• No results found

Nurturing nature : testing the three-hit hypothesis of schizophrenia Daskalakis, N.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Nurturing nature : testing the three-hit hypothesis of schizophrenia Daskalakis, N."

Copied!
21
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Daskalakis, N.

Citation

Daskalakis, N. (2011, December 8). Nurturing nature : testing the three-hit hypothesis of schizophrenia. Retrieved from https://hdl.handle.net/1887/18195

Version: Corrected Publisher’s Version

License: Licence agreement concerning inclusion of doctoral thesis in the Institutional Repository of the University of Leiden

Downloaded from: https://hdl.handle.net/1887/18195

Note: To cite this publication please use the final published version (if applicable).

(2)

Addendum

Environmental and tactile stimulation modulates the neonatal handling effect on adult rat spatial memory.

Nikolaos P. Daskalakis

1,2

, Maria Kaperoni

1

, Christos Koros

1

, E. Ronald de Kloet

2

, and Efthimia Kitraki

1,3

1

Laboratory of Histology & Embryology, University School of Medicine, University of Athens

2

Division of Medical Pharmacology, Leiden/ Amsterdam Center for Drug Research, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden University

3

Department of Basic Sciences, School of Dentistry, University of Athens

Int J Dev Neurosci. 2009 Dec;27(8):747-55.

(3)

Abstract

Handling of rat pups promotes their adult cognitive performance. However, new data suggest that individual components of the handling procedure, like exposure to novelty or tactile stimulation, have distinct lasting effects on behaviour. In this study we examined the interaction of early novelty exposure with a varying amount of tactile stimulation on spatial recognition memory and corticosterone secretion of adult male and female rats. A split litter design was used and the experimental animals were also compared to animal facility reared controls.

The experiment was conducted in two phases. In the first phase, we examined the effect of novel or home environment during the 15-min of neonatal handling, following 10 back-strokes. Tactile stimulation of 10 back-strokes combined with novelty exposure, enhanced novel arm discrimination in a Y-maze task in adult rats of both sexes compared to their siblings that stayed at

home, as well as to the animal facility reared controls. In the second phase, additional back-stroking (total of 20 back-strokes) reduced the Y-maze performance of males neonatally exposed to novelty, while the same treatment enhanced the performance of their siblings that stayed at home. Basal corticosterone levels, determined one week post Y-maze, were significantly increased only in the novelty exposed/10 back-stroked females compared to same sex non-handled controls. In contrast, 10 back- strokes combined with the home cage environment increased corticosterone in males. Increase to 20 back- strokes reversed the impact of neonatal environment on corticosterone levels.

These data suggest that the nature and intensity of the individual components of a mild early-life manipulation, like handling, are critical in modifying aspects of adult memory performance and basal adrenocortical function.

1. Introduction

There is accumulating evidence that adult rats exposed as pups to early-life experience during the perinatal period show altered behaviour and neuroendocrine function [1, 2]. For instance, an adverse experience, like prolonged maternal separation of 24 hours, impairs later life cognitive performance and enhances adrenocortical and emotional reactivity. On the contrary, mild manipulations, like neonatal handling, can lead, via developmental programming, to animals with a better coping with stressful situations later in life [1, 3-7]. Such handled animals, which are separated from their mother daily for 15 minutes over the first three weeks of life, exhibit as adults reduced anxiety. In response to a mild stressor they have a lower output of corticosterone, prolactin and adrenaline [7-10]. On the other hand, handled animals have increased levels of glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus and frontal cortex [11-13].

Accordingly, their limbic - hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (LHPA) axis feedback

sensitivity to corticosterone is enhanced, providing a more efficient termination of the

stress response. The aforementioned features of handled animals have been linked to

(4)

A

their generally improved performance in cognitive tasks [14, 15].

Neonatal handling consists of at least three different manipulations: brief maternal separation (up to 15min), touching (tactile stimulation) by the experimenter, and exposure to novelty. Firstly, the short term maternal separation eliminates olfactory cues as well as the tactile stimulation by the mother, for which the latter compensates at reunion [16, 17]. Secondly, touching by the experimenter during the separation can, to some extent, mimic the maternal tactile stimulation, but the various protocols are different in this respect. Finally, the additional exposure to novelty offers new olfactory and visual stimulation (from 2nd week on). Although the effect of neonatal handling on the adult animal’s physiology and behavior has been extensively studied since the mid

‘50s [6], only few studies have examined the impact of its separate components.

The established handling paradigm [7] was modified by introducing a split litter design, for studying the impact of exposure to novelty separately [18]. In this model, all pups are experiencing daily a short term separation from their mother as well as the experimenter’s touching. However, only half of the pups in each litter are exposed individually to novelty, while the rest are left in their home cage. The adult male rats, exposed to novelty exposure as part of this handling protocol, exhibited as adults enhanced spatial and non-spatial memory [18, 19] and showed increased hippocampal LTP [20]. More recently, by using the same paradigm [21], it was shown that brief exposure of pups to an unfamiliar environment produced in adult males an attenuated fear and stress response, as well as diminished sexual behavior. Females have been scarcely studied in this paradigm. Juvenile and adult female rats, that had experienced as pups prolonged absence of the mother while being placed in a novel environment, were less reactive to stressors as compared to female pups that stayed in their nest during maternal absence [22]. However, others did not observe this difference using the same protocol [23].

During prolonged (24h) maternal separation, artificial tactile stimulation of male pups, especially in the anogenital region, has been shown to reverse the central effects of separation in rats, while additional feeding restored corticosterone secretion [24-26].

Tactile stimulation can also enhance the maturation of cortical neurons [27], improve passive avoidance response [28] and spatial working memory [29]. In paradigms of one hour maternal separation, tactile stimulation of the dorsal area has been shown to reverse the effect of separation on anxiety-like behavior and pain sensitivity in adult rats of both sexes [30]. The lasting behavioural effects of tactile stimulation during the handling paradigm have not been studied.

The aim of this study was to examine the impact and the interaction of early novelty

exposure with a varying amount of tactile stimulation on spatial recognition memory

and basal corticosterone secretion of adult male and female animals (Fig. 1A for an

overview).Therefore the experiment was conducted in two phases. We first examined

the effect of novel or home environment, following 10 back strokes, on the above

parameters. The outcome was further investigated with an additional amount of tactile

(5)

stimulation (total of 20 back-strokes) preceding the daily novelty or home exposure.

