• No results found

Consistency and the unexpected

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Consistency and the unexpected"

Copied!
148
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Consistency and the unexpected

Noordewier, M.K.

Publication date: 2009

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Link to publication in Tilburg University Research Portal

Citation for published version (APA):

Noordewier, M. K. (2009). Consistency and the unexpected. Ridderprint.

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal

Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

(2)

Noordewier, M.K.

Publication date: 2009

Citation for published version (APA):

Noordewier, M. K. (2009). Consistency and the unexpected Ridderkerk: Ridderprint

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights.

• Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal

Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright, please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

(3)

1

In 2012, the Levelt Committee, formed to investigate the publications by mr. D.A. Stapel, has decided to label this publication as fraudulent as it does not meet the accepted requirements for scholarly quality which are also adhered to by Tilburg University. The publication is shown to enable social and scholarly meta-research.

M.K. Noordewier (2009), Consistency and the unexpected (dissertation)

Conclusion from the Levelt Committee:

Chapter 2, see article:

Noordewier, M.K., & Stapel, D.A. (2010). Affects of the unexpected: When inconsistency

feels good (or bad). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 642-654.

According to Mr. Stapel: fraudulent

Fake data collection (school data), data supplied by Mr. Stapel

Results extremely unlikely, e.g., too high effects, too clean data (no missings)

Chapter 3, see article:

Noordewier, M.K., & Stapel, D.A. (2011). Stereotype disconfirmation affect: When sweet

hooligans make you happy and honest salesmen make you sad. Basic and Applied Social

Psychology, 33,

1-6.

According to Mr. Stapel: fraudulent

Fake data collection (school data), data supplied by Mr. Stapel

(4)

2

According to Mr. Stapel: fraudulent

Fake data collection (school data), data supplied by Mr. Stapel

(5)
(6)
(7)
(8)
(9)
(10)
(11)
(12)

Personality,
taste,
price,
performance,
appearance...
People
have
a
 lot
 of
 expectancies
 about
 all
 kinds
 of
 future
 circumstances
 or
 events.
 But
 what
 happens
 when
 reality
 turns
 out
 to
 be
 different
 from
what
people
thought
it
would
be?
How
would
people
feel,
for
 instance,
when
they
heard
a
rumor
that
their
new
neighbor
is
rude
and
offensive,
but
 upon
 meeting
 her
 discover
 she
 is
 in
 fact
 very
 polite,
 pleasant
 and
 nice?
 Or,
 how
 would
people
respond
when
the
weatherman
forecasted
rain,
but
the
moment
they
 pull
the
curtains,
they
feel
the
sun
shining
on
their
face?
This
dissertation
is
on
how
 people
 respond
 to
 the
 unexpected.
 It
 answers
 the
 question
 how
 people
 feel
 about
 things
 that
 they
 did
 not
 anticipate
 and
 how
 they
 subsequently
 judge
 these
 unexpected
outcomes.



The
background
of
the
research
described
in
the
present
dissertation
lies
in
 consistency
 theories.
 Based
 on
 this
 perspective,
 the
 answer
 to
 the
 question
 about
 feelings
of
the
unexpected
seems
simple:
We
all
have
a
strong
need
to
understand,
 structure,
 and
 predict
 reality
 and
 as
 a
 result
 of
 this
 need
 for
 consistency
 it
 is
 unpleasant
 when
 reality
 does
 not
 match
 our
 expectancies.
 On
 the
 other
 hand,
 however,
 people
 like
 positive
 outcomes:
 They
 like
 friendly
 people,
 success,
 sunny
 weather,
 and
 pleasant
 tastes.
 Interestingly,
 there
 are
 numerous
 examples
 where
 reality
is
both
unexpected
and
positive
and
the
question
is
whether
people
feel
good
 about
 this
 outcome
 because
 it
 is
 positive
 or
 whether
 they
 feel
 bad
 about
 this
 outcome
because
it
is
inconsistent.
A
first
tentative
answer
to
this
question
can
be
 found
 in
 a
 classic
 and
 striking
 book
 on
 the
 consequences
 of
 people’s
 need
 for
 consistency
 in
 response
 to
 an
 outcome
 that
 is
 definitely
 more
 positive
 than
 expected…


 

When prophecy fails 

(13)

