• No results found

To what extent do socioeconomic inequalities in SRH reflect inequalities in bIurden of disease?: The HELIUS study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "To what extent do socioeconomic inequalities in SRH reflect inequalities in bIurden of disease?: The HELIUS study"

Copied!
10
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Tilburg University

To what extent do socioeconomic inequalities in SRH reflect inequalities in bIurden of

disease?

Galenkamp, H.; van Oers, J. A. M.; Stronks, K.

Published in:

Journal of Public Health DOI:

10.1093/pubmed/fdz173

Publication date: 2020

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to publication in Tilburg University Research Portal

Citation for published version (APA):

Galenkamp, H., van Oers, J. A. M., & Stronks, K. (2020). To what extent do socioeconomic inequalities in SRH reflect inequalities in bIurden of disease? The HELIUS study. Journal of Public Health, 41(4), e412-e420. https://doi.org/10.1093/pubmed/fdz173

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

(2)

e412 This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution NonCommercial-NoDerivs licence (© The Author(s) 2019. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of Faculty of Public Health. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial reproduction and distribution of the work, in any medium, provided the original work is not altered or transformed in any way, and that the work properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact journals.permissions@oup.com Henrike Galenkamp, PhD

Hans van Oers, Professor, PhD Karien Stronks, Professor, PhD

To what extent do socioeconomic inequalities in SRH reflect

inequalities in burden of disease? The HELIUS study

Henrike Galenkamp

1

, Hans van Oers

2,3

, Karien Stronks

1

1Department of Public Health, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, 22660 1100 DD Amsterdam, The Netherlands 2Tilburg School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Tranzo Scientific Center for Care and Welfare, Tilburg University, 90153 5000 LE Tilburg, The Netherlands 3National Institute for Public Health and the Environment, 1 3720 BA Bilthoven, The Netherlands

Address correspondence to Henrike Galenkamp, E-mail: H.galenkamp@amc.nl.

A B S T R A C T

Background Self-rated health (SRH), an attractive measure for health monitoring, shows persistent inequalities with regard to socioeconomic

status (SES). However, knowledge on the extent to which inequalities in SRH reflect inequalities in disease burden is lacking.

Methods Data come from the multi-ethnic HEalthy LIfe in an Urban Setting study (Dutch, South-Asian Surinamese, African Surinamese,

Ghanaian, Turkish or Moroccan origin, N= 19 379, aged 18–70). SES was defined by educational and occupational level. Disease burden was operationalized as chronic diseases, physical and mental functioning (measured with SF-12) and depressive symptoms (measured with PHQ-9). We applied logistic regression analyses and reported average marginal effects (AME).

Results Dutch origin participants with low educational or low occupational level had higher probabilities of reporting fair/poor SRH, compared

to the highest levels (AME= 0.20 95% CI: 0.13;0.27; and 0.12 (0.09;0.15), respectively). Associations were attenuated after adjusting for all disease burden indicators, to AME= 0.03 (0.01;0.04) and AME = 0.02 (−0.00;0.04). In all the non-Dutch origin groups, a larger part of the inequalities remained after adjustment.

Conclusion Socioeconomic inequalities in SRH are for a large part explained by higher disease burden in lower socioeconomic groups, but less

so in those with non-Dutch origin. Future research should examine if our conclusions also hold for trend data on inequalities in SRH.

Keywords chronic diseases, HELIUS study, mental health, self-rated health, socioeconomic inequalities

Introduction

Self-rated health (SRH) is an often used instrument to assess health status in health monitoring. It measures the perception of health and thus incorporates various aspects of health that people may have in mind, such as the absence or presence of chronic or acute diseases, problems with physical functioning, lifestyle factors and psychosocial factors.1–5 The fact that this single item health measure is relatively easy to measure and that it captures a wide range of health aspects whilst performing well in predicting mortality6,7 makes SRH an attractive health indicator in monitoring population health.

Socioeconomic inequalities in health, to the advantage of those with a higher socioeconomic status (SES), are also shown for SRH. For example, large inequalities in SRH8 and in healthy life expectancy (i.e. number of years expected to live in good SRH)9 exist between groups with high and low educational level, and these inequalities are persistent

over time.10 It is, however, poorly understood what these inequalities in SRH reflect.

