• No results found

The effects of Animal Assisted Therapy on autonomic and endocrine activity in adults with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The effects of Animal Assisted Therapy on autonomic and endocrine activity in adults with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial"

Copied!
9
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

General Hospital Psychiatry 72 (2021) 36–44

Available online 23 May 2021

0163-8343/© 2021 Published by Elsevier Inc.

The effects of Animal Assisted Therapy on autonomic and endocrine activity in adults with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial

Carolien Wijker a , b , * , Nina Kupper c , Ruslan Leontjevas b , Annelies Spek d , Marie-Jose Enders-Slegers b

a

GGZ Oost Brabant, Berlicumseweg 8, 5248 NT Rosmalen, the Netherlands

b

Faculty of Psychology, Open University of the Netherlands, the Netherlands

c

CoRPS – Center of Research on Psychological and Somatic disorders, Department of Medical & Clinical Psychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands

d

Autism Center of Expertise, Eemnes, the Netherlands

A R T I C L E I N F O Keywords:

Adults

Autism spectrum disorder Stress

Endocrinology

Cardiac autonomic control Dogs

A B S T R A C T

Objective: Stress and its sequelae are very common in adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) without an intellectual disability (ID). Animal-assisted therapy (AAT) has shown physiological stress-reductive effects in children with ASD. The aim of the current study was to examine the acute psychophysiological response to an AAT session, and to examine the longer-term stress-physiological effects of the intervention, up until 10 weeks post-treatment, in comparison to waiting-list controls.

Method: A randomized controlled trial with pre-intervention (T0), post-intervention (T1: 10 weeks) and follow- up (T2: 20 weeks) measurements of neuroendocrine and cardiovascular measures, was conducted in 53 adults with ASD (N = 27 in intervention arm; N = 26 in control arm). Within the intervention group, stress- physiological data were collected during the 5th therapy session (acute effects). Data were analyzed with mixed models for outcome measures cortisol, alpha-amylase, heart rate variability and sympathetic activity.

Results: The AAT interventional session was significantly associated with reduced cortisol levels (β = − 0.41, p = .010), while parasympathetic and sympathetic cardiovascular activity remained unaltered. No significant changes were found for stress-physiological measures at post-treatment time points.

Conclusions: Acute stress reduction, reflected in significant reduction in cortisol levels, was found during an AAT session in adults with ASD, without ID. More research is needed to explore to what extent the specific factors of AAT have contributed to the decrease in cortisol and whether stress reduction is possible for the longer-term.

1. Introduction

High levels of perceived stress and stress-related problems are very common in adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) without an intellectual disability (ID) [17]. These high levels of stress are associated with poor life outcomes in ASD, such as depression, anxiety and cardiac diseases [7]. Difficulties in initiating and maintaining social relation- ships and difficulties in cognitive switching, both core deficits in ASD [2], jeopardize adequate coping with stress [6,17,59]. Adults with ASD, who experience prolonged stress exposure, are at increased risk of chronic exhaustion, loss of skills and reduced tolerance for stimuli, which is called an ‘autistic burn-out’ [41].

Receiving adequate social support is associated with improved mental health in adults with ASD [6,59]. However, bullying and social exclusion are widely experienced by individuals with ASD, resulting in poor social support networks [45]. Due to these negative social experi- ences in the past, people with ASD (especially the higher functioning people, without ID), often try to hide or compensate one’s disabilities to increase social acceptation, behavior called social camouflaging [19,29]. The downside of social camouflaging is that it is a major source of stress. Recently, research on social camouflage showed strong asso- ciations with depression, and even suicide [8]. It has been reported as one of the main factors causing a burn-out in ASD, worsening of daily life functioning, such as loss of ability to talk, and poor executive

* Corresponding author at: GGZ Oost Brabant, Berlicumseweg 8, 5248 NT Rosmalen, the Netherlands.

E-mail address: carolien.wijker@ou.nl (C. Wijker).

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

General Hospital Psychiatry

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/genhospsych

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2021.05.003

Received 27 October 2020; Received in revised form 20 May 2021; Accepted 20 May 2021

(2)

functioning [41]. The high prevalence of stress and limited resources to reduce stress in adults with ASD without ID, puts them at risk for poor life outcomes [6,17]. Interventions targeting stress in adults with ASD, without ID thus are highly needed.

Only a limited number of studies in extant literature report on in- terventions that reduce stress in adults with ASD without ID, and studies using stress as outcome measure are lacking [46]. In our previous publication, an RCT on the effectiveness of Animal Assisted Therapy (AAT) in a sample of adults with ASD without ID, we showed signifi- cantly lower subjective stress levels after AAT as compared to waiting list controls [56]. To date, the effects of AAT on physiological stress in adults with ASD are unknown. However, in children with ASD, lower cortisol awakening responses (CAR) were observed during the stay of a specially trained and selected service dog in their home [52]. Further- more, in children with ASD who participated in therapy sessions incorporating goal-directed activities with horses, significant reductions in salivary cortisol were found [49].

Given the positive effects of AAT on acute stress reduction in children with ASD, and the lack of research on physiological stress reactivity in adults with ASD without ID, we aimed to explore whether physiological indicators of stress in this latter population change across an AAT session period (1 h), and hypothesized that the AAT session would be associated with a reduction in stress parameters.

Since this is the first study exploring associations between AAT and physiological stress in the adults with ASD without ID, we included a broad range of parameters, reflecting activation of the major physio- logical stress response systems. We included measures of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal axis (HPA axis) (cortisol), and the neural and neuro-endocrine branches of the sympathetic nervous system (alpha-amylase, systolic time intervals (i.e., PEP, LVET)), as well as the parasympathetic nervous system (HRV).

To examine whether possible intervention effects remained after the therapy, we performed a randomized controlled trial (RCT) in which physiological effects of AAT were tested as secondary outcomes in by comparing post-intervention (after 10 sessions) and follow-up effects (20-week follow-up period) to pre-intervention (T0). We hypothesized that participants in the AAT condition, compared to waiting list controls, would have reduced cortisol levels at post-treatment and that this would return to pre-intervention levels when therapy was not provided anymore [52]. Based on studies on mindfulness, we expected the sym- pathetic measures to follow a similar, but smaller-sized course, while parasympathetic activity is expected to show small-sized increases (i.e., somewhat more variability) at post-treatment, with a similar return to pre-treatment at follow-up [38,58].

