• No results found

Author: Sirin Yildiz Student number: S2351447 E-Mail: s.a.n.yildiz@student.rug.nl

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Author: Sirin Yildiz Student number: S2351447 E-Mail: s.a.n.yildiz@student.rug.nl "

Copied!
61
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Faculty of Economics and Business

Master of Science in Marketing Management MASTER THESIS

Author: Sirin Yildiz Student number: S2351447 E-Mail: s.a.n.yildiz@student.rug.nl

Supervisor: Dr. M. Moeini Jazani Co-assessor: prof. Dr. B.M. Fennis

13.01.2020

Word Count: 9005

(2)

TABLE OF CONTENTS

1. Introduction ... 4

2. Literature Review ... 6

2.1 Time framing and environmental concern ... 6

2.2 Environmental concern and behavioral intentions ... 12

2.3 Moderators ... 15

3. Conceptual Model ... 18

4. Methodology ... 19

4.1 Data Collection and Sample ... 19

4.2 Manipulation and Study Design ... 20

4.3 Measures ... 22

4.4 Results ... 24

4.5 Summary of Results ... 30

5. General Discussion ... 32

5.1 Theoretical implications ... 32

5.2 Managerial Implications ... 34

6. Future research and Limitations ... 36

7. Conclusion ... 38

8. References ... 39

9. Appendices ... 54

Appendix A ... 54

Appendix B ... 56

(3)

Abstract

Past research has shown the effect of time framing in different domains, such as the health and the food domain. This research examines the framing of time (i.e., granular vs. less granular expressions of duration) in a new domain, namely the environmental domain. In this research, it is proposed that individuals who are exposed to environmental risk expressed in days, as opposed to years, will display higher pro-environmental behavior.

Based on past research, we expected that day framing will result in more proximity, which will result in a higher perceived environmental concern, and therefore in a higher pro- environmental behavior. Accordingly, it is proposed that the relationship between time framing and pro-environmental behaviors is mediated by environmental concern.

Results of an extensive study largely support these predictions that time framing is an effective tool in communicating climate change risks. However, this effect is going in the opposite direction of what was expected. When the environmental risk is framed in years and therefore is less granular, individuals are more motivated to get engaged with

environmentally oriented behavior. Additionally, two moderators are included in this research; environmental self-efficacy and biospheric values, which both showed a non- significant effect on the relationship between time framing and environmental behavior.

Keywords: Temporal Framing, Granularity, Environmental Behavioural Intentions,

Environmental Concern, Biospheric Values, Environmental Self-Efficacy

(4)

1. Introduction

Pro-environmental behavior is a salient concern in contemporary society (Mahardika, Thomas, Ewing, & Japutra, 2019). Mounting scientific evidence suggests that human- induced climate change may pose a significant threat to humans and the wider environment (Whitmarsh, 2009).

Changing individuals’ environmental behavior can solve most of the environmental problems that we have encountered (Steg, Perlaviciute, & van der Werff, 2015). The current

generation's success in achieving sustainable patterns of living on the earth will determine whether the planet will retain a livable environment for future generations (Oskamp, 2007).

Therefore, there is a consensus that a change in behavior is needed to reduce environmental impacts and improve positive impacts on human health (De Young, 1993). The rapid development of the economy and increasing public exposure to environmental

lead to an increase in the expected number of people who will adopt pro-environmental behavior (Chen, De la Rosa, Peterson, Zhong, & Lu, 2016).

However, policies to achieve pro-environmental behavior have met with limited success, after decades of information campaigns and economic measures to encourage 'green' behaviors, the public is prepared to recycle, but few take action beyond this (Whitmarsh, 2009). Researchers and industry practitioners have developed different approaches to

promoting pro-environmental behavior, emphasizing different benefits. However, there have been mixed findings in terms of which approach is the most effective (Xu, Arpan & Chen, 2015). Research on energy consumption behavior highlights various psychological, social, economic, and physical barriers to fostering energy conservation (Whitmarsh, 2009);

therefore, environmental behavior cannot be achieved simply through information provision

and economic measures (Jackson, 2005).

(5)

Relevant here is, is that the way that information is framed is inevitable when communicating about any complex phenomenon (Graber, 2014), such as pro-environmental behavior.

Several researchers have conducted studies about the framing of information (Spence, Poortinga & Venables, 2010; Kees, 2011), a specific type of framing is temporal framing, which has been studied intensively by several researchers in different domains (Xu et al., 2015; Chandran & Menon, 2004). Nevertheless, there is high relevance to apply temporal framing specifically to the environmental domain and environmental risks in order to encourage pro-environmental behavior among individuals.

According to Chandran and Menon (2004), when a hazard is presented in a day frame, the risk is construed as more concrete and proximal compared with a year frame, enhancing the effectiveness of the message. Therefore, in this master thesis, we will examine the effect of framing environmental risk expressed in two different levels of granularity. Environmental risk expressed in days and environmental risk expressed in years, where objectively, the time is the same, just expressed in different units. The findings aim to achieve results that can be used to communicate environmental risk effectively.

The paper is structured as followed: Firstly, the article will discuss relevant literature, based upon that, four hypotheses are created, which will be combined into a conceptual model.

Secondly, the thesis will provide the methodology to test the hypothesis. Thirdly, the results will be provided in the general discussion, consisting out of the theoretical and the

managerial implications. After this, future research suggestions and limitations will be

provided. Lastly, the paper will conclude with the conclusion.

(6)

2. Literature Review

Based on previous literature, this literature review will provide extensive description and analysis of the relevant variables used in this thesis. Throughout the literature review, we will propose the relevant hypotheses being tested in this research.

2.1 Time framing and environmental concern

Our subjective assessments of the time that passes may be inaccurate (Grondin, 2010) since the perception of time is subjective (Larson, Larson and Katz,1991). This argument is reinforced by Fredrickson and Kahnemen (1993), who state that “subjective perceptions of time are different from objective changes.” Therefore, individuals’ subjective estimates of duration do not accurately map onto objective time (Zauberman, Kim, Malkoc and Bettman, 2009). Individuals have difficulty thinking about time as an independent dimension and often misjudge the duration of events (Zauberman et al., 2009). Indeed, research shows that

consumers’ mapping of objective duration onto subjective time is nonlinear and characterized by insufficient sensitivity to changes in duration.

Since time is perceived differently by individuals (Droit-Volet & Meck, 2007), it is possible

to influence the subjective perceptions of time (Baker & Cameron, 1996). Time perception

can be altered by contextual factors (Antonides, Verhoef, & Van Aalst, 2002). One of these

factors is the temporal context, where framed outcomes influence the effects of the message

(Gerend, & Cullen, 2008). Chandran & Menon (2004) argue that the framing of time can

evoke a different psychological process, and the manipulation of time perceptions could

influence decision making (Kim & Jang, 2017).Churchill, Good & Pavey (2014) identify

temporal framing as “the application of a time frame to a particular outcome.”