Marion
 Keech
 who
 had
 received
 a
 message
 from
 an
 alien
 creature
 called
 Sananda,
 saying
 that
 at
 midnight
 on
 the
 21st
 of
 December
 1955
 the
 bottom
 of
 the
 Atlantic
 Ocean
would
rise
and
as
a
consequence,
the
land
on
the
Atlantic
coast
would
flood,
 France
would
sink,
Russia
would
be
one
big
ocean
and
a
huge
wave
would
rage
over
 the
 Rocky
 Mountains.
 All
 this,
 as
 the
 prophecy
 proclaimed,
 would
 happen
 because
 earthly
creatures
needed
to
be
purified
and
a
new
order
was
about
to
rise.
Stanley
 Schachter,
a
researcher
working
together
with
Festinger,
infiltrated
in
the
group
to
 observe
what
would
happen
on
the
night
of
the
prophecy
would
fail.

 The
followers
of
Marion
Keech
believed
that
they
were
chosen
to
be
rescued
 from
this
disaster.
On
the
night
of
December
20th,
a
space
ship
would
come
to
pick
 them
up
and
bring
them
to
a
safer
place.
The
followers
of
Marion
Keech
were
normal
 and
healthy
citizens
who
had
a
place
in
society.
Nevertheless,
in
anticipation
of
the
 savior,
 people
 had
 quit
 their
 jobs,
 sold
 their
 houses
 or
 were
 alienated
 from
 their
 relatives.
 On
 the
 night
 of
 the
 predicted
 flooding,
 they
 all
 gathered
 in
 the
 house
 of
 Marion
Keech
awaiting
their
rescue.
However,
midnight
came
and
went
without
any
 sign
of
Sananda
or
a
space
ship,
nor
did
the
sea
show
any
signs
of
flooding.
How
did
 the
group
respond?
Were
they
happy
with
the
positive
outcome
that
the
earth
would
 not
be
washed
away?
Were
they
relieved
that
the
horrible
prophecy
turned
out
to
be
 false,
and
thousands
of
lives
were
saved?
No.
Instead
of
responding
with
happiness
 and
relief,
they
were
shocked
and
they
felt
terrible.
They
sat
there,
in
the
living
room
 of
Marion
Keech,
frozen
in
disbelief,
upset
and
dazed,
some
of
them
were
crying.



(14)

beliefs
 (and
 some
 of
 them
 kept
 receiving
 messages
 from
 other
 planets
 once
 in
 a
 while).


Why
were
these
people
initially
not
happy
with
the
fact
that
the
prophecy
 failed?
Reality
was
without
a
doubt
more
positive
than
what
was
predicted
(although
 some
 followers
 might
 have
 been
 excited
 about
 flying
 in
 a
 space
 ship).
 The
 story
 illustrates
 the
 strength
 of
 people’s
 need
 for
 consistency.
 People
 have
 beliefs,
 prophecies,
anticipations
and
expectancies
to
predict
a
future
state
of
affairs
(Roese
 &
Sherman,
2007).
When
reality
turns
out
to
be
different,
this
means
that
the
world
 is
 an
 unpredictable,
 unstructured
 and
 inconsistent
 place,
 and
 this
 is
 an
 unpleasant
 experience.
 Even
 when
 this
 unexpected
 reality
 is
 a
 world
 that
 is
 saved
 from
 destruction,
people
are
initially
shocked
and
confused.


The
need
for
consistency
is
thus
important
in
answering
the
question
how
 people
 respond
 to
 the
 unexpected.
 Is
 this
 need
 for
 consistency
 so
 strong
 that
 unexpected
outcomes
are
always
unpleasant
–
even
when
they
are
positive?
Would
 people
 always
 respond
 with
 negative
 affect
 to
 unexpected
 positive
 outcomes,
 like
 unexpected
 success,
 smiling
 faces,
 and
 kind
 others?
 Before
 I
 outline
 the
 specific
 predictions
of
the
research
in
the
present
dissertation
in
more
detail,
it
is
important
 to
discuss
some
relevant
theoretical
background.



Consistency theories 

(15)

1968).
 That
 is,
 even
 though
 not
 all
 the
 examples
 described
 below
 directly
 apply
 to
 responses
 to
 the
 unexpected,
 they
 will
 highlight
 the
 strength
 of
 the
 need
 for
 consistency,
 which,
 as
 we
 will
 see
 in
 this
 dissertation,
 appears
 to
 be
 the
 basic
 and
 primary
factor
in
responses
to
the
unexpected.