2. Methods 2.1. Design

This study had a single-blind randomized controlled trial (RCT) design and was conducted between January 2015 and July 2017. The RCT had two conditions: 1) the intervention condition and 2) a waiting list control condition. Participants were, blindly for the principle researcher, randomized into one of the conditions after the pre- intervention assessment. The current study was conducted as a sec- ondary analysis of the RCT which aimed to examine self-reported effects of AAT [55].

To explore acute effects, data were collected during the fifth therapy session only in participants that were randomized into the intervention condition. Data regarding cortisol and alpha-amylase were collected before the start (t0, 0 min) and at the end (t60, 60 min) of the therapy session. Cardiac autonomic control measures were recorded during the session (60 min). Since participants in the AAT condition received an active form of therapy, including movement, increased vigilance and physiological arousal was expected [9,33]. For this reason, no mea- surements were conducted in week 5 in the waiting list control

condition, as this would not provide a reliable comparison.

To explore the effect of AAT on the RCT’s secondary psychophysio- logical outcome measures, physiological data were collected at three measurement occasions: pre-intervention (T0), post-intervention (T1, 10 weeks after T0), and follow-up (T2, 20 weeks after T0) in both intervention and waiting list control groups.

Because of ethical considerations of a RCT with a waiting list control condition, we offered all participants in the waiting list control condi- tion AAT after T2. The medical ethics committee CMO region Arnhem- Nijmegen, the Netherlands approved the study (NL-number:

NL48974.091.14). The study is registered in the Dutch Trial Registry (number: NTR5938).

2.2. Participants

Participants were recruited in seven pre-planned batches from the mental health care organization GGZ Oost Brabant, the Netherlands and the Dutch organization for Autism (Nederlandse Vereniging voor Autisme (NVA)). For recruitment, we employed information flyers in the waiting room and verbal information from therapists. When prospective par- ticipants were interested in the study, they were screened for eligibility by the primary researcher (C.W.) using a standardized intake form. The screening comprised a review of inclusion and exclusion criteria, and the use of specific current medication (i.e. psychotropic, and beta-blocker use).

Participants were included when 1) they had a diagnosis ASD received from a registered psychologist or psychiatrist, evaluated by the Autistic Disorder Interview-Revised [32], a semi-structured interview to assess the DSM-5 criteria [2], combined with clinical observations, 2) were between 18 and 60 years of age, 3) without intellectual disability, i.

e. had IQ scores >80 which was (previously) established by a diagnostic researcher with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III or IV [53,54], and 4) were suffering from stress, reflected in scores of >19 on the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS, [10,11]) and/or scores of >132 on the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R, [4]). Because the main goal of the AAT was the reduction of stress and stress-related outcomes, only participants who had scores considered as high on these outcomes were included. The PSS is a self-report assessment instrument for perceived stress and has a good internal consistency and an adequate convergent validity [11]. The self-report SCL-90-R measures psychological and physical symptoms, such as depression, anxiety, agoraphobia and sleeping problems. The questionnaire has an excellent reliability and construct validity [4]. Norms and cut-off scores for the PSS and SCL-90-R were derived from respectively the global population [11] and Dutch population [4].

Participants were excluded when they 1) were at risk for psychosis or suicide as indicated by the treating psychologist or psychiatrist, 2) were currently institutionalized into a 24-h service home, 3) reported they had a severe allergy to dogs, fear of dogs, or aversion to dogs, 4) used corticosteroids, or 5) were participating in a treatment other than AAT during the study. If prospective participants chose to participate in the study, but were receiving treatment other than AAT during the screening procedure, they were asked to consult their therapist. When participa- tion was considered as safe by the therapist, participants paused their current treatment during the study period. They were extensively informed that they could withdraw from participation at any time without giving a reason and restart their treatment.

2.2.1. Sample size

Since this is the first study exploring the effects of AAT in the adult

ASD population, the study had an exploratory character. We aimed to

include 25 to 30 participants per condition, since this was regarded

sufficient to find moderate effects in previous exploratory research

[16,26]. From the 68 eligible participants, eight were excluded because

they did not meet the inclusion criteria, and seven declined to partici-

pate (Fig. 1). In total, 53 participants participated (N = 27 in AAT

(3)

condition, N = 26 in control condition) and provided their verbal and written informed consent.

While 27 participants were included in the intervention condition initially, two participants were deemed outliers and their data were excluded from analyses, due to 1) pregnancy [9,47] and 2) multiple personal problems and a stressful life event not related to the study.

Therefore, data of N = 25 participants were analyzed. In addition, due to device failure during the ECG/ICG measurement, one participant had missing data for all cardiac variables.

Regarding longer-term effects of AAT, N = 51 participants were included in analyses (N = 25 AAT condition, N = 26 control condition).

One participant in the control condition dropped out after pre- intervention measures due to physical illness and the need for inten- sive revalidation treatment (T0 data were included in analyses).

Furthermore, due to device failure of HRV measures, six data points in five participants were missing. These participants were all included in the analyses.

2.3. Outcome variables

The two key components of the human stress system are the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal axis (HPA axis) and the autonomic nervous system, which consists of a parasympathetic and sympathetic branch. The outcome measures were chosen carefully to represent these key components of the stress system, as cortisol reflects HPA axis ac- tivity, alpha-amylase reflects activity of the neuroendocrine branch of the sympathetic nervous system, while systolic time intervals (Pre- Ejection Period (PEP), Left Ventricular Ejection Time (LVET)) reflect the neural sympathetic branch affecting cardiac activation. In addition, root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) reflects influences of the parasympathetic nervous system.

2.3.1. Stress hormones

2.3.1.1. Cortisol. Is a hormone released by the HPA axis in response to

perceived stress. In addition, cortisol knows a diurnal curve, with an

increase in response to awakening, and reducing values during the day

Fig. 1. Flowchart design study.

(4)

until the nadir is reached just after midnight. An in-house competitive radio-immunoassay employing polyclonal anti-cortisol antibodies (K7348) and [1,2-3H(N)]-Hydrocortisone (PerkinElmer NET396250UC) was used as a tracer for cortisol. The lower limit of detection was 1.0 nmol/l and inter-assay variation was <7% at 3.3–30 nmol/l (n = 80).

Intra-assay variation was <4% (n = 10). Cortisol is expressed in nmol/l.

2.3.1.2. Alpha-amylase. In recent years, has emerged as a valid and reliable marker of sympathetic nervous system activity in stress research [1]. Alpha-amylase is defined in U/l and was determined with a Beckman-Coulter AU5811 chemistry analyzer (Beckman-Coulter Inc., Brea, CA). Samples were diluted 1000× with 0.2% BSA in 0.01 M phosphate buffer pH 7.0. Inter-assay variation was 2,2% at 210.000 U/l (n = 39).