(7)

According to Liberman and Trope (1998), the temporal frame of the message can influence how a message is construed. Perceptions of temporal distance systematically alter the way future events are construed and thus influence the evaluation and choices related to those future events (Liberman and Trope, 1998). Distal events seem more abstract and

decontextualized than proximal events and outcomes that occur in the distant future may have a less psychological impact than those expected to occur faster (Liberman & Trope,1998).

Relevant in the temporal framing literature is temporal discounting, which is the tendency of people to discount rewards as they approach a temporal horizon in the future or the past (Doyle, 2012). Temporal discounting can be expected to strongly affect people’s risk

judgments and their willingness to change appropriate behaviors (Gattig & Hendrickx, 2007).

These findings are reinforced by Nicolaij and Hendrickx (2003), in their study, they found that longer outcome delays result in lower average risk judgments and a decreased tendency to change appropriate behaviors. Assessments of temporal discounting often require

participants to choose between a smaller, immediate reward and a more substantial reward gained after a specified delay, such as £1 now or £5 after two weeks (Pavey & Churchill, 2017). There is a general tendency for diminished value to be placed on distant-future reward as oppose to near-future reward (Trope & Liberman, 2010). Humans discount outcomes that are uncertain, temporally delayed, spatially distant, and occurring to others (Gattig &

Hendrickx, 2007). The discounting processes involved appear to be similar across different outcome dimensions (Gattig & Hendrickx, 2007).

This information is highly relevant to the environmental domain since many environmental

problems are uncertain, have long-term consequences, and some have both immediate and

long-term adverse effects (Gärling & Schuitema, 2007). For instance, urban air pollution

from traffic may cause immediate odor annoyance as well as long-term health problems

(8)

(Gärling & Schuitema, 2007). Other environmental problems, such as the increase of

atmospheric greenhouse gas concentrations or the depletion of natural resources, such as oil, water, minerals, do not have severe immediate effects. However, in the long run, such risks may have catastrophic consequences (Gattig & Hendrickx, 2007).

To make individuals more aware of these environmental problems, framing information can be applied since framing can influence attitudes and behaviors by highlighting certain aspects of the situation or by changing how one might think and consider the presented information (Chan, 2019). Besides, temporal framing can be used to encourage pro-environmental behavior, since, for pro-environmental behaviors, it is that both negative and positive consequences are often somewhat uncertain and occur over the long-term and this restricts the ability of an individual to realize the benefits of behaving pro-environmental (Grønhøj,

&Thøgersen, 2011). Consequently, interventions designed to change the subjective perception of future time can be used as a strategy (Zauberman & Kim, 2011) in the

environmental domain. Subtle changes in the message could alter how long or short a future event might seem (Zauberman & Kim, 2011).

Temporal framing has been heralded as a promising strategy to make risks appear more

proximal and concrete and thus counter temporal discounting effects (Lo, Smith, Taylor,

Good & von Wagner, 2012). In order to use temporal framing to affect individuals their risk

judgments and their willingness to change behavior, it is relevant to mention that individuals

perceive differences between two values when those values are communicated with the usage

of different units (Wertenbroch, Soman, & Chattopadhyay, 2007). Therefore, in this research

paper, temporal framing is operationalized by presenting temporal distance of environmental

risk in different time units, namely in days versus years (Chandran & Menon, 2004). This

manner of temporal framing is that the same quantity of time can be expressed at different

levels of granularity, for example, “1 year,” “12 months,” or “365 days.” (Zhang & Schwarz,

(9)

2011). Although there is no rational reason to suppose that a week might differ from 7 days, it does (Adaval, Pandelaere, Briers, Lembregts, Bagchi, Li, & Coulter, 2013). This effect leads to errors in estimation that affect not only how we make progress in goals that are set but also affect, at a more fundamental level, how we perceive things (Adaval et al., 2013).

Therefore, we frame the risk of an environmental disaster happening in a similar subjective time yet expressed in different units. Namely, the time expressed in days and the time expressed in years. Temporal framing of objectively neutral reference periods leads to

differential subjective perceptions of the distance (Chandran & Menon, 2004). Therefore, the persuasive effect of a message can differ even when the content is the same, depending on how the contents are framed and delivered (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979).

Also, how messages are framed influences the extent to which they are attended to, how much knowledge the audience gain from them, and how positively such messages are evaluated (Graber, 2004; Lakoff, 2010). So, using the time frame as a unit influences the perception of an individual (Pandaleare, Briers and Lembregts, 2011), and the size of the unit is used to make an inference about the proximity of an event (Monga, & Bagchi, 2011). Fine- grained utterances increase confidence that the information is accurate (Zhang & Schwarz, 2011), resulting in environmental risk perceived as more accurate. We can state that expressing an attribute in a different unit leads to a more significant perceived difference if the unit is on an expanded or finer-grained scale (Pandelaere, Briers, & Lembregts, 2011).

Examples are that a donation seems smaller when it is framed using a per-day rather than a per-year format Gourville (1998), and individuals underestimate when they budget for a month rather than for a year (Ülkümen, Thomas, & Morwitz, 2008).

Once a future event is understood as having a greater relevance to the present, individuals

should be more willing to take action (Lewis and Oyserman, 2015). For the environmental

domain, if the environmental risk can have greater relevance to the present, as a result of how

(10)

time units are expressed, individuals should be more willing to take action in favor of the environment. Chandran & Menon (2004) show in their research that comparisons can be made between messages containing the sentence “every day” or “every year.” Objectively, the time frame does not convey information about the temporal distance. However, it has been argued that the day and year frames trigger different subjective temporal perceptions (Chandran & Menon, 2004). That systematically affect judgments of risk’ associated with events that are closer in time, more proximal, and thus more concrete and probable than those associated with a year frame (Chandran & Menon, 2004). Additionally, the study of Lewis and Oyserman (2015) showed that people say they will start saving four times sooner if told how many days rather than how many years they have until their child goes to college or until they want to retire. Changing the metric used to frame when the future begins should make the future feel psychologically relevant to the current situation by making it connected to and congruent with the present self (Lewis and Oyserman, 2015).

Individuals experience temporal granularity, the farther away a future event is, the fewer details can be imagined, the closer a future event is, the more details can be imagined (Lewis and Oyserman, 2015).

Additionally, compared with closer events, further events are typically considered using a more gross-grained time metric, such as years rather than days (Lewis and Oyserman, 2015).

So, the use of temporal reference frames, such as a day or year, is likely to trigger subjective

associations with a near or a distant future, respectively, that results in temporal distance

effects (Chandran & Menon, 2004). A day frame suggests that an event is closer in time and,

therefore, more proximal and thereby replicating a near-future event (Chandran & Menon,

2004). Contrastingly, a year frame moves the event further in time and is, therefore, less

proximal, replicating a distant future event, evoking a different psychological process from

the temporal of every year (Chandran & Menon, 2004).