In both phases, a split litter design was used. The experimental animals were also compared to non-handled, animal facility reared (FR) controls.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1 Subjects

18 female and 6 male Wistar rats (obtained at 6 weeks of age from Hellenic Pasteur Institute, Athens, Greece) were used for breeding. After a habituation period of two weeks, 3 females were put in a cage with one male rat Type IV (macrolon-polycarbonate cages with wire lid; 60 x 38 x 20 cm; containing sawdust bedding). Pregnancy was determined by vaginal smears obtained every morning and pregnant females were transferred in individual cages (home cage: macrolon- polycarbonate type III cages with wire lid; 42.5 x 26.6 x 18.5 cm) containing sawdust. The day of birth was defined as postnatal day 0 (= PND 0). We accepted litter with a maximal excess of one male or female and our litter size was 7-9 pups. The pups of experimental groups were exposed to neonatal manipulations from PND1 to PND21. In the control litters, the pups remained undisturbed with their dams in the housing room until the time of behavioral testing, receiving only the animal facility rearing (change of cage once per week; starting at PND10). In total, offspring of 13 litters was used in this study.

All animals were weaned at PND22, group housed with the same sex littermates and received the normal animal facility rearing afterwards. At PND90 they were tested in the Y-maze task and one week later they were sacrificed under basal conditions. Estrus phase was detected in sacrificed females by vaginal smears. Rats were housed under a 12:12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 07.00 h, illumination inside the cage: 20-30 lux, temperature: 22 ± 2 °C, relative humidity 60 ± 10%).

Food and tap water were provided ad libitum. All animal treatments were approved by the local ethical committee and have been carried out in agreement with ethical recommendation of the European Communities Council Directive of 24 November 1986 (86/609/EEC). All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering.

2.2 Early-life manipulations

Early-life manipulations consisted of 4 consecutive steps (see in Fig. 1B a detailed description):

i. Dams’ transfer out of the litter. Between 14:00 and 15:00h, dams were removed from their cage and placed in a same type cage, in the same room, at a distance from the home cage.

ii. Back-painting (tactile stimulation): In Experimental Phase I, back-painting, lasted 1min per pup with 10 passages of the marker (10 strokes, 10S). In Experimental Phase II, back- painting lasted 1min per pup with 20 passages of the marker (20 strokes, 20S).

iii. Novelty exposure (environmental stimulation) of 3min daily. Exposure to novelty was performed according to Tang [18]. Home and Novel pups within each experimental phase (see following section) were daily experiencing the brief maternal separation and the same amount of tactile stimulation. Novel pups were additionally exposed to a different context, compared to home pups, namely the novel environment in isolation from their littermates.

iv. Dams’ reunion.

All animals with the exception of AFR were back-painted. From day 2 to 21, the animals were

(6)

A

identified (on step 2) by the experimenter by the different colour in the back that was renewed daily. The two markers used had the same odour.

2.3 Experimental design

To study the impact of early novelty exposure on spatial recognition memory and basal corticosterone secretion of adult male and female animals we used a (treatment) x (sex) design and the treatment was comprised of three levels with a control group (animal facility rearing), a home group (back-stroked in the home cage), and a novel group (back-stroked and exposed to novelty).

To study, additionally, the interaction of early novelty exposure with a varying amount of tactile stimulation we conducted the experiment in two phases:

- In Phase I, 21 male and 21 female pups from 6 different litters received, on their back, 10 strokes (10S) during back-painting, with or without novelty exposure (‘10S-Novel’ and ‘10S-Home’ groups, respectively). Maternal separation lasted in total 15min.

- In Phase II, 16 male and 13 female pups from 4 different litters received on their back 20 strokes (20S) during back-painting, with or without novelty exposure (‘20S-Novel’ and ‘20S-Home’ groups, respectively). Maternal separation lasted in total 15min.

Early life manipulations Weaning Decapitation

in basal conditions Y- maze test

PND97 PND90

PND1-21 PND22

N N N N H H H H DAM

DAM

N N N N H H H H DAM N N

N N

H H H H ii. 1-min Back-painting

H H H H

TIME

i. Dam out iv. Dam back

N N N N

N N N N H H H H

iii. 3-min Novelty exposure

min0 1

min 14

min 15

10 min min

Figure 1. Experimental methods. (A) Time line of longitudinal study. (B) Sequential steps of early-life manipulations. (i) “Dam out”. The dams were transferred out of the litter. This step took approximately 1min (as indicated in the clock). (ii) Back-painting (tactile stimulation): Back-stroking, consisting of passages of a marker tip over the pups’ back, was used as daily tactile stimulation;

started 1min after the dam was removed and lasted 1min per animal. For all the pups of a litter, this step took approximately 9min (as indicated in the clock). (iii) Novelty exposure (environmental stimulation): Exposure to novelty was performed according to Tang [18] and started 10min after the dam was removed. A split-litter design was used so that half of the animals in each litter

(painted with a certain color during stroking) were put individually in “novel” cages (macrolon- polycarbonate 18 x 20 x 14 cm; containing fresh sawdust bedding), under heating lamps for 3min (Novel animals). The other half of the litter (painted with another color during stroking) remained for 3min in the home cage under heating lamps (Home animals). After 3min the Novel pups were transferred back to their home cage. This step took approximately 4min (as indicated in the clock).

During the transfer of Novel (back and forth), the home pups were also equally touched. (iv)

“Dams’ back” (dams’ reunion): Finally, the dams were returned in the home cage. This step took approximately 1min (as indicated in the clock).

A

B

(7)

In both phases of the experiment, the experimental animals were also compared to AFR controls.

Control litters were the same group of animals in both phases and consisted of 10 male and 13 female pups that derived from 3 different litters. For a synopsis of the different experimental groups according to the early-life manipulations see Table 1.

