• No results found

A spokesperson as the “face” of an organization in crisis: the effects of expressed guilt on corporate reputation

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "A spokesperson as the “face” of an organization in crisis: the effects of expressed guilt on corporate reputation"

Copied!
45
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

A spokesperson as the “face” of an organization in crisis:

The effects of expressed guilt on corporate reputation

Student Name: Tom de Groot

Student Number: 10556176

Supervisor: Joost Verhoeven

Graduate School of Communication

Master’s programme Communication Science

University of Amsterdam

Master’s Thesis

28 June 2018

(2)

ABSTRACT

Organizational crises are often very emotionally charged events. Whereas quite some

previous research has investigated emotional responses of stakeholders to a crisis, precious

little is known about the effects of expressed emotion by (spokespersons of) organizations,

while reacting to a crisis, on corporate reputations. The present study investigates to what

extent and how the expression of the emotion guilt in organizational crisis responses affects

corporate reputation. This is studied by means of a 2 (guilt message framing versus rational

message framing) x 2 (deny crisis response strategy versus rebuild crisis response strategy)

between-subjects factorial experimental design with 178 participants. Although, against

expectations, no direct positive effect of guilt message framing on corporate reputation was

found, an indirect effect of guilt message framing on corporate reputation was found via

perceived sincerity. In addition, an alignment between expressed guilt and crisis response

strategy was found to be important, but trait forgiveness in respondents appeared not to play a

significant role in the interaction between perceived sincerity and corporate reputation.

Keywords: crisis communication, emotions, message framing, corporate reputation, crisis response strategies.

Introduction

Although crises are very often emotional events, emotion has only recently been incorporated

into crisis communication research (Jin, 2009). The majority of the studies on emotions and

crisis communication focused on the emotional responses of stakeholders to a crisis rather

than the expression of emotions by organizations in their communication about a crisis (e.g.

Mcdonald, Sparks, & Glendon, 2010; Jin, Pang, & Cameron, 2012). However, a crisis may

(3)

of a crisis, organizational members may be under psychological pressure, media pressure and

time pressure and simultaneously experience emotional tumult (Van der Meer & Verhoeven,

2014). Under these conditions, unintentionally, a spokesperson may express his or her

inherent emotions.

According to Christensen and Cornelissen (2011), “parts of activities or the voice of

single individuals (e.g., managers) are taken to stand for or represent the whole organization”

(p. 385). The same might be true for spokespersons who represent an organization during a

crisis situation and express emotions over what has happened. In this way, a spokesperson

embodies the organization.Whereas spokespersons might choose to suppress emotions in

their communication about a crisis, expressing emotion in their crisis response strategies is a

better alternative since people tend to have more positive behavioural intentions towards an

organization when a corporate crisis message contains intensive emotional appeals as

compared to when a corporate crisis message contains no emotional appeals (Kim &

Cameron, 2011). Until now, not much is known about how the expression of emotion in crisis

response strategies by organizations can affect corporate reputation (Claeys, Cauberghe, &

Leysen, 2013). Corporate communication practitioners and researchers alike have long

overlooked the importance of emotional appeals in crisis communication (Van der Meer &

Verhoeven, 2014). The well managed expression of emotions by spokespersons, however, is

able to protect an organization’s reputation (Read, 2007).

The expression of the emotion guilt, in specific, could play a crucial role in protecting an organization’s reputation during a preventable crisis, in which the organization is blamed

for what has happened (Coombs, 2007). Previous studies have shown that guilt, “arguably the most exemplary moral emotion” (De Hooge, Nelissen, Breugelmans, & Zeelenberg, 2011, p.

462), stimulates prosocial behaviour toward a victim (Ketelaar & Au, 2003; Nelissen, Dijker

(4)

protect and enhance relationships by punishing wrongdoings and restore inequities

(Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatheron, 1994; Leith & Baumeister, 1998). The behaviour

following guilt is interpreted as behaviour motivated out of concern for other persons

(McCullough, Kilpatrick, Emmons, & Larson, 2001; Sheikh & Janoff-Bulman, 2010), and is

commonly linked to the stimulating of transgressors to think about how their actions influence

the well-being of other people (Haidt, 2003; Smith, 1759). In the case of a preventable crisis,

in which the organization has purposefully placed stakeholders at risk (Coombs & Holladay,

2002), expressing guilt in crisis responses may play a central role in restoring stakeholders

trust and repairing corporate reputations.

Previous research has shown that how an organization acts after a crisis, through its

crisis response strategy, goes a long way toward protecting their reputation (Coombs &

Holladay, 1996, 2008). Crisis response strategies pertain what management says and does

after a crisis, and form three groups based upon perceptions of accepting responsibility for a

crisis: denial, diminish, and rebuild (Coombs, 2006). In the present study, the deny crisis

response strategy and rebuild crisis response strategy will be employed in order to examine

the importance of alignment between crisis response strategy and expressed emotion. These

two crisis response strategies will be studied because rebuild is the most effective strategy

(Coombs, 2007; Kim, Avery, & Lariscy, 2009), whereas deny is the least effective (Coombs,

2007; Kim et al., 2009), yet the most often used strategy (Kim et al., 2009).

The key objective of this study is therefore to investigate the impact that guilt framed

crisis communication messages (compared to rationally framed crisis communication

messages) after an airplane crash due to negligence have on corporate reputation. Whereas

previous studies on emotional crisis communication have used fictional organizations, the

present study uses an existing organization, namely KLM (the flag carrier airline of the

(5)

crisis communication. An online experiment was conducted in order to fill in the gap in crisis

communication research regarding the impact of expressed guilt.. The research question is as

follows:

RQ: “To what extent and how does the expression of the emotion guilt in organizational crisis responses affect corporate reputation?”

Theoretical background

The theoretical background of this study starts with discussing the significance of protecting

corporate reputation. After that, crisis communication, chiefly based on Situational Crisis

Communication Theory (referred to as SCCT in the remainder of this paper) by Coombs

(2007), will be outlined. Third, the supposed impact of the expression of emotion/guilt in

crisis communication messages by organizations on reputation will be discussed. Fourth, light

is shed on the importance of perceived sincerity of crisis communication messages. Finally,

trait forgiveness (tendency to forgive) as a possible moderator between perceived sincerity

and corporate reputation will be discussed.

Corporate reputation

As reputation affects actions and outcomes, it is crucial for managers to find ways to shape

and manage reputation (Clardy, 2012). According to Wartick (1992), a reputation is an

aggregate evaluation stakeholders produce about to what extent an organization is meeting

stakeholder expectations based on its past behaviour. The failure of an organization to meet

expectations can be very problematic for organizations (Reichart, 2003). Reputations are

widely acknowledged as valuable, intangible assets that may permit firms to charge higher

(6)

(Fombrun & Shanley, 1990). Reputations develop through information stakeholders receive

about an organization (Fombrun & van Riel, 2004). Stakeholders receive this information

through interactions with the organization, mediated reports about the organization, and

second-hand information from other people (Coombs, 2007).

Organizational crises can be seen as the ‘moment of truth’ for reputations because they

are able to seriously harm stakeholders emotionally, physically and/or financially (Coombs,

2007). Stakeholders include employees, suppliers, customers, communities, financiers, NGOs,

environmentalists, governments, media, critics and others (Fassin, 2009). Striking examples

of organizational crises are plane crashes, ethical misdeed, financial frauds, and medical

failures. Crises tend to hurt reputations because they give stakeholders reasons to think

negatively about an organization (Coombs, 2006).

