• No results found

The mediating role of parenting behaviours in the relation between parent and infant anxiety

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The mediating role of parenting behaviours in the relation between parent and infant anxiety"

Copied!
33
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

 

   

THE  MEDIATING  ROLE  OF  PARENTING  

BEHAVIOURS  IN  THE  RELATION  BETWEEN  

PARENT  AND  INFANT  ANXIETY  

       

FACULTY  OF  SOCIAL  AND  BEHAVIOURAL  SCIENCES  

Graduate  School  of  Childhood  Development  and  Education  

                Masterscriptie  Orthopedagogiek   Pedagogische  en  Onderwijskundige  Wetenschappen   Universiteit  van  Amsterdam     L.D.  Willems   6097901     1st  Supervisor   Dr.  E.L.  Möller  /  M.  Metz,  MSc.     2nd  Supervisor   Dr.  E.I.  De  Bruin     Amsterdam,  April  2015  

(2)
(3)

Abstract  ...  4  

Samenvatting  ...  5  

Introduction  ...  6  

Parenting  behaviour  ...  7  

Paternal  and  maternal  parenting  differences  ...  9  

Social  referencing  and  modeling  ...  11  

This  study  ...  14  

Method  ...  16  

Participants  ...  16  

Procedure  ...  17  

Measures  ...  18  

Plan  for  data-­‐analysis  ...  22  

Data  analysis  and  results  ...  23  

Preliminary  checks  and  descriptive  statistics  ...  23  

Association  of  parenting  behaviours  with  parental  anxiety  and  infant  anxiety.  ...  24  

Discussion  ...  27  

References  ...  32    

(4)

Abstract  

The  mediating  role  of  parenting  behaviours  was  assessed  in  the  relation  between   parent  and  infant  anxiety  for  mothers  and  fathers  separately.  Eighty-­‐one  infants  aged   10-­‐15  months  were  encouraged  to  cross  the  deep  side  of  the  visual  cliff  by  their  mother   (N  =  40)  or  father  (N  =  41).  During  this  task  parent  and  infant  anxiety  were  observed.   During  two  free  play  tasks  (one  with  and  one  without  toys),  challenging,  overprotective   and  overcontrolling  parenting  behaviours  were  coded.  Additionally,  the  Comprehensive   Parenting  Behaviour  Questionnaire  (CPBQ-­‐1)  was  filled-­‐out  by  parents  to  assess  

challenging,  overprotective  and  overcontrolling  parenting.  More  parental  anxiety  was   assumed  to  relate  to  more  infant  anxiety  because  of  a  decrease  in  challenging  and   increases  in  overcontrolling  and  overprotective  parenting.  Mediation  was  assessed  with   PROCESS  for  SPSS  (Preacher  &  Hayes,  2004),  but  no  evidence  for  mediation  was  found.   A  borderline  positive  association  between  paternal  and  infant  anxiety  was  found.  A   slight  trend  for  a  relation  between  for  more  paternal  challenging  parenting  and  less   infant  anxiety  was  found.  Both  results  were  in  the  expected  direction.  Some  evidence   emerged  that  infants  of  anxious  fathers  are  more  anxious;  paternal  challenging  

parenting  may  form  a  buffer  for  infant  anxiety.  Mediation  might  emerge  when  children   are  older.    

   

(5)

Samenvatting  

De  mediërende  rol  van  opvoedingsgedrag  is  onderzocht  in  de  relatie  tussen  angst  van   ouders  en  baby’s  voor  vaders  en  moeders  apart.  Eenentachtig  baby’s  van  10-­‐15   maanden  werden  aangemoedigd  door  vader  (N  =  41)  of  moeder  (N  =  40)  om  de  diepe   zijde  van  de  visual  cliff  over  te  steken.  Angst  van  ouder  en  kind  werden  geobserveerd.   Middels  twee  vrij  spel  taken  (één  met  en  één  zonder  speelgoed)  werden  uitdagend,   overcontrolerend  en  overbeschermend  opvoedgedraging  gecodeerd.  Ook  werden   uitdagend,  overcontrolerend  en  overbeschermend  opvoedgedraging  gemeten  middels   de  Comprehensive  Parenting  Behaviour  Questionnaire  (CPBQ-­‐1).  Verondersteld  werd   dat  angstige  ouders  angstigere  baby’s  hebben  door  een  afname  van  uitdagend  

opvoedgedrag  en  een  toename  van  overcontrolerend  en  overbetrokken  opvoedgedrag.   Mediatie  werd  getest  door  middel  van  PROCESS  voor  SPSS  (Preacher  &  Hayes,  2004),   maar  niet  gevonden.  Een  borderline  positief  verband  werd  gevonden  voor  angst  van   vader  en  die  van  kind.  Een  trend  was  zichtbaar  dat  meer  uitdagend  opvoedgedrag  van   vaders  geassocieerd  was  met  minder  angst  van  het  kind.  Beide  resultaten  waren  in  de   verwachte  richting.  Zeer  voorzichtig  kan  gesteld  worden  dat  angstige  vaders  baby’s   hebben  met  meer  angst;  uitdagend  gedrag  van  vaders  zou  hier  een  buffer  voor  kunnen   vormen.  Mogelijk  vindt  mediatie  plaats  wanneer  kinderen  ouder  zijn.    

   

(6)

Introduction  

Anxiety   disorders   amongst   children   and   adults   in   the   Netherlands   and   other   western  societies  are  highly  prevalent  and  can  be  severe.  In  children  and  adolescents  5   to  10  percent  suffer  from  anxiety  disorders  (Carr,  2006;  Costello  et  al.,  2011;  Ferdinand   &   Dieleman,   2007;   Verhulst,   2001).   A   recent   meta-­‐analysis   by   Costello   et   al.   (2011)   reviewing  datasets  using  a  DSM-­‐III-­‐R,  DSM-­‐IV  or  ICD-­‐10  diagnosis  of  anxiety  disorders   found   the   following   prevalence   rates   for   all   anxiety   disorders   combined:   12,3%   in   elementary-­‐school-­‐age   children,   11%   in   13-­‐18   year   old   children   and   a   prevalence   of   10,2%  using  all  data  sets  with  age  ranges  between  2-­‐21  years  of  age.  Children  with  an   anxiety   disorder   are   at   high   risk   of   becoming   adults   with   an   anxiety   disorder   (Rapee,   Schniering,  &  Hudson,  2009).  

