• No results found

Reversal of the rise time cue in the affricate fricative contrast, an experiment on the silence of sound

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Reversal of the rise time cue in the affricate fricative contrast, an experiment on the silence of sound"

Copied!
7
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

V.J. van Heuven 181

R E V E R S A L OF THF. R I S F - Ί Ι Μ Ε CUE IN THE AFFRICATE-FR1CATIVE CONTRAST: AN E X P E R I M E N T ON THE SILENCE OF SOUND*

Vincent J. van Heuven

Dept. of Linguistics/Phonetics Laboratory, Leyden University, P.O. Box 9515, 2300 RA Leiden, I he Netherlands

1. INTRODUCTION

1.1. Cues underlymg the a f f r i c a t e - f r i c a t i v e contrast

T r a d i t i o n a l l y , Imguists distlnguish true consonants (or: obstruents) along a three term manner of articulation dimension: stop, fricative, and affricate. In this paper we shall be concerned with the contrast betweeri two of these categories: affricate versus fricative, äs in the word pair cho£ - shop. This contrast has multiple acoustic cues, which are listed m table I:

Table I, acoustic cues i n v o l v e d in the a f f r i c a t e - f r i c a t i v e contrast (1) Duration of the preeonsonantal v o w e l (Isenberg, 1978)

(2) Decay rate of the preconsonantal vowel amplitude (Debrock, 1977) (3) Duration of the pre-burst silent m t e r v a l (e.g., Kuipers, 1955;

Truby, 1955)

(4) Formant transitions of the preconsonantal v o w e l (Isenberg, 1978; Dorman, Raphael & Isenberg, 1980)

(5) Rise time of the f n c t i o n noise amplitude (e.g., Gerstman, 1957) (6) Duration of the fnction noise (e.g., Gerstman, 1957)

(7) Rise time of the post-consonantal v o w e l amplitude (Debrock, 1977) (8) Presence/absenoe of a r e l e a s e burst (Dorman et al., 1980)

Perceptual relevance has not been estabhshed for all of these acoustic correlates, let alone m a single expenment, but over the y e a r s the research has forused on three of them, and their trading relations: noise amplitude rise time (henceforth: rise time), noise duration (henceforth: duration), and the duration of the silent interval separating the precedmg vowel and the fnction noise (henceforth: interval). So far the f o l l o w i n g e f f e c t s have emerged:

I thank Peter Vroege for running the expenment described m this paper.

(2)

1 ί V.J. van lleuven Table II, Effects of cues involved in ehe affncate-fncative contrast

fncative affricate rise time gradual abrupt

duration relatively long relatively short mterval short (absent) long

The research on trade-offs between these cues has had a long history, beginning with Gerstman (1957). A re-analysis of bis data (van Heuven, 1979) has shown that duration and rise time can be traded only in the very narrow ränge between 90 and 130 ms noise duration, mdicaung that duration is the pnmary cue to the contrast, dt least for Stimuli with the c o n t r a s t in initial Position.

Tradings mvolvmg the mterval can only be e x a m m e d in a context with a segment (typically a vowel) preceding the contrast. Though there are several studles mampulating mterval äs a single Parameter (e.g., Kuipers, 1955, Truby, 1955), tradmg research has been quite limited. T w o - p a r a m e t e r studies involving the m t e r v a l (agamst duration) are described by Repp, Liberman, Eccardt & Pesetsky (1978), and by Dorman et al. (1980) mterval versus release burst, duration, and formant transitions. Regulär trading relations were estabhshed for the mterval parameter m all of these studies· the effect of a longer mterval cueing a f f r i c a t e could be offset by a more f r i c a t i v e - l i k e value for the competing parameter.

Tradings mvolvmg the rise time parameter in c o n t e x t have been investigated least of all. Yet, in one study rise time, too, was found to trade regularly w i t h mterval (Dorman, Raphael & l iberrnan, 1979, expenment also described m Dorman et al., 1980). Here the a f f n c a t e - f n c a t i v e c o n t r a s t was exammed in word-fmal Position (ditch dish) the effect of slower rise time cueing fricative could be counteracted by a longer mterval (37 vs. 57 ms m t e r v a l for cross-overs at 0 and 35 ms nse time, respectively).

