   the   truth   under   specific   circumstances.   For   instance,   according   to   Vrij   et   al.   (2008)   the   truth   needs   to   be   easily   accessed   from   memory,   in   order   to   elicit   an   automatic  response  that  is  difficult  to  inhibit.  Furthermore  the  researchers  noted  that  if   the   individual   is   motivated   to   be   believed,   credibility   will   be   taken   less   for   granted,   resulting   in   the   deceiver   trying   harder   to   monitor   one’s   own   behavior   and   that   of   the   conversational  partner.  This  process  will  add  to  the  cognitive  cost  of  lying.  Individuals   with  deception  experience  may  respond  to  these  cognitively  constraining  processes  in   different   ways   than   people   without   such   experience.   Moreover,   some   individuals   may   just  be  “good  liars”.  According  to  Vrij,  Granhag,  and  Porter  (2010)  the  best  liars  possess   several   important   features   that   may   cause   them   to   be   able   to   lie   with   more   ease   than   others.  For  instance,  they  argued  that  some  individuals  might  have  a  more  trustworthy   appearance  than  others,  making  it  easier  for  them  to  get  away  with  deception.  This  in   turn  might  lead  them  to  experience  less  emotions  during  the  act  of  deception.      

  The  best  liars  are  good  actors  who  can  behave  in  a  seemingly  trustworthy  way   (Vrij  et  al.,  2010).  Actors  may  have  an  enhanced  sense  of  self-­‐other  awareness  (Thomson   &   Jaque,   2012),   which   may   allow   them   to   monitor   their   own   behavior   as   well   as   the   behavior   of   others   with   more   ease.   When   professional   actors   take   on   a   role,   they   extensively  elaborate  on  the  script  through  imaginative  embellishment  (Noice  &  Noice,   2006),   allowing   them   to   “relive”   an   event   that   has   not   really   happened   to   them.  

(9)

Additionally,  they  adopt  the  perspective  of  the  character  they  are  portraying,  in  part  by   relating  the  character’s  thoughts  and  feelings  to  their  own  mental  state  (Noice  &  Noice,   2006).  Since,  the  actors  fully  adopt  the  role  they  have  to  play,  it  may  become  a  second   “truth”,   making   it   easy   for   them   to   act   natural   in   regard   to   the   role.   This   in   turn   may   result   in   actors   successfully   faking   an   impulsive   response   when   confronted   with   information   from   their   previously   prepared   role,   so   as   to   appear   convincing.   It   seems   that   in   order   to   appear   honest   a   liar   must   have   certain   nonchalance   toward   lying.   By   integrating  the  role  with  their  own  identity,  actors  may  take  their  credibility  for  granted   just  as  any  innocent  person  would,  making  them  appear  truthful  when  they  are  in  fact   guilty  of  the  act  of  deception.    

To  assess  the  relationship  between  private  self-­‐awareness  and  deception  ability   A.K.  Johnson  et  al.  (2005)  instructed  non-­‐professional  actors  to  portray  varying  levels  of   deception   on   film.   A   questionnaire   about   private   self-­‐awareness   was   administered   beforehand.   There   were   three   conditions   for   the   videos:   non-­‐deception,   faking   good   (exaggerating  positive  biographical  information),  and  faking  bad  (exaggerating  negative   biographical   information).   Undergraduate   students   were   presented   with   the   video   footage   after   which   they   rated   the   believability   of   the   actors.   They   were   instructed   to   both  indicate  whether  the  actors  were  lying  or  telling  the  truth  and  how  confident  the   actors   seemed   to   be.     Actors   with   higher   overall   private   self-­‐awareness   were   more   successful   in   deceiving   the   undergraduates.   It   has   been   proposed   that   enhanced   self-­‐ awareness  may  improve  one’s  Theory  of  Mind  (e.g.  Gallup,  1982,  cited  in  A.K.  Johnson  et   al.,  2005)  possibly  resulting  in  greater  deception  skills  (A.K.  Johnson  et  al.,  2005).  It  is   possible   that   actors   in   general   have   enhanced   self-­‐awareness   and   thus   are   better   at   deceiving  others.  After  all,  they  are  specifically  trained  at  pretending  to  be  someone  else.   According  to  Thomson  and  Jaque  (2012)  the  ability  of  actors  to  create  characters  would   be   beneficial   for   their   psychological   self-­‐other   awareness.   They   found   that   although   actors  seemed  more  psychologically  vulnerable,  they  were  indeed  more  psychologically   self-­‐other  aware  than  non-­‐actors.    