Below
I
will
start
with
describing
one
of
the
most
influential
theories
of
the
 past
fifty
years,
cognitive
dissonance
theory
(Festinger,
1957).
This
will
be
followed
 by
 a
 description
 of
 balance
 theory
 (Heider,
 1958),
 self‐verification
 theory
 (Swann,
 1983/1987)
 and
 a
 brief
 overview
 of
 other
 findings
 and
 perspectives
 that
 can
 be
 related
to
consistency
strivings.


Cognitive dissonance theory 

A
 year
 after
 the
 publication
 of
 When  Prophecy  Fails,
 Festinger
 published
 A  theory  of  Cognitive  Dissonance  (Festinger,
 1957),
 which
 starts
 with
 the
 proposition
 that
‘if
a
person
held
two
cognitions
that
were
psychologically
inconsistent,
he
or
she
 would
experience
dissonance
and
would
attempt
to
reduce
dissonance
much
as
one
 would
attempt
to
reduce
hunger,
thirst,
or
any
drive’
(see
also
Aronson,
1992).
Thus,
 cognitions
 that
 are
 relevant
 to
 one
 another
 can
 be
 consonant
 or
 dissonant.
 Two
 cognitions
are
consonant
if
one
follows
from
the
other
(“I
want
to
be
healthy
and
I
 eat
 a
 lot
 of
 fruit”),
 and
 they
 are
 dissonant
 if
 the
 opposite
 of
 one
 cognition
 follows
 from
the
other
(“I
want
to
be
healthy
and
I
smoke”).
Dissonance
is
a
state
of
tension
 that
 is
 psychologically
 uncomfortable
 (see
 also
 Elliot
 &
 Devine,
 1994),
 which
 motivates
people
to
reduce
dissonance.
The
greater
the
magnitude
of
the
dissonance,
 the
 greater
 the
 pressure
 to
 reduce
 dissonance.
 That
 is,
 the
 more
 discrepant
 two
 cognitions
 are,
 the
 greater
 the
 discomfort,
 and
 the
 more
 motivated
 people
 are
 to
 reduce
it.


(16)

using
 numerous
 methods,
 all
 demonstrating
 that
 dissonance
 can
 have
 intriguing
 consequences
and
that
people
employ
a
variety
of
strategies
to
keep
beliefs,
values,
 outcomes,
attitudes
and
behavior
consistent
with
each
other.
Here
are
a
few
classics:

 Induced  compliance.  When
 people
 are
 induced
 to
 act
 in
 a
 way
 that
 is
 contrary
 to
 their
 attitudes,
 the
 dissonance
 between
 attitudes
 and
 behavior
 can
 motivate
them
to
change
their
attitude
in
the
direction
of
their
behavior.
In
a
classic
 study
 (Festinger
 &
 Carlsmith,
 1959),
 participants
 were
 asked
 to
 tell
 a
 waiting
 participant
 that
 an
 extremely
 boring
 experiment
 was
 actually
 one
 of
 the
 most
 enjoyable
 experiences
 they
 had
 ever
 had
 in
 a
 research
 study
 (the
 study
 involved
 things
like
the
endless
turning
of
plugs
and
taking
of
several
dozen
spools
of
thread
 with
 one
 hand).
 Participants
 received
 either
 a
 large
 incentive
 ($20)
 or
 a
 small
 incentive
 ($1)
 to
 tell
 this
 lie.
 When
 given
 a
 large
 incentive,
 participants
 did
 not
 change
 their
 opinion
 about
 the
 experiment,
 whereas,
 when
 participants
 received
 a
 small
 incentive,
 participants
 changed
 their
 attitude
 in
 the
 direction
 of
 the
 lie.
 The
 explanation
for
this
effect
is
that
the
large
incentive
works
as
a
justification
for
the
lie
 (‘I
 am
 a
 truthful
 person,
 but
 this
 is
 a
 good
 pay
 for
 a
 small
 lie’),
 whereas
 a
 small
 incentive
 does
 not
 work
 in
 this
 manner
 and
 the
 logical
 way
 to
 restore
 the
 inconsistency
 is
 then
 to
 construct
 beliefs
 that
 are
 consistent
 with
 their
 lie
 (‘I
 am
 a
 truthful
person,
this
experiment
was
not
so
bad’).