Crisis communication

Although the vast majority of crisis communication literature (e.g., Benoit, 1997, 2005, image

repair theory; Bradford & Garrett, 1995; Cameron, Pang, & Jin, 2008, contingency theory;

Coombs, 2007, SCCT) has discussed various response strategies, the crisis response strategies

that are employed by arguably most researchers are Coombs’ (2007) SCCT strategies.

SCCT offers a framework for understanding how to maximize the reputational

protection abided by post-crisis communication and includes the crisis response strategies

deny, diminish and rebuild. The deny strategy includes ‘attack the accuser’, ‘denial’, and ‘scapegoating’ (Coombs, 2007). The three tactics have in common that they try to remove any

connection between the organization and the crisis (Coombs, 2006). The diminish strategy

includes ‘excuse’ and ‘justification’. Employing a diminish strategy, a crisis manager admits

there is an organizational crisis but tries to change the attributions stakeholders make about it

(7)

involves ‘compensation’, and ‘apology’ (Coombs, 2007). Rebuild strategies try to enhance

corporate reputation by offering material and/or symbolic forms of aid to victims of a crisis,

with as goal to take positive actions and offset the crisis (Coombs, 2007).

The level of responsibility for a crisis is decisive for which crisis response strategy is

appropriate to employ (Coombs, 2007). Coombs and Holladay (2002) developed three crisis

clusters based on how responsible the organization is for a crisis, which they refer to as: “attributions of crisis responsibility” (p. 168). These three clusters are: the victim cluster

(natural disasters, rumours, workplace violence, and product tampering/malevolence), the

accidental cluster (challenges, technical-error accidents, and technical-error product harm),

and the preventable cluster (human-error accidents, human-error product harm, organizational

misdeed management conduct, organizational misdeed with no injuries, and organizational

misdeed with injuries).

Emotional crisis communication

Although researchers in psychology have extensively studied how emotions influence

cognitive processing (e.g. Tiedens, 2001; Loseth & Dahl, 2017) and interactions (e.g. Robles

et al. 2016; Vanutelli & Balconi, 2015), it was only recently that the role of emotions was

researched in the field of crisis communication, public relations, and media framing (e.g. Van

der Meer & Verhoeven, 2014; Claeys et al., 2013; Kim & Cameron, 2011).

Emotions are widely viewed as internal mental states representing evaluative, valenced

reactions to events, agents, or objects that vary in intensity (Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1988).

They are commonly short-lived, intense, and directed at external stimuli (Fiske & Taylor,

1991). Consensus in psychological research suggests that emotion consists of five components, namely: “(a) cognitive appraisal or evaluation of a situation, (b) the

(8)

including behavioural intentions or readiness, and (e) a subjective feeling state” (Nabi, 1999,

p. 295). Furthermore, emotions influence important social functions such as ensuring the

social transmission of emotional interpretations of events (Klinnert, Emde, & Campos, 1986),

behaving in a way that influences other people and that may be meant to do so, either

unintentionally, or more strategically (Frijda, 1993), and evoking reactions in others (Frijda &

Mesquita, 1994).

By expressing emotion in crisis communication messages an organization displays its

human face (Van der Meer & Verhoeven, 2014). By showing its human face, an organization

may improve relationships with its stakeholders (Kelleher, 2009).The psychological process

of attributing human emotions, traits or intentions to non-human entities is also known as

anthropomorphism (Guthrie, 1993). People anthropomorphize nonhuman agents to achieve

mastery of an otherwise uncertain environment, making it more predictable and

understandable (Waytz & Morewedge, 2010). Furthermore, connecting brands with human

qualities directs stakeholders toward developing emotions parallel to other human

relationships (Kim, Park, & Kim, 2014). By humanizing brands, for instance by expressing

emotion in corporate communication, firms and organizations are thus able to gain stronger

attachments with consumers and stakeholders (Wen & Song, 2017).

Emotions may be expressed through the use of language, non-verbal communication,

such as facial expressions (Ekman, 1993), gestures, and/or posture (Wallbott, 1998). They are

an important form of communication, as they uncover information about the sender (Van der Meer & Verhoeven, 2014). The expression of emotion indicates to receivers the sender’s

emotional potential of a situation, whether the sender’s appraisal conformed to the norms or deviated from them, and defines the sender’s social position and role within one’s social

structure (Frijda & Mesquita, 1994). Research in the field of psychology has shown that

(9)

estimation of the sender’s intentions and actions toward them (Lewis, 2000; Van Kleef, De

Dreu, & Manstead, 2004).

Guilt is one of the few emotions that requires self-evaluation of the sender (Nabi,

1999). It usually serves a relationship-enhancing function and is characterized by an urge to

heal the situation, and cognitive preoccupation with reckoning ways to make things right so

the guilt may be vanished (Izard, 1977; Roseman, Wiest, & Swartz, 1994). Guilt can have

beneficial consequences because it makes people put the concerns of others above their own

(Haidt, 2003). Reparative action inclinations such as confessions, apologies, and attempts to

undo the harm done are targeted at restoring the relationship between the transgressor and the

victim (Caplovitz Barrett, 1995; Lewis, 1971, 1987; Lindsay-Hartz, 1984). When referring to

guilt, it is important to note that no guilt in a legal sense or blame needs to be involved, for

someone to feel and/or express it. Frijda and Mesquita (1994) give the example of the

long-lasting distress caused by having accidently killed a child who suddenly crossed the street in front of the driver’s car: even though it was not the driver’s fault to kill the child, he or she

might feel extremely guilty for the horrible thing that has happened.

Based on the fact that people who receive emotions communicated by a sender use this information to shape an estimation of the sender’s intentions and actions toward them (Lewis,

2000; Van Kleef et al., 2004), it is expected that when an organization expresses guilt in a

crisis response, stakeholders presume that the organization sincerely tries to rebuild the

damaged relationship with them, which will likely result in a less negative corporate

reputation. Hence, the following hypothesis is proposed:

H1. Guilt message framing results in a less negative corporate reputation than rational message framing.

(10)

Expressing emotion in a crisis response may not only directly influence corporate

reputation (in its function as a piece of information for the public), it may also impact how stakeholders interpret an organization’s crisis response message. Based on the maintaining

and repairing functions of guilt (e.g. Baumeister et al., 1994; Lewis, 1971), as well as the argument that people use received emotion to shape an estimation of a sender’s intentions and

actions toward them (Lewis, 2000; Van Kleef et al., 2004) it is expected that the combination

of expressed guilt and a rebuild response strategy after an organizational crisis will lead to a

less negative corporate reputation. The expressed emotion, guilt, would then be aligned with the organization’s intentions and actions: rebuilding the damaged relationship after their

wrongdoing. The following hypothesis is proposed:

H2a. Guilt message framing in combination with a rebuild crisis response strategy results in a less negative corporate reputation than rational message framing in combination with a

rebuild crisis response strategy.