Since many studies confirmed associations between SRH on the one hand, and morbidity or mortality on the other hand,11–13 it is easily and often implicitly assumed that inequalities in SRH reflect inequalities in what is often called ‘objective’ or ‘actual’ health status. However, it is unclear to what extent socioeconomic inequalities in SRH can be explained by conventional measures of more objective health, such as the presence of chronic diseases and limitations in everyday functioning. Does the gap in SRH reflect the higher actual burden of disease in lower socioeconomic groups, or does it merely reflect poorer living conditions, or other

(3)

SOCIOECONOMIC INEQUALITIES IN SRH AND DISEASE BURDEN e413

environmental or social conditions,14 which may also be incorporated in health assessments?

Two studies indeed concluded that inequalities in SRH largely reflect the higher disease burden in lower socioe-conomic groups, by investigating the explanatory value of chronic diseases and problems with functioning for socioe-conomic inequalities in SRH.15,16 Both studies also showed that significant inequalities remained. It is unsure, however, whether results are similar in a general population sample, since Leão et al. have included people aged 50 and over, and Simon et al. included a sample with chronic disease patients over-represented.15,16

The current study aims to assess the extent to which inequalities in SRH reflect burden of disease across socioe-conomic groups: chronic diseases, physical and mental functioning and depressive symptoms. Previous research has indicated that associations between SRH and its determinants may differ across demographic groups.17,18Therefore, we use data from a multi-ethnic cohort study based in Amsterdam and primarily perform analyses on respondents with Dutch ethnic origin, both in total and stratified by age and sex. In addition, we analyse whether results differ in non-Dutch origin groups.

Methods Sample

The aim and design of the HEalthy LIfe in an Urban Setting (HELIUS) study have been described in detail elsewhere.19,20 In brief, the HELIUS study is a multi-ethnic cohort study conducted in Amsterdam, the Netherlands. Subjects were randomly, stratified by ethnicity, selected from the Amsterdam municipal register and sent an invitation letter by mail. We were able to contact 55% of those invited, either by response card or after a home visit by an ethnically matched same-sex interviewer. Of those, 50% agreed to participate (60% amongst Dutch, 51% amongst Surinamese (South-Asian Surinamese and African Surinamese), 61% amongst Ghanaians, 41% amongst Turks and 43% amongst Moroccans). Non-response analysis revealed small differences in SES between participants and non-participants.20After a positive response, participants received a confirmation letter of the appointment for the physical examination, including a digital or paper version of the questionnaire. Participants who were unable to complete the questionnaire themselves were offered assistance from a trained ethnically matched interviewer.

Of all participants who completed the questionnaire and who took part in the physical examination (N = 22 165), we first excluded those not belonging to the six largest ethnic

groups (n = 548). We further excluded those with missing educational level (n = 195, 0.9%). Participants with missing occupational level (n = 3273, 15.1%), of whom 38% had no education or elementary education, were retained in the analysis as a separate category. Further excluded were those with missing values on SRH (n = 57, 0.3%) or on measures of disease burden (see next section): chronic diseases (n = 1264, 5.8%), other SF-12 items (n = 624, 2.9%) and PHQ-9 items (n = 100, 0.5%). Women, older participants, those from non-Dutch ethnic origin groups, those with lower educational or occupational level and with poorer physical or mental health more often had missing data on one or more of these variables. The final sample consisted of 19 377 participants, 4372 of Dutch, 2772 of South-Asian Surinamese, 3674 of African Surinamese, 1906 of Ghanaian, 3184 of Turkish and 3469 of Moroccan origin. The Medical Ethics Committee of the Amsterdam Academic Medical Center approved the study protocols. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Measurements

SRH was indicated by the first item of the SF-12: ‘In general, would you say your health is: Excellent, Very good, Good, Fair, or Poor’. SRH was dichotomized into fair/poor1versus good/very good/excellent (0) SRH.

SES was measured by educational level and occupational level. Educational level was based on the highest educa-tional qualification obtained, either in the Netherlands or in the country of origin, and categorized into four groups, (i) ‘none, or only primary education’, (ii) ‘lower vocational or lower secondary education’, (iii) ‘intermediate vocational or intermediate or higher secondary education’ and (iv) ‘higher vocational education and university’.