2.4 Behavioral Testing – Y-maze test

The animals’ ability for spatial recognition memory was assessed at PND 90 by using the Y-maze test [31, 32]. In brief, a black, wooden Y-maze apparatus, having three identical arms (50 x 16 x 32 cm) was used. Extra-maze only, visual cues (posters and laboratory glassware equipment) were placed around the maze and above the top of the black wooden sides. No visual cues were placed inside the maze, and the extra-maze cues were not equally visible from all points within the maze, thus enabling spatial orientation. The investigator was not visible by the rat. Rats were tested during the light phase of their cycle (between 9:00-13:00 h). The testing room was next to animals’ room. The floor of the maze was covered with sawdust bedding that was renewed for each animal using a mixture of sawdust that differed for males and females. The task consisted of two trials having a 4h intertrial interval. The same arm was always designated as a start arm, where the animal was introduced facing the wall of the arm. The other two arms were alternating as “Novel” and “Other” arm between rats to reduce arm-bias effects. In Trial 1, the novel arm was blocked with a black wooden guillotine. At the start of testing, a rat was placed in the start arm and the rat was allowed to explore the start and other arms for 15min. At the end of Trial 1, the rat was returned to its home cage and the bedding inside the maze was mixed, to reduce the possibility of using odors as a cue. In Trial 2, all arms were accessible and the rat was allowed to explore them for 5min. The animals were recorded with a video-camera adjusted on the ceiling of the room. The videos were analyzed blindly by two independent observers by using a computer software [33]. The dependent variables measured in the 1

st

and the whole 5min period of Trial 2 were: a) number of total entries b) % entries in each arm, c) % duration in each arm and in the center of the apparatus. Entry into an arm was counted when the head and two front paws were inside the arm, and the duration of an arm visit ended when the head and two front paws were outside of the arm again.

2.5 Corticosterone measurement

One week following Y-maze testing, the animals were decapitated under basal conditions between 13.00-15.00 h. Trunk blood was collected in heparinized tubes on ice and centrifuged at 14.000 rpm, for 5min, at 4 °C. Plasma was collected and kept at -80

o

C until use. Corticosterone levels were

ELM

Group name

CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

Experimental Phase I Experimental Phase II

Tactile Stimulation PND 2-21 Novelty exposure PND 2-21 Number of animals per group

Animal Facility

Rearing 10 back-strokes

daily 10 back-strokes

daily 20 back-strokes

daily 20 back-strokes daily

- - + - +

20 (10♂13♀) 21 (10♂11♀) 20 (11♂9♀) 15 (8♂7♀) 14 (8♂6♀)

Table 1

Synopsis of the experimental groups according to the different early-life manipulations (ELM)

Note: PND, postnatal day

(8)

A

determined by using a standard radioimmunoassay kit for small rodents (MP Biomedicals, LLC, NY, USA; sensitivity 1.25 ng/ml, intra-assay variation, 4.4%, interassay variation 6.5%).

2.6 Data analysis

Animals’ behaviour in Y-maze was analyzed in the 1

st

min and in the whole-5min- period of Trial 2, as it has been reported that animals might gradually loose their interest during a testing period [32]. The following behavioural measures were analysed:

- General locomotion and exploration behaviour during Y-maze test assessed by the total number of entries in all arms, during Trial 2.

- Novel arm discrimination during Y-maze test. To measure novel arm discrimination in the Y-maze test, a ‘Difference score’ was calculated by subtracting the % entries in Other arm from the % entries in Novel arm for each animal. Additionally the % entries in Other and Novel arms were compared.

- Strategy in Y-maze performance. Differences in the strategy of exploration were examined by analyzing the % of entries in the start arm and the % duration of stay in the center of the maze during the 1

st

min of Trial 2.

- First choice. As first choice was considered the first arm visited.

All the above variables, along with corticosterone measurements, were statistically evaluated by two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with ‘treatment’ and ‘sex’ as the independent variables.

When appropriate, Bonferroni or Least Significant Difference (LSD) posthoc analysis was applied (as specified in the legends). The preference for Novel versus Other arm was also analyzed by repeated two-way ANOVA with ‘arm’ as the within-subject factor. In this case, paired t-tests within the groups were applied for further comparisons of % entries between these two arms.

Experimental Phases I and II were analyzed separately. Choice of first arm entered after leaving the start was compared to chance (50%) by a binomial 2-tailed test [32, 34].

According to the vaginal smears obtained at sacrifice, females comprised a mixed population as to the estrus cycle in all groups. Since they had a comparable distribution of the phases among groups, we have pooled the data within each female group for all the parameters measured. The same non-treated animals were used as the control group in statistical analysis of Experimental Phases I and II. The level of significance was set at p<0.05. The statistical analysis was adjusted for non-equivalent groups.

3. Results

3.1 Effect of neonatal exposure to novelty following 10 back-strokes on Y-maze performance (Experimental Phase I)

3.1.1 General locomotion and exploration behaviour during Y-maze test

All animals showed the same motivation and ability to explore the Y-maze apparatus

during Trial 2, as assessed by the total number of entries in all arms that did not differ

significantly among groups (Fig. 2).

(9)

3.1.2 Novel arm discrimination during Y-maze test

Early-life exposure to novelty following 10 back-strokes significantly enhanced the adult animals’ performance in the Y-maze memory test (Fig. 3). Analysis of the difference score for the 1 st min and 5min of Trial 2 (Fig. 3A,B), gave an overall effect of treatment [1st min: F(2,53) = 5.00, p = 0.011; 5 min: F(2,54) = 9.33, p = 0.001], sex [1st min: F(1,53) = 4.59, p = 0.037; 5min: non significant] and treatment-sex interaction[

F(2,53) = 3.62, p = 0.034; 5min: non significant]. In general, Novel animals (10S-NOVEL) showed increased ability to discriminate the novel arm in the Y-maze, as depicted by their higher difference scores. Novel males had a higher score in the 1st and 5min of Trial 2, compared to controls (p = 0.004 and p = 0.029 respectively), but they did not differ significantly from the Home (10S-HOME) males (p = 0.084 and p = 0.22 for the 1st and 5min respectively). On the other hand, Novel females were superior to both Home females (p = 0.014 and p = 0.008 for the 1 st and 5min of Trial 2, respectively; Fig.

3A, B) and control females in the 5min period (p = 0.045; Fig. 3B). Home females were impaired, as compared to control females (p = 0.017) and Home males (p = 0.011) in the 1st min of Trial 2 (Fig. 3A).

Novel animals of both sexes entered more often the novel than the other arm, compared to Home animals and controls (Fig. 3C, D). More specifically, during the 1 st

0 2 4 6

8 MALE FEMALE

ENTRIES

Total Entries (Trial 2) 1 min

0 5 10 15 20

25

MALE FEMALE

ENTRIES

5 min

CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

Figure 2. General locomotion and exploration behaviour during Y-maze test in Experimental Phase I. Mean ± SEM of entries in all arms of the Y-maze during the 1

st

and whole 5min period of Trial 2. CON, animal facility reared controls;

10S-HOME, animals neonatally 10 times back- stroked in the home cage; 10S-NOVEL, animals neonatally 10 times back-stroked and exposed to novelty. No significant differences were observed (2-way ANOVA; significance accepted for p < 0.05).