Van der Meer and Verhoeven (2013) suggested that future researchers should

investigate whether less effective strategies become significantly more efficacious with the

communication of emotion. In the present study, it is however not expected that when guilt is

expressed in combination with a deny crisis response strategy, this will result in a more

positive corporate reputation. This is not expected because a mismatch between

communicated emotion and crisis response strategy might confuse stakeholders, likely

making them question the sincerity of the message’s content as well as the expressed emotion

(Van der Meer & Verhoeven, 2013). In this case, expressed guilt may not be perceived as

authentic, sincere and trustworthy because no action inclinations such as confessions,

apologies, and attempts to undo the harm are yielded by the organization in order to restore

(11)

H2b. Guilt message framing in combination with a deny crisis response strategy does not result in a less negative corporate reputation compared to rational message framing in

combination with a deny crisis response strategy.

On the contrary, and in line with hypothesis H2a., when the expression of guilt is

combined with a rebuild crisis response strategy, this may be perceived as trustworthy and in

sync by stakeholders. Caza, Zhang, Wang, & Bai (2015) found that leaders who were judged

more sincere, were simultaneously judged more trustworthy. Therefore, it is expected that

when the spokesperson expresses guilt in combination with a rebuild strategy, stakeholders

are more likely to believe the crisis message than when the spokesperson expresses guilt in

combination with a deny strategy. The following hypothesis is proposed:

H3. Guilt message framing in combination with a rebuild crisis response strategy results in higher perceived sincerity than guilt message framing in combination with a deny crisis

response strategy.

Perceived sincerity

Case studies by several authors indicate that the positive influence of emotional message

framing can be attributed to an increase in perceived (organizational) sincerity (e.g. Benoit &

Brinson, 1999; Kauffman, 2008; Legg, 2009; Claeys et al. 2013; Caza, et al., 2015). The

communication of emotion(s) can improve the degree to which an organization is perceived as

sincere (Caza et al., 2015) and trustworthy (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). Subsequently,

information obtained from a trustworthy sender has a greater impact than information

obtained from an untrustworthy sender (Hovland & Weiss, 1951). Kim (2011), for example,

found that sincerity of a message, in a corporate communication context, can positively impact stakeholders’ evaluations of an organization. A good example of this are corporate

(12)

social responsibility (CSR) activities; they are only positive for corporate reputation when

consumers or stakeholders attribute sincere motives to the organization’s CSR actions (Yoon,

Gürhan-Canli, & Schwarz, 2006).

In relation to the above, Sandlin and Gracyalny (2018) found that positive judgements about a public figure’s reputation related to perceptions of apology sincerity. An attempt to be

forgiven is considered sincere if it is perceived by the recipient as heartfelt and if the emotion

that is expressed appears genuine (Risen & Gillovich, 2007). Based on the assumptions made

in previous research about the impact that perceived sincerity of (emotional) messages has on

forgiveness, trust and reputation, it is expected that the positive impact of expressed guilt on an organization’s corporate reputation may be due to a mediating effect of perceived sincerity.

The following hypothesis is proposed:

H4. The positive effect of guilt message framing on corporate reputation is mediated by perceptions of sincerity.

Trait forgiveness

Forgiveness has been identified as one of the most efficacious instruments of repairing

relationships after a transgression (Adams & Inesi, 2016). Whereas a transgression causes a

debt that the transgressor owes to the victim, forgiveness imparts the victim’s willingness to

renounce that debt and repair the relationship (Exline et al., 2004). A longitudinal study by

Riek, Luna, and Schnabelrauch (2014) showed that guilt forecasts forgiveness-seeking behaviours. Transgressor’s guilt then acts as the key psychological steppingstone between

intent and desires for forgiveness (Adams & Inesi, 2016). Tendency to forgive likely differs

from person to person; whereas some people might forgive others relatively easy for their

wrongdoing, other people might have more trouble with this. Desmet, De Cremer, and Van

(13)

extent to which perceived remorse functions as a prerequisite for trust and cooperation. In line

with this, it is expected that the effect that perceived sincerity of the crisis communication

messages in this study have on corporate reputation is moderated by trait forgiveness in

respondents. Hence, the following hypothesis is proposed:

H5. The positive effect of perceived sincerity on corporate reputation is moderated by the trait forgiveness in respondents. Figure 1. Conceptual Framework

Methods

Design

This study uses a 2 (guilt versus rational message framing) x 2 (deny versus rebuild crisis

response strategy) between-subjects factorial experimental design to investigate the

hypotheses. Message Framing (Guilt vs. Rational) Corporate Reputation Perceived Sincerity

Crisis Response Strategy (Rebuild vs. Deny)

Trait Forgiveness

(14)

Stimuli

The crisis situation involved a fictitious plane crash in Stockholm, Sweden. Four scenarios

manipulated Message Framing and Crisis Response Strategy. Participants read a newspaper

article about the plane crash: 71 people had died when a new Boeing 737 plane of KLM

coming in on Stockholm Arlanda Airport swerved off a runway, crashed, and burst into flames. In the end of the newspaper article it was mentioned that several new Boeing 737’s

had problems with the landing gear, and that flight operators were advised to inspect their

planes. After the newspaper article, participants were exposed to one of the four conditions:

the crisis communication response by the spokesperson of KLM about the airplane crash.

These existed out of a combination of ‘no emotion + deny’, ‘guilt + rebuild’, ‘guilt + deny’, and ‘no emotion + rebuild’. The newspaper article (see Appendix A. for full newspaper

article) existed out of text, and the crisis responses by the spokesperson of KLM (see

Appendix B. for all four crisis responses) existed out of text accompanied with a picture

showing the facial expression of the spokesperson.

Participants and procedure

The participants for this experiment were recruited from the researcher’s own network

through Facebook, WhatsApp and E-mail, resulting in a sample of (N = ) 178 respondents

who fully completed the questionnaire. Participants received a message inviting them to fill in

an online questionnaire in Qualtrics. They were randomly divided across the four conditions.

At the start of the questionnaire, respondents were asked to rate KLM’s corporate reputation

(pre-crisis). After that, they were exposed to the newspaper article and asked to rate corporate

reputation (post-crisis) again. Finally, they were exposed to the crisis response by KLM and

asked to rate corporate reputation (post-response) one last time. After having read the crisis

(15)

well as their personal tendency to forgive. The questionnaire ended with a manipulation check

and demographics (see Appendix C. for the full questionnaire).

Participants had an average age of 26.80 years (SD = 0.75). Furthermore, 51.10

percent of the respondents were male (N = 91) and 48.90 percent were female (N = 87). Most

participants originated from The Netherlands (N = 146), followed by Germany (N = 5), Italy

(N = 5), and France (N = 3). The majority of them, 88.2 percent, went to higher education (HBO/university). In order to check if respondent’s age, gender, level of education and

nationality was comparable between the four conditions, randomization checks were

conducted. First, an One-Way ANOVA indicated that the mean age between the four

conditions was not significantly different: F(3, 174) = 2.03, p = 0,112. Second, three

Chi-Square tests for gender (X2 (3) = 3.32, p = 0.345), level of education (X2 (9) = 4.94, p = 0.839)

and nationality (X2 (54) = 56.80, p = 0.371) indicated that they were not significantly different

between the four conditions either.

Variables

Message Framing (independent variable). In the guilt framed crisis message, the

spokesperson expressed that KLM feels “extremely guilty” for the “horrible” plane crash. The emotion guilt was expressed through language (e.g. explicitly stating to feel “extremely

guilty” as well as using emotional loaded adjectives such as “horrendous tragedy” and

“beloved passengers”) and facial expression. The rationally framed message on the other hand

was more direct and presented the same information in a more straightforward and objective manner (e.g. “incident” instead of “horrible crash” and “passengers” instead of “beloved

passengers”). Besides that, the spokesperson had an expressionless face.