 

The  current  experiment  

The  research  question  we  aimed  to  answer  with  the  current  experiment  was  whether   the   cognitive   cost   of   lying   is   shaped   by   an   individual’s   experience   with   deception.   For   this   purpose   we   chose   to   compare   the   deception   skills   of   a   group   with   deception   experience   (actors)   to   a   group   without   such   experience   (non-­‐actors).   We   specifically   expected  actors  to  be  better  at  concealing  their  own  identity  compared  to  non-­‐actors  in   part   because   of   their   experience   with   pretending   to   be   someone   else.   Deception   skills   were   measured   using   three   reaction   time   based   autobiographical   deception   tests:   the   autobiographical  CIT  (aCIT),  the  aIAT,  and  the  autobiographical  SLT  (aSLT).    

 

Methods    

Participants  

Eleven  actors  (3  males),  and  22  non-­‐actors  (4  males)  participated  in  this  study  (23  of   them   received   €7,50   for   their   participation,   the   rest   of   the   participants   cooperated   voluntarily).  Participants  were  classified  as  “actors”  if  they  had  at  least  2  years  of  acting   experience.   The   participants   were   recruited   through   flyers,   posters,   and   through   the   Internet.    

   

(10)

Apparatus  

All  stimuli  were  presented  on  a  PC  using  Inquisit  4  software  (2013).      

Procedure    

Profile  

Up   to   one   week   prior   to   testing,   participants   received   a   profile   containing   fictitious   autobiographical  information  (e.g.  Your  name  is  Sam;  You  were  bitten  in  your  leg  by  a   Dalmatian  when  you  were  little,  see  Appendix  A).  They  were  instructed  to  memorize  this   profile  so  that  they  could  adopt  the  fictitious  identity  on  the  day  of  testing.  Participants   were   not   informed   about   the   questioning   format   (used   on   the   day   of   testing)   beforehand.    

 

Memory  check  

On  the  day  of  testing,  participants’  knowledge  of  the  fictitious  identity  was  first  tested  by   means   of   a   questionnaire   (see   Appendix   B).   If   participants   made   mistakes   on   the   questionnaire,   they   were   instructed   to   read   through   the   profile   thoroughly   within   the   next  five  minutes.  If  participants  still  made  mistakes  after  this,  the  experimenter  went   over  the  answers  with  the  participant,  after  which  testing  began.    

 

Deception  tests    

Autobiographical  Concealed  Information  Test  (aCIT)    

The  aCIT  (e.g.  Rosenfeld,  Biroschak,  &  Furedy,  2006;  Verschuere  et  al.,  2010)  consisted   of  1  practice  block  (12  trials)  and  2  test  blocks  (108  items  per  block).  Each  test  block   contained   9   target   items     (about   the   fictitious   identity),   9   probe   items   (about   the   real   identity  of  participants),  and  36  irrelevant  items  (see  Appendix  C),  presented  in  random   order.  Hence,  the  total  amount  of  test  trials  was  216.  Inter  stimulus  intervals  were  500,   800,  or  1000  milliseconds  in  randomized  order,  to  prevent  participants  from  preparing   their  responses  and  showing  rhythmic  response  patterns  (Seymour  et  al.,  2000).  On  top   of   the   screen   the   sentence   “Do   you   recognize   this   stimulus?”   appeared.   Participants   were  instructed  to  hide  their  true  identity  during  this  test  by  answering  with  “yes”  to  all   target  items  and  with  “no”  to  all  other  items  (probe  items  and  irrelevant  items).  Answers   were  given  by  pressing  the  corresponding  key  (“z”  for  yes  and  “m”  for  no,  or  vice  versa)   on  a  keyboard.  The  responses  were  to  be  made  as  quickly  yet  as  accurately  as  possible.      