Van Heuven (1983) studied the effects of rise time and duration In word-mitial position (chop - Shop) for isolated Stimuli, and for the same tokens embedded m a carrier Why oon't you say ... again? For

(3)

1.2. Possible causes for the reversal

In my ( 1 9 8 3 ) papp r I explamed this cunous reversal of the rise time cue in c o n t e x t äs an e f f e c t of forward masklng. This seemed a reasonable h y p o t h e s i s , smce the pre-burst vowel had been recorded in an utterance p i e o e d i n g a chop token, i.e., with a relatively abrupt mtensity o f f - r a m p äs is charactenstic of pre-stop v o w e l s (Debrock, 1977). Plornp ( 1 9 6 4 ) , arnong others, has shown that the human ear is r e l a t i v e l y msertsmve to auditory Stimulation shortly a f t e r the abrupt termlnation of a high mtensity acoustic event. A subsequent Iow mtensity sound w i l l not be heard during this period of masklng, or will at least appear w e a k e r than when presented in Isolation. ' The masklng period may e x t e n d for äs long äs 250 ms, though the effect rapidly d e c a y s ovei Urne.

Due to m a s k i n g , then, our h s t e n e r s might have heard a brief mterval of s i l e n c e (or reduced energy), suppressing and replacing the Iow mtensity noise onset of the f r i c t i o n sound. The smoother the noise onset, the longer the noise would remain below the masking threshold, c r e a t m g a longer perreived gap äs w e l l äs a shorter perceived noise duration, which two illusions then conspired to cue a f f r i c a t e .

H o w e v e r , m s p i t e of the prima facie attractiveness of this account, there are complications t h a t may force us to reconsider. For masking to occ ur it is necessary that the frequency distribution of the masker (here· v o w e l ) comcides or o v e r l a p s with that of the probe (here: f r i c t i o n n o i s e ) . I hus, in Plomp ( 1 9 6 4 ) pure tones were masked by w h i t e noise (see further, Resnick, Weiss & Heinz, 1979). In my v o w e l - n o i s e s e q u e n c e s U seems unlikely that the masker and probe frequency d i s t r i b u t i o n s w e r e s u f f i c i e n t l y sirmlar to cause strong masking e f f e c t s thf> v o w e l /ei/ has rnost of its energy below 2500 H/, w h e r e a s the /sh/ noise has its energy concentrated above this value (Heinz & S t e v e n s , 1 9 6 1 ) .

As an a l t e r n a t i v e e x p l a n a t i o n for the reversal of the rise time cue in c o n t e x t l now propose the followmg: Let us assume that it is a necessary condition for hsteners to perceive an a f f r i c a t e that the Stimulus c o n t a m a h r i e f mterval of silence (or reduced energy) immediately p r e c e d m g the friction noise, so äs to reflect the presence of an a r t i c u l a t o r y c losure ( c f . Dorman et al., 1979). If such a silent mterval is a b s e n t from the physical Stimulus, the listener may have to remterprete the a t o u s t u · Signal t r y i n g to s a t i s f y the condition for a silent m t e r v a l . It is < onoeivable, then, that he will consider the Iow energy portion in the smooth noise onset to be the silent mterval.

(4)

» . J · V d i i l l e u v e n

1.3. Approach

lf the cue reversa! is indeed a m a t t e r of r e i n t e r p r e t i n g the a v a i l a b l e

cues so äs to perceive a s i l e n t i n t e r v a l , it should not m a t t e r to the

listener w h e t h e r the Iow i n t e n s i t y p o r t i o n of the S t i m u l u s is located in

the noise onset, or in the o f f s e t of the preceding vowel, äs l o n g äs

the energy dip occurs at the V C - b o u n d a r y , i.e., at a p o i n t in t i m e

w h e r e a stop-closure can be located.