Crisis Response Strategy (moderator). In the deny crisis response strategy, the

(16)

responsibility for the incident” and “Boeing failed to deliver us a safe plane”. In the rebuild

crisis response strategy, the spokesperson of KLM used apology and compensation in his

crisis response: “KLM apologises for the crash” and “we will provide professional support”.

Perceived Sincerity (mediator). Perceived Sincerity was measured by asking the

participants to rate the sincerity of the spokesperson’s crisis response. It was measured using a

7-point Likert scale by Aaker (1997). The scales ranged from (1) “Not at all descriptive” to (7) “Extremely descriptive”. The scale had to be filled in for the following traits: (1) honest,

(2) sincere, and (3) sentimental. After a Principal Component Analysis (referred to as PCA in

the remainder of this paper) and Reliability Analysis including the three items to check if the variable is reliable with a Cronbach’s Alpha higher than 0.70 (see results in Table 1.), one

workable variable was computed for Perceived Sincerity. A Descriptives analysis showed that

the new variable has a minimum of 1.00 and a maximum of 6.33 (M = 4.14, SD = 1.18).

Perceived Sincerity was not perfectly normally distributed with a Shapiro-Wilk of p = 0.005.

Trait Forgiveness (moderator). The Heartland Forgiveness Scale by Thompson,

Snyder, and Hoffman (2005) was employed to measure trait forgiveness in respondents. The

scale included items as “With time I am understanding of others for the mistakes they’ve made” and “When someone disappoints me, I can eventually move past it” (see Appendix C7.

for the other two items which were reversed coded). Respondents had to indicate on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from (1) “Almost always false of me” to (7) “Almost always true of me”,

whether they agreed or not. Before computing the variable Trait Forgiveness, again a PCA

and Reliability Analysis were run including the four items to make sure the variable is reliable

(see results in Table 1.). A Descriptives analysis showed that the newly computed variable has

a minimum of 1.50 and a maximum of 7.00 (M = 4.80, SD = 1.03). As for Perceived

Sincerity, Trait Forgiveness was not perfectly normally distributed with a Shapiro-Wilk of p =

(17)

Table 1. Results of PCA and Reliability Analyses

Variable Eigenvalue % of Variance

explained Number of Items Cronbach’s Alpha Pre-crisis Corporate Reputation 2.80 (1 factor) 56.03 5 (>0.45) 0.80 Post-crisis Corporate Reputation 2.74 (1 factor) 54.71 5 (>0.45) 0.79 Post-response Corporate Reputation 3.07 (1 factor) 61.37 5 (>0.45) 0.94 Perceived Sincerity 2.05 (1 factor) 68.37 3 (>0.45) 0.76 Trait Forgiveness 2.26 (1 factor) 56.61 4 (>0.45) 0.74

Corporate Reputation (dependent variable). The Organizational Reputation Scale

(Coombs & Holladay, 2002) was used to measure Corporate Reputation. The scale included

items as “KLM is concerned with the well-being of its public” and “Under most

circumstances, I would be likely to believe what KLM says” (see Appendix C2. for the other

three items which were reversed coded). The anchors for the scale ranged between (1)

Strongly disagree and (5) Strongly agree. Pre-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.85, SD =

0.55), Post-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.43, SD = 0.62) and Post-response Corporate

Reputation (M = 3.35, SD = 0.73) were all reliable. In Table 1. you can find the results of the

PCA and Reliability Analyses. It must be noted that results, again, have to be interpreted

carefully because the Shapiro-Wilk was p = 0.000 for all three: the variables were thus not

perfectly normally distributed.

Multicollinearity tests were done to check whether there are no high intercorrelations

(18)

Forgiveness). This was done by means of performing a Regression analysis three times using “Collinearity diagnostics”. Since all scores (VIF = 1.00 for Trait Forgiveness, VIF = 1.05 for

Crisis Response Strategy and VIF = 1.01 for Message Framing) were lower than 3.00, no

multicollinearity issues were observed.

Manipulation check

Previous to sending out the online experiment to all participants, a small-scaled test was done

by asking sixteen acquaintances to fill in the questionnaire and note down feedback. This

feedback was then used to improve the questionnaire and create the final version.

A manipulation check was performed in order to find out whether the respondents

were aware of the fact that the crisis communication message they read either included a

Deny Crisis Response Strategy or a Rebuild Crisis Response Strategy and either Guilt

Message Framing or Rational Message Framing. Respondents had to indicate on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from (1) “Strongly disagree” to (5) “Strongly agree” whether they

(dis)agreed with items as “The spokesperson of KLM used compensation and apology in the

crisis communication message” and “The spokesperson of KLM was emotional and expressed

guilt in the crisis communication message” (see Appendix C8. for the full scale).

An Independent-Samples T-Test was performed in order to find out whether the

manipulation had been successful. The results confirmed that respondents realized that KLM’s spokesperson used a Deny Crisis Response Strategy (M = 3.89, SD = 0.98) instead of

a Rebuild Crisis Response Strategy (M = 1.98, SD = 0.93) in the deny conditions (Levene’s

Test indicated equal variances: F = 0.63, p = 0.430): t(176) = 13.31, p = 0.000, and a Rebuild

Crisis Response Strategy (M = 3.73, SD = 0.93) instead of a Deny Crisis Response Strategy

(M = 2.52, SD = 1.11) in the rebuild conditions (Levene’s Test indicated unequal variances: F

(19)

KLM’s spokesperson expressed guilt (M = 3.62, SD = 1.00) instead of no emotion (M = 2.40,

SD = 1.15) in the guilt conditions (Levene’s Test indicated equal variances: F = 2.91, p =

0.090): t(176) = -7.55, p = 0.000, and that KLM’s spokesperson expressed no emotion (M =

3.51, SD = 0.99) instead of guilt (M = 2.55, SD = 1.00) in the rational conditions (Levene’s

Test indicated equal variances: F = 0.08, p = 0.774): t(176) = 6.39, p = 0.000. The

manipulation check was thus successful.

Results

Pre-crisis, post-crisis, and post-response corporate reputation

Because corporate reputation was measured at three different stages in the questionnaire, first

a Paired Samples T-Test was run in order to see whether corporate reputation got significantly

worse after respondents read the newspaper article about the KLM crash in Stockholm

compared to before they read the newspaper article. There was indeed a significant change

between Pre-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.85, SD = 0.55) and Post-crisis Corporate

Reputation (M = 3.43, SD = 0.62): t(177) = 10.35, p = 0.000. After that, a second Paired

Samples T-Test was run including Post-crisis Corporate Reputation and Post-response

Corporate Reputation to see whether corporate reputation changed significantly after

respondents read the crisis response. In Figure 2. you can see that in the ‘no emotion + deny’

condition, Post-response Corporate Reputation was significantly worse than Post-crisis

Corporate Reputation, in the ‘guilt + rebuild’ condition, a distinct (positive) trend toward

significance occurred between Post-crisis Corporate Reputation and Post-response Corporate

Reputation, and in the ‘guilt + deny’ condition and ‘no emotion + rebuild’ condition. The

changes between Post-crisis Corporate Reputation and Post-response Corporate Reputation

(20)

Figure 2. Results of Paired- Samples T-Test for Corporate Reputation T1 T2 T3

No motion + deny Guilt + rebuild Guilt + deny No emotion + rebuild

Testing of hypotheses

The conceptual model used in this paper reconciled with model 22 in PROCESS, and so an

analysis was run with the variables Y (Post-response Corporate Reputation), X (Message

Framing – categorical), M (Perceived Sincerity), W (Crisis Response Strategy – categorical),

and Z (Trait Forgiveness). In some cases, additional analyses were run next to the PROCESS

analysis.