A  raised  vulnerability  to  anxiety  has  been  found  in  children  of  parents  with  an   anxiety  disorder  (Turner,  Beidel,  &  Costello,  1987).  In  a  study  where  children  aged  7  to   12   years   of   anxious   parents,   depressed   parents,   a   combination   of   anxious/depressed   parents   and   those   of   non-­‐diagnosed   parents   were   compared,   it   was   found   that   a   significantly   larger   amount   of   children   of   parents   with   a   psychiatric   diagnosis   had   an   increased  risk  of  psychopathology  (Beidel  &  Turner,  1997).  Offspring  of  parents  with  an   anxiety   disorder   alone   showed   to   have   more   anxiety   disorders,   whereas   children   of   depressed  and  anxious/depressed  parents  showed  a  broader  range  of  psychopathology.   From  a  different  point  of  view,  Cooper,  Fearn,  Willets,  Seabrooke  and  Parkinson  (2006)   assessed   the   anxiety   disorders   of   parents   of   85   children   diagnosed   with   an   anxiety   disorder   and   compared   them   to   the   parents   of   45   control   children   with   no   current   anxiety  disorder.  A  significantly  higher  rate  of  anxiety  disorders  was  found  in  parents  of   children  with  an  anxiety  disorder  themselves.    

This  intergenerational  transmission  of  anxiety  can  be  explained  by  two  factors:   vulnerability   factors   such   as   genetic   factors   or   having   an   anxious   temperament,   and   environmental  factors  (Murray,  Creswell,  &  Cooper,  2009).  The  latter  includes  adverse   life   events,   modelling,   information   transfer   and   parenting   behaviours.   This   study   will   explore   the   possibility   of   a   mediating   role   of   parenting   behaviours   in   the   relation   between   parent   and   infant   anxiety.   Parenting   behaviours   and   modelling   will   be   discussed.    

(7)

Parenting  behaviour  

Many  studies  report  on  the  association  between  parenting  behaviours  and  child  anxiety   (Chorpita  &  Barlow,  1998;  McLeod,  Wood,  &  Weisz,  2007;  Rapee,  1997;  Wood,  McLeod,   Sigman,   Hwang,   &   Chu,   2003).   The   most   studied   parenting   behaviours   in   association   with  child  anxiety  are  parental  control  and  rejection  (McLeod  et  al.,  2007;  Wood  et  al.,   2003).    

Parental   overcontrol,   overprotection,   overinvolvement   and   intrusiveness   have   been  used  in  the  literature  interchangeably,  generally  under  the  umbrella  term:  control.   These   restrictive   parenting   behaviours   are   characterized   by   excessive   regulation   of   children’s  activities  and/or  routines,  high  levels  of  intrusiveness,  encouragement  of  the   child’s  dependency  on  parents  and  discouragement  of  the  child’s  independent  problem   solving   (Barber,   1996;   Chorpita,   Brown,   &   Barlow,   1998;   Maccoby,   1992;   Majdandžić,   De  Vente,  &  Bögels,  2010;  Van  Der  Bruggen,  Stams,  &  Bögels,  2008).  According  to  some   theoretical  models  (Chorpita  &  Barlow,  1998;  McLeod  et  al.,  2007;  Wood  et  al.,  2003),   overcontrol   can   increase   child   anxiety   by   preventing   children   to   explore   their   surroundings   independently,   by   reducing   their   feelings   of   competence,   and   by   increasing   children’s   interpretation   of   their   environment   as   threatening.   Autonomy   granting  can  be  seen  as  the  opposite  of  parental  control  and  therefore  promotes  child   independence.  Autonomy  granting  is  often  studied  instead  of,  or  in  addition  to,  parental   control  (Clark  &  Ladd,  2000;  Ginsburg,  Grover  &  Ialongo,  2008;  Majdandžić  et  al.,  2010;   Steinberg,  Elmen  &  Mounts,  1989).    

In  a  meta-­‐analysis  by  Möller,  Nikolić,  Majdandžić,  De  Vente,  &  Bögels  (in  press),  a   distinction   is   made   between   overcontrolling   and   overprotective   parenting   and   it   is   assumed   that   they   affect   child   anxiety   in   different   ways.   Overcontrolling   parenting   restricts   the   child   by   needlessly   helping   or   interfering   with   the   child’s   behaviour   without  taking  their  needs,  interests  or  feelings  into  account.  Overprotective  parenting   limits  the  child’s  exposure  to  new  objects,  people  or  situations  by  taking  into  account   the   child’s   health   and   safety   in   an   excessively   cautious   and   protective   manner.   In   the   meta-­‐analysis,  paternal  and  maternal  differential  parenting  behaviours  are  both  related   to   child   anxiety.   Results   showed   that   while   there   were   no   significant   correlations   between   maternal   or   paternal   overcontrol   and   child   anxiety,   for   maternal   overprotective   parenting   and   child   anxiety   a   significant   relation   was   found   and   for   paternal   overprotective   parenting   and   child   anxiety   a   borderline   significant   but  

(8)

stronger   correlation   was   found,   indicating   that   overcontrolling   and   overprotective   parenting  behaviours  indeed  relate  differently  to  child  anxiety.    

Parental  rejection  is  characterized  by  hostility  and  negativity  towards  the  child,   lack   of   warmth,   lack   of   responsiveness   and   criticising   reactions   towards   the   child   (McLeod  et  al.,  2007;  Rapee,  1997;  Wood  et  al.,  2003).  Rejecting  parenting  behaviours   enhance  child  anxiety  by  continuous  negative  responses,  thereby  increasing  the  child’s   feelings  of  incompetence  and  worthlessness  (McLeod  et  al.,  2007;  Rapee,  1997;  Wood  et   al.,  2003).  

The  meta-­‐analytic  review  conducted  by  McLeod  et  al.  (2007)  illustrated  that  the   overall   link   between   parenting   and   child   anxiety   is   small.   However,   when   looking   merely  at  observational  data  instead  of  questionnaires,  a  medium  effect  size  was  found   for  the  association  between  parental   overcontrol  and  child  anxiety  while  only  a  small   effect   size   was   found   for   the   relation   between   parental   rejection   and   child   anxiety.   Similar   results   have   been   found   in   a   review   by   Wood   et   al.   (2003).   Parental   rejection   could  be  more  strongly  related  to  child  depression,  while  parental  overcontrol  could  be   more  related  to  child  anxiety  (Rapee,  1997).  Furthermore,  in  the  meta-­‐analysis  by  Van   der   Bruggen   et   al.   (2008)   examining   the   association   of   parent   and   child   anxiety   and   parental   overcontrol,   a   medium   effect   size   between   child   anxiety   and   parental   overcontrol  was  also  found.  