Therefore, c h a n g i n g the vowel offset from a b r u p t to g r a d u a l

should increase the number of affricate judgments when the vowel is

closely followed by the f r i c t i o n sound; moreover, g r a d u a l vowel o f f s e t

and noise onset should r e i n f o r c e one another, since both m a n i p u l a i i o n s

can be reinterpreted äs the s i l e n t i n t e r v a l . M a s k i n g , on the other

h a n d should disappear w h e n the vowe! i n t e n s i t y decays g r a d u a l l y , and

no effect of m a s k i n g should o b t a i n at all when the vowel o f f - r a m p is

longer than some 70 ms ( c f . van den Broecke & van Heuven, 1983). As

a result the noise rise time cue should reverse a f t e r an a b r u p t vowel

terminat'ion, but f u n c t i o n n o r m a l l y (i.e., äs in S t i m u l u s i n i t i a l p o s i t i o n )

w h e n the vowel decay is smooth. Thus, v a r y i n g noise tise time versus

vowel decay time in a t w o - p a r a m e t e r study w i l l a l l o w us to choose

between the competing ex p l a n a t i o n s .

2. METHOD

A m a l e n a t i v e Speaker of R . P . - E n g l i s h recorded the sentence

Why dpn't you say shop again observing normal Intonation and t i m i n g .

The audiosignal was d i g i t i s e d (10 k H z , 12 bits, 4.5 kHz LP c u t - o f f ) and

stored in Computer memory (DEC Micro PDP-11/23). Using a d i g i t a l

tape e d i t i n g program a new utterance was created by c o n c a t e n a t i n g

parts of the d i g i t a l record, äs f o l l o w s .

The utterance was t r u n c a t e d a f t e r the word say at a p o s i t i v e

going zero crossing at the end of the g l o t t a l period whose i n t e n s i t y

was no less than 90 percent of the peak value reached throughout the

vowel. As it happened, t h i s e l i m i n a t e d the f i n a l three g l o t t a l periods

from the vowel. To this a b r u p t vowel c e r m i n a t i o n were appended a 10

ms silent i n t e r v a l and a 60 ms Stretch of steady state [sh]-noise, gated

out from the centre portion of the o r i g i n a l f r i c a t i v e . This in turn was

followed by the f i n a l 20 ms of f r i c t i o n before the C V - b o u n d a r y in

shop and the remainder of the o r i g i n a l utterance. As a r e s u l t the

f r i c t i o n noise had an a b r u p t onset, and lasted for 80 ms, i n c l u d i n g the

t r a n s i t i o n into the f o l l o w i n g vowel. The p a r t i c u l a r t e m p o r a l

O r g a n i s a t i o n was chosen so äs to create a S t i m u l u s t h a t could be

i n t e r p r e t e d äs either shop or chop.

(5)

lu J 8 different ranclorn Orders, preceded by 10 practice

separated by 2 s interstirnulus intervals (offset to onset).

items, and

The entire tape was played in a quiet room over headphones to 5 audiometrically normal adult n a t i v e English listeners. They were instructed to decide for each Stimulus whether they perceived shop or chop w i t h binary forced choice.

3. RESULTS AND CONCLUSION

Percent f r i c a t i v e judgements was determined for each of the 25 Stimulus types (N = ΊΟ judgments per Stimulus type). Figure l plots these percentages äs a joint function of v o w e l o f f s e t (horizontal dimension) and noise onset ( v e r t i c a l dimension).

Let us c o n c e n t r a t e , f i r s t of all, on the r e s u l t s obtained f o r S t i m u l i w i t h a b r u p t l y terminating vowels. Mere we notice that abrupt noise onset is associated w i t h f r i c a t i v e , and gradual onset. w i t h a f f r i c a t e . This e f f e c t runs counter to w h a t is usually claimed in the literature (where gradual noise onset is a cue for f r i c a t i v e ) , but replicates our earlier results for this parameter w i t h shup/chop tokens presented in a spoken c o n t e x t . A p p a r e n t l y , both the abrupt noise onset ond the 10 ms of p h y s i c a l silence are c o m p l e t e l y m a s k e d by the p r e c e d i n g v o w e l . Both t o ourselves and to our subjects the transition of v o w e l into c o n s o n a n t sounded p e r f e c t l y smooth. ao

60

l 20 20 60 80

VOWEL DECAY TIME (MS) FIGURE l, Percent chop responses äs a function of the rise time of the f r i c t i o n noise and decay time of the

preconsonantal vowel. The phoneme boundary separating a f f r i c a t e (shaded) from f r i c a t i v e (open) areas is d r a w n through the 50% cross-over points, which were determined by linear

Interpolation between Stimuli straddling the boundary.