Hypothesis H1, against all expectations, has to be rejected because no significant main

effect was found between Guilt Message Framing and Rational Message Framing on

Post-response Corporate Reputation: F(6, 171) = 0.02, B = 0.00, t = 0.02, p = 0.982. In order to

Pre-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.85, SD = 0.55) Post-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.43, SD = 0.62) t(177): 10.35, p = 0.000 Post-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.49, SD = 0.76) Post-response Corporate Reputation (M = 3.13, SD = 0.86) t(43): 3.28, p = 0.002 Post-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.38, SD = 0.54) Post-response Corporate Reputation (M = 3.56, SD = 0.60) t(43): -1.83, p = 0.074 Post-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.42, SD = 0.60) Post-response Corporate Reputation (M = 3.28, SD = 0.68) t(42): 1.31, p = 0.197 Post-crisis Corporate Reputation (M = 3.44, SD = 0.59) Post-response Corporate Reputation (M = 3.42, SD = 0.70) t(46): 0.32, p = 0.754

(21)

double check, also an Independent-Samples T-Test was run. Given a violation of the Levene’s

Test for homogeneity of variances, F(1, 176) = 2.79, p = 0.987, a T-Test not assuming

homogeneous variances was calculated. The results indicated that there was no significant

difference between Guilt Message Framing and Rational Message Framing on Post-response

Corporate Reputation: t(1, 176) = -1.34, p = 0.181.

Hypothesis H2a, which predicted that “guilt message framing in a rebuild crisis

response strategy results in a less negative corporate reputation than rational message framing

in a rebuild crisis response strategy”, has to be rejected too. An Univariate Regression using

Split File for Crisis Response Strategy with Post-Response Corporate Reputation as

dependent and Message Framing as independent variable (Levene’s Test: F(1, 89) = 1.80, p =

0.183) was insignificant: F = 1.16, p = 0.284. Hypothesis H2b, which predicted that “guilt

message framing in a deny crisis response strategy does not result in a less negative post-crisis

corporate reputation compared to rational message framing in a deny crisis response strategy”,

is accepted. The Univariate Regression (Levene’s Test: F(1, 85) = 3.68, p = 0.058) indicated

that the differences between the two was indeed insignificant for deny: F = 0.83, p = 0.365.

Hypothesis H3, which predicted that “guilt message framing in combination with a

rebuild crisis response strategy results in higher perceived sincerity than guilt message

framing in combination with a deny crisis response strategy”, is rejected: the results of an

Univariate Regression using Split File for Message Framing with Perceived Sincerity as dependent variable and Crisis Response Strategy as independent variable (Levene’s Test: F(1,

85) = 0.53, p = 0.470) indicated an insignificant difference between deny and rebuild: F =

3.18, p = 0.078.

In regards to hypothesis H4, which predicted that “the positive effect of guilt message

framing on post-crisis corporate reputation is mediated by perceptions of sincerity”, the

(22)

Corporate Reputation through perceived sincerity. According to the confidence intervals (CI =

0.08, 0.34) there is a 95 percent chance that there is a positive effect that is higher than 0.00.

Furthermore, both the direct effect of Guilt Message Framing on Perceived sincerity (F(3,

174) = 7.62, B = 0.72, t = 2.99, p = 0.003) and Perceived Sincerity on Post-response

Corporate Reputation (F(6, 171) = 12.85, B = 0.48, t = 2.67, p = 0.008) were significant.

Finally, hypothesis H5, which predicted that the effect of Perceived Sincerity on

Post-response Corporate Reputation is moderated by the Trait Forgiveness in respondents, is

rejected. The PROCESS results indicated that the interaction effect between Perceived

Sincerity, Trait Forgiveness and Corporate Reputation was not significant: F(6, 171) = 12.85,

B = -0.04, t = -0.93 , p = 0.357.

Discussion

The objective of this study was to fill in the gap regarding the effects of guilt message framing

on corporate reputation in times of a preventable crisis. Against expectations, the present

study did not reveal that corporate reputation is (directly) positively affected by a crisis

response in which the spokesperson of the organization expresses guilt. This is in contrast

with recent studies in which was found that the expression of other emotions such as sadness

(Claeys et al., 2013) and shame and regret (Van der Meer & Verhoeven, 2014) can result in

less reputational damage compared to expressing no emotion in a crisis response.

A first explanation for having to reject hypothesis H1 and H2a could be the following: according to Coombs’ (2007) crisis situation model of SCCT, “as stakeholders attribute

greater crisis responsibility to the organization, their perceptions of the organizational

reputation will decline” (p. 168). Even though I argued before that when expressing guilt, no

guilt in a legal sense and/or actual sense of being to blame needs to be involved (the example

(23)

respondents attributed more crisis responsibility to the organization when the spokesperson

stated that the organization “feels (extremely) guilty”. In this way, the negative effect of

attributing crisis responsibility to the organization might have liquidated the positive effect of

expressing genuine guilt over what has happened, even when a rebuild crisis response strategy

was employed.

A second, methodological, explanation could be that corporate reputation is often

linked to reputational capital (Fombrun & Van Riel, 2004), which is, just as in a bank account

(Alsop, 2004; Dowling, 2002), accumulated over time. Building up reputation is thus a long

process. Therefore, measuring corporate reputation in a longitudinal study would have been

more appropriate. The scale used in the present study might not give an accurate picture of

what corporate reputation really entails.

A third explanation could be that other factors, not included in the conceptual model,

influenced the expression of guilt on corporate reputation and perceived sincerity (hypothesis

H3). For instance, the fact that persuasion knowledge is active in a public. This knowledge

helps receivers of corporate information how, when and why firms, organizations, or

governments try to influence them (Friestad & Wright, 1994). It is therefore plausible that

respondents felt like the main objective of KLM’s spokesperson behind rebuilding the relationship with stakeholders was to keep the organization’s reputation intact because of

financial reasons, more than sincerely feeling guilty for the many deaths that the airplane

crash caused. Another factor could be that because the spokesperson expressed guilt, respondents made assumptions about his and indirectly the organization’s (since the

spokesperson embodies the organization) character: emotions play a significant part in

determining character (Starkey, 2015). Simultaneously, character is used by a public to make

a judgement about reputation (Mishina, Block, & Mannor, 2012). Because the expression of

(24)

Salkovskis, 1991), respondents might have judged the mental and moral qualities of the

spokesperson/KLM as impotent, possibly explaining why expressing guilt did not positively

affect corporate reputation.