(11)

As mentioned, individuals construe proximal events in more concrete and contextualized terms, while distal events are construed in abstract, decontextualized terms (Trope &

Liberman, 2000). Message framing focused on a day could be used to increase the perceived temporal proximity to the outcome (Chandran &Menon, 2004). Chandran and Menon (2004) argue that a hazard seems more proximal when it is expressed in terms of a per-day rather than a per-year basis; the per-day formulation makes the hazard seem closer in time because a day is shorter than a year.

If we apply this theory for the environmental risk, we can, therefore, state that:

H1: Temporal framing will affect environmental concern such that these judgments are

higher when the duration to environmental risks is expressed in a day frame as opposed to a

year frame.

(12)

2.2 Mediator and Dependent Variables: Environmental concern (ME) and pro-environmental behavior (DV)

Many environmental problems are related to human behavior. Therefore individual action in the marketplace can remedy environmental problems (Paavola, 2001). Environmentally significant behavior can reasonably be defined by its impact: the extent to which it changes the availability of materials or energy from the environment or alters the structure and dynamics of ecosystems or the biosphere itself (Stern,1997). Particular behavior, such as clearing forest or disposing of household waste, directly or proximally, causes an

environmental change (Stern, Young, & Druckman,1992). Where other behavior is

environmentally significant indirectly, by shaping the context in which choices are made that directly cause environmental change (Rosa & Dietz, 1998).

A more thorough understanding of the relationship between concern and behavior is

necessary if marketers and public policymakers are to reduce environmentally harmful

behavior (Kilbourne & Pickett, 2008). Kilbourne & Pickett (2008) found in their study that

when concern increases, consumption behavior might become more environmentally

sensitive, and consequently, consumers may change purchase patterns, tilting them toward

going greener. Minton & Rose (1997) reinforce this argument and state that "what makes

people more inclined to behave in environmentally friendly ways is an awareness of various

environmental problems and the consequences of their behavior in response." "This may

result in a willingness to relocate their resources such as time, money, and attention, to make

personal behavior more environmentally friendly" (Minton & Rose, 1997). Additionally,

individuals become engaged in environmental issues and perform pro-environmental

behavior since they are more concerned about the adverse consequences of environmental

problems for themselves, others, or the biosphere (Hansla, Gamble, Juliusson & Gärling,

2008).

(13)

Thus it has generally been found that there is a positive correlation between environmental concern and environmentally friendly behavior (Straughan and Roberts, 1999). Therefore, mitigating environmental problems requires behavioral changes by individuals all over the world. Encouragingly, studies have shown that the majority of the world's population is aware of environmental problems and supports environmental protection (Milfont & Schultz, 2016). If the majority of individuals are concerned about the environment, the collective concern could be manifested by individual pro-environmental behavior (Sapci & Considine, 2014). Pro-environmental behavior best can be described as 'behavior that is undertaken to change the environment (Stern, 2000) and is a mixture of self-interest and concern for other people, the next generation, other species, or whole ecosystems (Bamberg, & Möser, 2007).

Since environmental concern is an important issue ( Diekmann & Franzen, 2019),

influencing individuals, their environmental behavior should be of interest. Subjective risk plays a role in influencing individuals their behavior (Rundmo, 1999), which is also applicable to the environmental risk domain. Since environmental concern significantly affects environmental behavior (Rundmo, 1999). Kilbourne and Pickett (2008) argue that when concern increases, consumption behavior might become more environmentally sensitive. An example of a particular behavior is that individuals who perceive that

environmental problems constituted a severe threat to health were more likely to engage in environmental practices, such as recycling, conserving water, buying environmentally friendly products, and limiting their driving (Baldassare & Katz, 1992). Therefore, we can state that personal environmental threats had a significant role in predicting overall

environmental practices (Baldassare & Katz, 1992).

Another example is that environmentally concerned households tend to be more conservative

in their use of energy (Sapci & Considine, 2014). Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera (1987)

found a significant and positive correlation between general measures of environmental

(14)

concern and environmental practice. Individuals who demonstrate strong environmental concerns are more likely to undertake waste recycling, green purchasing behavior, and lower energy consumption (Lin & Huang, 2012). Consumers who have environmental concerns are willing to behave in a more environmentally friendly way to help environmental protection (Sapci & Considine, 2014). Lin and Huang (2012) found in their study that when presented with certain conditions or situations, such as perceived worsening environmental threats, or the availability of subsidies or discounts for green products, consumers with severe

environmental concerns are more willing to behave in a pro-environmental way. Therefore, consumers who are concerned more with the environment are more willing to make efforts to change (Lin & Huang, 2012).

We assume that in a day framing, an environmental risk will be perceived as more severe, which will lead to individuals being environmentally concerned in comparison when the environmental risk would be communicated in a year frame. From the theory altogether, we can state that individuals who are environmentally concerned behave in a more pro-

environmental responsible way.

Therefore, we propose:

H2: Perceived environmental risk will mediate the effect of temporal framing on pro- environmental behavior, such that in a day (vs.year) frame, consumers show increased pro- environmental behavioral intention.

(15)

2.3 Moderators

Two moderators were included in this research, environmental self-efficacy and biospheric values, to examine the relationship between framing and behavioral intention more deeply.

Environmental Self-efficacy

Self-efficacy can be defined as the belief in an individual's capacity to organize and guide the courses of action required to tackle certain situations in the immediate future (Wood &

Bandura, 1989). Perceived self-efficacy is a significant determinant of intention, which further influences performance (Bandura, 1997). A higher level of self-efficacy in specific tasks indicates more excellent capabilities and confidence in performing the tasks and is likely to increase an individual behavioral intention to take action (Huang, 2016).

Several researchers argue that self-efficacy or effectiveness can affect people's environmental behavior (Lee, 2011; Lee et al., 2014; Tabernero & Hernández, 2011).

Environmental self-efficacy has a positive direct effect on pro-environmental behavior (Huang, 2016). Bandura (1997) argues that self-efficacy in the environmental domain is strengthened when people master the specific skills required to engage in pro-environmental behavior and are verbally persuaded of their ability to perform such behavior. An example is from the research from Huang (2016), he found that when individuals have higher levels of self-efficacy in mitigating global warming, they are likely to be more interested in obtaining updated information for possible solutions and in acting. Additionally, Meinhold & Malkus (2005) argue that pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors are stronger among individuals with high levels of self-efficacy. Individuals engage in and feel better about pro-

environmental acts, and therefore their self-efficacy in their lives will increase (Meinhold &

Malkus, 2005). Therefore, environmental self-efficacy may influence the relationship of

temporal framing on environmental behavioral intentions since individuals who are high in

(16)

environmental self-efficacy are involved in pro-environmental behavior. Accordingly, those individuals are less likely to be influenced by the time frame in comparison with individuals who score low on environmental self-efficacy.