A  fairly  new  parenting  construct  is  challenging  parenting  behaviour.  Challenging   parenting  behaviour  entails  behaviours  of  the  parent  towards  the  child  where,  during   playful   interaction,   the   child   is   challenged,   teased,   excited,   stimulated   or   encouraged   towards   competition   and   risk-­‐taking   behaviour   (Bögels   &   Perotti,   2011;   Bögels   &   Phares,   2008).   In   a   recent   study   by   Majdandžić,   Möller,   De   Vente,   Bögels,   &   Van   den   Boom   (2014)   a   distinction   is   made   between   more   physical   forms   of   challenging   behaviours,  such  as  rough-­‐and-­‐tumble  play,  and  more  verbal  forms,  such  as  teasing  or   stimulating   risk-­‐taking   behaviour,   which   both   play   a   role   in   the   construct.   So   far,   hypotheses   have   been   made   that   challenging   parenting   behaviour   buffers   for   child   anxiety,   exposing   the   child   to   arousal   at   an   early   age   and   in   a   safe   and   comfortable   manner   (Bögels   &   Perotti,   2011;   Bögels   &   Phares,   2008).   However   to   this   point,   little   research   had   been   done   (Majdandžić   et   al.,   2014;   Möller,   Majdandžić,   &   Bögels,   in   press).   In   both   of   these   studies   (Majdandžić   et   al.,   2014;   Möller   et   al,   in   press),   significant   relationships   have   been   found   to   child   anxiety,   although   in   different  

(9)

directions  for  fathers  and  mothers.  For  fathers  a  negative  relationship  was  found,  while   a  positive  relationship  for  mothers  was  found.  This  suggests  that  the  more  challenging   behaviour   fathers   display,   the   less   anxious   their   children   are,   while   the   more   challenging  behaviour  mothers  display,  the  more  anxious  their  children  are  (although  in   the   study   by   Möller   et   al.   (in   press)   the   positive   relation   for   mothers   was   only   borderline  significant).  

It   is   possible   that   mothers   and   fathers   differ   in   the   extent   to   which   they   show   different   types   of   parenting   behaviours   (Bögels   &   Perotti,   2011).   This   will   be   further   described  in  the  next  paragraph.  

 

Paternal  and  maternal  parenting  differences  

In   a   review   on   the   evolutionary   basis   of   sex   differences   in   parenting   (Möller,   Majdandžić,  De  Vente,  &  Bögels,  2013)  it  is  described  how  fathers  and  mothers  differ  in   their   parental   roles   due   to   assumed   different   specializations   developed   during   evolution.   Where   males   are   characterized   by   more   risk-­‐taking   behaviour,   are   more   strongly  orientated  to  social  dominance  and  are  more  competitive,  females  tend  to  be   more  risk-­‐aversive,  caring  and  empathic  or  sensitive  to  the  needs  of  others  (Bögels  &   Phares,  2008;  Möller  et  al.,  2013).  These  specializations  are  expected  to  be  reflected  in   the  way  parents  parent,  as  fathers  and  mothers  are  likely  to  model  and  actively  teach   their  offspring  their  own  gender-­‐roles.  According  to  Paquette  (2004)  for  instance,  the   father-­‐child  relationship  can  be  characterized  by  an  activation  relationship  as  opposed   to   the   mother-­‐child   attachment   relationship.     The   father-­‐child   activation   relationship   “satisfies   the   child’s   need   to   be   stimulated,   to   overcome   limits,   and   to   learn   to   take   chances   in   contexts   in   which   the   child   is   confident   of   being   protected   from   potential   dangers”  (Paquette,  2004,  p.  202).  This  is  put  into  practice  during  the  before  mentioned  

rough-­‐and-­‐tumble   play   (RTP)   in   which   fathers   surprise   their   children,   momentarily  

destabilize  them  and  encourage  children  to  take  risks  (Paquette,  2004).  The  attachment   relationship   (in   reference   to   Bowlby,   1969   in   Paquette,   2004)   entails   the   child’s   confidence  in  the  parent’s  response  to  the  child’s  basic  needs,  calming  and  comforting  in   times  of  stress,  and  offering  warmth.    

As   described   in   the   review   by   Möller   et   al.   (2013),   several   studies   have   found   that   fathers   spend   more   time   playing   with   their   children   than   mothers,   that   paternal  

(10)

play   is   more   often   more   physical   (RTP)   than   the   play   of   mothers,   and   compared   to   fathers,  mothers’  play  is  more  often  characterized  by  pretend  play.  For  example,  a  study   by  Lamb  (1977)  revealed  that  infants  as  young  as  7  to  12  months  are  equally  attached   to   both   parents   and   that   both   parents   hold   their   babies   for   equal   amounts   of   time.   Interestingly,  the  reason  parents  pick  up  their  babies  differed  from  each  other.  Mothers   picked  up  and  held  their  babies  for  caretaking  and  soothing  practices,  whilst  fathers  did   this  mainly  for  play  interactions  (Lamb,  1977).  A  side  note  in  the  review  by  Möller  et  al.   (2013)  is  that  some  studies  have  found  very  small  to  no  difference  in  the  type  of  play   fathers  and  mothers  engage  in,  or  in  the  sensitivity  and  responsiveness  of  fathers  and   mothers.  However,  none  of  the  studies  described  found  that  fathers  are  more  sensitive   than  mothers.    

It  is  hypothesized  that  mothers,  based  on  their  evolutionary  acquired  skills,  are   specialized  in  the  child’s  internal  world  providing  warmth  and  protection,  providing  in   the  child’s  basic  needs  (e.g.,  feeding,  changing  diapers)  and  caring  and  soothing  when   the   child   is   in   stress.   Fathers   on   the   other   hand   are   thought   to   be   specialized   in   the   external   world   encouraging   the   child   to   push   and   overcome   limits,   engaging   in   more   competitive  and  challenging  play  (Bögels  &  Perotti,  2011;  Bögels  &  Phares,  2008).  It  is   also   hypothesized   that   paternal   challenging   behaviour   is   a   protective   factor   for   the   development   of   child   anxiety   because   it   lets   children   gain   experience   in   dealing   with   ambiguous  situations  in  a  safe  manner  (Bögels  &  Perotti,  2011;  Bögels  &  Phares,  2008).   In   line   with   this,   Bögels   and   Perotti   (2011)   pose   that   fathers   who   stimulate   their   children   for   risk   taking   and   competitive   behaviours   through   RTP,   enhance   the   coping   skills   of   their   children   through   positive   experiences   (Möller   et   al.,   2013)   and   prepare   them  for  competing  in  the  outside  world  later  in  life.  

Some  researchers  suggest  that  what  is  a  protective  factor  in  paternal  parenting   behaviour,  could  be  a  risk  factor  in  maternal  behaviour  and  vice  versa  (Bögels  &  Perotti,   2011;  Bögels  &  Phares,  2008,  Möller  et  al.,  2013).  In  a  longitudinal  study  conducted  to   examine   the   independent   influence   of   paternal   and   maternal   parenting   behaviour   on   child   social   anxiety   it   was   found   that   paternal   challenging   behaviour   predicted   less   social   anxiety   at   4,5   years,   whereas   maternal   challenging   behaviour   predicted   more   social  anxiety,  suggesting  that  the  same  parenting  behaviours  can  be  either  a  risk  factor   or   a   protective   factor   in   the   development   of   child   social   anxiety   (Majdandžić   et   al.   2014).    