Autobiographical  Implicit  Association  Test  (aIAT)  

The  aIAT  consisted  of  sentences  about  the  true  identity  of  the  participants,  the  fictitious   identity  from  the  profile,  or  sentences  with  known  ground  truth  that  were  either  true  or   false  (see  Appendix  D).  There  were  four  corresponding  categories  (Self,  Other,  True,  and   False,   respectively).   Sentences   were   presented   in   the   middle   of   a   computer   screen.   Participants   were   instructed   to   place   each   sentence   in   the   corresponding   category   presented  in  the  top  corners  of  the  computer  screen.  Answers  were  given  by  pressing   the   corresponding   key   on   a   keyboard   (“e”   for   categories   presented   on   the   left   of   the   screen,   “i”   for   categories   presented   on   the   right   of   the   screen).   The   test   consisted   of   three   blocks:   attribute   practice,   congruent,   and   incongruent.   In   the   attribute   practice   block  (20  trials)  participants  were  familiarized  with  the  buttons.  In  the  congruent  block   (20  practice  trials,  40  test  trials)  the  category  True  was  combined  with  the  category  Self   (both   requiring   button   press   “e”)   and   the   category   False   was   combined   with   the  

(11)

category  Other  (both  requiring  button  press  “i”).  In  the  incongruent  block  (20  practice   trials,   40   test   trials)   the   category   True   was   combined   with   the   category   Other   (both   requiring  button  press  “e”)  and  the  category  False  was  combined  with  the  category  Self   (both  requiring  button  press  “i”).  Hence,  the  congruent  block  required  a  truth  response   (i.e.  real  autobiographical  sentences  are  true;  fictitious  autobiographical  sentences  are   false)   whereas   the   incongruent   block   required   a   lie   response   (i.e.   fictitious   autobiographical   sentences   are   true;   real   autobiographical   sentences   are   false).   The   inter  stimulus  interval  was  250  milliseconds.  There  was  no  response  deadline.  All  trials   required  a  correct  response.  If  participants  made  a  mistake,  a  red  cross  would  appear  at   the  bottom  of  the  screen,  indicating  that  the  participant  had  to  give  the  correct  response   in  order  for  the  test  to  continue.    

     

Autobiographical  Sheffield  Lie  Test  (aSLT)  

This  test  consisted  of  two  item  types:  Other  Identity  (fictitious  autobiographical  items   referring   to   the   profile;   see   Appendix   A)   and   Own   Identity   (autobiographical   items   referring   to   the   participant).   There   were   two   practice   blocks   containing   36   unrelated   control   items   with   known   ground   truth   in   randomized   order   borrowed   from   Verschuere,  Prati,  &  De  Houwer  (2009)  (see  Appendix  E).  The  purpose  of  the  practice   blocks   was   to   familiarize   participants   with   the   buttons.   Participants   were   presented   with  either  an  “L”  or  a  “T”  indicating  whether  they  had  to  lie  or  tell  the  truth  about  the   upcoming   sentence.   The   sentence   was   then   presented   in   the   middle   of   the   computer   screen.  The  letter  (“L”  or  “T”)  remained  on  the  computer  screen  until  the  next  trial  to   ensure  that  participants  would  not  forget  whether  they  had  to  lie  or  tell  the  truth  about   the   presented   sentence.   Answers   were   given   by   pressing   the   corresponding   key   on   a   keyboard  (“4”  for  yes  and  “6”  for  no  or  vice  versa).  During  the  first  practice  block  (12   trials)  the  order  of  truth  and  lie  trials  was  fixed.  In  the  second  practice  block  (16  trials)   this  order  was  scrambled  in  order  to  ensure  that  participants  could  no  longer  anticipate   whether  they  should  tell  the  truth  or  lie  to  the  stimulus.  The  test  block  consisted  of  72   truth  trials  and  72  lie  trials  in  randomized  order.  During  truth  trials  participants  had  to   answer   truthfully   (i.e.   say   yes   to   all   Own   Identity   items;   say   no   to   all   Other   Identity   items).   During   lie   trials   participants   had   to   deny   their   own   identity   (i.e.   say   yes   to   all   Other   Identity   items;   say   no   to   all   Own   Identity   items).   The   time   between   the   presentation  of  the  cue  and  the  presentation  of  the  stimulus  ranged  between  500  and   1500   milliseconds   in   order   to   prevent   rhythmic   responding   (Seymour   et   al.,   2000).   There  was  no  response  deadline;  hence  a  response  was  required  in  order  for  the  next   stimulus  to  appear.    