(6)

'. DiSCUSSION

Though the masking hypothesis has been convincingly f a l s i f i e d , should be noted that some measure of masking still persists. It was bserved that ihe 10 ms s i l e n t gap separating vowel and fnction sound äs inaudible, which e f f e c t has to be ascribed to masking. Apart from US temporal resolutiori of the intensity envelope was excelient iroughout the expenment, since even the insertion of a mere 20 ms oise on-ramp or v o w e l o f f - r a m p brought about a 22% morease <>i ffncate judgments (cf. f.gure I). Ihus .t would seem that the effects f forward maskmg m the present speech c o n t e x t are «"™*'ν m.ted which findimt is .n Une wuh e.g. SUe & van Nierop (1970) and et al showed that vowel onto consonant masking

'

v o a n d consonants ,„ norma, speech , s I w a y s less than this value.

Secondly our resuits bear out that a silent mterval is mdeed a oressarv condi'tion for the perception of the affricate (or stop) nner When such an mterva! is physically absent, low intensity ortions of the Stimulus flankmg the VC-boundary are reinterpreted äs ilence. This behaviour seems to support the View that dunng speech ercepuon the acoustic cues are evaluated in the hght of what the .stener knows about articulation. One wonders if reinterpretation of ues could be used in a more pnncipled way to examine the relative •nportance of multiple cues in phonetic contrasts. Clearly, if one c ue an be reinterpreted äs an other, but not vice versa, the on-negotiable cue is the strenger of the two. In this light our resuits ndicate once more that rjse time is a manner cue of limited mportance, especially for contrasts occurrmg in connected speech.

Thirdly, "sound" reinterpreted äs "silence" provides a less vowerful cue to a f f r i c a t e than physical silence does. We may observe hat affricate judgments plateau at 70-75%, which means that the .ffricate end of the Stimulus space was not highly convmcmg. Oiven he resuits of other studies much better exemplars of affncates can be ^enerated if a proper penod of silence is inserted between vowel and

nction burst.

(7)

e x a g g e r a t e the p e r e c e i v e d l e n g t h of silence so äs to make the S t i m u l u s

fit a stop p e r c e p t .

R E F E R E N C E S

1. Broecke, M . P . R . van den and Broek, D. van der (1978). M a t c h i n g

slopes w i t h e l e c t r o n i c switches. Progress Report of the Institute

of P h o n e t i c s U t r e c h t 3.2. 24-26. ~

2. Broecke, M . P . R . van den and H e u v e n , V . J . van (1983). E f f e c t and

a r t i f a c t in the a u d i t o r y p e r c e p t i o n of rise and decay time: speech

and n o n - s p e f c h . P e r c e p t i o n and Psychophysics 33. 305-313.

3. Debrock, M. ( 1 9 7 7 ) . An acoustic c o r r e l a t e of the force of

a r t i c u l a t i o n . J o u r n a l of Phonetics 5, 61-80.

4. D o r m a n , M . F . , R a p h a e l and L.J., L i b e r m a n , A.M. (1979). Some

e x p e r i m e n t s on the sound of s i l e n c e in phonetic perception

J o u r n a l of the A c o u s t i c a l Society of A m e r i c a 65. 1518-1532.

5. D o r m a n , M . F . , R a p h a e l , L.J. and Isenberg, D. (1980). Acoustic cues

for a f r i c a t i v e - a f f r i r a t e c o n t r a s t in w o r d - f i n a l Position, Journal

of Phonetics 8, 397-405.

6. G e r s t m a n , L.J. ( 1 9 5 7 ) . Perceptual d i m e n s l o n s for the friction

portions of r e r t a i n speech sounds. U n p u b l i s h e d Ph.D. dissertation

N e w Y o r k U m v e r s i l y .