Cortisol and alpha-amylase are both positively related to perceived stress [44,51]. Saliva sampling is evaluated as reliable, valid, non- invasive and not stressful [23]. Both samples were collected with the Sarstedt Salivette (article number 51.1534.500).

2.3.2. Cardiac autonomic control

The Vrije Universiteit Ambulatory Monitoring System (VU-AMS 4.6) was used to record a continuous electrocardiogram (ECG) and imped- ance cardiogram (ICG) [14], using a 7-electrode configuration and liquid gel electrodes (Kendall, Medcat, the Netherlands). The event button on the device was used to indicate start and end times of the resting period at T0, T1 and T2 (each 30 min duration) and start and end times of the therapeutic session (1 h), during which cardiac autonomic control was assessed. During the resting period measurements, partici- pants remained seated. Data were exported to the VU-DAMS software (version 3.2), which automatically detected all markers in the ECG. All R-peak markers were visually checked, and adjusted when necessary.

The signal was visually checked for artifacts (e.g. premature atrial or ventricular contractions), which were removed prior to scoring.

2.3.2.1. Heart rate variability. In total, three variables were extracted for analyses, in part based on recommendations of the Task Force of The European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology [50]. From the corrected ECG signal, period averages were calculated for heart period (IBI), which combines cardiac sympathetic and parasympathetic influences. In addition, the standard deviation of all N–N intervals (SDNN) and the root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) were calculated as time domain measures of heart rate variability, and reflect parasympathetic cardiac tone, an important measure to assess effects of stress reduction.

2.3.2.2. Sympathetic activity. From the ICG signal, systolic time in- tervals (Pre-Ejection Period (PEP) and Left Ventricular Ejection Time (LVET)) were derived. PEP is a noninvasive index of cardiac sympathetic drive and is defined as the time interval in milliseconds between the onset of ventricular depolarization (Q-wave onset in the ECG) to the opening of the aortic valves (B-point in the ICG). LVET is defined as the time interval in milliseconds between the opening of the aortic valves (B-point in the ICG) and closing of the aortic valves (X-point in the ICG).

Systolic time intervals were manually scored from ensemble averages of the ICG of each protocol period using the VU-AMS interactive scoring software. Scoring procedures for impedance cardiography have been published previously [27,28]. PEP and LVET are strongly (inversely) associated with cardiac sympathetic activity and are recommended to observe sympathetic changes in a non-invasive way [35].

2.4. AAT program

AAT is a goal-directed intervention, in which an animal is present.

AAT has the main aim to improve well-being in humans [20]. In our AAT, ten weekly 1-h one-on-one sessions were offered to the

participants, guided by a certified health care professional. A trained dog was present throughout. Information on the selection and training of the therapy dogs and guidelines regarding their welfare is described elsewhere [57]. Our AAT program incorporated 13 trained therapy dogs, aged between 2 and 10 years old, who were selected by experi- enced dog trainers from the Dutch service dog foundation (Stichting Hulphond Nederland). The dogs had the following breeds: Labrador, Labrador crossbreed, Poodle, Golden retriever, Golden retriever cross- breed and German wirehaired pointer. To protect well-being of the dogs, strict guidelines were followed, such as working a maximum of two hours per day (non-consecutive) with a maximum of two days per week depending on the breed, age, and fitness of the dog. The program was executed by 3 healthcare professionals with a college or university de- gree in mental health care and with a minimum of five years working experience with adults with ASD, without ID. Since the therapist also fulfilled the role as animal handler, they were also certified in dog behavior.

A semi-structured therapy protocol was used during the study to limit inter-therapist differences [55]. It has been hypothesized that touching the animal reduces stress [40] and expression of non-verbal communication (body posture, gestures) improves social communica- tion [25]. Therefore, the therapy protocol contained several exercises aiming to reduce stress and improve social communication. The thera- pist provided exercises to the participant. The participant was chal- lenged to execute the exercises together with the therapy dog. For example, the participant was asked to have the dog respond to several commands, such as letting the dog sit of giving a paw. These commands could be given verbally, but the participants were also challenged to perform this exercise without spoken language. Other examples of ex- ercises are, letting the dog retrieve materials, providing tempting ma- terial to the dog and trying to prevent the dog giving into the temptation, or guiding the dog without a leash through a parkours with obstacles.

Again, participants were challenged to execute those exercises first with spoken language, but in a second round they were challenged to perform the same exercises without spoken language, to improve their non- verbal communication. The final 10 to 15 min of each session, partici- pants were given time to physically interact with the therapy dog in the form of petting or grooming the dog. Participants were encouraged to focus their attention and communication towards the dog, and were sitting down during this period. When possible, the therapist withdrew in order to optimize contact between the participant and the dog.

However, when the participant needed encouragement to remain focused on the dog, the therapist helped facilitating this interaction.

More detailed information on the therapy protocol and program fidelity is provided elsewhere [55,57].

2.5. Procedure

Assessments were executed at the mental health care center GGZ Oost Brabant. Due to cortisol and alpha amylase’s circadian rhythm [37,51], we standardized the time of day of the assessments, with as- sessments taken between 1 and 4 PM. We adhered to a strictly stan- dardized data sampling protocol. Participants were asked not to eat, drink, or exercise an hour before the assessments.

To examine changes between pre-, post- and follow-up assessments,

salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase samples and data on cardiac auto-

nomic activation were collected during T0, T1, and T2 for both the

intervention and waiting-list control condition. Participants were

scheduled for three appointments (T0, T1 and T2). At each appointment,

they were asked to wait in the waiting room of the mental health care

center for 15 min to acclimatize from their travel. At their pre-scheduled

time, the principal researcher or a research assistant invited the

participant to the assessment room and connected the participant to the

VU-AMS device. When the device was connected and a trial recording

was considered adequate, the participant was asked to sit down in a

chair and the first saliva sample (t0) was collected. Participants received

(5)

a small plastic tube that contained a cotton role. Participants were asked to put the cotton role in their mouth without touching it with their hands and then gently move the cotton role in their mouth for two minutes until it was at least moderately saturated. Time was monitored and documented by the principal researcher, or research assistant. After two minutes, participants were asked to put the cotton role back into the tube without touching the role with their hands. The plastic tube was handed over to the principal researcher or research assistant and was immediately stored in a cool box for a maximum of four hours. The Salivettes were labeled with a code for blind analysis by the laboratory analyst. Participants were then asked to read magazines in silence for 30 min. After 30 min, a second saliva sample was collected using the same procedure as the first sampling and the recording of the VU-AMS was ended. Saliva samples were stored at − 20

C and brought to the labo- ratory of the Wilhelmina University Children Hospital of Utrecht, the Netherlands, where they are stored for a maximum of 90 days at − 80

C and analyzed by a laboratory analyst.