In contrast to hypothesis H1, H2a, and H3, it is comprehensible that hypothesis H2b

was accepted, since expressed emotion should be aligned with the type of crisis response

employed (Van der Meer & Verhoeven, 2013). When guilt is expressed in combination with a

deny response, this creates confusion. In this case, it would have been better if the

spokesperson suppressed feelings of guilt and/or expressed another emotion such as sadness

or grief in order to minimize attribution of crisis responsibility but at the same time still show the organization’s human face. However, the strategic suppression of emotions, often referred

to as expressive suppression (Gross & Levenson, 1993), is not an easy task. Expressive

suppression is response-focused and intervenes once the emotion is already under way. It

requires repeated endeavours to manage emotional responses because they continuously ascend, challenging the individual’s resources (Cutuli, 2014). Expressive suppression comes

relatively late in the emotion-generative process, and amends the behavioural aspect of the

emotional response without deflating the subjective and physiological experience of negative

emotion (Cutuli, 2014). Repeated efforts deplete cognitive resources to the damage of social

performances and produce a sense of discrepancy between inner experience and outer

expression of the communicator (Higgins, 1987). Thus, whereas in theory we could argue that

the spokesperson should have suppressed his feelings of guilt and/or express another emotion such as sadness or grief, in practise a spokesperson’s resources might run out during an

impactful situation like an airplane crash resulting in the inability of suppressing inherent

emotions.

Even though no direct effect of guilt message framing on corporate reputation was

(25)

perceived sincerity. These results are in line with the research of Claeys et al. (2013), who

found a mediation effect of emotional message framing on reputation via sincerity. According

to Baron & Kenny (1986), one should not continue with the mediation when the main effect

of the independent variable on the dependent variable is insignificant, because chances of

finding a mediation effect are nearly zero. However, more advanced ways to test mediation,

such as Hayes (2013), allow continuing, and with success: even though it might not be

allowed to call it a mediation, still a significant indirect effect was observed. The positive

indirect effect implies that when a spokesperson/organization delivers a crisis response

message that is considered sincere, stakeholders may be more likely to forgive the

organization for its wrongdoing and evaluate its reputation less negatively. Since “sincerity is

a specific element taken into regard by the public when it estimates the overall credibility of a communicator or message” (Claeys et al., 2013, p. 305), the findings of the present study

suggest that organizational credibility is enhanced by the sincere expression of the emotion

guilt.

Even though perceived sincerity had a positive effect on corporate reputation, this was

not moderated by trait forgiveness in respondents. In this kind of situations, when a variable

no longer has an effect on the dependent variable, a ceiling effect often is the explanation.

However, the mean trait forgiveness was not exceedingly high. Thus, sincerity was likely

important to most respondents, no matter how forgiving they are.

Limitations and suggestions for future research

Comparable experiments on emotional crisis communication done in the past (e.g. Kim &

Cameron, 2011; Claeys, 2013, Van der Meer & Verhoeven, 2014) solely used written texts.

Therefore, in the present study, respondents were exposed to written crisis responses

(26)

make the expression of guilt even clearer. When expressing emotion, however, also often

gestures, postures and tone of voice are used (Ekman, 1993; Wallbott, 1998). Whereas I did

not have the means to shoot videos in which a (fictional) spokesperson reads out the crisis

response message, employing such a richer medium including audio-visual features might

give a much more accurate picture of how the expression of a certain emotion can affect

corporate reputation. Even better would be setting up a field experiment, since it has the

advantage that outcomes are observed in a natural setting rather than in contrived setting.

A second limitation is that the present study mainly used participants who were in

their mid-twenties, went to higher education and were collected from the researcher’s own

network. According to Patzer (1996), college students are an acceptable sample when findings

serve as a foundation to be further investigated with other types of participants and/or when

the research handles fundamental questions such as cognitive information processing. Readers

should, nonetheless, keep in mind that the sample is not a perfect representation of the

population.

The third limitation is that the present study let the spokesperson of KLM express only

one emotion. Investigating the impact of guilt was highly relevant, as it is described as

arguably the most exemplary moral emotion (De Hooge et al., 2011) and a crucial mechanism

by which relationships are maintained and repaired (Baumeister et al., 1994): especially the

latter is extremely important in crisis management (Coombs, 2007). Future research, however,

should investigate the impact of the expression of other emotions as well. Until now only

negative emotions such as sadness, anger, regret, shame, and guilt have been investigated in

relation to crisis communication. It would also be interesting to see what happens when

positive emotions such as hope, altruism, relief, or optimism are expressed in crisis

(27)

Lastly, even though in Coombs’ (2007) “crisis situation model of SCCT” (p. 166),

emotions are included, he only seems to take into account the emotions that stakeholders

experience during a crisis, and not organizational members. In addition, the present study

expanded emotional crisis communication literature by finding an indirect effect between the

expression of guilt and corporate reputation through perceived sincerity. In earlier research,

Kim and Cameron (2011) argued that credibility perceptions should be considered in crisis

response research as well. It is argued in the present study that organizational credibility,

through perceived sincerity, is enhanced by the expression of the emotion guilt. Future

research should therefore create a more complete conceptual model for SCCT in which also causal relationships between organizational emotions (besides stakeholders’ emotions),

credibility/(emotional) sincerity and corporate reputation or behavioural intentions are

included.

Theoretical and managerial implications

Eisinger and Mills (1968) found that a communicator with an extreme position on the same

side of an issue as the receiver will be perceived as more sincere and competent. Since a

strong reputation can be (partly) based on how competent someone is (Thelwell et al., 2013),

it is useful for corporate communication practitioners to realize that when their expressed

emotions are aligned with the emotions that stakeholders are experiencing, perceived sincerity

and corporate reputation will probably be the strongest.

By expressing emotion in crisis responses an organization shows its human face, and

in doing so might improve its connection with stakeholders (Kelleher, 2009). Expressing

emotion in crisis responses might, however, backfire when stakeholders perceive the

expression of emotion to be insincere (Van der Meer & Verhoeven, 2014). Communication

(28)

strategic reasons like protecting the reputation: it might be considered as fake and

hypocritical, possibly damaging corporate reputation. One could thus ask him or herself how

strategically the expression of emotion in crisis responses should be approached.

The findings regarding the indirect effect between guilt message framing and

corporate reputation via perceived sincerity could also be used in other fields of

communication (research) such as political communication and persuasive

communication/marketing. Political leaders for example give a lot of speeches about varying

topics. A vast amount of them might be emotionally loaded. The importance of emotional

sincerity on reputation is likely as important for political leaders as it is for corporations.

Caza, et al. (2015) discuss the example of Hillary Clinton, who gave a speech on January 7,

2008 in New Hampshire, in which she was “tearing up” (Breslau, 2008). Her emotional

sincerity appeared to be as important as the emotion itself, drawing much more attention than her speech’s content. The present study provides proof that the sincerity of the expressed

emotion is even more important than the emotion itself. People, working as spokespersons,

politicians or marketers, and who bring out messages to large publics, should bear in mind

that in order to enhance positive reputations, faked expression of emotion should be avoided.

The importance of the findings regarding emotional sincerity also have important

implications for the (research) field of ‘emotional labour': the managing of the

expression/suppression of emotion in the workplace (Wharton & Erickson, 1993). Roles that

specifically require emotional labour, amongst others, include: public administrators, social

workers, doctors, nurses, teachers and service-based jobs such as receptionists and people

who work in hospitality (Guy & Newman, 2004). Previous research (e.g. Schaubroeck &

Jones, 2004) already indicated that employer demands to express positive emotions are related

to health symptoms such as emotional exhaustion, decrease in job satisfaction and burnout.