A

B

(10)

A

min (Fig. 3C) of testing Novel and control animals entered more often the novel than the other arm (p = 0.006 for Novel males; p = 0.045 for control males; p = 0.036 for Novel females and p = 0.049 for control females). In the whole 5min period (Fig. 3D), only Novel animals still showed preference for the novel arm (p = 0.019 for Novel males and p = 0.001 for Novel females). Home animals did not show any arm preference for novel or other arm during the test.

3.1.3 Strategy in Y-maze performance

Differences in exploring the start arm and the center of the apparatus were detected

1 min

0 20 40

60 OTHER

NOVEL

MALE FEMALE

33,3

  

% ENTRIES

5 min

0 20 40 60

OTHER NOVEL

MALE FEMALE

33,3

 

% ENTRIES

-5 5 15 25

35 MALE FEMALE

Difference score

ac ab

b

a c$

ab 1 min

Novel arm discrimination

-5 5 15

25 MALE FEMALE

a ab

b

a a

b 5 min

Difference score

CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

CON 10S-HOME10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

Figure 3. Novel arm discrimination during Y-maze test in Experimental Phase I. Mean ± SEM of (A, B) Difference score (% entries in Novel arm - % entries in Other arm) and (C, D) of % entries in the Other and Novel arm during the 1

st

and whole 5min period of Trial 2. CON, animal facility reared controls, 10S-HOME, animals neonatally 10 times back-stroked in the home cage; 10S-NOVEL, animals neonatally 10 times back-stroked and

exposed to novelty. In (A, B), different letters (one or combination of two) over the bars denote significantly different groups within the same sex (2-way ANOVA and LSD posthoc test). In (C, D), stars over horizontal lines denote significant differences between arms for each animal group (repeated 2-way ANOVA and paired t-tests). Doted lines define chance level; significance accepted for p < 0.05.

A

B

C

D

(11)

among groups in the 1 st min [ANOVA treatment effect: F(2,51):4.23, p=0.021, sex:

F(1,51):4.44, p=0.041, treatment - sex F(2,51):7.71, p=0.001 ; Fig. 4A ], but not the 5min period of Trial 2. Home males visited the start arm significantly less than the Novel (p

= 0.024) and control (p = 0.001) male groups and the female Home group (p = 0.001).

No significant differences were detected among female groups. Concerning the time spent in the center during the 1 st min [ANOVA treatment effect: F(2,55):3.357, p=0.43 ; Fig. 4B], both Home and Novel males spent less time in the center than controls (p = 0.001 and p = 0.029, respectively) (Fig. 4B). No difference in this parameter was detected in female groups.

3.1.4 First choice

Only male rats of the Novel group showed a trend (p=0.065) in entering first the novel than the other arm (Table 2), but the same trend was found towards the right arm compared to the left one (p=0.065; Table 2). Control males entered first more the right arm (p=0.021) compared to the left one, independently of its novelty (Table 2). In the females’ first choice there was not a specific novel-other or right-left preference.

0 20 40

60 MALE FEMALE

b

a a$

a a

33,3

ab

% E N TR IE S

START ARM (1 min)

0 10 20

30 MALE FEMALE

b b

a

% D U R AT IO N

CENTER (1 min)

CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

Figure 4. Strategy in Y-maze performance for animals of Experimental Phase I. Mean ± SEM of (A)

% entries in the Start arm and (B) % duration in the Center of the apparatus during the 1st min of Trial 2. CON, animal facility reared controls, 10S-HOME, animals neonatally 10 times back-stroked in the home cage; 10S-NOVEL, animals neonatally

10 times back-stroked and exposed to novelty.

Dotted line in (A) defines chance level. Different letters over the bars denote significantly different groups within the same sex (2-way ANOVA and Bonferroni posthoc test). Significance accepted for p < 0.05.

A

B

(12)

A

3.2 Effect of neonatal exposure to novelty following 20 back-strokes on Y-maze performance (Experimental Phase II)

3.2.1 General locomotion and exploration behaviour during Y-maze test

All animals showed the same motivation and ability to explore the Y-maze apparatus during the 1st min of Trial 2 (Fig. 5). An overall effect of ‘treatment’ on the total number of entries was obtained for the whole 5min period of Trial 2 [F(2,43) = 5.65, p = 0.007].

Further analysis per sex showed that the effect of treatment was marginally non- significant in males [F(2,21) = 3.47, p = 0.052], with 20 back-strokes Novel (20S-NOVEL) males showing a trend for less total entries versus controls (p = 0.055). No significant differences were observed between Novel and Home males. In females, an analogous trend was observed towards reduced exploration versus control, which did not reach significance.

3.2.2 Novel arm discrimination during Y-maze test

Early novelty or home exposure in combination with 20 back-strokes significantly modified the animals’ ability for spatial recognition memory in adulthood (Fig. 6).

Analysis of the difference score for the 1 st min and 5min of Trial 2, gave an overall effect of treatment [1st min: F(2,47) = 3.29, p = 0.047; 5min: F(2,45) = 6.22, p = 0.004 ]. In the 1st min, there was a trend of 20 back-stroked Home (20S-HOME) animals to outperform Novel (20S-NOVEL) animals, which however did not reach significance when further analyzed per sex (Fig. 6A). In the 5min period (Fig. 6B), males 20S-Home significantly outperformed 20S-Novel (p = 0.021) and control (p = 0.019) animals of the same sex (F(2,24) = 4.52, p = 0.023). An analogous trend in 20S-Home females did not reach significance (p = 0.079).

Home animals of both sexes, as well as controls entered more often the novel

Experimental group Novel arm as 1st choice Right arm as 1st choice

CON male 10S-HOME male 10S-NOVEL male CON female 10S-HOME female 10S-NOVEL female

60.0% (6/10) 80.0% (8/10) 90.9% (10/11)b 46.2% (6/13) 66.7% (7/11) 44.4% (4/9)

90.0% (9/10)a 20.0% (2/10) 81.8% (9/11)b 61.5% (8/13) 33.3% (4/11) 77.8% (7/9) Note. Given are percentages of rats entering the

novel arm or the right arm first after leaving the start arm. First number in parentheses indicates the number of rats that entered the novel or right arm first and the second number indicates the total number of rats tested.

a

P<0.05, compared

to chance (50%);

b

P=0.065, compared to chance (50%). CON, animal facility reared controls;

20S-HOME, animals neonatally 20 times back- stroked in the home cage; 20S-NOVEL, animals neonatally 20 times back-stroked and exposed to novelty.