Occupational level was classified according to the Dutch Standard Occupational Classification system,21 which pro-vides an extensive systematic list of all professions in the Dutch system. Occupational level consisted of five categories, based on job title and job description, including a question on fulfilling an executive function. Because of low numbers, the lowest two categories were combined, resulting in these four categories: (i) elementary/lower, (ii) intermediate, (iii) higher and (iv) academic. Missing occupational level was included as a separate category, as this category may also include respondents who never had a job. Since there were no cases with fair/poor SRH in higher and academic levels in the Ghanaian group, levels 2, 3 and 4 had to be combined in this group.

Disease burden was operationalized as the number of chronic diseases, level of physical and mental functioning and depressive symptomatology.

(4)

The first disease variable represented the number of chronic diseases, based on self-reported presence of a list of 20 chronic disease(s) in the past 12 months (Appendix 1). A second disease variable was created with additional information that was available in HELIUS on a selection of diseases. This information was used to define additional diseases (obesity), or to add to self-report (hypertension, myocardial infarction, angina and diabetes). These four conditions were coded as ‘yes’ if one or both of the self-reported and measured definitions were positive and as ‘no’, if both were negative. As a result, the second disease variable theoretically ranged from 0 to 21. Hypertension was defined as systolic BP ≥140 mmHg, or diastolic BP ≥90 mmHg, or being on antihypertensive medication or self-reported hypertension. Diabetes was defined on the basis of self-report, elevated fasting glucose (≥7 mmol/l), and/or the use of glucose lowering medication. Obesity was defined as a body mass index higher than 30 kg/m2 (measured weight divided by measured height squared). Myocardial

infarctionand angina pectoris were defined according to the Rose questionnaire.22

As indicators of physical and mental functioning, we used two sub scales of the SF-12.23 Physical and Mental Component Summary Scores (PCS and MCS) were calculated using pre-viously published scoring coefficients,24 and were used for descriptive purposes. Because the sub scales PCS and MCS cannot be calculated without the first item of SF-12 (SRH; the main outcome measure of this study), the 11 remaining items of the SF-12 were included individually in all regression models.

Depressive symptomswere measured with the PHQ-9.25The PHQ-9 consists of nine items, with a response scale varying from zero (never) to three (nearly every day), and was used as a sum score for this study (range 0–27). If one of the items was missing, the mean score of the other eight items was used to replace the missing item. If more than one item was missing, the variable was considered missing.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics include means (SD), medians (IQR) and percentages. Logistic regression analysis was conducted with fair/poor SRH as the outcome measure. Educational level and occupational level were the main predictors, in models that included a different number of health variables as covariates in each subsequent step. The first model and all subsequent models included age and sex. The final model included all health variables as covariates. Average marginal effects (AME) for each educational and occupational level on fair/poor SRH were reported for all models.26 AMEs can be interpreted as the average increase in probability of fair/poor SRH over all

values of the covariates. We based conclusion regarding group differences on a comparison of AMEs and their associated 95% confidence intervals.

Because the magnitude and meaning of socioeconomic inequalities in health might depend on ethnicity,27 and this may affect analyses stratified by age and sex, we performed analyses in two samples. First, we conducted regression mod-els in those with Dutch ethnic origin in men and women and in those aged up to 49 and 50 and over. Second, regression models were conducted on all participants, stratified by eth-nicity.

Analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) and STATA (StataCorp. 2017. Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP). The level of statistical significance was set at P < 0.05.

Results

Table 1shows characteristics of the study sample, by eth-nicity. Rather large inequalities were found within the Dutch origin group according to educational and occupational level (Appendix Table S1). For example, highest and lowest preva-lence of fair/poor SRH was 30.5% (educational level 1) and 5.3% (educational level 4). Compared to the number of chronic diseases, PHQ-9 and physical component of the SF-12, the mental component score of the SF-12 showed a less clear gradient across socioeconomic groups.

In the Dutch origin sample, we observed a substantial reduction in the inequalities in fair/poor SRH, after adjust-ment for burden of disease (Table 2). The AME for lowest versus highest educational level decreased from 0.20 to 0.03, and the AME for lowest versus highest occupational level decreased from 0.12 to 0.02. This indicates that having the lowest versus the highest educational level is associated with a 20% higher probability of rating one’s health as fair or poor, and this decreases to 3% if the differences in disease burden are taken into account. This 3% higher probabil-ity was no longer statistically significant. Remarkably, the number of chronic diseases attenuated the socioeconomic inequalities in SRH to about the same extent as the phys-ical SF-12 items. Mental SF-12 items and the PHQ-9 sum score contributed least to the explanation of inequalities in SRH.