(29)

public would also not desire overly positive and faked emotions from for example store clerks

and waiters/waitresses. If this notion could be further investigated in future

psychological/social research, it could be beneficial for both service providers and service

receivers, and prevent serious health problems among employees.

Conclusion

This study suggests a new (SCCT) conceptual framework of crisis communication theory in

which emotions from the side of the organization as well as emotional sincerity or credibility

are added. It furthermore magnified the body of literature on emotional crisis communication

by providing both theoreticians and managers with implications on expressing guilt in

corporate messages, but nonetheless encourages further research into the expression of

emotion in crisis communication. The apprehension that emotional sincerity is more

important than the expressed emotion itself as well as its implications for

managers/researchers in the field of corporate, political and persuasive communication are

repeatedly stressed. Furthermore a novel insight about emotional sincerity outside the ‘communications spectrum’; in emotional labour, is introduced.

References:

Aaker, J. L. (1997). Dimensions of brand personality. Journal of Marketing Research, 34(3), 347-356.

Adams, G. S., & Inesi, M. E. (2016). Impediments to forgiveness: victim and transgressor attributions of intent and guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(6), 866-881. doi:10.1037/pspi0000070

Alsop, R. J. (2004). The 18 immutable laws of corporate reputation: creating, protecting,

and repairing your most valuable asset. New York, NY: Free Press.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243-267. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243

(30)

Benoit, W. L. (1997). Image repair discourse and crisis communication. Public Relations

Review, 23, 177-180.

Benoit, W. L. (2005). Image restoration theory. In R. L. Heath (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Public Relations: Volume 1 (pp. 407-410). CA: Thousand Oaks.

Benoit, W. L., & Brinson, L. (1999). Queen Elizabeth’s image repair discourse: Insensitive royal or compassionate queen. Public Relations Review, 25(2), 145-156.

doi:10.1016/S0363-8111(99)80159-3

Bradford, J. L., & Garrett, D. E. (1995). The effectiveness of corporate communicative responses to accusations of unethical behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 14, 875-892.

Breslau, K. (2008, January 6). Hillary tears up. Newsweek (Retrieved October 27, 2010, from http://www.newsweek.com/id/85609).

Cameron, G. T., Pang, A., & Jin, Y. (2008). Contingency. In T. Hansen-Horn & B. Neff (Eds.), Public relations: From theory to practice (pp. 134-157). Boston: Pearson Allyn & Bacon.

Caplovitz Barrett, K. (1995). A functionalist approach to shame and guilt. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt,

embarrassment, and pride (pp. 25-63). New York, NY: Guilford Press

Caza, A., Zhang, G., Wang, L., & Bai, Y. (2015). How do you really feel? Effect of leaders' perceived emotional sincerity on followers' trust. The Leadership Quaterly, 26(4), 518-531. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.05.008

Christensen, L. T., & Cornelissen, J. (2011). Bridging corporate and organizational communication: Review, development and a look into the future. Management

Communication Quarterly, 25, 383–414.

Claeys, A-S., Cauberghe, V., & Leysen, J. (2013). Implications of stealing thunder for the impact of expressing emotions in organizational crisis communication. Journal of

Applied Communication Research, 41(3), 293-308.

doi:10.1080/00909882.2013.806991

Clardy, A. (2012). Organizational reputation: Issues in conceptualization and measurement.

Corporate Reputation Review, 15(4), 285-303. doi:10.1057/crr.2012.17

Coombs, W. T. (2006). The protective powers of crisis response strategies: Managing reputational assets during a crisis. Journal of Promotion Management, 12, 241-259. doi:10.1300/J057v12n03_13

Coombs, W. T. (2007). Protecting organization reputations during a crisis: The development and application of situational crisis communication theory. Corporate Reputation

Review, 10(3), 163–176.

Coombs, W. T., & Holladay, S. J. (1996). Communication and attributions in a crisis: An experimental study in crisis communication. Journal of Public Relations Research, 8, 279– 295.

Coombs, W. T., & Holladay, S. J. (2002). Helping crisis managers protect reputational assets: Initial tests of the situational crisis communication theory. Management

Communication Quarterly, 16, 165-186.

Coombs, W. T., & Holladay, S. J. (2008). Comparing apology to equivalent crisis response strategies: Clarifying apology’s role and value in crisis communication. Public

Relations Review, 34, 252-257. doi:10.1016=j.pubrev.2008.04.001

Cutuli, D. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression strategies role in the emotion regulation: an overview on their modulatory effects and neural correlates.

(31)

De Hooge, I. E., Nelissen, R. M. A., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2011). What is moral about guilt? Acting “prosocially” at the disadvantage of others. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 100(3), 462-473. doi: 10.1037/a0021459

De Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 1025-1042. doi:10.1080/02699930600980874

Desmet, P. T. M., De Cremer, D., & Van Dijk, E. (2011). Trust recovery following voluntary or forced financial compensations in the trust game: The role of trait forgiveness.

Personality and Individual differences, 51, 267-273. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.027

Dowling, G. (2002). Creating corporate reputations: Identity, image, and performance. New York: Oxford University Press.

Eisinger, R., & Mills, J. (1968). Perception of the sincerity and competence of a

communicator as a function of the extremity of his position. Journal of Experimental

Social Psychology, 4, 224-232.

Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48, 376-379. Exline, J. J., Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., Campbell, W. K., & Finkel, E. J. (2004). Too

proud to let go: Narcissistic entitlement as a barrier to forgiveness. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 894-912. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.894

Fassin, Y. (2009). The stakeholder model refined. Journal of Business Ethics, 84(1), 113-135. doi:10.1007/s10551-008-9677-4

Fiske, T., & Taylor, E. (1991). Social cognition. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Fombrun, C. J., & Shanley, M. (1990). What is in a name? Reputation building and corporate strategy. Academy of Management Journal, 33(2), 233-259. doi:10.2307/256324 Fombrun, C. J., & van Riel , C. B. M. (2004). Fame & fortune: How successful companies

build winning reputations. New York: Prentice-Hall Financial Times.

Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The persuasion knowledge model - how people cope with persuasion attempts. Journal of consumer research, 21(1), 1-31.

Frijda, N. H. (1993). The place of appraisal in emotion. Cognition and emotion, 7, 357-388. Frijda, N. H., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions of emotions. In S.

Kitayama, & H. Marcus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual

influence (pp. 51–87). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: physiology, selfreport and expressive behavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970-986.

doi:10. 1037//00223514.64.6.970

Guy, M. E., & Newman, M. A. (2004). Women's jobs, men's jobs: Sex segregation and emotional labor. Public Administration Review, 64(3), 289-298.

doi:10.1111/j.1540-6210.2004.00373.x

Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852-870). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychological

Review. 94(3), 319-340. doi:10.1037//0033-295x.94.3.319

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process

analysis: a regression-based approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press

Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15(4), 635-650.

Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.