Table 2

First choice in Experimental Phase I

(13)

than the other arm during the 1st min of Trial 2 (p = 0.042 , 0.045, 0.030 and 0.049 for the comparisons between Other and Novel arm in 20S-HOME males, control males, 20S-HOME females and control females, respectively) (Fig. 6C). At the end of 5min, 20S-Home males were still showing a preference for visiting novel versus other arm (p

= 0.044, Fig. 6D), while this preference was lost over time in the female 20S-Home and control groups. 20S-Novel animals of both sexes showed no preference for novel versus other arm in either time point.

3.2.3 Strategy in Y-maze performance

No differences in exploring the start arm were observed among the groups of Phase II (data not shown). Treatment [F(2,43) = 4.19, p = 0.023] and sex [F(1,43) = 10.02, p = 0.003] significantly affected the time spent in the center of the maze during the 1 st but not the whole 5min period of Trial 2 (Fig. 7). Further analysis showed that 20S-Home females spent more time in the center compared to 20S-Novel females (p = 0.046). In addition, 20S-Novel females spent less time in the center compared to 20S-Novel males (p = 0.033).

0 2 4 6 8

ENTRIES

1 min

MALE FEMALE

Total Entries (Trial 2)

0 5 10 15 20 25

ENTRIES

5 min

MALE FEMALE

CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL

CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL

Figure 5. General locomotion and exploration behaviour during Y-maze test in Experimental Phase II. Mean ± SEM of entries in all arms of the Y-maze during the 1st and whole 5 min period of Trial 2. CON, animal facility reared controls, 20S-HOME, animals neonatally 20 times back-

stroked in the home cage; 20S-NOVEL, animals neonatally 20 times back-stroked and exposed to novelty. No significant differences were observed (2-way ANOVA and Bonferroni posthoc test;

significance accepted for p < 0.05).

A

B

(14)

A

3.2.4 First choice

No specific novel-other or right-left preference was observed for the 20 back- stroked male groups (Table 3). Control males, as mentioned in Experimental Phase I, entered first the right arm (p=0.021) compared to the left one, independently of its novelty. In female rats, the 20 back-stroked Novel animals showed a preference for entering first

-5 5 15 25

35

1 min

MALE FEMALE

Difference score

Novel arm discrimination

-5 5 15 25 35

b

a

a

MALE FEMALE

5 min

Difference score

1 min

0 20 40

60 OTHER

NOVEL 33,3

 

MALE FEMALE

% ENTRIES

5 min

0 20 40

60 OTHER

NOVEL 33,3

MALE FEMALE

% ENTRIES

CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

Figure 6. Novel arm discrimination during Y-maze test in Experimental Phase II. Mean ± SEM of (A,B) Difference score (% entries in Novel arm - % entries in Other arm) and (C,D) of % entries in the Other and Novel arm during the 1st and whole 5 min period of Trial 2. CON, animal facility reared controls; 20S-HOME, animals neonatally 20 times back-stroked in the home cage; 20S-NOVEL, animals neonatally 20 times back-stroked and exposed to novelty. In (A,B), different letters (one

or combination of two) over the bars denote significantly different groups within the same sex (2-way ANOVA and LSD posthoc test). No significant differences were observed in the 1st min period for both sexes and in the 5min period for females. In (C,D), stars over horizontal lines denote significant differences between arms for each animal group (repeated 2-way ANOVA and paired t-tests). Doted lines define chance level;

Significance accepted for p < 0.05.

A

B

C

D

(15)

the right arm (p=0.031) compared to the left (Table 3), while no specific preference was observed for the other female groups.

3.3 Basal corticosterone levels (Experimental Phase I & II)

Early-life exposure to novelty following 10 back-strokes (Phase I) significantly affected the animals’ basal corticosterone levels in adulthood in a sexually dimorphic manner (ANOVA treatment effect: F(2,53):6.15, p=0.010, sex effect: F(1,53):32.44, p=0.001; Fig. 8A). Home (10S-HOME) males and Novel (10S-NOVEL) females had elevated corticosterone levels compared to same sex controls (p = 0.043 and 0.007, respectively).

0 10 20 30 40

% DURATION a

ac bc$

CENTER (1 min)

MALE FEMALE

CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL

Figure 7. Strategy in Y-maze performance for animals of Experimental Phase II. Mean ± SEM of

% duration in the Center of the apparatus during the 1

st

min of Trial 2. CON, animal facility reared controls; 20S-HOME, animals neonatally 20 times back-stroked in the home cage; 20S-NOVEL, animals neonatally 20 times back-stroked and

exposed to novelty. Different letters (one or combination of two) omit over the bars denote significantly different groups within the same sex;

$: denotes sex difference between same treatment groups (2-way ANOVA and Bonferroni posthoc test;

significance accepted for p < 0.05).

Experimental group Novel arm as 1st choice Right arm as 1st choice CON male

20S-HOME male 20S-NOVEL male CON female 20S-HOME female 20S-NOVEL female

60.0% (6/10) 75.0% (6/8) 62.5% (5/8) 46.2% (6/13) 85.7% (6/7) 66.7% (4/6)

90.0% (9/10)a 75.0% (6/8) 62.5% (5/8) 61.5% (8/13) 71.4% (5/7) 100.0% (6/6)a

Table 3

First choice in Experimental Phase II

Note. Given are percentages of rats entering the novel arm or the right arm first after leaving the start arm. First number in parentheses indicates the number of rats that entered the novel or right arm first and the second number indicates the total number of rats tested.

a

P<0.05, compared

to chance (50%);

b

P=0.065, compared to chance (50%). CON, animal facility reared controls;

20S-HOME, animals neonatally 20 times back-

stroked in the home cage; 20S-NOVEL, animals

neonatally 20 times back-stroked and exposed to

novelty.

(16)

A

No significant differences were observed between Home and Novel animals of either sex. All female groups had higher corticosterone levels from the respective male ones (p = 0.001, 0.029, 0.001 for control, 10S-HOME and 10S-NOVEL, respectively).