(11)

Regarding   the   intergenerational   transmission   of   anxiety,   Bögels   and   Phares   (2008)   describe   a   model   in   which   parental   anxiety   affects   the   paternal   and   maternal   role  in  different  ways.  This  hypothesis  is  further  elaborated  in  a  formal  model  (Bögels  &   Perotti,   2011).   For   instance,   because   women   are   more   risk-­‐aversive,   caring,   empathic   and   sensitive   to   the   needs   of   others,   being   anxious   does   not   necessarily   change   their   habits,   only   strengthens   them.   On   the   other   hand,   men   are   more   risk-­‐taking,   competitive   and   seek   out   social   dominance.   Being   anxious   could   possibly   alter   this   behaviour  very  much.  Therefore,  an  anxious  mother  would  still  be  capable  of  fulfilling   her   soothing,   caring   and   protecting   role   but   an   anxious   father   is   less   capable   of   stimulating,  competitive,  risk  taking  and  social  dominant  behaviour.    

 

Social  referencing  and  modeling  

One  way  to  investigate  the  intergenerational  transmission  of  anxiety  is  through  social   referencing   studies.   Social   referencing   is   a   skill   infants   develop   around   the   age   of   ten   months  (Feinman  &  Lewis,  1983;  Walden  &  Ogan,  1988).  When  infants  look  towards  the   emotional  expressions  of  someone  else  in  an  ambiguous  situation,  usually  an  important   figure  like  a  parent,  and  use  this  information  to  understand  the  unfamiliar  situation,  this   is  called  social  referencing  (Feinman,  1982).  When  infants  reference  and  subsequently   use   the   new   understanding   of   an   ambiguous   situation   to   alter   their   behaviour,   this   is   called  modeling  (Murray  et  al.,  2009).    

Many   studies   have   investigated   this   phenomenon   with   young   children   to   see   whether  they  are  influenced  by  their  parents’  reaction  to  a  non-­‐social  stimulus  or  social   stimulus   (De   Rosnay,   Cooper,   Tsigaris,   &   Murray,   2006;   Feinman   &   Lewis,   1983;   Mumme,   Fernald,   &   Herrera,   1996;   Murray   et   al.,   2008;   Walden   &   Ogan,   1988).   An   example  of  a  social  referencing  study  with  a  non-­‐social  stimulus  is  the  study  of  Gerull   and   Rapee   (2002).   Here,   30   toddlers   were   presented   with   two   fear-­‐provoking   animal   toys,  which  were  alternately  combined  with  negative  or  positive  maternal  reactions  to   the  toy,  such  as  vocal,  facial,  and  bodily  gestures.  The  key  aim  of  this  study  was  to  see  if   fear/avoidance  behaviour  of  the  child  in  reaction  to  the  toy  would  persist  when  mother   stopped  her  negative  emotional  signalling  after  a  1-­‐minute  and  10-­‐minute  delay.  Results   indicated  that  children  indeed  show  significantly  increased  fear  and  avoidance  to  a  toy   after   negative   maternal   signalling   as   opposed   to   positive   maternal   signalling.   These  

(12)

differences   were   also   significant   after   the   1-­‐minute   and   10-­‐minute   delay,   suggesting   that   the   children   formed   an   association   between   the   toy   and   mothers’   reaction.   In   a   similar   study   (Engliston   &   Rapee,   2007),   positive   maternal   modelling   towards   a   fear-­‐ relevant  stimulus  decreased  the  fear  acquisition  of  toddlers  in  an  experimental  design.   Here,   77   toddlers   and   their   mothers   where   assigned   to   one   of   three   pre-­‐exposure   conditions:  a  modelling  group,  a  stimulus-­‐only  group  and  a  control  group.  The  control   group  was  not  presented  with  a  fear-­‐relevant  stimulus  but  was  allowed  free  play  time.   Results  revealed  that  toddlers  in  the  positive  modelling  condition  significantly  differed   from  the  two  other  groups:  these  toddlers  showed  increased  positive  reactions  to  the   toy  in  both  the  post-­‐test  and  follow-­‐up  trial  compared  to  the  baseline  trial.  Toddlers  in   the  stimulus  only  and  control  condition  showed  decreased  positive  reactions  towards   the  toy  during  post-­‐test  and  follow-­‐up  trial.  These  results  suggest  that  positive  maternal   modelling   can   be   seen   as   a   protective   factor   in   the   acquisition   of   fear   in   toddlers   (Engliston  &  Rapee,  2007).    

Social  referencing  studies  using  a  social  stimulus  examine  infants’  response  to  an   unfamiliar  person.  For  example,  De  Rosnay  et  al.  (2006)  examined  infants’  response  to  a   stranger  in  an  experimental  design.  First  the  stranger  approached  and  interacted  with   the  mother  in  full  view  of  the  infant.  Next,  the  stranger  approached  the  infant  while  the   mother   was   asked   to   disregard   the   child   and   read   a   magazine.   This   experiment   was   conducted  with  each  mother-­‐infant  pair  twice,  each  time  with  a  different  male  stranger.   On  one  occasion,  mothers  were  asked  to  respond  to  the  stranger  in  a  socially  anxious   way,  and  on  the  other  occasion,  mothers  were  asked  to  respond  in  a  non-­‐anxious  way.   Mothers  were  previously  trained  to  act  socially  anxious  and  non-­‐socially  anxious  during   the   interaction.   Infants’   responses   towards   the   stranger   showed   significantly   more   fearfulness   and   avoidance,   and   showed   less   positive   emotional   tone   after   the   socially   anxious  mother-­‐stranger  interaction,  compared  to  their  responses  to  the  stranger  after   the  non-­‐socially  anxious  mother-­‐stranger  interaction.    

Another   social   referencing   study   using   a   social   stimulus   (Murray   et   al.,   2008)   examined   the   intergenerational   transmission   of   social   anxiety   using   a   longitudinal   design.   The   response   given   to   a   stranger   by   infants   of   mothers   with   (N   =   79)   and   without  social  phobia  (N  =  77),  were  assessed  at  10  months  and  14  months  of  age.  At   each  time,  a  female  stranger  first  approached  the  mother  for  a  short  conversation  in  full   view  of  the  infant.  Next,  the  stranger  approached  and  interacted  shortly  with  the  infant,  

(13)

and  subsequently  picked-­‐up  the  child,  while  the  mother  could  intervene  whichever  way   she   pleased.   Results   revealed   that,   at   10   and   at   14   months,   socially   anxious   mothers   where  more  anxious  towards  the  stranger  in  both  mother-­‐stranger  and  infant-­‐stranger   interactions,   and   these   mothers   where   less   engaged   with   the   stranger   during   the   mother-­‐stranger   interaction   and   less   encouraging   during   the   infant-­‐stranger   interaction.   Infants   of   socially   anxious   mothers   showed   increased   avoidance   and   fearfulness   towards   the   stranger   over   the   four-­‐month   period,   compared   to   infants   of   non-­‐socially   anxious   mothers.   These   two   studies   have   found   evidence   for   infants’   capability   of   using   maternal   emotional   signals   in   a   direct   and   indirect   manner   and   subsequently  altering  their  own  behaviour  during  their  infant-­‐stranger  interaction.    