(17)

Free choice. Third,
the
motivation
to
keep
cognition
and
behavior
consistent
 can
result
in
(biased)
feelings
that
a
good
choice
has
been
made.
For
instance,
when
 people
make
a
(free)
choice
between
two
products
that
are
equally
desirable
(e.g.,
a
 toaster
or
a
coffeemaker),
the
moment
they
choose
one
option,
they
reject
the
other.
 The
 rejection
 of
 an
 attractive
 alternative
 creates
 dissonance
 (Brehm,
 1956)
 and
 to
 reduce
 this,
 people
 typically
 concentrate
 on
 or
 downplay
 positive
 and
 negative
 aspects
 of
 the
 different
 alternatives
 in
 such
 a
 way
 that
 the
 chosen
 alternative
 is
 evaluated
more
positively,
supporting
the
choice
made
(but
see
also
Zeelenberg,
van
 Dijk,
 Manstead,
 &
 van
 der
 Pligt,
 2000).
 Due
 to
 similar
 reasoning,
 people
 who
 have
 placed
 a
 bet
 have
 greater
 confidence
 in
 the
 choice
 they
 made
 (e.g.,
 that
 the
 horse
 they
 bet
 on
 is
 going
 to
 win)
 than
 people
 who
 are
 about
 to
 place
 a
 bet
 (Knox
 &
 Inkster,
1968).



Belief  disconfirmation.  Finally,
 and
 especially
 relevant
 in
 regard
 to
 the
 research
in
the
present
dissertation,
belief
disconfirmation
can
result
in
dissonance,
 which
motivates
people
to
find
ways
to
reduce
it.
In
a
famous
study,
people
were
led
 to
believe
that
they
would
fail
or
succeed
on
a
task
(guessing
out
which
one
of
two
 people
on
a
picture
is
more
likely
to
be
schizophrenic;
Aronson
&
Carlsmith,
1962).
 After
 this
 expectancy
 was
 induced,
 participants
 received
 false
 feedback
 that
 they
 were
 very
 successful
 or
 not.
 The
 dissonance
 between
 expectancy
 and
 reality
 motivated
 participants
 to
 influence
 the
 results,
 such
 that,
 when
 given
 the
 opportunity,
people
who
expected
to
fail
changed
correct
answers
to
incorrect
ones
 to
 achieve
 the
 failure
 they
 anticipated,
 whereas
 people
 who
 expected
 to
 succeed
 changed
 wrong
 answers
 to
 correct
 ones
 to
 achieve
 success.
 Thus,
 people
 who
 are
 confronted
with
an
outcome
that
is
inconsistent
to
their
expectancy
are
motivated
to
 change
the
outcome
when
possible.



(18)

they
 change
 their
 attitudes,
 shift
 their
 preferences,
 alter
 their
 beliefs
 or
 create
 the
 outcome
 they
 anticipated.
 With
 these
 strategies,
 people
 make
 reality
 consistent
 again
and
possible
negative
feelings
are
eliminated.


The  when  and  why  of  dissonance.  From
 the
 day
 that
 cognitive
 dissonance
 theory
entered
the
field,
the
question
why
dissonance
is
unpleasant
and
under
which
 exact
 conditions
 dissonance
 is
 experienced
 has
 been
 debated.
 As
 a
 result
 of
 this,
 several
boundary
conditions
have
been
proposed
and
the
theory
went
through
quite
 a
few
revisions
in
the
past
fifty
years
(see
for
an
overview,
Cooper,
2007;
Harmon‐ Jones
&
Mills,
1999).
It
has
been
proposed
that
in
order
to
experience
dissonance,
the
 self
needs
to
be
involved
(Aronson,
1968;
1992)
and
decision
freedom
needs
to
be
 high
 (Linder,
 Cooper,
 &
 Jones,
 1967).
 Furthermore,
 the
 new  look  version
 of
 dissonance
proposed
that
people
need
to
feel
personally
responsible
for
producing
 foreseeable
aversive
consequences
(Cooper
&
Fazio,
1984),
which
in
turn
has
been
 challenged
 by
 findings
 by
 Harmon‐Jones
 and
 colleagues
 showing
 that
 aversive
 consequences
 (that
 people
 are
 personally
 responsible
 for)
 are
 not
 necessary
 for
 dissonance
to
occur
(Harmon‐Jones,
2000;
Harmon‐Jones,
Brehm,
Greenberg,
Simon,
 &
Nelson,
1996).
A
recent
view
on
dissonance
proposes
an
action‐based
model
that
 addresses
the
motivation
underlying
dissonance
effects
(Harmon‐Jones,
1999),
and
 this
is
especially
relevant
to
our
question
how
people
feel
when
they
are
confronted
 with
 unexpected
 outcomes.
 Specifically,
 Harmon‐Jones
 (1999;
 2000;
 see
 also
 Harmon‐Jones,
Amodio,
&
Harmon‐Jones,
in
press)
proposes
that
when
a
behavior,
 belief,
attitude,
or
value
is
regarded
as
a
meaningful
truth,
people
are
committed
to
it
 and
 they
 will
 experience
 dissonance
 when
 they
 encounter
 information
 that
 is
 inconsistent
 with
 it.
 This
 dissonance
 is
 there
 because
 the
 sense
 of
 being
 able
 to
 predict
 and
 control
 outcomes
 may
 be
 threatened
 and
 this
 might
 interfere
 with
 the
 requirement
 to
 act
 consistently
 and
 effectively.
 The
 action‐based
 model
 of
 dissonance
 thus
 broadens
 dissonance
 theory
 again,
 as
 it
 states
 that
 the
 mere
 discrepancy
between
cognitions
can
result
in
dissonance.