Exposure to novelty following 20 back-strokes (Phase II) reversed the corticosterone pattern observed in experimental groups of Phase I (ANOVA treatment effect:

F(2,39):4.10, p=0.026, sex effect: F(1,39):31.38, p=0.001, treatment - sex interaction: : F(2,39):4.22, p=0.023 ; Fig. 8B). Novel (20S-NOVEL) males had elevated corticosterone levels, as compared both to control and Home (20S-HOME) males (p < 0.0001 for both). Corticosterone levels of 20S-Novel males did not differ from 20S-Novel females.

20S-Home females had higher corticosterone levels than female control (p = 0.049), as well as, than 20S-HOME males (p < 0.0001). Finally, control females had higher corticosterone levels than control males (p = 0.001).

0 200 400

600 MALE FEMALE

C or tic os te ro ne (n g/ m l)

a

b ab a$

ab$

b$

Basal corticosterone levels

0 200 400 600

C or tic os te ro ne (n g/ m l)

a$

b$

a a

b

ab

MALE FEMALE

CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL CON 10S-HOME 10S-NOVEL

CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL CON 20S-HOME 20S-NOVEL

Figure 8. Basal corticosterone levels. Mean ± SEM of plasma corticosterone levels in animals from Experimental Phase I (A) and II (B) determined one week after Y-maze test. CON, animal facility reared controls; 10S-HOME, animals neonatally 10 times back-stroked in the home cage; 10S-NOVEL, animals neonatally 10 times back-stroked and exposed to novelty; 20S-HOME, animals neonatally

20 times back-stroked in the home cage;

20S-NOVEL, animals neonatally 20 times back- stroked and exposed to novelty. Different letters (one or combination of two) omit over the bars denote significantly different groups within the same sex; $: denotes sex differences between same treatment groups (2-way ANOVA and Bonferroni posthoc test; Significance accepted for p < 0.05).

A

B

(17)

4. Discussion

The beneficial effects of neonatal handling, comprised of brief maternal separation, artificial tactile stimulation of the pup and exposure to novelty [6], on adult brain function, have extensively been studied in rodents. However, the effects of the individual components of handling and their possible interaction have not been examined in detail. This study aimed to examine whether the amount of novelty exposure and tactile stimulation, during the handling procedure, is reflected in later behaviour and circulating corticosterone levels. To diminish the effects of maternal care we adopted a split litter design, previously used to study the effects of novelty exposure in adult behaviour and physiology: half of the pups stayed in the home cage and the other half in a novel cage [18].

4.1 Spatial memory: inverted U-shaped curve effect of mild early-life manipulations The main finding of the present study is that novelty in the handling paradigm enhances adult spatial recognition memory, and that the amount of tactile stimulation in the form of back-stroking can modify this effect. 10 times back-stroking “allows”

the beneficial effect of novelty on memory performance, while 20 times back-stroking counteracts this effect. Interestingly, 20 times back-stroking had an enhancing effect on memory of adult rats that stayed during neonatal handling in their home cage, denoting that the overall amount of stimulation (novelty and stroking), received by the pups during the handling procedure, appears to be critical for the adult outcome. A minimal amount of neonatal stimulation, as well as over-stimulation, were disadvantageous for spatial recognition memory in adulthood. The above data suggest the existence of an inverted U-shaped curve effect of mild early manipulations, such as novelty and tactile stimulation, on adult spatial performance.

In previous studies it was shown that novelty exposure enhances the water maze

performance of male rats, as compared to their home-cage siblings receiving the

same amount of handling [18, 19]. The Y-maze data of Phase I are in compliance with

this finding. We have further shown that the early novelty experience has the same

beneficial effect on female spatial recognition memory, and is superior in comparison

to minimal neonatal stimulation (animal facility rearing). The superiority of 10S-Novel

animals in Y-maze task is not due to changes in their general locomotion activity, as the

number of total entries was similar in all animal groups. Early exposure to novelty has

been shown to enhance LTP in the CA1 area of the hippocampus in adulthood [20] and

to reduce emotional reactivity in the open field [18]. These findings could account for

the better performance of 10S-Novel animals in the Y–maze test. Furthermore, it was

reported [19] that male rats exposed to novelty can mount an additional corticosterone

response to an unexpected event. These animals correspond to the 10S-Novel males in

our study, and their presumed more effective HPA-axis’ coping could have helped them

(18)

A

to outperform 10S-Home in the Y-maze test.

4.2 Endocrine correlates

The HPA-axis of animals exposed to novelty has been suggested [19, 35] to be similar to that of the handled rats. However, a study comparing directly these two manipulations is missing. The only neuroendocrine evidence so far concerns corticosterone levels. In our study, 3 month old male 10S-Home and 10S-Novel rats did not differ in circulating corticosterone levels, in agreement with previous reports [19].

Compared to non-handled controls, only 10S-Home males had higher corticosterone levels, in compliance with the previously reported enhanced emotional reactivity of 10S-Home males versus 10S-Novel (Tang, 2001). Notably, additional stimulation reversed the situation, with 20S-Novel males having higher corticosterone levels from both 20S-Home and non-handled controls. This increase in basal hormone levels suggests a higher impact of early-life manipulations for this group at the level of HPA- axis as well. An opposite effect of novelty was observed in females, which was again reversed by additional back-stroking. The pattern in females implies a more sensitive HPA-axis response in the first place (10S-Novel females versus 10S-Home), which is not further responding after additional stroking (20S-Home and 20S-Novel did not differ in hormone levels).

4.3 Gender differences on corticosterone and spatial performance

Basal corticosterone levels of adult controls were higher in females, as expected [36]. In males, higher hormone levels coincided with poor memory performance (groups 10S-HOME and 20S-NOVEL), while in females increased corticosterone levels were in parallel with enhanced spatial memory (groups 10S-NOVEL and 20S-HOME).

Although in our study no direct correlation can be made between corticosterone levels and animals’ performance, as we have determined the hormone one week following Y-maze, previous studies have shown that stress levels of corticosterone impair spatial recognition memory in male rats and facilitate it in females. [37, 38]. The reason for this dimorphism is not clear. This may be due to a counteraction exerted by the higher estrogen levels of females to the impairing effects of corticosterone in spatial memory.