To explore the effects on physiological stress during an AAT session, data were collected for all participants in the intervention condition during the fifth session. At the beginning (t0 min) and at the end of the session (t60 min) saliva was collected following the same procedure as during the other assessment occasions. Participants were asked to sit down during the two-minute collection of their saliva. ECG and ICG were recorded continuously during this hour using the VU-AMS device.

We chose to assess the biological markers during the fifth session, because by then, participants were expected to have accustomed to the therapy setting, resulting in less confounding factors, such as stress caused by a new situation, person, setting, or environment [49]. To control for circadian rhythms, therapy sessions were only scheduled in the afternoon between 1 and 4 PM.

2.6. Data preparation

Regarding the acute effects of AAT on physiological outcome mea- sures, we selected the first and last 5 min of the recording (when also saliva was collected). A recording time-window of 5–10 min is sufficient to examine autonomic activation of the heart. Moreover, SDNN during such a short time frame can be considered to reflect parasympathetic activity [50].

To explore longer-term effects of 10 AAT sessions on physiological outcome measures of stress, we selected the second saliva sample as our pre-intervention measure. Regarding the cardiac data, the middle 10 min of the 30-min resting period were selected from the T0, T1 and T2 measurements.

We removed 21 datapoints from analyses that fell outside the range above or below three standard deviations determined at pre- intervention assessment (T0). Standardized centered scores were calculated for remaining data with means and standard deviations at T0.

Estimates resulting from the mixed linear models were, therefore, standardized and may be considered betas (β).

2.7. Statistical analysis

SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0 was used for descriptive statistics and building mixed models.

2.7.1. Acute AAT effects

Separate mixed linear models were built for repeated measurements of cortisol, alpha-amylase, Heart period, SDNN, RMSSD, PEP and LVET, with time as a fixed linear effect [t0, t60]. After exploring the effects for covariates, no improvements were found in our model (checked using Likelihood ratio tests), and for this reason, we only presented the most parsimonious models without covariates.

2.7.2. Longer-term effects of AAT

Separate mixed models that accounted for repeated measurements

were built for cortisol, alpha-amylase, Heart period, SDNN, RMSSD, PEP and LVET, with intervention condition as a fixed effect [yes/no] (further referred to as model 1). In the next model, model 2, we added the cat- egorical variable for time as fixed effect [time points T1 and T2 compared to T0], and, in model 3, the interaction of time with inter- vention condition was added. We present the estimated effects of the best-fit model and the significance-values of the interaction term of model 3 if this was not the best-fit model. Furthermore, as preplanned, we adjusted the best-fit model for the following covariates age [years], sex [male/female], having a dog at home at T0 [yes/no], WAIS total IQ, changes in psychotropic medication during the study [yes/no], and use of betablockers [yes/no]. Due to high adherence rates (98%), the pre- planned covariate ‘adherence’ was not used in analyses.

Considering the exploratory character of this research regarding covariates, both the most reduced model and the most complete model with covariates are presented in the result section. We did not correct for multiple testing.

3. Results

Pre-intervention characteristics, including physiological variables, of the study sample are presented in Table 1, stratified by group.

3.1. Acute AAT intervention effects

Results on acute effects during an AAT session are presented in Table 2. Mixed linear models showed a significant decrease of cortisol at the end of the therapy session compared to the start of the session (β =

0.41, 95% CI -0.72 to − 0.11; p = .010). The increase in alpha-amylase was considered interesting (statistical trend), though not significant (β

= 0.12, 95% CI -0.02 to 0.26; p = .090). Regarding the ECG and ICG variables, no significant acute changes in response to the AAT session results were found.

3.2. Longer-term AAT intervention effects

Fig. 2 displays longer-term effects of the intervention per outcome variable and stratified for condition. Furthermore, beta estimates, 95%

Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values of the best-fit model, model 1 (checked using Likelihood ratio tests), are presented in Table 3. Esti- mated effects were almost identical in both the unadjusted models and the models adjusted for potential covariates. For all models, interaction terms for time points and the intervention condition did not improve the fit of models, indicating that there were no significant within-person changes due to the intervention from pre- to post-intervention and follow-up.

3.3. Covariates

Analysis revealed a sex-related effect for PEP and cortisol (trend), as Table 1

Pre-intervention characteristics.

Variable AAT Mean (SD) Control Mean (SD)

Age 38 (12.49) 39.96 (10.34)

IQ 103.08 (13.69) 101.42 (14.17)

Male 12 17

Dogowner 9 8

Betablocker 4 0

Cortisol (nmol/l) 10.80 (3.82) 10.38 (3.81)

Alpha-amylase (U/l; x 1000) 222.21 (114.99) 238.92 (142.39)

Heart rate (bpm) 78.01 (12.02) 74.92 (10.63)

SDNN (msec) 45.23 (16.81) 49.41 (19.53)

RMSSD (msec) 30.19 (17.80) 29.32 (18.37)

PEP (msec) 86.93 (11.88) 84.49 (9.22)

LVET (msec) 271.80 (30.66) 287.20 (23.99)

(6)

both outcomes were higher in males compared to females (PEP: β = 0.73, 95% CI 0.23 to 1.24; p = .005; cortisol, β = 0.53, 95% CI − 0.02 to 1.07; p = .056). Participants who did not use betablockers had a shorter PEP, i.e. increased sympathetic cardiac drive, compared to participants using beta-blockers (β = − 1.43, 95% CI -2.34 to − 0.53; p = .003) and older age was associated with lower heart rate variability (SDNN: β =

0.03, 95% CI -0.06 to − 0.01; p = .018; RMSSD: β = − 0.03, 95% CI -0.06 to 0.00; p = .075). Ten percent of the participants changed their psychotropic medication during the study. Yet, the number of partici- pants who used betablockers, and the dose of their medication, remained unchanged during the study. Analysis revealed no significant associations for change in psychotropic medication, having a dog at home at T0 and total IQ with the outcome variables.

4. Discussion

The current study explored the acute psychophysiological response to a single AAT session in adults with ASD without ID. In addition, the longer-term psychophysiological effects of the intervention were tested using a randomized controlled trial design with a waiting-list control group and three time points (pre-treatment, post-treatment and 10-week follow-up).