Across Dutch origin subgroups according to age and sex, AMEs from models before and after adjustment for health variables appeared quite similar (Fig. 1). This was indicated by largely overlapping confidence intervals for men versus women and for the younger versus older age group. In ethnic minority groups, overall larger inequalities between the higher

(5)

SOCIOECONOMIC INEQUALITIES IN SRH AND DISEASE BURDEN e415

Table 1 Characteristics of the sample

N = 19379 Dutch origin, N = 4372 South-Asian Surinamese origin, N = 2772 African Surinamese origin, N = 3674 Ghanaian origin, N = 1906 Turkish origin, N = 3184 Moroccan origin, N = 3469 Mean (SD)/n (%)/median [IQR] Mean (SD)/n (%)/median [IQR] Mean (SD)/n (%)/median [IQR] Mean (SD)/n (%)/median [IQR] Mean (SD)/n (%)/median [IQR] Mean (SD)/n (%)/median [IQR] Age 46.0 (14.0) 45.1 (13.5) 47.5 (12.6) 44.4 (11.2) 39.9 (12.2) 40.1 (12.9) Female sex 2372 (54.3) 1504 (54.3) 2222 (60.5) 1144 (60.0) 1750 (55.0) 2108 (60.8) Educational level Level 1 (lowest) 141 (3.2) 373 (13.5) 193 (5.3) 542 (28.4) 967 (30.4) 1044 (30.1) Level 2 605 (13.8) 908 (32.8) 1266 (34.5) 762 (40.0) 792 (24.9) 607 (17.5) Level 3 958 (21.9) 823 (29.7) 1343 (36.6) 471 (24.7) 930 (29.2) 1179 (34.0) Level 4 2668 (61.0) 668 (24.1) 872 (23.7) 131 (6.9) 495 (15.5) 639 (18.4)

Occupational level missing 253 (5.8) 310 (11.2) 318 (8.7) 266 (14.0) 735 (23.1) 910 (26.2)

Level 1 (lowest) 683 (15.6) 1095 (39.5) 1386 (37.7) 1420 (74.5) 1469 (46.1) 1308 (37.7) Level 2 960 (22.0) 772 (27.8) 1195 (32.5) 149 (7.8) 599 (18.8) 757 (21.8) Level 3 1612 (36.9) 460 (16.6) 675 (18.4) 52 (2.7) 277 (8.7) 409 (11.8) Level 4 864 (19.8) 135 (4.9) 100 (2.7) 19 (1.0) 104 (3.3) 85 (2.5) Fair/poor SRH 393 (9.0) 822 (29.7) 784 (21.3) 369 (19.4) 1043 (32.8) 1247 (35.9) Median no of chronic diseases 1 [0–2] 2 [1–5] 2 [1–4] 2 [1–3] 3 [1–6] 2 [1–4] Median PHQ-9 score 3 [1–5] 4 [1–7] 3 [0–5] 2 [0–5] 5 [2–9] 4 [2–8] SF-12 physical component 51.0 (7.6) 46.9 (9.6) 48.5 (8.9) 48.1 (8.6) 45.8 (10.4) 46.3 (10.1) SF-12 mental component 51.0 (8.6) 47.6 (10.9) 50.2 (9.9) 49.3 (9.5) 45.1 (11.0) 45.8 (10.7)

and lower SES groups were observed, whereas smaller pro-portions were explained by the specific health factors (Fig. 2). An exception was the group with Ghanaian origin, probably due to the different categorization of SES. In particular in Turks the inequalities were largest, and in Ghanaians the pro-portion that could not be explained by specific health factors was largest (i.e. small difference in AME between unadjusted and adjusted models).

Sub group models that include each health variable sepa-rately are shown inAppendix Tables S2–S10. These results confirm that the patterns that were found for educational level also apply to occupational level.

Discussion

Main finding of this study

This study examined to what extent socioeconomic inequali-ties in SRH reflect inequaliinequali-ties in the burden of disease. Gen-erally, inequalities in SRH were for the most part explained by inclusion of more specific measures of disease burden. However, the extent to which this was true varied across

demographic sub groups. Results in particular suggested that in ethnic minority groups, inequalities in SRH are relatively less accounted for by the specific health factors that were included in this study.