Therefore, we propose:

H3: The effect of framing will be more significant among people with low environmental self- efficacy.

Biospheric Values

Individuals differ in their amount of protecting the environment (Chan, 2019), and personal values can explain people's pro-environmental actions and in which these actions may be more aligned with nature, the biospheric values (de Groot & Steg, 2008). Biospheric values emphasize the quality of nature and the environment independently from the benefits it provides for human beings (Steg and De Groot, 2012). Individuals who endorse biospheric values, therefore, assign importance to the intrinsic value of the ecosystem, which translates into a moral imperative to act pro-environmentally (Stern and Dietz, 1994).

Biospheric values are considered to be important antecedents of environmental behavior (Werff, Steg, Keizer, 2013). Schultz, Gouveia, Cameron, Tankha, Schmuck, & Franek (2005) relate pro‐environmental intention and behavior positively to altruistic and biospheric

concern. Furthermore, several researchers have shown that individuals with strong biospheric

values are more like to have pro-environmental intentions and to act pro-environmentally

(Steg & Groot, 2012). Previous research showed that biospheric values had been related to

pro-environmental behavior (Schultz & Zelezny, 1998), acceptability of climate change

policies (Nilsson, von Borgstede & Biel, 2004) and environmental activism (Steg et al.,

2011). Therefore, biospheric values may moderate the relationship between temporal

framing and environmental behavioral intentions. Individuals who are high in biospheric

(17)

values are already related to pro-environmental behavior and therefore are less likely to be influenced by the time frame in comparison with individuals who carry low biospheric values.

Therefore, we propose:

H4: The effect of framing will be more significant among people with low biospheric values.

(18)

3. Conceptual Model

Based on the literature review and the two proposed hypotheses, a conceptual model is constructed. In this conceptual model, temporal framing and duration are the independent variables, which will influence environmental behavioral intentions, where this effect is mediated through the environmental concern. Furthermore, the moderator's biospheric values and environmental self-efficacy influence the relationship that temporal framing has on environmental behavioral intentions.

Figure 1: Conceptual Model

(19)

4. Methodology

This part of the thesis will provide information about the methodology that is used to test the four proposed hypotheses. Besides, it will provide information about the effect of the

influence of the mediator's environmental concern and the two moderators' biospheric values and environmental self-efficacy. The methodology will contain the data collection and sample, manipulation of the study design, measures, results, and will conclude with a summary of the results.

4.1 Procedure Data Collection and Description Sample

To acquire information about the relationship between time framing and personal

environmental behavioral intention, where this relationship is mediated by perceived

environmental concern, a questionnaire was conducted. In December 2019, data was

collected through a survey via the online survey platform Qualtrics. Participants received a

payment for their participation. The study initially contained 363 participants. Several

attention questions were incorporated into the questionnaire, and those who failed these

quality checks (N=53) were excluded from the analysis, resulting in a final sample of 310

participants. The participants consisted for 42,6% (N = 132) out of male, 57,1% (N = 177)

out of females and 0,3% (N = 1) was specified as “other”. The average age of the participants

in the study is 37, with a standard deviation of 12.134 and a mean of 37,35. The minimum

age is 19 years, and the participant with the maximum age was 74 years. Regarding the

participants their political orientation, 18,4 % (N = 57) identified themselves as Republic,

41,6% (N=129) as Democrat, 32,6 % (N = 101) as Independent, 1,9% (N =6) as other and

lastly 5,5% (N = 17) of the respondents did not have a political preference.

(20)

With reference to employment, 45,5 % (N = 141) was full-time employed,18,1 % (N= 56) was part-time employed, 15,8% (N= 49), self-employed, 7.1% (N=11) unemployed but looking for a job, of the respondents 3,9% ( N=12) was retired, 4,5% ( N=14) identified themselves as housewife/househusband 14 4,5 and lastly, 5,2% (N= 16) was unable to work

& others.

4.2 Manipulation and study design

In order to test the hypothesis, a 2 (temporal frame: day vs. year) x 2 (objective duration: 27 vs. 41 years) design was used. In this research, a 2-factor between-subject experimental design was conducted. This thesis consists of 2 independent variables, namely, time framing, and objective duration. Both variables are categorical and consist of 2 levels. First, the perception of time was manipulated by using different expressions of time, namely day versus year. The duration was manipulated, consisting of the two different levels, 27 years and 41 years. The reason we included two different durations of time was to examine the generalizability of our framing effect across different objective times.

The participants were randomly assigned to one of the four different conditions. In which they read a news article on climate change's impacts occurring in 27 years expressed in days (N = 76), 27 years expressed in years (N =81), 41 years expressed in days (N =83) and 41 years expressed in years (N =70). The news article is as followed: “What makes this report particularly unique is that it provides concrete estimates of the so-called point of no return based on our current trajectory of emissions,” says Thomas Wirth, the IPCC spokesperson.

According to the report, scientists forecast that in (9,855 days/14,965 days/27 years/41

years)

(21)

We will reach a crucial threshold beyond which it will be impossible to recover the planet back to a normal state. In the US alone, the increased frequency and variety of natural disasters such as ultra-intense hurricanes Harvey, Florence, and Dorian as well as massive and extreme wildfires, floods and droughts across the country are pointed as harbingers of a warming globe and irreversible climate change threshold we are reaching in the next (9,855 days/14,965 days/27 years/41 years). "It is possible to limit global warming to 1.5 degrees Celsius but doing so would require unprecedented changes in our lifestyles and major transformations in the ways our global society functions and interacts with natural

ecosystems," the report says. Lowering emissions to this degree would require widespread transitions in energy, urban, transportation, infrastructure, agricultural, and industrial systems in the next (9,855 days/14,965 days/27 years/41 years). "International cooperation, as well as individuals' adoption of preventive measures, are imperative to limit emissions and, therefore, global warming and its devastating impacts. Decisions made today will have lasting consequences for future generations", says Miriam Perkins, a scientist at the Climate Change Research Center at Yale. The next (9,855 days/14,965 days/27 years/41 years) will be critical in the evolution of these efforts.

The news article highlighted environmental risk occurring in the future, and the article was

inspired by the scenario used by Schuldt, Rickard, and Yang (2019) in their study about how

departure dates influence climate change engagement. The stories across all four conditions

were identical. The time framing and duration were different. In the article, there was an

emphasis on framing the risk, and the article highlighted multiple times the moment when

environmental risk would occur.

(22)

4.3 Measures

After exposure to the article, participants were provided with a questionnaire; we state the questions in order of their presentation. Furthermore, it is stated from which articles the questions and the scales are derived, and information is provided on which scale is used.