In line with our expectations, we found that the AAT session signif- icantly reduced the level of cortisol with a standardized estimated effect that was considered of medium size (Cohen., 1988). Counter to our expectations, alpha-amylase showed a trending increase. The measures of cardiac autonomic control remained unaltered, suggested no para- sympathetic and sympathetic cardiac changes associated with AAT.

Regarding the longer-term effect of AAT, the analyses did not reveal significant effects of the intervention condition compared to the control condition, and did not reveal significant differences between the post- intervention and the 20-week follow-up when compared to the pre- intervention.

Our study results are consistent with studies in children with ASD, in which acute cortisol reductions were found in response to the AAT intervention [49,52]. In the study of Tabares [49] no longer-term effects were assessed. Similar to our results, Viau and coworkers also reported the absence of longer-term intervention effects. During the AAT inter- vention a trained service dog was placed into the homes of children with ASD. Cortisol awakening response (CAR) decreased during the inter- vention period. However, when the dog was removed from their homes, the CAR of the children jumped back to pre-intervention levels soon [52].

Due to the high perceived stress in adults with ASD, without ID, the acute reduction of cortisol during the AAT session is a promising finding

within this study population. In neurotypical individuals, acute cortisol reductions were found during physical interaction with a dog [39,40].

During this physical interaction, besides the reduction of cortisol, oxytocin, a hormone associated with attachment and bonding (released after a gentle, pleasant touch or grooming [3,13]) increased. In our study, participants ended the AAT session with 10–15 min of petting and grooming the dog. This physical interaction, might have increased levels of oxytocin and therefore, contributed to the reduction of cortisol.

Although more research is needed to examine the relation between physical interaction and stress-reduction, it could be one of the expla- nations for the reduction in cortisol found after the AAT session.

Furthermore, previous analyses revealed that AAT participants rated the intervention as joyful [57]. In line with this, adherence rates were very high (98%), which seems to indicate that participants were very motivated to receive the intervention. Another potential explanation for the AAT induced cortisol reduction may lie in the non-judgmental perception of the dogs (Kruger & Serpell., 2010), reducing the need to employ camouflaging strategies [29]. Since camouflaging is associated with poor life outcomes, reducing camouflaging strategies is strongly recommended [41]. It would be interesting for future research to explore the relation between AAT and the employment of camouflaging strategies.

An important limitation to these AAT session-related findings is that no control group was included. The interpretation of the reduction in cortisol should, therefore, be done with caution. While the cortisol reduction might be due to increased oxytocin, induced by the petting and grooming of the dog at the end of the AAT therapy session [40], or a reduction of the perceived stress, the cortisol’s circadian rhythm should be considered [23,37]. At the start of the AAT session, the observed cortisol levels were high, suggesting the presence of stress, either acute or chronic (around 10 nmol/L on average, while typical resting cortisol levels lie around 1 nmol/L during the afternoon, where they remain (around 1 nmol/L, 50th percentile; [36]) during the entire afternoon.

Therefore, the moderate decrease we found in cortisol might be attrib- uted to the effects of the intervention, rather than to circadian in- fluences. However, due to the lack of a control group and the high cortisol levels at the start of the AAT session (which might have increased likelihood of finding a decrease of acute stress over time), a suggestion of causality on the acute stress reduction and AAT should be made with caution. Future research could compare intervention condi- tions with and without a dog (treatment as usual; i.e. CBT or mindful- ness), as this might provide more specific information on the presence of a dog on stress reduction.

Besides cortisol, counter to our expectations, an increasing trend was found for alpha-amylase and there were no significant effects on cardiac autonomic control. HRV is inversely related to vigilance and arousal [9,33]. Increased alpha-amylase is often associated with higher arousal which might imply high stress levels, yet, can also be caused by increased activity and vigilance. Speculating on the reason for the observed increase in alpha-amylase, and considering the discrepancy between decreased cortisol on one hand, indicating reduced stress, and increased alpha-amylase on the other hand, suggesting increased sym- pathetic activation, this effect might be the result of increased vigilance and general arousal during the active exercises in AAT. In future AAT research, using physiological measures as outcomes, it would be rec- ommended to control for level of activity.

Regarding longer-term effects, the aim of the current RCT was to reduce stress, and reduce the resting physiological (unchallenged) pre- intervention level. No significant effects were found on all outcome variables. Comparing our study findings to literature describing other stress-reducing interventions, such as mindfulness, it seems to prove quite difficult to reduce the resting baseline, as pre- and post- intervention effects were mostly absent for measures of autonomic arousal [38,42,58]. In general, establishing longer-term changes in physiological stress systems seems challenging [31]. Though it has been argued that the resting physiological baseline is to a large extent Table 2

Intervention effects (short-term).

Variable Start session End session Estimated effect

(95% CI) p value

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) (Z-score) Cortisol 10.04 (3.14) 8.74 (3.14) -0,41 (− 0,72 to

− 0,11) 0,010*

Alpha-

amylase 337.60

(238.38) 364.80

(261.64) 0,12 (− 0,02 to 0,26) 0,090 Heart rate 82.20 (15.21) 81.05 (15.34) -0,08 (− 0,27 to 0,12) 0,424 SDNN 58.37 (26.57) 55.80 (29.26) -0,10 (− 0,36 to 0,17) 0,463 RMSSD 28.19 (19.09) 28.25 (17.65) 0,09 (− 0,10 to 0,28) 0,385 PEP 87.06 (12.56) 88.38 (15.14) 0,11 (− 0,08 to 0,29) 0,247

LVET 273.57

(34.72) 276.73

(35.64) 0,09 (− 0,07 to 0,26) 0,267 Notes: Mean raw scores (sd) per variable at the beginning (t0 minutes) and end (t60 minutes) of session 5 are presented. Cortisol (nmol/l), Alpha-amylase (U/l;

x 1000), Heart rate (bpm), SDNN (msec), RMSSD (msec), PEP (msec), and (LVET (msec). Furthermore, the estimated effects with 95% confidence intervals are presented in Z-scores with p-values of the basic model without covariates.

*

p-values <.05 were considered as significant.

(7)

determined by genetic components [43], chronic stress tends to increase resting state arousal levels which is a characteristic of allostatic load [34]. Interventions such as mindfulness or AAT may not be robust enough to accomplish changes in chronic stress if practicing is dis- continued. Above reasoning might explain why only acute, and no longer-term effects were found. Furthermore, as the stress physiological system in the context of chronic stress is quite complex, it is up for discussion whether measures of physiological stress are valid outcome measure to evaluate effectiveness of interventions that aim to restore rest and relaxation.