What is already known on this topic

SRH is an often used instrument in population health mon-itoring. Therefore, in view of the development of policies aiming to reduce inequalities in health, it is important to have knowledge on what differences in SRH represent and what interventions are needed to reduce inequalities in SRH. Research over the past decades has pointed to inequalities in the development of diseases28,29and in physical and mental functioning.30–32 In addition, inequalities were observed in health behavior33and recovery from health problems.34Two previous studies have focused on the explanation of SES inequalities in SRH in specific populations, in particular older people and in those with chronic diseases.15,16 Simon et al. observed that in a sample with predominantly chronically ill participants, subjective health aspects (psychosomatic symp-toms and perceived discomfort/stress) explained more of the

(6)

Fig. 1. AME for educational level on fair/poor SRH, in Dutch respondents by sex and age.

inequalities than objective health aspects (chronic diseases and functional limitations).15

What this study adds

The current study was performed in a general population sample, as opposed to previous studies on this topic. In contrast to the results of Simon et al.,15 we observed that chronic diseases and physical functioning explained most of the SES inequalities in Dutch origin respondents, which might be related to the inclusion of predominant chronically ill people in their study, and to a different operationalization of health aspects.

Regarding the population groups with non-Dutch origin, we found that inequalities in SRH more often persisted after

taking into account the distribution of chronic diseases and problems with physical and mental functioning across socioe-conomic groups. Reasons for a significant remaining associ-ation of educassoci-ational and occupassoci-ational level with SRH may include that physical and mental disease burden was not mea-sured optimally, or that other, unmeamea-sured, health factors play a role. For example, if anxiety was measured as in-depth as was depression with the PHQ-9, or if the severity of chronic diseases would have been measured this might have led to a better explanation of socioeconomic health inequalities.

The remaining part of inequalities in SRH could further be due to structural unfavourable circumstances in the low SES groups, such as an unsafe environment or unfavourable financial situation, or to individual factors such as lifestyle factors and personality. In particular in African Surinamese,

(7)

SOCIOECONOMIC INEQUALITIES IN SRH AND DISEASE BURDEN e417

Fig. 2. AME for educational level on fair/poor SRH, non-Dutch respondents by ethnicity.

(8)

Ta b le 2 Association between SES (educational a nd occupational level) a nd fair/poor SRH, Dutch o rigin sample, n = 4372 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Model 7 Adjusted for A ge, sex Age, sex, chronic diseases self-report Age, sex, chronic diseases self-report + measured a Age, sex, sf-12 physical Age, sex, sf-12 mental Age, sex, phq-9 A ge, sex, chronic d iseases self-report + measured a, sf-12 physical, sf-12 m ental, phq-9 AME (95% CI) AME (95% CI) AME (95% CI) AME (95% CI) AME (95% CI) AME (95% CI) AME (95% CI) Education Level 1 0 .20 (0.13;0.27) 0.06 (0.02;0.11) 0.04 (0.00;0.08) 0.07 (0.03;0.11) 0.10 (0.05;0.14) 0.11 (0.06;0.16) 0.03 (− 0.00;0.06) 2 0 .11 (0.08;0.14) 0.05 (0.03;0.08) 0.04 (0.02;0.07) 0.04 (0.02;0.06) 0.06 (0.04;0.09) 0.08 (0.05;0.11) 0.03 (0.01;0.04) 3 0 .04 (0.02;0.06) 0.01 (− 0.00;0.03) 0.01 (− 0.01;0.03) 0.01 (− 0.00;0.03) 0.02 (0.01;0.04) 0.03 (0.01;0.05) 0.00 (− 0.01;0.02) 4( re f) Occupation Missing 0.08 (0.03;0.13) 0.04 (− 0.00;0.08) 0.03 (− 0.01;0.07) 0.03 (− 0.00;0.07) 0.04 (− 0.00;0.07) 0.04 (0.00;0.08) 0.01 (− 0.02;0.05) Level 1 0.12 (0.09;0.15) 0.05 (0.02;0.07) 0.04 (0.01;0.06) 0.05 (0.02;0.07) 0.06 (0.03;0.08) 0.08 (0.06;0.11) 0.02 (− 0.00;0.04) 2 0 .07 (0.04;0.09) 0.03 (0.01;0.06) 0.03 (0.01;0.05) 0.03 (0.01;0.05) 0.04 (0.02;0.07) 0.05 (0.03;0.07) 0.02 (− 0.00;0.04) 3 0 .02 (0.00;0.04) 0.01 (− 0.01;0.03) 0.01 (− 0.01;0.03) 0.01 (− 0.01;0.03) 0.01 (− 0.01;0.03) 0.02 (− 0.00;0.04) 0.01 (− 0.01;0.03) 4( re f)

Moroccan and Ghanaian participants, a quite small propor-tion of fair/poor SRH ratings were accounted for by a higher disease burden in lower socioeconomic groups. This result might point to the relevance of other health aspects, not measured in this study, or to a different view on health, where non-health related factors are needed to explain why lower socioeconomic groups have poorer SRH.