Perceived Concern (Mediator). After exposure to the scenario, perceived concern, the mediator, was assessed. Using a total of 12 questions derived from Van der Linden (2014 &

2015), McRight (2010), and Hornsey et al. (2015). Participants responded on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = Not at all agree, 7 = Very much agree). The questionnaire included items such as “I feel the consequences of climate change are inevitable for future generations” and Climate change is an important issue.”On average, the participants were quite concerned about environmental risk after reading the news article (M=5,56, SD=1.58).

Behavioral intentions (DV). On a 7-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly disagree, 7 = Strongly agree), personal behavior intentions were measured. The scale that measured environmental behavioral intention contained 19 items and was derived from Gifford & Comeau (2011) and Tobler et al. (2012). The questionnaire included items such as “I am willing to save

electricity (e.g., by switch off my lights when not in use or by using energy-efficient light bulbs” and “I am prepared to greatly reduce my energy use to help tackle climate”. In

general, participants were also quite intended concerning their behavioral intentions (M=5,38, SD=1,07).

Self-Efficacy (Moderator). Environmental self-efficacy was measured with four items on a

5-point Likert scale (1=Strongly disagree, 5=Strongly agree). These four items were derived

from past research (Heath & Gifford, 2006; Benjamin, Por, and Budescu, 2017). The

(23)

difference to alleviate the negative effects of global warming” and “There is very little I can do to mitigate the negative effect of global warming." Generally, participants scored

moderately on environmental self-efficacy (M=3,18, SD=0,59).

Biospheric Values (Moderator). Lastly, on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly disagree, 5

= Strongly agree), 6 items were used to measure participants ‘Biospheric Values'. This scale was derived from Dunlap (2000), Ziegler (2017), and Whitmarsh (2011). The questionnaire included items such as “Humans were meant to rule over the rest of nature," and "Plants and animals have the same right to exist as humans." Participants in general were in favor of these values (M = 3, 86, SD=0,79).

Demographics. The questionnaire concluded with several questions to identify the

demographics of the sample size. Items included participants to specify their age, gender,

income, political orientation, education, ethnicity, and description of their job.

(24)

4.4 Results

Personal Environmental Behaviour. To test Hypothesis 1 and to analyze the impact of time framing and duration on personal environmental behavior, a two-way ANOVA analysis was conducted. The two-way ANOVA not only aims at assessing the main effect of each

independent variable but also identifies if there is any interaction between them (Casella, 2008). Therefore, the two-way ANOVA analysis tested for an interaction effect between the two factors. The result from this two-way ANOVA (appendix table B.1) shows that there was no interaction between time framing and duration on environmental behavior (F (310) = 0.025, p =0.874, η2p = .020. Additionally, the main effect of the factor duration is highly insignificant on environmental personal intentions. (F (310) = 0.074, p=0,786, η2p = .020 and therefore has become irrelevant as a factor. The 2-way ANOVA analysis support that there is a significant main effect of time framing on environmental personal behavioral intentions (F (310) = 5.752, p <0.017 η2p = .020. In figure 1, it is observed that when the environmental risk occurs in a year frame, expressed in blue, personal environmental behavior is

significantly higher, compared to when the risk was communicated in a day frame, expressed in red. These results show that the time framing is effective in motivating behavioral

intentions, regardless of the duration (27 or 41 years). However, the direction of the effect is the opposite of what we had initially hypothesized. That is when the durations were

expressed in years (vs. days) people showed a higher tendency to engage in behavioral

changes.

(25)

27 years 41 years Figure 1

As noticed, the results of the two-way ANOVA analysis showed that there was no interaction effect and that the factor framing has a significant main effect on personal environmental behavior. Therefore, we collapsed our data across durations and analyzed our data only using the framing (year vs. day) as the IV in the subsequent analysis.

To assess the effect of temporal framing on the personal environmental behavior in both framing conditions, two separates one-way ANOVA analysis was conducted. Namely, the effect of time framing in the 27 years condition and the 41 years condition.

Framing 27 years. To test the effect of time framing on personal environmental behavior for 27 years, a one-way ANOVA analysis was conducted. According to Malhotra (2009), the one-way ANOVA provides an understanding of the influence of one nominal independent variable on an interval dependent variable. The results of this analysis can be found in

appendix table B.2 The result of the one-way ANOVA showed a marginally significant effect (F(310) = 2.585, p = 0.110),)η2p = 0.10). Participants in the 27 years conditions showed that personal behavior significantly was influenced when environmental risk was framed in a year as opposed to a day frame.

Blue = Day Frame

Red = Year Frame

(26)

Framing 41 years. Secondly, a one-way ANOVA Analysis for 41 years was conducted. The findings are presented in appendix table A.3. The result of the one-way ANOVA showed a significant difference (F(310) =3,173 p= 0.077., η2p = .050), Participants in the 41 years conditions showed that time framing significantly influenced personal behavior when environmental risk was framed in a year opposed to a day.

Based on both analyses, we can conclude that there was a main effect of framing on each duration. Additionally, if we compare both durations, The One-way Anova analysis was, in both cases, significant. Yet the effect of time framing in the condition of 41 years is stronger compared to the 27 years condition (although this effect did not qualify a significant

interaction). In the following analyses, we continue exploring data while the data is collapsed across the duration conditions.

Mediation Analyses. Therefore, in order to test Hypothesis 2, a mediation analysis was conducted by using the PROCESS Macro model 4 in SPSS (Hayes, 2013). Meditation test if the relationship between time framing and personal environmental behavior can be explained by environmental concern.

1

From the mediation analysis, Appendix B table 7, we can derive that the relationship

between the independent variable, time framing, and the mediator, the concern is significant.

1 1Firstly, we conducted a mediation analysis for both durations separately. 27 years was marginally significant mediated, and 41 was significantly mediated. These can be found in Appendix A 5 and A 6. Considering both mediations 27 and 41 years, have the same pattern of results, the durations are combined in the analysis, to have a larger sample size and have higher predictive power. For each duration separately, the results can be found in the appendix.

(27)

(B=0,4557, SE=0.1178, p=0.0108<0.05). The effect of the mediator's concern on the independent variable's environmental behavior is highly significant (B=0.4528, SE=0.0288, p=0.000<0.05), concern is highly significant in predicting personal behavioral intentions.

The direct effect of the independent variable, time framing on the dependent variable is not significant. (B=0.0819, SE=0.097, P=0.3678 >0.05). The indirect effect of X and Y is significant, which is confirmed by a bootstrap analysis (b = .2063, SE = .0819, 95% CI = .0449 to .370. Based on the analyses, we can state that there is an indirect significance since this 95% confidence interval is not including any zero values. Therefore, the entire effect of time framing is being conveyed to the dependent variable, trough the mediator concern, which is a sign of mediation. The result of the mediation analysis provides support for the second hypothesis. This mediation result supports our hypothesis 2.