It is of note that with regard to the longer-term effects of AAT, we did find subjective reductions in perceived stress in this study sample [56].

It has been recommended in literature to use objective outcomes over subjective self-reports, due to difficulties in meta-cognitive thinking in ASD [18,22]. Nevertheless, ASD literature displays more studies in which an acute physiological stress response remained absent, while participants were exposed to a task which was perceived as stressful. For example, children with ASD who were accompanied by a dog reported less stress during a challenging task compared to the two control con- ditions: parent support or being alone. However, cortisol levels were Fig. 2. Levels of physiological stress measures for control and intervention group.

Note: Graphs in this figure show the means (SEM) of alpha amylase, cortisol, heart rate variability (SDNN, RMSSD), heart rate, and systolic time intervals (PEP, LVET)

across the follow-up period (T0 - pre-intervention, T1 after intervention (=+10 weeks), and 10 weeks thereafter. Controls = open marker, intervention =

solid marker.

(8)

similar for all conditions, reflecting no physiological stress-reducing effect of the dog. Furthermore, Jansen et al. [21] found no acute cortisol reactivity in adults with ASD while performing a task which was perceived as highly stressful. To date, it is unclear how these discrep- ancies can be interpreted, especially considering the wide range of methodology used in studies measuring cortisol (Taylor & Corbett., 2014). Therefore, we recommend to combine physiological measure- ments with self-reports to measure effects of AAT interventions.

Furthermore, in line with mindfulness studies, it might be interesting to include a longer follow-up as it might be possible that ASD participants need more time to implement learned stress-reducing skills into the daily life, resulting in delayed effects [12].

Regarding the covariates in this study, in a general population, males often show higher cortisol responses to psychosocial stress [24,26]. To date, conclusions on sex-related effects of cortisol in the ASD population have to be drawn with caution, since most studies were small, and pri- marily consisted of males (Taylor & Corbett., 2014). Since our study was of a larger sample size and included an almost equal number of male and females, findings on possible sex-effects are important to mention. A recent meta-analysis on sex-related cortisol reactivity, described that although baseline cortisol did not seem to differ between males and females, cortisol reactivity to psychosocial stress was higher in males [30]. In our study, males demonstrated higher levels of cortisol throughout (acute and longer-term). Possible stress induced arousal was minimized since only data were included in our analyses that were collected after sitting for 30 min while silently reading low arousal magazines, alone, in a laboratory setting. To gain more insight into sex differences in the neuro-endocrine system in people with ASD without ID, more research is needed. Furthermore, in our study males displayed longer PEP, lower cardiac sympathetic resting levels, compared to fe- males. Again, more research is needed to explore this more thoroughly.

With regard to the present study, several limitations should be mentioned. First, we acknowledge the relatively small study sample. As previously mentioned, it might be challenging to detect reductions in post-intervention autonomic arousal levels. Therefore, it is important that future studies include larger sample sizes, which might detect smaller effect-sizes. A second limitation is that we only included adults without intellectual disability, aged between 18 and 60, without being institutionalized or having fear or severe allergy to dogs. Furthermore,

we did not collect information on ethnicity. Therefore, conclusions to- wards the global population of adults with ASD, without ID should be made with caution. Another limitation is that we did not correct for possible confounding factors on the physiological outcome measures (especially on the HRV variables), such as body mass index and smoking [15,48], owing to the small sample size. This might have affected our results and should be considered as covariates in future research.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study, exploring intervention effects of AAT on physiological arousal and reactivity outcomes in adults with ASD, without ID. The adherence rates were very high and only one person dropped-out of the study due to physical illness, which seems to indicate that participants were very motivated to receive the AAT. Furthermore, program fidelity of the AAT was previ- ously assessed as very good [57]. Therefore, AAT seems to be a prom- ising intervention for the adult ASD population without ID. Despite the lack of significant changes on longer term physiological arousal, this exploratory study revealed acute decreases in cortisol in response to the AAT session. Considering the commonly experienced chronically high stress levels in adults with ASD, without ID, this intervention can be considered as a clinically relevant contribution to existing interventions.

However, future research is highly recommended to explore effects and working-mechanisms of AAT in the adult ASD population in more detail.

Author statement

All authors have read and approved the the manuscript. None of the authors have conflicting interests with industry or financial associations to disclose.

Funding

This work was supported by ‘Stichting Olim’ of the mental health care organization GGZ Oost Brabant, grant number D14800.

Acknowledgements

Author contributions: C.W. designed the study and co-designed the intervention, wrote the paper, was responsible for data collection and processing, data analysis, and for reporting the study results. N.K. and R.

L. assisted in the design, physiological data processing, and statistical analysis of the study and co-wrote the paper. M.-J.E.-S. and A.S. assisted in the design of the study and co-wrote the paper. All authors have read and approved the manuscript. None of the authors have conflicting in- terests with industry or financial associations to disclose.

References

[1] Ali N, Nater UM. Salivary alpha-amylase as a biomarker of stress in behavioral medicine. Int J Behav Med 2020;27(3):337–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529- 019-09843-x.

[2] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.

[3] Amico JA, Vollmer RR, Karam JR, Lee PR, Li X, Koenig JI, et al. Centrally administered oxytocin elicits exaggerated grooming in oxytocin null mice.

Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2004;78(2):333–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

pbb.2004.04.006.

[4] Arrindell WA, Ettema JHM. SCL-90-R symptom checklist. Amsterdam: Pearson;

2003.

[6] Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Mazefsky CA, Eack SM. The combined impact of social support and perceived stress on quality of life in adults with autism spectrum disorder and without intellectual disability. Autism 2018;22(6):703–11.

[7] Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Mazefsky CA, Minshew NJ, Eack SM. The relationship between stress and social functioning in adults with autism spectrum disorder and without intellectual disability. Autism Res 2015;8(2):164–73. https://doi.org/

10.1002/aur.1433.

[8] Cage E, Troxell-Whitman Z. Understanding the reasons, contexts and costs of camouflaging for autistic adults. J Autism Dev Disord 2019;49(5):1899–911.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-03878-x.

[9] Chamchad D, Horrow JC, Nakhamchik L, Arkoosh VA. Heart rate variability changes during pregnancy: an observational study. Int J Obstet Anesth 2007;16(2):

106–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijoa.2006.08.008.

Table 3

Intervention effects (long-term).