The effect of early novelty exposure on adult spatial recognition memory was

generally the same for both sexes under the same treatment. Sex differences were

observed in the behavioral strategy used during the memory test (as those concerning

the visits of the start arm or the center of the apparatus). Studies on the influence of

estrogens on memory give equivocal results, with some studies showing improvement,

impairment or no effect [for review, see [39, 40]]. The estrus phase of female rats can

contribute to their cognitive performance depending on the task used and the context

of learning. For example, although it has been reported that stressed females at

proestrus differ in their performance in the Y-maze task from those in estrus [41], this

has not been observed for non-stressed female animals [37]. In the Y-maze task, control

(19)

males and females (at different estrus phases) do not show significant differences in performance [38]. In the current study, we used a mixed population of intact females, randomly assigned as to the estrus cycle and we found no significant differences in memory performance of the control animals, in agreement with the previous findings.

4.4 Maternal care

Although we used a split litter design, our results do not completely exclude the contribution of maternal care. In the classical handling paradigm, handled animals receive greater amounts of maternal care at the reunion than the non-handled [17, 42].

In the novelty paradigm, it was proposed that the mothers were retrieving more the home-staying pups [19]. However, this maternal care difference was not found by others [43] and was not confirmed in a following study [44]. Additionally, Tang’s experimental litters did not have an equal sex distribution and this could be a confounding factor, as it has been shown that dams differentially care for their male or female pups [45, 46].

5. Conclusion

In conclusion, this study provided evidence that the nature and intensity of the individual components of a mild early-life manipulation, like handling, affect outcome in aspects of spatial memory and basal adrenocortical function of adult rats. Transient variations in early-life experience could thus lead to behavioural variation or behavioural plasticity of the response towards a later-life event.

6. Acknowledgment

This research was supported by grant 70/3/8372 of the Greek General Secretariat of Research & Technology.

7. References

[1] Champagne, D. L., Ronald de Kloet, E., Joels, M. Fundamental aspects of the impact of glucocorticoids on the (immature) brain. Seminars in fetal & neonatal medicine. 2009,14:136-42.

[2] de Kloet, E. R., Sibug, R. M., Helmerhorst, F. M., Schmidt, M. V. Stress, genes and the mechanism of programming the brain for later life. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2005,29:271-81.

[3] Lehmann, J., Pryce, C. R., Jongen-Relo, A. L., Stohr, T., Pothuizen, H. H., Feldon, J. Comparison of maternal separation and early handling in terms of their neurobehavioral effects in aged rats.

Neurobiology of aging. 2002,23:457-66.

[4] Plotsky, P. M., Thrivikraman, K. V., Nemeroff, C.

B., Caldji, C., Sharma, S., Meaney, M. J. Long-term consequences of neonatal rearing on central

corticotropin-releasing factor systems in adult male rat offspring. Neuropsychopharmacology.

2005,30:2192-204.

[5] DeNelsky, G. Y., Denenberg, V. H. Infantile stimulation and adult exploratory behaviour in the rat: effects of handling upon visual variation- seeking. Animal behaviour. 1967,15:568-73.

[6] Levine, S. Infantile experience and resistance to physiological stress. Science (New York, N.Y.

1957,126:405.

[7] Meaney, M. J., Aitken, D. H., Bodnoff, S. R., Iny, L.

J., Sapolsky, R. M. The effects of postnatal handling

on the development of the glucocorticoid receptor

systems and stress recovery in the rat. Progress

in neuro-psychopharmacology & biological

psychiatry. 1985,9:731-4.

(20)

A

[8] Nunez, J. F., Ferre, P., Escorihuela, R. M., Tobena, A., Fernandez-Teruel, A. Effects of postnatal handling of rats on emotional, HPA-axis, and prolactin reactivity to novelty and conflict.

Physiology & behavior. 1996,60:1355-9.

[9] Levine, S., Haltmeyer, G. C., Karas, G. G., Denenberg, V. H. Physiological and behavioral effects of infantile stimulation. Physiology &

behavior. 1967 2:5.

[10] Meerlo, P., Horvath, K. M., Nagy, G. M., Bohus, B., Koolhaas, J. M. The influence of postnatal handling on adult neuroendocrine and behavioural stress reactivity. Journal of neuroendocrinology.

1999,11:925-33.

[11] Meaney, M. J., Aitken, D. H. The effects of early postnatal handling on hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor concentrations: temporal parameters.

Brain research. 1985,354:301-4.

[12] Meaney, M. J., Diorio, J., Francis, D., LaRocque, S., O'Donnell, D., Smythe, J. W., et al. Environmental regulation of the development of glucocorticoid receptor systems in the rat forebrain. The role of serotonin. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1994,746:260-73; discussion 74, 89-93.

[13] Viau, V., Sharma, S., Plotsky, P. M., Meaney, M.

J. Increased plasma ACTH responses to stress in nonhandled compared with handled rats require basal levels of corticosterone and are associated with increased levels of ACTH secretagogues in the median eminence. J Neurosci. 1993,13:1097-105.

[14] Kosten, T. A., Lee, H. J., Kim, J. J. Neonatal handling alters learning in adult male and female rats in a task-specific manner. Brain research.

2007,1154:144-53.

[15] Stamatakis, A., Pondiki, S., Kitraki, E., Diamantopoulou, A., Panagiotaropoulos, T., Raftogianni, A., et al. Effect of neonatal handling on adult rat spatial learning and memory following acute stress. Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands).

2008,11:148-59.

[16] Villescas, R., Bell, R. W., Wright, L., Kufner, M.

Effect of handling on maternal behavior following return of pups to the nest. Developmental psychobiology. 1977,10:323-9.

[17] Pryce, C. R., Bettschen, D., Feldon, J.

Comparison of the effects of early handling and early deprivation on maternal care in the rat.

Developmental psychobiology. 2001,38:239-51.

[18] Tang, A. C. Neonatal exposure to novel environment enhances hippocampal-dependent memory function during infancy and adulthood.

Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.

2001,8:257-64.

[19] Tang, A. C., Akers, K. G., Reeb, B. C., Romeo, R.

D., McEwen, B. S. Programming social, cognitive, and neuroendocrine development by early exposure to novelty. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006,103:15716-21.

[20] Tang, A. C., Zou, B. Neonatal exposure to novelty enhances long-term potentiation in CA1 of the rat hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2002,12:398- 404.

[21] Benetti, F., Andrade de Araujo, P., Sanvitto, G. L., Lucion, A. B. Effects of neonatal novelty exposure on sexual behavior, fear, and stress-response in adult rats. Developmental psychobiology.

2007,49:258-64.

[22] Rees, S. L., Steiner, M., Fleming, A. S. Early deprivation, but not maternal separation, attenuates rise in corticosterone levels after exposure to a novel environment in both juvenile and adult female rats. Behavioural brain research.