Moderation Analysis. This research consisted of 2 moderators that may have influenced the relationship of the independent variable time framing on the dependent variable personal environmental behavior. To explore if the moderator's environmental self-efficacy and biospheric values separately are moderating the relationship between the independent variable and dependent variable, two separate moderation analysis were conducted.

(28)

Environmental Self-Efficacy. To identify whether environmental self-efficacy moderates the relationship between time framing on pro-environmental behavior, moderation analysis was conducted. This was done by using the PROCESS Macro model 1 in SPSS (Hayes, 2013).

The output can be found in Appendix B, Table 8. From the analysis we can conclude that the interaction is not significant within environmental values, (F (310) F= 0,1192 P>0,05 P = 0.7302. From figure 2, we can derive that among those who score low and average on environmental self-efficacy, the effect of framing is significant. The framing of time does not influence individuals that score above average.

Effect SE T P

Below Average .3128 .1594 1.9621 .0507

Average .2739 .1126 2.4327 .0156

Above Average .2350 .1593 1.4752 .1412

Figure 2

Biospheric values. The second moderator, biospheric values, showed a similar pattern.

Repeatedly we used the PROCESS Macro model 1 in SPSS (Hayes, 2013). The output can be found in Appendix B,Table 9. From the analysis we can conclude that the interaction is not significant (F (310) F= 0,3904 P>0,05 P = 0.5326. Although the interaction was not

significant, we looked into the effect of framing on the DV at different levels of the

moderator. These results are provided in figure 3; we can derive that among those who have low environmental values, the effect of framing is marginally significant. Such that a year has led to more pro-environmental behavior as opposed to a day frame. Among individuals who have high environmental values, framing has not further increased their pro-

environmental behavior, since this is not significant.

(29)

Effect SE T P Below Average .2419 .1415 1.7097 .0883

Average .1795 .0990 1.8135 .0707 Above Average .1172 .1397 .8389 .4022

Figure 3

Demographics. To show that the main effect of framing on personal environmental behavior remains significant, even after controlling for participants their age, gender, and income, an ANCOVA analysis was conducted. The three demographic variables were included as a covariate. From the analysis, we can state that the analysis was robust, which also can be seen in figure 4. Firstly, gender was included as a covariate. The analysis showed that framing still has a significant effect on the DV, (F(310) =4,667 p= 0.032., η2p = .100) Secondly, age was included as a covariate, and the analysis showed that framing has a significant effect on the DV, (F(310) =5,756, p= 0.017., η2p = .050), Lastly, the third covariate was included, income, the analysis showed that framing has a significant effect on the DV, namely (F(310) =5,713, p= 0.017., η2p = .050),

SS MS F P-

Value

Time Framing

SS

Time Framing

F

Time Framing

P-Value Gender 4.473 4.473 4.017 0.046 5.196 4.667 0.032

Age 1.074 1.074 0.955 0.329 6.438 5.756 0.017

Income 0,012 0,012 0.011 0.918 6.44 5.713 0.017

Figure 4

(30)

4.5 Summary of results

For this analysis, it was expected that participants exposed to risk expressed in days opposed to years would show higher pro-environmental behavior. Additionally, it was expected that this effect was mediated by environmental concern. Our two-way ANOVA analysis displays that time framing significantly influences pro-environmental behavior. However, the findings of the analysis showed a relationship in the opposite direction. Environmental risk framed in a year opposed to days resulted in significantly higher environmental behavior. The two-way ANOVA analysis also showed that the duration has no significant effect and therefore

becomes irrelevant as a factor.

Furthermore, the two separate one-way ANOVA analysis both found a significant effect of time framing on personal environmental intentions, across both durations, 27 years and 41 years. Therefore, we can state that there is nothing specific for 41 years. The findings can be generalized since, across both durations, the same pattern has occurred. In the 27 years as well as in the 41 years condition, it applies that individuals are more motivated to get engaged with environmentally oriented behavior when environmental risk is expressed in years opposed to days. Therefore, the findings may be generalized across durations since the pattern is consistent across both durations.

The mediation analysis showed that concern is fully mediating the relationship between time framing and behavior, and the two-way ANOVA analysis has shown that when risk is expressed in years, it resulted in more concern. Therefore, time expressed in more

granularity decreases concern and intention to get involved in environmental behavior and

therefore explains why individuals are more or less concerned, depending on the framing

condition. Since in a day frame risk is identified as occurring further in the future compared

to a year frame, a day frame will result in less concern since it is perceived as further away.

(31)

Concerning moderation, the influence of environmental self-efficacy on the relationship was not significant. Furthermore, the composition of the interaction shows that among those with low personal efficacy, framing was more effective. This is consistent with the expectations since, for individuals who are high in environmental self-efficacy, it is expected that regardless of framing, they are concerned about the environment. These findings are

consistent with our theory. Meinhold & Malkus (2005) argue that pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors are stronger among individuals with high levels of self-efficacy.

This is also relevant for the second moderator, biospheric values. It was found that among those who have low environmental values, the effect of framing is marginally significant.

They were more concerned and showed an increased tendency to get engaged with

environmentally oriented behavior significantly. Additionally, individuals that scored high on environmental values were not influenced by the framing of time since they are concerned anyhow. These findings are consistent with the literature since individuals with strong biospheric values are more likely to have pro-environmental intentions and to act pro- environmentally (Steg & Groot, 2012).

Across both moderation analysis, we can identify that the framing is influencing individuals

who scored low on environmental values and environmental self-efficacy. Lastly, the

robustness analysis showed that the main effect on the DV was still significant after

controlling for the covariates, gender, age, and income.

(32)

5. General Discussion

The next section will provide a discussion of the results and will present the theoretical and managerial implications. Additionally, the discussion of the limitations is included as well as suggestions for future research.

5.1 Theoretical Implications

This research paper started with the framework that expressing risk in higher granularity would result in more serious environmental concerns and higher pro-environmental personal behavioral intentions. Based on previous research, it was hypothesized that framing

environmental risk in a day frame opposed to a year frame would have resulted in a feeling of higher urgency and concern. An example of previous research is the study of Chandran &

Menon (2004), their findings support the main idea of this paper as they state that "everyday framing makes risk appear more proximal and concrete than every year framing." In this study, we found the opposite to be accurate; environmental risk framed in years opposed to environmental risk framed in days has resulted in more severe concern and pro-

environmental behavioral intentions. Findings of the present research contribute, it demonstrates that expressing risk in more granularity decreases the intention to get

environmentally involved compared to expressing risk in a year frame, and it is demonstrated that this effect is fully mediated by environmental concern. Support for this opposing

prediction may be influenced by 'numerosity.' Numerosity may have affected the perception of individuals rather than the idea of granularity. Resulting in a risk that is perceived as 'closer.' According to the numerosity perspective, differences are magnified when the choice of units leads to larger numbers (Monga & Bagchi, 2011). Numerosity is indeed driven by the salience of numbers and not units (Monga, & Bagchi, 2011). Furthermore, numerosity

research suggests that changes are magnified by small versus large units because a change

(33)

from 7 to 21 versus 1 to 3 seems larger (Monga & Bagchi, 2011). In this research paper, it, therefore, may be that small units, communicating the risk in days, are magnified compared to the large units, communicating the risk in years, which results in the day frame risk being perceived to be further in the future. This goes along with the argument that higher values elicit perceptions of larger quantity (Stone, Sieck, Bull, Yates, Parks, and Rush, 2003).