It has been argued that SRH measures something different in individuals according to their SES, hampering the validity of SRH as a proxy measure to compare their physical and mental health status.35–39 Our results show that in those with Dutch origin, the 20% elevated risk on poorer SRH for the lower educated was reduced to 3% if their higher disease burden was taken into account. Due to its non-specific wording, SRH captures a range of different health problems, as well as their accumulation. SRH may thus be useful for obtaining insight in socioeconomic health inequalities, for example, in the context of complex health interventions that are targeted at multiple health aspects. At the same time, this conclusion might not hold for ethnic minority populations, and this should be examined in further research. It should also be noted that our findings apply to one moment in time. Its validity for monitoring health inequalities over time should be examined with longitudinal or trend data.

Limitations of this study

Our findings should be viewed in light of some limitations. First, disease burden was almost only measured by self-report, except for some of the chronic diseases. If there would be relevant health-related reporting differences between socioe-conomic groups, this would influence both the outcome SRH and the selected explanatory variables. Previous research, however, is not consistent with regard to reporting differences (i.e. differential associations between SRH and indicators of disease burden according to SES) and their direction.37,40–42 Thus, the extent to which reporting differences have influ-enced our results is expected to be limited. Second, those who did not participate in the study, or were excluded because of missing data had slightly lower SES,20 were more often members of ethnic minority groups and had poorer physical and mental health. Inclusion of a healthier sample probably influenced representativeness of our descriptive data, but probably not the strength of the associations that were found. Third, we have not included more specific questions on func-tional limitations, which are major health problems in older age groups. In HELIUS, questions on activities of daily living (ADL) and on functional limitations were only measured in those aged 55 and over. In the 1422 Dutch respondents that responded to those questions, we found that the remaining AME for the lowest educational level was 0.02 (instead of

(9)

SOCIOECONOMIC INEQUALITIES IN SRH AND DISEASE BURDEN e419

0.03), and the remaining AME for lowest occupational level was 0.00 (instead of 0.02). There is thus a substantial overlap between these additional questions and the variables that were included in our study, but some additional variance could have been explained by additional questions on daily functioning.

Conclusion

SRH is a concise health measure that has shown consistent predictive value for morbidity and mortality.6The results of this study showed that more specific indicators of disease burden account for most of the educational and occupational inequalities in SRH in people with Dutch origin. Future stud-ies should examine whether these conclusions also hold for trend data on inequalities in SRH. In respondents with non-Dutch origin, larger part of the inequalities remained after adjustment for specific health aspects. It should be examined which health aspects influence SRH in these groups.

Supplementary data

Supplementary dataare available at the Journal of Public Health online.

Acknowledgements

The HELIUS study is conducted by the Amsterdam UMC, location Academic Medical Center, and the Public Health Service of Amsterdam. Both organizations provided core support for HELIUS. We are most grateful to the participants of the HELIUS study and the management team, research nurses, interviewers, research assistants and other staff who have taken part in gathering the data of this study.

Funding

This work was supported by the Dutch Heart Foundation (2010T084), the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw: 200500003), the European Union (FP-7: 278901), and the European Fund for the Integration of non-EU immigrants (EIF: 2013EIF013). The study reported here was additionally supported by a grant from the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO: 319-20-002).

References

1 Fayers PM, Sprangers MAG. Understanding self-rated health. Lancet 2002;359(9302):187–8.

2 Simon JG, de Boer JB, Joung IMA, et al. How is your health in general? A qualitative study on self-assessed health. Eur J Public Health 2005;15(2):200–8.

3 Peersman W, Cambier D, De Maeseneer J, et al. Gender, educational and age differences in meanings that underlie global self-rated health. Int J Public Health2012;57(3):513–23.