Model 1: Model 2: Interaction

Adjusted for

covariates Non-adjusted

basic model Variable

(Z-score) Estimated effect (95% CI) p

value Estimated effect (95% CI) p

value p value Cortisol − 0.09 (− 0.41

to 0.23) 0.566 − 0.14 (− 0.45

to 0.17) 0.372 0.878 Alpha-

amylase 0.02 (− 0.26 to

0.30) 0.883 0.00 (− 0.27 to

0.27) 0.995 0.789

Heart rate − 0.10 (− 0.34

to 0.15) 0.435 − 0.09 (− 0.33

to 0.15) 0.463 0.306 SDNN 0.22 (− 0.05 to

0.49) 0.116 0.22 (− 0.05 to

0.48) 0.111 0.558

RMSSD 0.01 (− 0.27 to

0.29) 0.927 0.03 (− 0.24 to

0.31) 0.809 0.605

PEP − 0.11 (− 0.34

to 0.12) 0.326 − 0.11 (− 0.34

to 0.12) 0.345 0.464 LVET 0.08 (− 0.14 to

0.29) 0.482 0.08 (− 0.13 to

0.29) 0.460 0.149

Notes: The adjusted model shows the estimates (in Z-scores) for a model cor-

rected for gender [male/female], age, WAIS total IQ, having a dog at home at T0

[yes/no], change in medication during the study [yes/no], and use of beta-

blockers [yes/no]. The basic model shows the estimates (Z-scores) for a model

without covariates. p-values for both models and for the interaction [time x

intervention] are presented.

(9)

[10] Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress.

J Health Soc Behav 1983. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404.

[11] Cohen S, Williamson G. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In: Spacapan S, Oskamp S, editors. The social psychology of health:

Claremont symposium on applied social psychology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage;

1988.

[12] Ditto B, Eclache M, Goldman N. Short-term autonomic and cardiovascular effects of mindfulness body scan meditation. Annal Behav Med 2006;32(3):227–34.

https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324796abm3203_9.

[13] Dunbar RI. The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010;34(2):260–8.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001.

[14] de Geus EJC, Willemsen GHM, Klaver CHAM, van Doornen LJP. Ambulatory measurement of respiratory sinus arrhythmia and respiration rate. Biol Psychol 1995;41(3):205–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(95)05137-6.

[15] Ehrenwald M, Wasserman A, Shenhar-Tsarfaty S, Zeltser D, Friedensohn L, Shapira I, et al. Exercise capacity and body mass index - important predictors of change in resting heart rate. BMC Cardiovasc Disord 2019;19(1):307. https://doi.

org/10.1186/s12872-019-01286-2.

[16] Hertzog M. Considerations in determining sample size for pilot studies. Res Nurs Health 2008;31(2):180–91.

[17] Hirvikoski T, Blomqvist M. High self-perceived stress and poor coping in intellectually able adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 2015;19(6):

752–7.

[18] Hufnagel C, Chambres P, Bertrand PR, Dutheil F. The need for objective measures of stress in autism. Front Psychol 2017;8(JAN):8–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/

fpsyg.2017.00064.

[19] Hull L, Petrides KV, Allison C, Smith P, Baron-Cohen S, Lai MC, et al. “Putting on my best normal”: social camouflaging in adults with autism spectrum conditions.

J Autism Dev Disord 2017;47(8):2519–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017- 3166-5.

[20] International Association of Human-Animal Interaction Organizations. IAHAIO white paper: the IAHAIO definitions for animal assisted intervention and animal assisted activity and guidelines for wellness of animals involved. 2014. p. 1–10.

[21] Jansen LMC, Gispen-De Wied CC, Wiegant VM, Westenberg HGM, Lahuis BE, Van Engeland H. Autonomic and neuroendocrine responses to a psychosocial stressor in adults with autistic spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 2006;36(7):891–9.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0124-z.

[22] Kievit RA, Geurts HM. Autism and perception of aware- ness in self and others: two sides of the same coin or dissociated abilities? Cogn Neurosci 2011;2(2):119–20.

[23] Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1994;

19(4):313–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(94)90013-2.

[24] Kirschbaum C, Wrist S, Hellhammer DH. Consistent sex differences in cortisol responses to psychological stress. Psychosom Med 1992;54:648–57.

[25] Kruger KA, Serpell JA. Animal-assisted interventions in mental health: definitions and Theoretical foundations. In: Handbook on animal-assisted therapy; 2010.

https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-381453-1.10003-0.

[26] Kudielka BM, Hellhammer J, Hellhammer DH, Wolf OT, Pirke KM, Varadi E, Pilz J, Kirschbaum C. Sex differences in endocrine and psychological responses to psychosocial stress in healthy elderly subjects and the impact of a 2-week dehydroepiandrosterone treatment. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1998;83(5):1756–61.

https://doi.org/10.1210/jcem.83.5.4758.

[27] Kupper N, Willemsen G, Boomsma DI, De Geus EJC. Heritability of indices for cardiac contractility in ambulatory recordings. J Cardiovasc Electrophysiol 2006;

17(8):877–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-8167.2006.00535.x.

[28] Kupper N, Willemsen G, Posthuma D, De Boer D, Boomsma DI, De Geus EJC.

A genetic analysis of ambulatory cardiorespiratory coupling. Psychophysiology 2005;42(2):202–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00276.x.

[29] Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Ruigrok AN, Chakrabarti B, Auyeung B, Szatmari P, et al.

Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in men and women with autism. Autism 2017;21(6):690–702.

[30] Liu J, Ein N, Peck K, Huang V, Pruessner JC, Vickers K. Sex differences in salivary cortisol reactivity to the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST): a meta-analysis.

Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017;82:26–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

psyneuen.2017.04.007.

[31] Looser RR, Metzenthin P, Helfricht S, Kudielka BM, Loerbroks A, Thayer JF, et al.

Cortisol is significantly correlated with cardiovascular responses during high levels of stress in critical care personnel. Psychosom Med 2010;72(3):281–9.

[32] Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1994. https://doi.org/

10.1007/BF02172145.

[33] Marques P, Maluf-Filho F, Kumar A, Matuguma SE, Sakai P, Ishioka S. Long-term outcomes of acute gastric variceal bleeding in 48 patients following treatment with cyanoacrylate. Dig Dis Sci 2008;53(2):544–50. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10620- 007-9882-5.

[34] Mauss D, Jarczok MN, Fischer JE. The streamlined allostatic load index: a replication of study results. Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands) 2016;19(6):553–8.

https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2016.1219718.