According to Pelham, Sumarta & Myaskovsky (1994), individuals are especially sensitive to numerosity as a cue for judging quantity. That is, people sometimes judge amount or

likelihood based on the number of units into which a stimulus is divided without fully considering other important variables such as the size of the units (Pelham et al., 1994), which may have been the case in this research. This argument is reinforced by Stone et al.

(2003), they state that individuals judge the magnitude of a quantitative expression by focusing on foreground information, such as the number at the expense of background information such as the unit. Additionally, Adaval et al. (2013) likewise argue that the unit effect occurs because consumers focus on the number rather than the type of units in which information is expressed. Therefore, in this research, the unit effect may have occurred.

Participants may have focused on the number that was used in the temporal framing, which is more significant in days compared to years. Support for this argument is that consequently, individuals learn to use largeness of a number to infer size, ignoring other cues (Bagchi & Li, 2010). Participants in the day frame condition may have focused on the largeness of the number 9855 days as opposed to the number 27 in the year frame. Therefore, the day frame may be perceived further in time compared to the risk framed in years.

Individuals process information passively and do not consider that the same information may have been framed differently (Monga & Bagchi, 2011). In this master thesis, it is showed that the framing of time, indeed has a significant effect on behavior in the environmental domain.

However, in the opposite direction, as proposed. The study demonstrated that when

(34)

environmental risk is communicated in years, it will influence individuals their

environmental, behavioral intentions. Therefore, it shows that framing an environmental risk in a year is a useful way to evoke, enhance, and stimulate environmental behavior and

intentions. These findings contribute to previous research about framing and provide the new insight that framing risk in the environmental domain is more effective when the risk is framed in years as opposed to days. Furthermore, this research also showed that this effect is fully mediated by environmental concern.

5.2 Managerial Implications

Based on the findings of this research, we can provide managers and policymakers with recommendations on how to communicate global climate change. Many organizations strive to make organizational processes and procedures more pro-environmental (Ruepert, Keizer,

& Steg, 2017). Besides that, this is internally relevant for the company; it is also of concern for marketing managers and policymakers that communicate environmental risk to the public.

Managers should be aware of messaging strategies that can ultimately mobilize

environmentally significant behavior, including climate change mitigation and adaptation

behaviors (Jarreau, Altinay, & Reynolds, 2017). Managers must be aware that the framing of

environmental risk might play a role in individuals' pro-environmental behavior. Based on

our findings, it can be stated that the framing of time can be used as a tool to enhance

marketing communication practices that are focused on environmental relevant products,

such as gas and energy. Furthermore, the results of our study are likewise relevant for

institutions that communicate environmental risks such as the Worldwide Fund for Nature.

(35)

This study provides insight into the managers; the findings from this study can be used to communicate environmental risk so that individuals change their environmental behavior.

Environmental risk is often perceived as far, and the consequences are often somewhat uncertain and occur over the long-term, which restricts the ability of an individual to realize the benefits of behaving pro-environmental (Grønhøj, &Thøgersen, 2011). The paper

provides a better understanding of how individuals can perceive environmental risk as closer

and therefore feel more urge to behave in a pro-environmentally way, with the usage of

temporal framing. The study suggests that environmental risk and the consequences should

be communicated in years to evoke pro-environmental behavior. According to the analysis,

customers perceive an environmental risk closer when the risk was framed in years compared

to days. Something that is of high importance to take into account when environmental risk is

communicated.

(36)

6. Limitations and Future research

The research that was conducted is not without limitations. These limitations also can provide a starting point for new paths of research. Firstly, the study consisted of 310 participants, and the individuals were randomized into four different conditions. Therefore, assuming the external validity of the findings, the results of the study can be generalized across other situations, individuals, and times (Aronson, Wilson, Akert, Fehr, 2004).

Nevertheless, to make the findings more externally valid, it could be of interest to focus on a larger sample size. Concerning the composition of the sample, all participants are from the United States. It may be of concern to conduct this research on individuals that are from other parts of the world to identify if this study has the same results in different countries.

Specifically, because the environmental risk is affecting individuals across the globe, and it is of importance that human beings are aware of how to communicate most effectively to stimulate and evoke pro-environmental personal behavior.

Concerning the internal validity of this study, the extent to which a piece of evidence supports a claim about cause and effect in the context of a particular study (Reis & Judd, 2000), the temporal manipulation only occurred once in this study. For future research, temporal manipulation can be made more salient and mentioned multiple times, besides in the manipulation, which can be done through the entire study emphasizing the day frame and the year frame.

In this research, we have found that time framing has influenced the perception of individuals in the environmental domain. Furthermore, we argued that granularity would result in time being perceived as shorter compared to the expression of time in less a granular expression.

The findings of the analysis showed that the opposite is accurate. Future research could,

therefore, focus on both viewpoints. Considering we have two opposing theoretical

predictions, namely moving towards communication with high granularity and

(37)

communication that is less granular, future research can highlight both predictions and explore which manner is best to communicate environmental risk most effectively.

Furthermore, while consumers may differ in how familiar they are with certain attribute information, they also may differ in their ability to process quantitative information (Pandelaere et al. 2011). Notably, some individuals appear to be more "numerate" than

others. Some studies indicate that numerate people are less likely to exhibit the framing effect (Peters, Västfjäll, Slovic, Mertz, Mazzocco & Dickert, 2006). Therefore, it may also be of interest to incorporate individual characteristics in this study, where you test both predictions to see to which extent personal characteristics are relevant in the perception of time in the environmental domain.

Besides, in this research, we used day framing and year framing, it may be of interest to explore what will happen if we move towards a more non-granular unit in expressing future time, such as a specific date. In future research, we can mention the date of occurrence of environmental risk, for example, the year 2048 instead of the duration expressed in days or years. This argument is based upon the pattern that we found in this research, that expressing risk in less granularity has resulted in more significant perceived concern and higher pro- environmental personal behavioral intentions.

Temporal framing has been explored in different domains, such as the food domain (Siddiqui, May and Monga, 2011) and health risk domain (Chandran & Menon, 2004).

This research contributed to explore framing in the environmental domain. For future

research, it may be of interest to examine day and year framing and granularity in other

domains where communication and influencing individuals is crucial.