Another  type  of  stimulus  used  in  social  referencing  studies  is  the  visual  cliff.  The   visual  cliff  is  a  large  table  with  a  variable  drop-­‐off  in  the  middle,  both  sides  covered  in   chequered   linoleum.   The   entire   table   is   covered   with   a   Plexiglas   surface.   The   shallow   side  has  the  Plexiglas  surface  directly  on  top  of  the  chequered  surface,  and  on  the  deep   side  the  Plexiglas  hangs  some  distance  above  the  chequered  surface.  The  visual  cliff  was   first   used   in   studies   examining   depth   perception   and   fear   of   heights   in   animals   and   infants,  and  later  on  used  to  examine  social  referencing  behaviours  in  infants  (Adolph  &   Kretch,   2012).   In   a   renowned   social   referencing   study   by   Sorce,   Emde,   Campos   and   Klinnert  (1985),  infants  were  placed  on  the  shallow  side  of  the  visual  cliff  while  mother   stood  behind  the  deep  side  of  the  cliff,  on  the  other  side.  Mother  placed  an  attractive  toy   in   front   of   her   on   the   deep   side   of   the   cliff   and   was   instructed   to   show   a   previously   trained   happy,   fearful,   interested,   angry   or   sad   facial   expression   when   her   infant   approached   the   edge   in   the   middle   of   the   visual   cliff.   In   the   happy   and   interested   condition,   most   infants   crossed   the   cliff   (74%   and   73%),   while   none   of   the   infants   crossed  in  the  fearful  condition  and  only  few  (11%)  crossed  in  the  anger  condition.  In   the  sadness  condition  11%  of  the  infants  crossed  the  cliff.  During  pilot-­‐testing,  different   cliff  heights  were  used  to  define  an  appropriate  height  in  which  infants  were  hesitant   but   not   completely   avoidant   of   the   cliff,   and   where   they   referenced   their   mothers   frequently.  This  was  a  drop  of  30  cm.  In  addition,  a  different  group  of  infants  was  placed   on  the  table  with  no  drop-­‐off  on  the  other  side,  and  mothers  were  instructed  to  pose  a   fearful  expression  when  their  infant  reached  the  middle  of  the  table.  All  infants  crossed   the   table,   of   which   83%   did   so   without   referencing   their   mothers   at   all.   This   study  

(14)

suggests   that   infants   only   reference   when   the   situation   is   ambiguous   enough   and   are   influenced  in  their  behaviour  by  maternal  emotional  signalling  (Sorce  et  al.,  1985).    

A  different  study  using  the  visual  cliff  in  a  social  referencing  paradigm,  examined   the   strength   of   different   types   of   cues   coming   from   mothers   (Vaish   &   Striano,   2004).   Expressing   only   positive   emotional   signals,   mothers   encouraged   their   infants   to   cross   the   visual   cliff   using   three   different   types   of   cues:   facial   expressions   only,   vocal   expressions  only  and  a  third  condition  of  facial  and  vocal  cues  simultaneously.  Infants   crossed  the  cliff  in  all  three  conditions,  but  crossed  the  cliff  faster  in  the  voice  plus  vocal   condition   and   vocal   only   condition   compared   to   the   voice   only   condition,   suggesting   that  a  vocal  cue  is  more  potent  than  a  facial  cue  alone.    

 

This  study  

In   the   current   study   it   was   hypothesized   that   there   is   a   relation   between   parental   anxiety  and  infant  anxiety  and  that  parental  anxiety  is  related  to  parenting  behaviours.   It   was   furthermore   hypothesized   that   parenting   behaviours   have   different   effects   on   infant   anxiety   in   the   visual   cliff   paradigm   depending   on   parent   gender   due   to   evolutionary  based  roles.  According  to  evolutionary  hypotheses,  fathers  are  specialized   in   being   more   challenging   whilst   mothers   tend   to   be   more   caregiving   (Möller   et   al.,   2013).  When  parents  follow  up  on  these  models,  their  role  in  the  socialization  of  their   offspring  can  be  a  protective  factor  in  their  child’s  development  of  anxiety.  When  they   do   not,   possibly   due   to   their   own   anxiety,   this   may   become   a   risk   factor   for   the   development  of  child  anxiety  (Bögels  &  Perotti,  2010;  Bögels  &  Phares,  2008).    

In   this   study,   an   explanation   for   the   intergenerational   transmission   of   anxiety   through   a   possible   mediating   role   of   parenting   behaviours   is   sought.   Challenging,   overprotective   and   overcontrolling   parenting   behaviours   of   fathers   and   mothers   are   linked  to  the  anxiety  displayed  by  parent  and  infant  during  the  visual  cliff  experiment.   Parenting   behaviours   of   fathers   and   mothers   were   assessed   during   a   free   play   task   conducted   prior   to   the   visual   cliff   task.   Parenting   behaviours   were   also   assessed   by   means   of   questionnaires.   Although   the   parenting   behaviour   measures   were   retrieved   from   a   different   task   than   the   anxiety   measures,   it   was   assumed   that   the   parenting   behaviours  displayed  during  the  free  play  task  were  comparable  to  the  behaviours  the  

(15)

infants   have   grown   accustomed   to   from   their   parents   in   their   home-­‐setting   (Gardner,   2000).    

For   mediation   to   occur,   four   conditions   should   hold   (Baron   &   Kenny,   1986).   First,   the   predictor   variable   parental  anxiety   should   significantly   predict   the   mediator   variable   parenting   behaviour.   Second,   the   predictor   variable   should   also   significantly   predict   the   proposed   outcome   variable   infant   anxiety.   Third,   the   mediator   variable   should   significantly   predict   the   outcome   variable   and   fourth;   the   predictor   variable   should   significantly   predict   the   outcome   variable   less   strongly   with   the   mediator   variable   present   in   the   model.   These   conditions   are   tested   in   the   following   research   questions.  

The  first  question  addressed  is  the  following.  Is  parental  anxiety  associated  with  

challenging,   overprotective   and   overcontrolling   parenting   behaviours?   It   was  

hypothesized   that   anxiety   of   the   parent   is   associated   with   the   challenging,   overprotective  and  overcontrolling  parenting  behaviours  they  might  show.  Parents  with   higher  rates  of  anxiety  might  show  more  overprotective  and  overcontrolling  behaviours   and  less  challenging  parenting  behaviours  than  parents  with  lower  levels  of  anxiety.    