Jin, Y. (2009). The effects of public’s cognitive appraisal of emotions in crises on crisis coping and strategy assessment. Public Relations Review, 35, 310-313. doi:10.1016/j.pubrev.2009.02.003

(32)

Jin, Y., Pang, A., & Cameron, G. T. (2012). Toward a publics-driven, emotion-based conceptualization in crisis communication: unearthing dominant emotions in multi staged testing of the integrated crisis mapping (ICM) model. Journal of Public

Relations Research, 24(3), 266-298. doi:10.1080/1062726X.2012.676747

Kauffman, J. (2008). When sorry is not enough: Archbishop Cardinal Bernard Law’s image restoration strategies in the statement on sexual abuse of minors by clergy. Public

Relations Review, 34(3), 258-262. doi:10.1016/j.pubrev.2008.03.001

Kelleher, T. (2009). Conversational voice, communicated commitment, and public relations outcomes in interactive online communication. Journal of Communication, 59(1), 172-188. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.01410.x

Ketelaar, T., & Au, W. T. (2003). The effects of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information

interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cognition & Emotion, 17, 429-453. doi:10.1080/ 02699930143000662

Kim, H-S. (2011). A reputational approach examining publics' attributions on corporate social responsibility motives. Asian Journal of Communication, 21(1), 84-101.

doi:10.1080/01292986.2010.524230

Kim, S., Avery, E. J., & Lariscy, R. W. (2009). Are crisis communicators practicing what we preach? An evaluation of crisis response strategy analyzed in public relations research from 1991 to 2009. Public Relations Review, 35, 446-448.

Kim, H. J., & Cameron, G. T. (2011). Emotions matter in crisis: The role of anger and sadness in the publics’ response to crisis news framing and corporate crisis response.

Communication Research, 38(6), 826-855. doi:10.1177/0093650210385813

Kim, K., Park, J., & Kim, J. (2014). Consumer–brand relationship quality: When and how it helps brand extensions. Journal of Business Research, 67(4), 591-597.

Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social referencing: The infant’s use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental

Psychology, 22, 427-432.

Legg, K. L. (2009). Religious celebrity: An analysis of image repair discourse. Journal of

Public Relations Research, 21(2), 240-250. doi:10.1080/10627260802557621

Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Empathy, shame, guilt, and narratives of

interpersonal conflicts: Guilt-prone people are better at perspective taking. Journal of

Personality, 66, 1-37. doi:10.1111/1467- 6494.00001

Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York, NY: International Universities Press.

Lewis, H. B. (1987). Shame and the narcissistic personality. In D. L. NatJimon (Ed.), The

many faces of shame (pp. 93-124). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Lewis, K. M. (2000). When leaders display emotion: How followers respond to negative emotional expression of male and female leaders. Journal of Organizational

Behaviour, 21, 221-234.

Lindsay-Hartz, J. (1984). Contrasting experiences of shame and guilt. American Behavioral

Scientist, 27, 689-704. doi:10.1177/ 000276484027006003

Loseth, V. S., & Dahl, J. (2017). The influence of emotions on cognitive processing, and the importance of retrospective evaluations (Master’s thesis). Retrieved from

https://brage.bibsys.no/xmlui/handle/11250/2483579

McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S. D., Emmons, R. A., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 249-266.

doi:10.1037/00332909.127.2.249

McDonald, L. M., Sparks, B., & Glendon, A. I. (2010). Stakeholder reactions to company crisis communication and causes. Public Relations Review, 36(3), 263-271.

(33)

Milgrom, P., & Roberts, J. (1982). Predation, reputation, and entry deterrence. Journal of

Economic Theory 27(2), 280-312. doi:10.1016/0022-0531(82)90031-X

Mishina, Y., Block, E.S., & Mannor, M. J. (2012). The path dependence of organizational reputation: How social judgement influences assessments of capability and character.

Strategic Management Journal, 33, 459-477. doi:10.1002/smj.958

Nabi, R. L. (1999). A cognitive-functional model for the effects of discrete negative emotions on information processing, attitude change, and recall. Communication Theory, 9(3), 292-320.

Nelissen, R. M. A., Dijker, A. J., & De Vries, N. K. (2007). How to turn a hawk into a dove and vice versa: Interactions between emotions and goals in a give-some dilemma game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 280-286.

doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.01.009

Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press

Patzer, G. L. (1996). Experiment-research methodology in marketing types and applications. Westport, CT: Quorum books

Read, K. (2007). “Corporate pathos”: New approaches to quell hostile publics. Journal of

Communication Management, 11, 332-347.

Reichart , J. (2003). A theoretical exploration of expectational gaps in the corporate issue construct. Corporate Reputation Review, 6, 58-69.

Reynolds, M., & Salkovskis, P. M. (1991). The relationship among guilt, dysphoria, anxiety and obsessions in a normal population - An attempted replication. Behaviour

Research and Therapy, 29(3), 259-265. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(91)90116-K

Riek, B. M., Luna, L. M. R., & Schnabelrauch, C. A. (2014). Transgressors’ guilt and shame: A longitudinal examination of forgiveness seeking. Journal of Social and Personal

Relationships, 31, 751–772. doi: 10.1177/0265407513503595

Risen, J. L., & Gilovich, T. (2007). Target and observer differences in the acceptance of questionable apologies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(3), 418-433. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.418

Robles, T. F., Carroll, J. E., Bai, S., Reynolds, B. M., Esquivel S., & Repetti, R. L. (2016) Emotions and family interactions in childhood: Associations with leukocyte telomere length. Psychneuroendocrinology, 63, 343-350. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.018 Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals

differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 206-221.

Sandlin, J. K., & Gracyalny, M. L. (2018). Seeking sincerity, finding forgiveness: YouTube apologies as image repair. Public Relations Review, 44(3), 393-406.

doi: 10.1016/j.pubrev.2018.04.007

Schaubroeck, J. & James, R. J. (2000). Antecedents of workplace emotional labor dimensions and moderators of their effects on physical symptoms. Journal of Organizational

Behaviour, 21, 163-183.

Sheikh, S., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (2010). The “shoulds” and “should nots” of moral emotions: A self-regulatory perspective on shame and guilt. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 36, 213-224. doi:10.1177/ 0146167209356788

Smith, A. (1759). The theory of moral sentiments. London, England: Miller.

Starkey, C. (2015). Emotion and character. In Miller, C. B., Furr, R. M., Knobel, A., & Fleeson, W. (Ed.), Character: New directions from philosophy, psychology and

theology (pp 192-214). New York, NY: Ofxford University Press.

Thelwell, R. C., Page, J. L., Lush A., Greenlees, I. A., Manley, A. J. (2013). Can reputation biases influence the outcome and process of making competence judgments of a

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

The main purpose of this study was to answer the following question: “What is the influence of positive and negative message framing in an advertisement on online purchase

The most commonly employed fishing techniques were handlines (26.77%), traditional baskets (25.81%) and drag nets (22.26%), followed by gill nets (17.10%) and, to a much

H3: A positive projected organizational reputation in combination with an internal conflict situation will result in a less negative evaluation of the communication climate

The use of an emotional message frame leads to a) more trust in the organization b) less anger c) more sympathy, compared with the use of a rational message frame. The use of a

H2: Emotional framing leads to less a) reputational harm, b) secondary crisis communication, c) secondary crisis reactions, as compared to rational framing. H3: A crisis

Delineating the conditions and contexts under which using humor as a crisis response could benefit or damage a company's reputation will not only instruct public

A 2 (message type: humorous versus non-humorous crisis response message) × 2 (response subject: personal identity versus organizational identity) x 2 (responsible versus

Although the rela- tion between overall board diversity and corporate reputation has been subject to research before, whether and how the distinct dimensions of