(38)

7. Conclusion

The purpose of this study was to explore how the framing of time affects environmental

concern and pro-environmental behavior. The present research provides framing of time in a

new domain, the environmental domain. The findings of the paper suggest that time framing

does significantly affect pro-environmental behavior, when environmental risk is framed in a

year, as opposed to a day frame. This effect is fully mediated by environmental concern.

(39)

8. References

Adaval, R., Pandelaere, M., Briers, B., Lembregts, C., Bagchi, R., Li, X., ... & Coulter, K. S.

(2013). Numerosity and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(5), xi-xiv.

Antonides, G., Verhoef, P. C., & Van Aalst, M. (2002). Consumer perception and evaluation of waiting time: A field experiment. Journal of consumer psychology, 12(3), 193-202.

Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., Akert, R. M., & Fehr, B. (2004). Social psychology (2nd Canadian ed.). Toronto, ON, Canada: Pearson Education.

Bagchi, R., & Li, X. (2010). Illusionary progress in loyalty programs: Magnitudes, reward distances, and step-size ambiguity. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(5), 888-901.

Baker, J., & Cameron, M. (1996). The effects of the service environment on affect and consumer perception of waiting time: An integrative review and research propositions.

Journal of the Academy of marketing Science, 24(4), 338.

Baldassare, M., & Katz, C. (1992). The personal threat of environmental problems as predictor of environmental practices. Environment and Behavior, 24(5), 602-616.

Bamberg, S., & Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A

new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. Journal of

environmental psychology, 27(1), 14-25.

(40)

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Macmillan.

Bandura, A., & Wood, R. (1989). Effect of perceived controllability and performance standards on self-regulation of complex decision making. Journal of personality and social psychology, 56(5), 805.

Benjamin, D., Por, H. H., & Budescu, D. (2017). Climate change versus global warming:

who is susceptible to the framing of climate change?. Environment and Behavior, 49(7), 745- 770.

Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S., &

Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365.

Casella, G. (2008). Statistical design. Springer Science & Business Media.

Chan, E. Y. (2018). Climate change is the world's greatest threat–In Celsius or Fahrenheit?.

Journal of Environmental Psychology, 60, 21-26.

Chan, H. W. (2019). When do values promote pro-environmental behaviors? Multilevel evidence on the self-expression hypothesis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 101361.

Chandran, S., & Menon, G. (2004). When a day means more than a year: Effects of temporal

framing on judgments of health risk. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(2), 375-389.

(41)

Chen, X., De la Rosa, J., Peterson, M. N., Zhong, Y., & Lu, C. (2016). Sympathy for the environment predicts green consumerism but not more important environmental behaviours related to domestic energy use. Environmental conservation, 43(2), 140-147.

Churchill, S., Good, A., & Pavey, L. (2014). Promoting the avoidance of high-calorie snacks.

The role of temporal message framing and eating self-efficacy. Appetite, 80, 131-136.

De Groot, J. I., & Steg, L. (2008). Value orientations to explain beliefs related to

environmental significant behavior: How to measure egoistic, altruistic, and biospheric value orientations. Environment and Behavior, 40(3), 330-354.

De Young, R. (1993). Changing behavior and making it stick: The conceptualization and management of conservation behavior. Environment and behavior, 25(3), 485-505.

Diekmann, A., & Franzen, A. (2019). Environmental concern: A global perspective. In Einstellungen und Verhalten in der empirischen Sozialforschung (pp. 253-272). Springer VS, Wiesbaden.

Dietz, T., Kalof, L., & Stern, P. C. (2002). Gender, values, and environmentalism. Social science quarterly, 83(1), 353-364.

Dillon, R. L., Tinsley, C. H., & Cronin, M. (2011). Why near‐miss events can decrease an

individual's protective response to hurricanes. Risk Analysis: An International Journal, 31(3),

440-449.

(42)

Doyle, J. R. (2012). Survey of time preference, delay discounting models. Delay Discounting Models (April 20, 2012).

Droit-Volet, S., & Meck, W. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(12), 504-513.

Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G., & Jones, R. E. (2000). New trends in

measuring environmental attitudes: measuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm:

a revised NEP scale. Journal of social issues, 56(3), 425-442.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of personality and social psychology, 65(1), 45.

Gärling, T., & Schuitema, G. (2007). Travel demand management targeting reduced private car use: effectiveness, public acceptability and political feasibility. Journal of Social Issues, 63(1), 139-153.

Gattig, A., & Hendrickx, L. (2007). Judgmental discounting and environmental risk

perception: Dimensional similarities, domain differences, and implications for sustainability.

Journal of Social Issues, 63(1), 21-39.

Gerend, M. A., & Cullen, M. (2008). Effects of message framing and temporal context on

college student drinking behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1167-

1173.

(43)

Gifford, R., & Comeau, L. A. (2011). Message framing influences perceived climate change competence, engagement, and behavioral intentions. Global Environmental Change, 21(4), 1301-1307.

Gifford, R., & Nilsson, A. (2014). Personal and social factors that influence pro‐

environmental concern and behaviour: A review. International Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 141-157.

Gourville, J. T. (1998). Pennies-a-day: The effect of temporal reframing on transaction evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 395-408.

Graber, D. (2004). Mediated politics and citizenship in the twenty-first century. Annu. Rev.

Psychol., 55, 545-571.

Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: a review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561-582.

Grønhøj, A., & Thøgersen, J. (2011). Feedback on household electricity consumption:

learning and social influence processes. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 35(2), 138-145.

Grønhøj, A., & Thøgersen, J. (2012). Action speaks louder than words: The effect of personal attitudes and family norms on adolescents’ pro-environmental behaviour. Journal of

Economic Psychology, 33(1), 292-302.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Our findings underscored that a strong therapeutic alliance between treatment team members and parents is an effective common process fac- tor of child semi-residential

The second aim of this study was to identify if there was a difference between field dependent and field independent students in the degree they applied the naïve impetus theory

BETREFT : Ontwerp van koninklijk besluit tot wijziging van het koninklijk besluit van 14 maart 1991 waarbij aan de griffiers van de hoven en de rechtbanken van de Rechterlijke

In zijn hoedanigheid van voogdijminister van het Gewestelijk Agentschap voor Netheid vraagt de Minister bij brief van 20 oktober 2000 de Commissie een advies uit te brengen omtrent

Het aan de Commissie ter advies voorgelegde ontwerp van koninklijk besluit heeft tot doel de Vlaamse Landmaatschappij toegang te verlenen tot het Rijksregister van de

11 De memorie van toelichting inzake artikel 4 is zeer duidelijk op dit punt... d) De Commissie wenst een specifieke opmerking te maken die steunt op de aangewende legistieke

• Compute the luminosity that this star had at its onset of core hydrogen burning.. • Compute the average density of this star at

In welke mate heeft u accent gelegd op de informatievoorziening van deze mogelijkheid aan de studenten om de activiteiten af te stemmen op de beschikbare vrije tijd van de student?.