(19)

Balance theory 

Balance
theory
(Heider,
1958)
proposes
that
people
try
to
maintain
balance
 among
 their
 beliefs,
 cognitions,
 and
 ‘sentiments’
 (see
 also
 Newcomb,
 1968).
 According
 to
 Heider
 (1958,
 p.
 176),
 “a
 balanced
 state
 is
 a
 situation
 in
 which
 perceived
 units
 (entities
 experienced
 as
 belonging
 together)
 and
 the
 experienced
 sentiments
(attitudes,
in
the
more
common
usage)
co‐exist
without
stress;
there
is
 thus
 no
 pressure
 toward
 change,
 either
 in
 the
 cognitive
 organization
 or
 in
 the
 sentiment”.
Heider
furthermore
stated
that
people
will
exert
psychological
energy
to
 achieve
 or
 restore
 balance
 in
 relationships.
 A
 typical
 example
 is
 that
 if
 your
 two
 friends
detest
each
other,
you
have
an
imbalanced
set
of
relationships
and
you
are
 motivated
to
restore
the
balance
(for
example
by
getting
them
to
like
each
other
or
 by
ending
one
of
your
friendships).



Balance
 theory
 received
 support
 from
 findings
 that
 when
 people
 have
 an
 attitude
toward
one
person
(“Fritz
is
nice”)
and
then
learn
what
this
person
thinks
of
 a
yet
unfamiliar
other
(“Theodore
is
rude”),
people
usually
form
an
attitude
toward
 this
unfamiliar
person
that
results
in
a
balanced
triad
(“I
do
not
like
Theodore”
e.g.,
 Aronson
 &
 Cope,
 1968;
 see
 also
 Gawronski,
 Walther,
 &
 Blank,
 2005).
 In
 general,
 people
 are
 thus
 motivated
 to
 keep
 beliefs
 about
 themselves,
 another
 person
 and
 third
 person
 or
 object
 in
 harmony,
 that
 is,
 in
 positive
 relation
 with
 each
 other.
 A
 typical
 application
 of
 balance
 theory
 can
 be
 found
 in
 advertizing
 contexts,
 where
 oftentimes
famous
spokespersons
are
used
to
recommend
certain
products.
If
most
 people
like
the
famous
person,
they
will
also
like
the
product
because
disliking
this
 product
results
in
a
state
of
imbalance.
Thus,
in
this
sense,
consumers
do
not
need
to
 be
convinced;
they
should
just
be
confronted
with
a
product
in
the
hands
of
someone
 they
like.
 Self­verification theory


(20)

and
 organize
 this
 quest
 for
 consistency.
 That
 is,
 Swann
 states
 that
 people
 want
 to
 confirm
 their
 self‐views
 not  as
 an
 end
 in
 itself,
 but
 as
 a
 means
 of
 bolstering
 their
 perception
 that
 the
 world
 is
 predictable
 and
 controllable.
 Thus,
 self‐verification
 processes
are
driven
by
a
desire
for
prediction
and
control
and
in
an
effort
to
exert
 control
over
their
current
situation,
people
rely
heavily
on
their
(chronic)
self
views
 (Swann,
1990).