 

Results  

 

Participants  

All  reported  results  for  the  aCIT  are  with  n  =  32  (7  males),  for  the  aIAT  with  n  =  33,  and   for  the  aSLT  with  n  =  31  (7  males).  The  ages  of  the  participants  ranged  between  19  and   62   years   old.   All   except   two   participants   (1   actor,   1   non-­‐actor)   spoke   Dutch.3  Acting   experience  for  the  non-­‐actors  ranged  between  0  hours  and  250  hours,  whereas  acting   experience  among  the  actors  ranged  between  500  hours  and  10000  hours  (see  Table  1).    

 

                                                                                                               

(12)

Table  1      

Means  and  Standard  Deviations  Per  Variable  for  Actors  and  Non-­‐actors  with  the  p-­‐values   for  the  Differences  Between  Actors  and  Non-­‐actors.    

      Actors       non-­‐actors          p-­‐value   total         n  =  11       n  =  22          (t-­‐tests)   n  =  33   Age       38.36  (16.08)     28.91(11.01)            .06                         32.06(13.44)   Gender*  (%  male)   27.3%       18.2%                        .56         21.2%   Acting  experience     3559.09(4193.26)   28.77(55.71)        <.01**                        1205.55(2890.15)   (hours)   Fake  Identity  ***   48.86(89.32)     4.53(6.50)            .04                       16.46(48.45)   (frequency)   Preparation  time   3.09(2.77)     2.41(5.58)            .71                              2.64(4.79)     (days)  

*  For  gender  Spearman’s  Chi  squared  (X2)  was  used.    

**  Cohen’s  f  =  .52.4    

***  This  measure  indicates  the  participants’  estimates  of  the  total  amount  of  times  they  pretended  to  be   someone   else.   Due   to   incomplete   questionnaire   data,   7   participants   were   missing   for   the   Fake   Identity   measure.    

 

aCIT  

All   practice   trials   were   excluded   from   the   analyses.   The   response   latencies,   and   error   rates  were  analyzed  separately  using  a  2  by  2  mixed  analysis  of  variance  (ANOVA)  with   Acting   (Actor   versus   Non-­‐actor)   as   a   between-­‐subjects   and   Item   type   (Probe   versus   Irrelevant)  as  a  within-­‐subjects  measure.  

 

Preliminary  analyses  

A   correlational   analysis   was   conducted   in   order   to   examine   whether   Age   was   a   confounding  factor.  If  Age  did  not  correlate  with  both  Acting  (Actor  vs.  Non-­‐actor)  and   the  CIT  effect  (RT  Probe  –  RT  Irrelevant,  RT-­‐PI),  it  would  not  be  taken  into  account  in  the   main  analyses.  There  was  a  significant  correlation  between  Age  and  the  CIT  effect  (r  =   .35,  p  <  .05.  The  correlation  between  Age  and  Acting  was  marginally  significant,  r  =  .34,  p   =   .06.   The   correlation   between   Age   and   the   amount   of   Acting   experience   in   hours   however,  was  significant,  r  =  .62,  p  <  .01.  Despite  the  fact  that  the  correlation  between   Age  and  Acting  was  only  marginally  significant,  age  was  included  as  a  covariate  in  the   main  analyses  due  to  the  significant  correlation  with  the  amount  of  acting  experience  in   hours.    