7. H e i n z , J . M . and Stevens, K . N . ( 1 9 6 1 ) . On the properties of

voiceless f r i c a t i v p consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society

of A m e r i c a 33. 589-596.

8. H e u v e n , V . J . van ( 1 9 7 9 ) . The r e l a t i v e c o n t r i b u t i o n of rise time,

steady t i m e , and o v e r a l d u r a t i o n of noise bursts to the a f f r i c a t e

-f r i c a t i v e d i s t i n c t i o n i n E n g l i s h . I n J.J. Wol-f a n d D.H. K l a t t (eds.),

ASA*50 Speech C o m m u n i c a t i o n Papers. The Acoustical Society of

A m e r i c a , New" York", 307-311.

9. Heuven, V . J . van (1983). Rise t i m e and duration of friction noise

äs p e r c e p t u a l cues i n the a f f r i c a t e - f r i c a t i v e contrast in English.

In M . P . R . van den Broecke, V . J . van Heuven and W. Zonneveld

(eds.), Sound s t r u c t u r e s , studies for Antonie Cohen. Forls,

D o r d r e c h t , I ~ 4 l - f 5 7 . ~

10. Isenberg, D. ( 1 9 7 8 ) . E f f e c t of s p e a k i n g rate on the relative

d u r a t i o n of stop closure and f r i c a t i v e noise. H a s k i n s Laboratories

Status R e p o r t on Speech Research SR-55/56, 63-79.

11. K u i p e r s , A . H . ( 1 9 5 5 ) . A f f r i c a t e s in intervocalic Position, Haskins

L a b o r a t o r i e s C j u a r t e r l y Progress Report 15, a p p e n d i x 6.

12. Plomp, R. ( 1 9 6 4 ) . R a t e of decay of auditory Sensation. Journal of

the A c o u s t i c a l S o c i e t y of America 36, 277-284.

13. Repp, B.H., Liberrrian, A.L., Eccardt, T., and Pesetsky, D. (1978).

Perceptual I n t e g r a t i o n of acoustic cues for stop, a f f r i c a t e , and

f r i c a t i v e m a n n e t · . J o u r n a l of E x p e r i m e n t a l Psychology: Human

Perception atid Perfu_rrn_ance_4, 621-637.

14. R e s n i c k , S.B., W e i s s , M.S. and H e i n z , J.M. (1979). M a s k i n g of

f i l t e r e d noise b u r s t s by s y n t h e t i c vowels. J o u r n a l of the

A c o u s t i c a l Society of America 66, 674-677.

15. Slis, I.H. ancTRierop, D.J.P.J. van (1970). On the forward masking

t h r e s h o l d of v o w e l s in V C - c o m b i n a t l o n s . 1PO Annual Progress

Report 5. 6 8 - 7 2 .

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

The conceptual model sketches the main research question which is aimed at finding out the influences of resistors and enablers on collaborative behaviours, and how

Day of the Triffids (1951), I Am Legend (1954) and On the Beach (1957) and recent film adaptations (2000; 2007; 2009) of these novels, and in what ways, if any,

function of rise time (vertically) and duration of the friction portion (hori- zontally), separated out for slow and fast speech rate in panels A and B, respectively, and

Contrast enhanced MRI plays a crucial role in the diagnosis of cochlear pathology associated with sensorineural hearing loss and may directly impact patient management..

As one of the most famous severely hearing impaired people the world has known, Ludwig van Beethoven was chosen for the illustration.. The music,”Freudvoll und Leidvoll”,

Er werden drie onderzoeken ingesloten in het Cochrane systematische literatuuronderzoek, met in totaal 135 deelnemers bij wie het effect van SSRI’s op slaapproblemen werd vergeleken

In 2008 en 2009 zijn op 3 momenten over een afstand van 20 m het aantal kevers geteld (tabel 2) Op 15 juli zijn de aantallen volwassen aspergekevers geteld vóór en na een

One example is the provision on ‘serious offence’ in Directive 96/26/EC on the admission to the occupation of road haulage operator and road passenger transport operator and