[35] Michael S, Graham KS, Oam GMD. Cardiac autonomic responses during exercise and post-exercise recovery using heart rate variability and systolic time intervals-a review. Front Physiol 2017;8:1–19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2017.00301.

[36] Miller R, Stalder T, Jarczok M, Almeida DM, Badrick E, Bartels M. The CIRCORT database: reference ranges and seasonal changes in diurnal salivary cortisol

derived from a meta-dataset comprised of 15 field studies.

Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016;73:16–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

psyneuen.2016.07.201.

[37] Nater UM, Rohleder N. Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: current state of research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009;34(4):486–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.01.014.

[38] Nyklíˇcek I, Mommersteeg PM, Van Beugen S, Ramakers C, Van Boxtel GJ.

Mindfulness-based stress reduction and physiological activity during acute stress: a randomized controlled trial. Health Psychol 2013;32(10):1110–3. https://doi.org/

10.1037/a0032200.

[39] Odendaal JSJ. Animal-assisted therapy - magic or medicine? J Psychosom Res 2000;49:275–80.

[40] Odendaal JSJ, Meintjes RA. Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. Vet J 2003;165:296–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/

S1090-0233(02)00237-X.

[41] Raymaker DM, Teo AR, Steckler NA, Lentz B, Scharer M, Delos Santos A. “Having all of your internal resources exhausted beyond measure and being left with no clean-up crew”: defining autistic burnout. Autism Adulthood 2020;2(2):132–43.

https://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2019.0079.

[42] Ruiz-Robledillo N, Bellosta-Batalla M, Moya-Albiol L. Lower cardiovascular reactivity to acute stress in informal caregivers of people with autism spectrum disorder than in non-caregivers: implications for health outcomes. Int J Psychophysiol 2015;98(1):143–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

ijpsycho.2015.07.011.

[43] Schreiber JE, Shirtcliff E, Van Hulle C, Lemery-Chalfant K, Klein MH, Kalin NH, et al. Environmental influences on family similarity in afternoon cortisol levels:

twin and parent-offspring designs. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2006;31(9):1131–7.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.07.005.

[44] Schumacher S, Kirschbaum C, Fydrich T, Str¨ohle A. Is salivary alpha-amylase an indicator of autonomic nervous system dysregulations in mental disorders? A review of preliminary findings and the interactions with cortisol.

Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013;38(6):729–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

psyneuen.2013.02.003.

[45] Shtayermman O. Peer victimization in adolescents and young adults diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome: a link to depressive symptomatology, anxiety symptomatology, and suicidal ideation. Issues Compr Pediatr Nurs 2007;30:

87–107.

[46] Spain D, Harwood L, O’Neill L. Psychological interventions for adults with autism spectrum disorders: a review. Adv Autism 2015;1(2):79–86. https://doi.org/

10.1108/AIA-05-2015-0007.

[47] Stein PK, Hagley MT, Cole PL, Domitrovich PP, Kleiger RE, Rottman JN. Changes in 24-hour heart rate variability during normal pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1999;180(4):978–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-9378(99)70670-8.

[48] Sumartiningsih S, Lin HF, Lin JC. Cigarette smoking blunts exercise-induced heart rate response among young adult male smokers. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2019;16(6):1032. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16061032.

[49] Tabares C, Vicente F, S´anchez S, Aparicio A, Alejo S, Cubero J. Quantification of hormonal changes by effects of hippotherapy in the autistic population.

Neurochem J 2012;6(4):311–6. https://doi.org/10.1134/S1819712412040125.

[50] Task Force. Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Circulation 1996;93:1043–65.

[51] Taylor JL, Corbett BA. A review of rhythm and responsiveness of cortisol in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014;49 (1):207–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.015.

[52] Viau R, Arsenault-Lapierre G, Fecteau S, Champagne N, Walker CD, Lupien S.

Effect of service dogs on salivary cortisol secretion in autistic children.

Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010;35(8):1187–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

psyneuen.2010.02.004.

[53] Wechsler D. WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.

[54] Wechsler D. WAIS-IV administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX:

Psychological Corporation; 2008.

[55] Wijker C, Spek AA, Leontjevas R, Verheggen T, Enders-Slegers M-J. The effectiveness of animal assisted therapy in adults with autism Spectrum disorder:

study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Autism Open Access 2017;7(5).

https://doi.org/10.4172/2165-7890.1000221.

[56] Wijker C, Leontjevas R, Spek A, Enders-Slegers M-J. Effects of dog assisted therapy for adults with autism spectrum disorder: an exploratory randomized controlled trial. J Autism Dev Disord 2019;0(0):0. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019- 03971-9.

[57] Wijker C, Leontjevas R, Spek A, Enders-Slegers MJ. Process evaluation of animal- assisted therapy: feasibility and relevance of a dog-assisted therapy program in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Animals 2019;9(12). https://doi.org/

10.3390/ani9121103.

[58] Wolever RQ, Bobinet KJ, McCabe K, Mackenzie ER, Fekete E, Kusnick CA, et al.

Effective and viable mind-body stress reduction in the workplace: a randomized controlled trial. J Occup Health Psychol 2012;17(2):246–58. https://doi.org/

10.1037/a0027278.

[59] Zimmerman D, Ownsworth T, O’Donovan A, Roberts J, Gullo MJ. High-functioning

autism spectrum disorder in adulthood: A systematic review of factors related to

psychosocial outcomes. J Intellect Dev Disabil 2018;43(1):2–19.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

AAQ-II: Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; APD: Avoidant personality disorder; AQ: Autism-Spectrum Quotient; ASC: Autism spectrum condition; AVPDSI: Avoidant Personality

In this chapter we have seen that musical endeavours on board of the Dutch East India Company’s ships were part of the proceedings on board, and they part of the pastime

Deleted Scale Variance if Item Deleted Corrected Item-Total Correlation Squared Multiple Correlation Cronbach's Alpha if Item Deleted. I have found that a person rarely deserves

In order to put the effects revealed in an RCT on an animal-assisted therapy (AAT) program in adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) into the context of clinical practice and

The remarkable adher- ence to the therapy program by study participants and the program’s clinically relevant effects indicate that AAT with dogs can be used to reduce perceived

TVEC is an oncolytic, immune-enhanced herpes simplex virus type 1, selectively infecting cancer cells and destroying the cells by direct effects on metabolic processes

The algorithm allows to efficiently compute properties of product lines whose feature model consists of a feature tree and a small number of &#34;requires&#34;

Deze kunnen momenteel met het hier beschreven model niet of nauwelijks onderzocht worden (het gaat hier dus niet om die factoren die bepalend zijn voor het bereikbare niveau,