  The  reliability  of  the  aCIT  was  assessed  by  means  of  the  odd-­‐even  method  used  in   Noordraven  and  Verschuere  (2013).  The  split-­‐half  correlation  of  RT-­‐PI  was  r  =  .75.    

   

                                                                                                               

4  According  to  Cohen  (1992)  f-­‐values  from  .10  represent  small  effects,  values  from  .25  represent  medium   effects,  and  values  from  .40  represent  large  effects.    

(13)

Main  analyses    

Reaction  Times  

Due  to  a  preset  response  deadline  of  800  ms  there  was  no  outlier  analysis  conducted.  All   time-­‐outs  were  excluded  from  the  analyses.  Errors  were  only  discarded  for  the  response   latency  analyses.    

There   was   a   main   effect   of   Item   Type,   F(2,   58)   =   27.68,   p   <   .01,   f   =   .96.   Participants   responded   slower   to   probe   items   (M   =   600,   SD   =   45),   than   to   irrelevant   items  (M  =  535,  SD  =  43).  However,  the  interaction  between  Item  Type  and  Acting  was   not  significant,  F(2,  58)  =  1.09,  p  =  .34,  f  =  .04.  The  difference  in  reaction  times  between   actors  and  non-­‐actors  was  also  non-­‐significant,  F(1,  30)  =  .55,  p  =  .46,  f  =  .02  (see  Figure   1).  

 

   

Figure  1.  Mean  Reaction  Times  and  Standard  Errors  Per  Item  Type  of  the  aCIT  for  Actors   and  Non-­‐actors.    

 

Error  rates  

There  was  a  main  effect  of  Item  Type,  F(2,  58)  =  9.41,  p  <  .01,  f  =  .33.  Participants  had  a   higher  error  rate  for  probe  items  (M  =  3.13,  SD  =  3.87)  as  compared  to  irrelevant  items   (M  =  .81  SD  =  1.06).  The  interaction  between  Item  Type  and  Acting  was  non-­‐significant,   F(2,   58)   =   .70,   p   =   .50,   f  =   .02.   The   differences   in   error   rates   between   actors   and   non-­‐ actors  were  also  non-­‐significant,  F(1,  29)  =  .21,  p  =  .65,  f  =  .01  (see  Figure  2).  5      

 

                                                                                                               

5  In   line   with   the   suggestion   made   by   Simmons,   Nelson,   and   Simonsohn   (2011)   we   checked   whether   excluding   Age   as   a   covariate   would   yield   similar   results.  When   Age   was   not   included   as   a   covariate   for   both  the  reaction  time  analyses  and  the  error  rate  analyses  the  main  effects  of  item  were  still  significant.   The  interactions  between  Item  and  Actor  were  still  non-­‐significant.  The  differences  in  reaction  times  and   error  rates  between  actors  and  non-­‐actors  were  also  still  non-­‐significant.    

  400   450   500   550   600   650   Probe   Irrelevant   R ea ct io n  T im es     (m s)   Item  Type   Actors   Non-­‐actors  

(14)

   

Figure  2.  Mean  Error  Rates  and  Standard  Errors  Per  Item  Type  of  the  aCIT  for  Actors  and   Non-­‐actors.    