The  second  question  is:  Is  parental  anxiety  associated  with  infant  anxiety?  It  was   hypothesized   that   anxiety   of   the   parent   is   positively   associated   with   infant   anxiety.   Thus,  more  parental  anxiety  was  expected  to  be  related  to  more  infant  anxiety  and  less   parental  anxiety  was  expected  to  be  related  to  less  infant  anxiety.  

Thirdly:   Is   parenting   behaviour   associated   with   infant   anxiety?   It   was   hypothesized  that  challenging,  overprotective  and  overcontrolling  parenting  behaviours   are   associated   with   the   anxiety   of   the   child.   Challenging   parenting   behaviours   were   expected  to  relate  negatively  to  infant  anxiety:  more  challenging  parenting  behaviours   were   thought   to   be   associated   with   less   infant   anxiety.   Overprotective   and   overcontrolling   parenting   behaviours   were   expected   to   relate   positively   with   infant   anxiety:  more  overprotective  and  overcontrolling  parenting  behaviours  were  thought  to   be  associated  with  more  infant  anxiety.    

Fourth:   Does   parenting   behaviour   mediate   the   relationship   between   parental  

anxiety   and   infant   anxiety?   It   was   hypothesized   that   the   three   different   parenting  

behaviours   (challenging,   overprotective   and   overcontrolling   parenting),   mediate   this   relationship.   In   other   words,   the   positive   relationship   between   parental   and   infant   anxiety  is  better  explained  by  its  relationship  to  parenting  behaviours.  More  anxiety  in  

(16)

parents   is   associated   with   a   decrease   in   their   challenging   behaviours   and   increase   in   their  overcontrolling  and  overprotective  behaviours.  In  turn,  less  challenging  and  more   overcontrolling  and  overprotective  parenting  behaviours  are  expected  to  be  associated   with  infants  showing  more  anxiety.    

Fifth:   Are  these  relations  different  for  fathers  and  mothers?   It   was   hypothesized   that   these   mechanisms   work   in   different   ways   for   fathers   and   mothers   due   to   evolutionary  specializations  of  men  and  women  (Bögels  &  Phares,  2008;  Möller  et  al.,   2013).  Paternal  challenging  behaviours  were  expected  to  be  associated  with  less  infant   anxiety,  while  maternal  challenging  behaviours  were  expected  to  not  be  associated  with   infant  anxiety  or  even,  in  accordance  to  Majdandžić  et  al.  (2014),  in  the  opposite  way.   Paternal  overprotective  and  overcontrolling  parenting  behaviours  were  expected  to  be   associated   with   more   infant   anxiety   and   so   were   maternal   overprotective   parenting   behaviours.  Therefore,  all  analyses  will  be  done  separately  for  fathers  and  mothers  and   challenging,  overprotective  and  overcontrolling  parenting  behaviours  (see  Figure  1  and     2).   In   accordance   with   these   hypotheses,   paternal   anxiety   was   expected   to   have   a   stronger  effect  on  infant  anxiety  than  mothers,  through  the  mediating  role  of  parenting   behaviours.           Paternal   parenting   behaviour             Maternal   parenting   behaviour                                                   Paternal  

anxiety         anxiety  Infant     Maternal  anxiety         anxiety  Infant  

                     

Figure  1.  Mediation  by  paternal  challenging  

behaviour.     Figure  2.  Mediation  by  maternal  challenging  behaviour.  

 

Method   Participants    

Participants   were   81   infants   (40   girls   and   41   boys)   between   10   and   15   months   who   were  randomly  invited  to  the  lab  once,  with  father  (N  =  41)  or  mother  (N  =  40).  In  table   1,  characteristics  of  infants  and  their  parents  that  visited  the  lab  are  illustrated.  First,   total  and  mean  scores  are  presented  of  all  participants.  Next,  total  and  mean  scores  are   divided   for   infants   participating   with   mother   or   with   father.   Visiting   fathers   and  

(17)

mothers  differed  only  in  their  fulltime  work  status,  with  a  significantly  larger  amount  of   fathers   working   fulltime   than   mothers,   which   is   characteristic   for   the   situation   in   the   Netherlands  (Wielers  &  Raven,  2013).    

 

Table  1.  

Characteristics  of  participants  

Infants   Total     N  =  81   With  mother     N  =  40   With  father     N  =  41   t  (df)  or     Χ2  (df)   Girls  –  N  (%)   40  (49,4%)   19  (47,5%)   21  (51,2%)   .11  (1)  

Age  infant;  in  months  –  M  (SD)   11,88  (1,25)   11,88  (1,30)   11,87  (1,21)   .03  (79)  

Parent   Total     N  =  81   Mothers     N  =  40   Fathers     N  =  41   t  (df)  or     Χ2  (df)  

Age  parent;  in  years  –  M  (SD)   35,66  (4,82)   34,84  (3,96)   36,39  (5,49)   -­‐1.42  (76)   Number  of  children  –  M  (SD)   1,45  (.77)   1,36  (.67)   1,54  (.85)   -­‐1.03  (76)   Married/living  together  –  N  (%)   75  (92,6%)   37  (92,5%)   38  (92,7%)   .00  (1)   Born  in  Netherlands  –  N  (%)   61  (75,3)   29  (72,5%)   32  (78,0%)   .34  (1)   Education  level  –  M  (SD)  a   7,23  (1.08)   7,23  (1.20)   7,23  (1,27)   .00  (76)  

Working  fulltime  –  N  (%)   31  (38,3%)   5  (12,5%)   26  (63,4%)   22.22  (1)*  

Note:  Chi  square  tests  were  performed  for  proportions,  independent  samples  t-­‐tests  for  means.  a  On  a  scale  from  0  

(primary  education)  to  8  (university).  *  p  <  .001.  

 

Procedure  

All  participants  were  recruited  by  means  of  handing  out  flyers  by  childcare  centres,  and   an   information   letter   distributed   by   the   Amsterdam   municipality   to   parents   who   just   had  a  baby.  Once  the  infant  was  able  to  crawl  at  10  to  15  months  of  age,  parents  were   contacted  to  schedule  an  appointment  to  visit  the  lab.  Parents  were  sent  a  confirmation   letter  of  the  scheduled  appointment,  information  about  the  lab  visit  and  the  first  set  of   questionnaires   (one   for   each   parent)   via   mail.   Both   parents   filled   out   questionnaires,   but  only  one  parent  visited  the  lab  due  to  memory  effects  of  the  infant  on  the  cliff.  The   visiting  parent  was  asked  to  bring  both  individually  filled  out  questionnaire  booklets  to   the   lab.   The   lab   visit   took   place   at   the   Research   centre   of   Child   Development   and   Education   where,   after   a   short   introduction   and   signing   of   the   informed   consent,   the   following  tasks  where  conducted  in  the  observation  room:  five  minutes  of  free  play  with   toys,   five   minutes   of   free   play   without   toys   and   the   visual   cliff   experiment.   All   tasks   where   filmed   with   three   video   cameras,   which   were   operated   from   the   registration   room  behind  a  one-­‐way  screen.  The  tasks  in  the  lab  lasted  approximately  30  minutes.   After  the  experimental  tasks  the  second  and  final  set  of  questionnaire  booklets  (one  for  

(18)

home   and   send   back   once   filled   out.   The   visiting   parent   was   reimbursed   all   travel   expenses,   received   a   copy   of   all   video   recordings   of   the   experimental   tasks   and   could   choose  between  a  compensation  of  €10,-­‐  or  a  donation  of  the  same  amount  to  Orange   Babies,   an   organisation   helping   HIV   infected   mothers   and   their   babies   in   Africa.   The   infant  received  a  small  toy.    