2006,175:383-91.

[23] Pryce, C. R., Bettschen, D., Bahr, N. I., Feldon, J. Comparison of the effects of infant handling, isolation, and nonhandling on acoustic startle, prepulse inhibition, locomotion, and HPA activity in the adult rat. Behavioral neuroscience.

2001,115:71-83.

[24] Pauk, J., Kuhn, C. M., Field, T. M., Schanberg, S. M. Positive effects of tactile versus kinesthetic or vestibular stimulation on neuroendocrine and ODC activity in maternally-deprived rat pups. Life sciences. 1986,39:2081-7.

[25] van Oers, H. J., de Kloet, E. R., Whelan, T., Levine, S. Maternal deprivation effect on the infant's neural stress markers is reversed by tactile stimulation and feeding but not by suppressing corticosterone. J Neurosci. 1998,18:10171-9.

[26] Suchecki, D., Rosenfeld, P., Levine, S. Maternal regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the infant rat: the roles of feeding and stroking. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1993,75:185-92.

[27] Schapiro, S., Vukovich, K. R. Early experience effects upon cortical dendrites: a proposed model for development. Science (New York, N.Y.

1970,167:292-4.

[28] Zhang, M., Cai, J. X. Effects of neonatal tactile stimulation and maternal separation on the anxiety and the emotional memory in adult female rats.

Zoological Research. 2006,27:7.

[29] Zhang, M., Cai, J. X. Neonatal tactile stimulation enhances spatial working memory, prefrontal long- term potentiation, and D1 receptor activation in adult rats. Neurobiology of learning and memory.

2008,89:397-406.

[30] Imanaka, A., Morinobu, S., Toki, S., Yamamoto, S., Matsuki, A., Kozuru, T., et al. Neonatal tactile stimulation reverses the effect of neonatal isolation on open-field and anxiety-like behavior, and pain sensitivity in male and female adult Sprague-Dawley rats. Behavioural brain research.

2008,186:91-7.

[31] Conrad, C. D., Galea, L. A., Kuroda, Y., McEwen, B. S. Chronic stress impairs rat spatial memory on the Y maze, and this effect is blocked by tianeptine pretreatment. Behavioral neuroscience.

1996,110:1321-34.

[32] Dellu, F., Mayo, W., Cherkaoui, J., Le Moal, M., Simon, H. A two-trial memory task with automated recording: study in young and aged rats. Brain research. 1992,588:132-9.

[33] Spruijt, B. M., Gispen, W. H. Behavioral

sequences as an easily quantifiable parameter

in experimental studies. Physiology & behavior.

(21)

1984,32:707-10.

[34] Conrad, C. D., Lupien, S. J., Thanasoulis, L.

C., McEwen, B. S. The effects of type I and type II corticosteroid receptor agonists on exploratory behavior and spatial memory in the Y-maze. Brain research. 1997,759:76-83.

[35] Tang, A. C., Reeb, B. C., Romeo, R. D., McEwen, B. S. Modification of social memory, hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal axis, and brain asymmetry by neonatal novelty exposure. J Neurosci.

2003,23:8254-60.

[36] Atkinson, H. C., Waddell, B. J. Circadian variation in basal plasma corticosterone and adrenocorticotropin in the rat: sexual dimorphism and changes across the estrous cycle.

Endocrinology. 1997,138:3842-8.

[37] Conrad, C. D., Jackson, J. L., Wieczorek, L., Baran, S. E., Harman, J. S., Wright, R. L., et al.

Acute stress impairs spatial memory in male but not female rats: influence of estrous cycle.

Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior.

2004,78:569-79.

[38] Conrad, C. D., Grote, K. A., Hobbs, R. J., Ferayorni, A. Sex differences in spatial and non- spatial Y-maze performance after chronic stress.

Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2003,79:32- 40. [39] Packard, M. G. Posttraining estrogen and memory modulation. Hormones and behavior.

1998,34:126-39.

[40] Williams, C. L., Meck, W. H. The organizational effects of gonadal steroids on sexually dimorphic spatial ability. Psychoneuroendocrinology.

1991,16:155-76.

[41] Korol, D. L., Kolo, L. L. Estrogen-induced changes in place and response learning in young adult female rats. Behavioral neuroscience.

2002,116:411-20.

[42] Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman, A., et al. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress.

Science (New York, N.Y. 1997,277:1659-62.

[43] Macri, S., Mason, G. J., Wurbel, H. Dissociation in the effects of neonatal maternal separations on maternal care and the offspring's HPA and fear responses in rats. The European journal of neuroscience. 2004,20:1017-24.

[44] Akers, K. G., Yang, Z., DelVecchio, D. P., Reeb, B. C., Romeo, R. D., McEwen, B. S., et al. Social competitiveness and plasticity of neuroendocrine function in old age: influence of neonatal novelty exposure and maternal care reliability. PLoS ONE.

2008,3:e2840.

[45] Moore, C. L., Morelli, G. A. Mother rats interact differently with male and female offspring. Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

1979,93:677-84.

[46] Claessens, S., Daskalakis, N., Oitzl, M. S.,

De Kloet, E. R., Champagne, D. Development

of individual differences: Impact of early-life

experience. FENS Abstr. 2008,4:059.6.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Drug Research (LACDR) and the Leiden University Medical Center (LUMC), under the supervision of Prof. In 2006, he did a clinical research internship in

Nurturing Nature: Testing the three-hit hypothesis of schizophrenia Nikolaos Daskalakis.. Thesis, Leiden University December

The newborn rat’s stress system readily habituates to repeated and prolonged maternal separation, while continuing to respond to stressors in context dependent fashion. Chapter 3

It appeared that the experience of being kept in isolation in a novel environment during repeated maternal separation, rather than the maternal absence per se, caused priming of

(c) MS + Chronic stress: Choy and van den Buuse studied how early and later life stress affects the schizophrenia phenotype of adult Wistar rats.. 24-hMD was used as early-life

We sacrificed rat pups in two different testing conditions: basal levels (basal) and 8h of separation (separated). 2C): to determine the effects of repeated separation in home context

In the present study we demonstrated that the stressful experience of peer deprivation in a novel cage during repeated MS (NOVEL SEP) rather than the maternal absence experience

The specific objectives of this project were stated in Chapter 1, Par 1.2. The project evaluation was discussed in accordance with these objectives. A future perspective is