 

Supplementary  analyses  

Because   of   the   small   sample   size,   we   also   conducted   dimensional   analyses,   neglecting   the  factor  actor  versus  non-­‐actor,  and  relating  acting  experience  with  the  RT-­‐PI  across   groups.   A   partial   Pearson   correlation   analysis   was   conducted   controlling   for   age.   The   correlation  between  the  RT-­‐PI  and  acting  experience  was  non-­‐significant,  r  =  .03,  p  =  .90.   These  results  indicate  that  actors  did  not  hide  their  own  identity  better  than  the  non-­‐ actors  did.    

  In  order  to  examine  whether  Preparation  time  (during  which  participants  could   rehearse   their   fictitious   identity)   had   a   beneficial   effect   on   deception   skills   a   dimensional   analysis   was   conducted   relating   preparation   time   (in   days)   with   RT-­‐PI.   A   partial  Pearson  correlation  was  conducted  controlling  for  Age.  The  correlation  between   the   RT-­‐PI   and   preparation   time   was   non-­‐significant,   r   =   -­‐.18,   p   =   .34,   indicating   that   participants’  deception  skills  did  not  benefit  from  rehearsing  their  fictitious  identity.    

 

Conclusion  

In  line  with  what  is  typically  found,  the  above  results  indicate  that  lying  is  more  difficult   than  telling  the  truth.  Against  our  expectations  however,  actors  did  not  show  superior   deceptive   skills   as   compared   to   non-­‐actors   for   this   test.   Furthermore,   the   amount   of   preparation  time  did  not  have  any  effect  on  participants’  performance  on  the  aCIT.      

aIAT  

The  attribute  practice  block  was  excluded  from  the  analysis.  Since  there  was  a  built-­‐in   penalty  for  error  trials  (a  correct  response  was  required  after  an  error)  adding  a  penalty   to  these  trials  was  no  longer  necessary.  This  built-­‐in  penalty  allowed  us  to  include  error   latencies   in   the   analyses   (Greenwald   et   al.,   2003).     The   aIAT   was   scored   using   the   improved  scoring  algorithm  (D1)  described  in  Greenwald  et  al.  (2003).  This  algorithm   consisted   of   the   following   adjustments.   The   practice   blocks   were   included   in   the   analyses.   Latencies   over   10,000   ms   were   discarded.6  Mean   reaction   times   (RTs)   were   calculated   per   block.   The   RT   differences   between   incongruent   and   congruent   practice   (block  6  –  block  3)  and  test  blocks  (block  7  –  block  4)  were  calculated.  Pooled  standard   deviations  (SDs)  were  calculated  for  the  practice  (SDs  of  block  6  and  block  3  combined)   and  test  blocks  (SDs  of  block  7  and  4  combined).  The  difference  scores  for  practice  and                                                                                                                  

6  There  were  no  latencies  below  400  ms,  hence  removal  of  fast  trials  was  not  necessary.    

0   1   2   3   4   5   6   Probe   Irrelevant   Er ro r   ra te s   (% )   Item  Type   Actors   Non-­‐actors  

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Publisher’s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers) Please check the document version of this publication:.. • A submitted manuscript is

The author criticizes the statistical properties of the conventional Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) regression technique in the presence of outliers and firm heterogeneity.

Procentueel lijkt het dan wel alsof de Volkskrant meer aandacht voor het privéleven van Beatrix heeft, maar de cijfers tonen duidelijk aan dat De Telegraaf veel meer foto’s van

The hopes and ideas that are connected to big data and the friction that comes along result to the core question of this thesis: how might predictive data mining

gcpraat v.'ord nic, maar oor die vredesnet waarmee Moskou die Amcrikaansc politick gevang het en waarin Washington nou hul- pcloos hcen en weer spartel. Die&gt;

With regard to regulatory focus theory and social comparison, when regulatory focus and comparison are aligned and thus either eagerness or vigilance is experienced, when

In what Way does Price and Quantity Framing affect the Probability of a Consumer to Engage in Ethical Consumption?.

This would be comparable to the finding in the current study that there was not a statistically signifi- cant difference for memory in the source monitoring task between the