 

Measures  

Parenting  behaviours  –  self-­‐reported  

For   assessment   of   self-­‐reported   parenting   behaviours   several   scales   of   the   Comprehensive   Parenting   Behaviour   Questionnaire   1-­‐year   version   (CPBQ-­‐1;   Majdandžić,   De   Vente,   &   Bogels,   2013)   were   used.   All   items   were   rated   on   a   5-­‐point   Likert-­‐scale,   ranging   from   1   (totally   not   applicable)   to   5   (Completely   applicable).   The   questionnaire   consists   of   the   following   scales:   Challenging   behaviour,   Overprotective   behaviour,   Overcontrolling   behaviour,   Warmth,   Negativity,   Negative   discipline   and   Positive  discipline.  In  this  study,  only  Challenging  behaviour  (46  items),  Overprotective   behaviour  (18  items)  and  Overcontrolling  behaviour  (10  items)  were  used.  The  scale  for   Challenging   behaviour   consisted   of   items   regarding:   teasing,   rough-­‐and-­‐tumble   play,   encouragement   of   risk   taking,   social   daring,   competition,   encouragement   of   assertiveness,   and   challenging   modeling.   The   scale   for   Overprotective   behaviour   consisted   of   items   regarding   limiting   exposure   of   new   objects,   people   or   situations   to   the   infant   and   the   scale   for   Overcontrolling   behaviour   consisted   of   items   regarding   needlessly   helping   the   infant   or   interfering   with   the   infants   behaviour   regardless   of   their   needs.   Reliability   ranged   from   acceptable   to   good   for   most   variables.   α’s   for   Challenging   behaviour   were   .82   for   mothers   and   .79   for   fathers.   For   overprotective   parenting,  α’s  were  .75  for  mothers  and  .63  for  fathers.  For  overcontrolling  parenting   α’s  were  48  for  mothers  and  .50  for  fathers.  This  means  internal  consistency  was  quite   low  but  used  for  further  analysis  regardless.    

 

Parenting  behaviours  –  observed  

Procedure.  During  two  sessions  of  free  play,  lasting  five  minutes  per  session,  parenting  

behaviour  towards  their  infant  was  scored  in  an  unstructured  setting  while  parent  and   infant   could   get   used   to   the   laboratory   setting   in   a   comfortable   manner.   The   first  

(19)

session   was   free   play   with   toys,   entailing  a   box   with   hand   puppets,   a   small   book   that   makes   sounds,   a   soft   ball   and   a   set   of   magnet   blocks.   Parent   and   infant   were   free   to   choose   which   toys   they   played   with.   The   second   session   was   free   play   without   toys.   Here,  parent  and  infant  where  given  only  a  set  of  cushions  to  play  with.  Each  session   started  the  moment  the  test  leader  closed  the  door  after  leaving  the  observation  room.   Instructions  by  the  test  leader  entailed  parent  and  child  could  play  with/without  toys  as   they  were  used  to.  Each  session  finished  after  5  minutes.  

Coding.   A   total   of   8   different   parenting   dimensions   where   coded   in   intervals   of   one  

minute   using   a   5-­‐point   Likert   scale.   The   coding   protocol   is   based   on   the   Meso   Behavioural   Rating   System   for   Families   with   young   children   (MeBRF,   Mahoney   et   al.,   2000)   but   adapted   for   use   in   this   particular   study.   In   this   study   only   the   parenting   dimensions   challenging   parenting   behaviour,   overcontrolling   and   overprotecting  

parenting  behaviour  where  used.    

The   dimension   of   challenging   parenting   behaviour   is   new   and   based   on   the   theory   of   Paquette   (2004),   elaborately   described   earlier   in   this   paper.   Examples   of   challenging  parenting  behaviour  during  both  free  play  tasks  are:  teasing  the  infant  with   toys  or  cushions,  mildly  scaring  the  infant  with  or  without  toys,  hide-­‐and-­‐seek  games,   lifting  the  infant  up  high,  challenging  the  infant  to  walk,  tickling  and  biting  games.  These   behaviours  are  given  scores  indicating  levels  of  frequency  and  intensity,  ranging  from  1   (none  or  minimal)  to  5  (very  often  or  very  high).    

The   dimension   of   overcontrolling   parenting   behaviour   is   partly   based   on   the   Intrusiveness   scale   from   Erickson   (Egeland,   Erickson,   Clemenhagen-­‐Moon,   Hiester,   &   Korfmacher,   1990;   Erickson,   Sroufe,   &   Egeland,   1985),   and   measures   the   extent   to   which   parents   help   or   intervene   in   the   child’s   activity   when   this   is   not   necessary.   Examples  of  overcontrolling  parenting  behaviour  are:  choosing  which  toy  to  play  with   for  the  infant,  taking  away  toys  from  the  infant  to  play  with  the  toys  themselves,  offering   too   many   toys   to   the   child   at   once,   feeding   or   hugging   the   child   when   he/she   doesn’t   want  to.  These  behaviours  are  given  scores  indicating  levels  of  frequency  and  intensity,   ranging  from  1  (none  or  minimal)  to  5  (very  often  or  very  high).  

The   dimension   of   overprotective   parenting   behaviour   is   partly   based   on   the   Involvement   scale   by   Hudson   &   Rapee   (2001),   and   measures   the   extent   to   which   parents   protect   their   child   in   an   overly   manner.   This   can   be   seen   in   how   the   parent   physically  handles  his/her  infant  and  remarks  the  parent  makes.  Examples  are:  cleaning  

(20)

the  infants  face  when  this  is  not  important,  making  remarks  about  tiredness  of  the  child,   excessively   holding   the   child   when   trying   to   walk   or   crawl   to   prevent   him/her   from   falling,   excessively   making   remarks   like   ‘watch   out’,   ‘be   careful’,   ‘not   so   fast’.   These   behaviours  are  given  scores  indicating  levels  of  frequency  and  intensity,  ranging  from  1   (none  or  minimal)  to  5  (very  often  or  very  high).  