4 Krause NM, Jay GM. What do global self-rated health items measure. Med Care1994;32(9):930–42.

5 Garbarski D. Research in and prospects for the measurement of health using self-rated health. Public Opin Q 2016;80(4):977–97.

6 Jylhä M. What is self-rated health and why does it predict mortality? Towards a unified conceptual model. Soc Sci Med 2009;69(3):307–16. 7 Mossey JM, Shapiro E. Self-rated health: A predictor of mortality

among the elderly. Am J Public Health 1982;72(8):800–8.

8 Subramanian SV, Huijts T, Avendano M. Self-reported health assess-ments in the 2002 world health survey: How do they correlate with education? Bull World Health Organ 2010;88:131–8.

9 Storeng SH, Krokstad S, Westin S, et al. Decennial trends and inequali-ties in healthy life expectancy: The HUNT study, Norway. Scand J Public Health2018;46(1):124–31. doi:10.1177/1403494817695911

10 Hu Y, van Lenthe FJ, Borsboom GJ, et al. Trends in socioeconomic inequalities in self-assessed health in 17 European countries between 1990 and 2010. J Epidemiol Community Health 2016;70(7):644–52. doi:

10.1136/jech-2015-206780

11 Verropoulou G. Key elements composing self-rated health in older adults: A comparative study of 11 European countries. Eur J Ageing 2009;6(3):213–26.

12 Cott CA, Gignac MAM, Badley EM. Determinants of self rated health for Canadians with chronic disease and disability. J Epidemiol Community Health1999;53(11):731–6.

13 Singh-Manoux A, Martikainen P, Ferrie J, et al. What does self rated health measure? Results from the British Whitehall II and French Gazel cohort studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 2006;60(4):364–72. 14 Molarius A, Berglund K, Eriksson C, et al. Socioeconomic conditions, lifestyle factors, and self-rated health among men and women in Sweden. Eur J Pub Health 2007;17(2):125–33.

15 Simon JG, Van De Mheen H, Van Der Meer JBW, et al. Socioeconomic differences in self-assessed health in a chronically ill population: The role of different health aspects. J Behav Med 2000;23(5):399–420. 16 Leão T, Perelman J. Depression symptoms as mediators of inequalities

in self-reported health: The case of southern European elderly. J Public Health2017:40(4):756–63

17 Benyamini Y, Leventhal EA, Leventhal H. Gender differences in pro-cessing information for making self-assessments of health. Psychosom Med2000;62(3):354–64.

18 Schnittker J. When mental health becomes health: Age and the shifting meaning of self-evaluations of general health. Milbank Q 2005;83(3):397–423.

19 Stronks K, Snijder MB, Peters RJ, et al. Unravelling the impact of ethnicity on health in Europe: The HELIUS study. BMC Public Health 2013;13(1):1–10. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-13-402

20 Snijder MB, Galenkamp H, Prins M, et al. Cohort profile: The healthy life in an urban setting (HELIUS) study in Amsterdam, the Netherlands. BMJ Open 2017;7:e017873). doi:

10.1136/bmjopen-2017-017873

(10)

21 Statistics Netherlands. Standaard Beroepenclassificatie 2010, 2010. 22 Rose G, McCartney P, Reid DD. Self-administration of a

question-naire on chest pain and intermittent claudication. Br J Prev Soc Med 1977;31(1):42–8. doi:10.1136/jech.31.1.42

23 Gandek B, Ware JE, Aaronson NK, et al. Cross-validation of item selection and scoring for the SF-12 health survey in nine countries: Results from the IQOLA project. J Clin Epidemiol 1998;51(11):1171–8. 24 Ware JE, Keller SD, Kosinski M. Sf-12: How to Score the Sf-12 Physcial and Mental Health Summary Scales: QualityMetric Incorporated, 1998.

25 Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. The PHQ-9. J Gen Intern Med 2001;16(9):606–13. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

26 Mood CJE Sr. Logistic regression: Why we cannot do what we think we can do, and what we can do about it. 2010;26(1):67–82.

27 Perini W, Agyemang C, Snijder MB, et al. Ethnic disparities in educa-tional and occupaeduca-tional gradients of estimated cardiovascular disease risk: The healthy life in an urban setting study. Scand J Public Health 2018;46(2):204–13. doi:10.1177/1403494817718906

28 Dalstra JA, Kunst AE, Borrell C, et al. Socioeconomic differences in the prevalence of common chronic diseases: An overview of eight European countries. Int J Epidemiol 2005;34(2):316–26.