For  the  three  different  parenting  dimensions  (for  both  tasks  separately),  a  final   score   was   computed   by   averaging   across   the   five   interval   scores.   Then,   a   final   mean   score   for   challenging   behaviour   was   made   by   averaging   the   means   of   challenging   behaviour  during  both  tasks.  The  same  was  done  for  a  final  score  of  overcontrolling  and   overprotective   parenting   behaviour.   Internal   consistency   of   the   two   variables   for   challenging   parenting   was   .25,   for   the   two   variables   of   overcontrolling   parenting   .12   and  for  the  two  variables  for  overcontrolling  behaviour  .42.  Internal  consistencies  for   the  observed  parenting  behaviours  are  very  low,  but  both  tasks  can  elicit  different  types   of  parenting  behaviours  to  so  they  form  a  robust  measure  together.    

Coding  was  carried  out  by  two  trained  students.  Of  the  observed  data,  20%  was   double   coded   to   obtain   inter-­‐rater   reliability   (IRR).   IRR   for   challenging   parenting   behaviour  was  .97  during  both  tasks,  .81  for  overcontrolling  parenting  during  free  play   with  toys  and  .78  during  free  play  without  toys  and  IRR  was  1  for  overprotection  during   both  free  play  with  toys  and  without  toys,  indicating  high  levels  of  agreement.    

 

Parental  and  infant  anxiety  –  observed    

Procedure.  The  visual  cliff  is  a  table  covered  in  a  plexiglass  surface,  split  in  two  sides.  A  

shallow  side,  where  the  plexiglass  rests  directly  on  a  red  and  white  chequered  surface,   and  a  shallow  side,  where  the  chequered  surface  drops  30  cm  down  while  the  plexiglass   surface   continues   on   the   same   height.     A   20   cm   high   border,   also   made   of   plexiglass,   surrounds  the  visual  cliff  table.  The  test  leader  introduced  the  task  with  an  instruction.   The  parent  was  explained  to  place  their  infant  on  the  table  with  their  feet  touching  the   back   border   of   the   table   and   subsequently   walk   over   to   the   other   side   of   the   table   to   stand  on  the  marked  spot  on  the  floor.  The  parent  was  asked  not  to  make  any  remarks   about   the   table   until   he   or   she   was   standing   on   the   spot   and   the   child   had   looked   at   him/her.   After   the   child   had   looked   at   the   parent,   which   the   test   leader   cued   to   the   parent  by  raising  her  hand,  the  parent  could  start  encouraging  their  child  to  crawl  over   to   them.   The   parent   was   not   allowed   to   cross   his/her   arms   over   the   table   or   tap   the  

(21)

surface  to  indicate  the  safety  of  the  table.  The  test  leader  was  present  in  the  room  and   stood   behind   a   pillar   during   the   task,   making   no   eye   contact   with   parent   or   child   but   keeping  the  safety  of  the  situation  in  check.  The  task  ended  when  one  of  the  following   situations  occurred:  1)  the  child  had  crossed  the  deep  side  and  touched  the  border,  2)   the   parent   picked   up   the   child   at   the   end   of   the   deep   side,   3)   when   10   minutes   had   passed  without  the  child  reaching  the  end  of  the  cliff,  or  4)  when  the  child  became  too   upset  or  the  parent  indicated  no  longer  wanting  to  proceed  with  the  task.    

Coding   parental   anxiety.   The   measure   for   parental   anxiety   consists   of   a  

combination  of  three  measures  of  observed  anxiety.  For  the  coding  of  facial  expression   of   anxiety   the   AFFEX   system   (Izard,   Dougherty   &   Hembree,   1983)   was   used.   Facial   expressions   (e.g.   big   eyes,   lifted   eyebrows,   stiff   lips),   bodily   expressions   (e.g.   tense   posture,  raised  shoulders,  nervous  movement)  and  verbal  expressions  of  anxiety  (tone   and   content   where   both   taken   into   account,   for   example   ‘oh   oh’,   ‘watch   out!’)   were   scored  every  10-­‐second  interval  on  a  scale  from  0  to  3.  Higher  scores  indicated  a  higher   frequency  or  intensity  of  each  expression  of  anxiety  (0  =  absence  of  the  behaviour;  1  =   light  expression  of  the  behaviour;  2  =  clear  expression  of  the  behaviour;  3  =  clear  and   intense  expression  of  the  behaviour).  The  scoring  started  the  moment  the  parent  stood   on   the   marked   spot   behind   the   deep   side   of   the   cliff   and   the   infant   had   looked   at   the   parent,   when   social   referencing   could   commence.   All   interval   scores   on   each   of   the   three   expressions   of   anxiety   were   averaged   for   a   single   final   score   for   each   variable.   Internal  consistency  of  the  three  variables  (α  =  .72)  was  good;  a  mean  score  for  parental   anxiety  was  made  from  the  three  averaged  variables.    

Coding  infant  anxiety.   For   the   coding   of   facial   expression   of   anxiety   the   AFFEX  

system   (Izard,   Dougherty   &   Hembree,   1983)   was   used.   Infant   anxiety   is   similarly   a   combined  measure  of  three  separate  measures  of  observed  anxiety.  Facial  expressions   (e.g.  wide  eyes,  stiff  lips),  bodily  expression  (e.g.  stiff  muscles,  head  ducked  in  shoulders,   holding   breath)   and   vocal   expressions   of   anxiety   (e.g.   whining,   crying)   were   scored   every   10-­‐second   interval   on   a   scale   from   0   to   3.   Higher   scores   indicated   a   higher   frequency  or  intensity  of  each  expression  of  anxiety  (0  =  absence  of  that  behaviour;  1  =   light  expression  of  that  behaviour;  2  =  clear  expression  of  that  behaviour;  3  =  clear  and   intense   expression   of   that   behaviour).   The   coding   of   the   infant   variables   started   the   moment  the  parent  placed  the  infant  on  the  cliff  and  the  infant  touched  the  cliff  in  any   way.  All  interval  scores  on  each  of  the  three  expressions  of  anxiety  were  averaged  for  a  

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

This study examines the relation between CSR and total CEO compensation through the mediating effect of corporate financial performance (CFP) measured in stock

The second major finding of the study is that the link between infant avoidance and expressed parental anxiety did not significantly differ between mothers and fathers, indicating

In an earlier paper, we found evidence to suggest that there should be made a distinction between the role and function of MAs (Moossdorff, 2012). The current paper

Het negatieve effect van vormgeving van een boodschap op de attitude ten opzichte van het thema van deze boodschap wordt door politieke interesse verzwakt; het toch al

Het doel van huidig onderzoek was inzichtelijk maken op welke wijze vrijheidsbeperkende maatregelen effectiever ingezet kunnen worden bij Orthopedagogisch Behandelcentrum Reik,

For assessing the second research question, bivariate correlation analysis was completed for determining the level of relatedness between self-compassion and the three

The objective of this research is to determine how three effective leadership behaviours performed by team leaders of hierarchical teams relate to the team and individual

In short, chapter 2 investigated how teachers’ characteristics—in particular their perceptions of bullying—were related to the number of self-reported victims in their