29 Katikireddi SV, Skivington K, Leyland AH, et al. The contribution of risk factors to socioeconomic inequalities in multimorbidity across the lifecourse: A longitudinal analysis of the Twenty-07 cohort. BMC Med 2017;15(1):152.

30 Hansen T, Slagsvold B, Veenstra M. Educational inequalities in late-life depression across Europe: Results from the generations and gender survey. Eur J Ageing 2017;14(4):407–18. doi:

10.1007/s10433-017-0421-8

31 Cerigo H, Quesnel-Vallée A. The social epidemiology of socioeco-nomic inequalities in depression. In: Cohen NL (ed.) Public Health Perspectives on Depressive Disorders. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 2017, 117.

32 Huisman M, Kunst A, Deeg D, et al. Educational inequalities in the prevalence and incidence of disability in Italy and the Nether-lands were observed. J Clin Epidemiol 2005;58(10):1058.e1–58.e10. doi:

10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.12.011

33 Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Salonen JT. Why do poor people behave poorly? Variation in adult health behaviours and psychosocial char-acteristics by stages of the socioeconomic lifecourse. Soc Sci Med 1997;44(6):809–19.

34 Tanaka A, Shipley MJ, Welch CA, et al. Socioeconomic inequality in recovery from poor physical and mental health in mid-life and early old age: Prospective Whitehall II cohort study. J Epidemiol Community Health2018;72(4):309–13. doi:10.1136/jech-2017-209584

35 Delpierre C, Lauwers-Cances V, Datta GD, et al. Using self-rated health for analysing social inequalities in health: A risk for underesti-mating the gap between socioeconomic groups? J Epidemiol Community Health2009;63(6):426–32.

36 Delpierre C, Kelly-Irving M, Munch-Petersen M, et al. SRH and HrQOL: Does social position impact differently on their link with health status? BMC Public Health 2012;12, 19(1). doi:

10.1186/1471-2458-12-19

37 Dowd JB, Zajacova A. Does self-rated health mean the same thing across socioeconomic groups? Evidence from biomarker data. Ann Epidemiol2010;20(10):743–9. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2010.06.007

38 Dowd JB, Todd M. Does self-reported health bias the measurement of health inequalities in U.S. adults? Evidence using anchoring vignettes from the health and retirement study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2011;66B(4):478–89.

39 Huisman M, Deeg DJH. A commentary on Marja Jylhä’s “what is self-rated health and why does it predict mortality? Towards a unified conceptual model” (69:3, 2009, 307-316). Soc Sci Med 2010;70(5):652–4. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.11.003

40 Kempen GIJM, Ormel J, Brilman EI, et al. Adaptive responses among Dutch elderly: The impact of eight chronic medical condi-tions on health-related quality of life. Am J Public Health 1997;87(1): 38–44.

41 Galenkamp H, van Oers HA, Kunst AE, et al. Is quality of life impair-ment associated with chronic diseases dependent on educational level? Eur J Pub Health2019;29(4):634–39.

42 Stafford M, Soljak M, Pledge V, et al. Socio-economic differences in the health-related quality of life impact of cardiovascular conditions. Eur J Public Health2012;22(3):301–5.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

A number of new inequalities on the entropy rates of subsets and the relationship of entropy and 3” norms are also developed, The intimate relationship between

Implementation of dynamic optimisation proved successful, as the average performance increase present on the refrigeration system for summer and winter was five per cent

Op basis van dit eenmalige onderzoek kan nog niet de conclusie worden getrokken dat de vertering van een rantsoen met biologisch geteelde voeders slechter is.. Daarvoor is

De deelnemers gaan met elkaar in gesprek over manieren om informatie over zorg en ondersteuning op het juiste moment dichter bij de burger te beleggen.. Dit doen we aan de hand van

For decades, communities of people living with HIV and other key populations have been driving the global AIDS response forward, demanding access to affordable medicines

According to our life-cycle model, co-payments and bequest saving thus jointly explain why higher SES households perceive a larger welfare gain from differences LTC needs and

Systematische analyse van de oorzaken van onaangepaste produktiesystemen is een eerste vereist om te komen tot een juiste beoordeling van de mogelijk- heden om inconsequenties op

Accredited supervisors are expected to demonstrate that they can develop or agree to a programme of research that is suitable for a research degree; recruit and select an