Stable
self‐conceptions
give
people
confidence
that
they
are
able
to
navigate
 through
 daily
 life.
 For
 this
 reason,
 events
 that
 confirm
 people’s
 self‐conceptions
 boost
 their
 feelings
 of
 security,
 whereas
 events
 that
 disconfirm
 them
 produce
 a
 discomforting
 feeling
 that
 they
 may
 not
 know
 themselves
 after
 all
 (see
 Swann,
 1990).
Given
that
people
want
to
be
able
to
predict,
control,
and
structure
the
world,
 the
notion
or
possibility
that
they
may
be
a
different
kind
of
person
than
they
always
 thought
they
were
creates
a
tension
that
does
not
feel
good.



A
 consequence
 of
 self‐verification
 processes
 is
 that
 people
 prefer
 self‐ verifying
 interaction
 partners
 to
 self‐enhancing
 partners
 (Swann,
 Hixon,
 Stein‐ Seroussi
 &
 Gilbert,
 1990;
 Swann
 &
 Pelham,
 1988;
 1990).
 That
 is,
 when
 people
 are
 given
 a
 choice
 between
 partners
 that
 provide
 favorable
 or
 unfavorable
 feedback,
 they
 choose
 the
 partner
 providing
 feedback
 that
 is
 consistent
 to
 the
 way
 they
 see
 themselves
(thus,
people
who
have
negative
self‐views
prefer
negative
feedback
over
 positive
feedback).
In
line
with
this
is
the
finding
that
a
large
majority
of
people
with
 low
 self‐esteem
 prefers
 negative
 feedback
 to
 positive
 feedback
 (Giesler,
 Josephs,
 &
 Swann,
1996).
In
Chapter
5
of
this
dissertation,
the
relation
between
self‐esteem
and
 need
for
consistency
is
investigated
in
more
detail.


And there is more 

(21)

Preference  for  familiarity  and  fluency.
 First,
 people
 appear
 to
 have
 a
 preference
for
familiar
and
predictable
phenomena.
This
manifests
itself
in
a
positive
 evaluation
 of
 stimuli
 that
 have
 been
 seen
 before
 (mere
 exposure;
 Zajonc,
 1968).
 Related
to
this
is
processing
fluency,
which
can
be
characterized
as
the
first
stage
at
 which
 the
 cognitive
 system
 registers
 a
 confirmed
 or
 disconfirmed
 expectancy
 (see
 e.g.,
Roese
&
Sherman,
2007).
That
is,
the
experience
of
high
fluency
is
caused
by
the
 smooth
 flow
 of
 incoming
 information
 that
 closely
 matches
 either
 expected
 or
 past
 information
 (e.g.,
 Whittlesea
 &
 Williams,
 2001),
 whereas
 the
 experience
 of
 dysfluency
 is
 the
 result
 of
 the
 detection
 of
 a
 mismatch.
 This
 is
 proposed
 to
 be
 a
 functional
equivalent
of
an
alarm
system
(Lieberman,
Gaunt,
Gilbert,
&
Trope,
2002).


Consistency in behavior. People
are
motivated
to
behave
in
a
consistent
way.
 For
example,
when
people
answer
a
question
(e.g.,
“are
you
going
to
buy
a
computer
 in
 the
 next
 six
 months”)
 affirmatively,
 this
 increases
 the
 chance
 that
 people
 will
 behave
consistent
with
the
answer
(mere
measurement
effect;
Levav
&
Fitzsimons,
 2006).
Related
to
this
is
the
fact
that
when
people
comply
with
a
small
request
(e.g.,
 display
 a
 three‐inch‐square
 sign
 bearing
 the
 words
 ‘be
 a
 safe
 driver’
 on
 their
 window),
they
are
more
likely
to
subsequently
comply
with
a
bigger
request
(have
a
 large
billboard
with
the
slogan
“Drive
Carefully”
in
their
lawn
for
a
week)
compared
 to
 people
 who
 did
 not
 comply
 with
 the
 small
 request
 (foot‐in‐the‐door
 effect:
 Freedman
&
Fraser,
1966).
Based
on
reasoning
like
“I
am
apparently
a
person
who
 does
this
sort
of
things”,
people
thus
behave
in
ways
that
are
consistent
with
their
 previous
behavior
(see
also
Cialdini,
1984).


(22)

&
Oleson,
1995;
Weber
&
Crocker,
1983;
Yzerbyt,
Coull,
&
Rocher,
1999).
Similarly,
 when
confronted
with
stereotype‐incongruent
information,
people
often
reinterpret
 it
 as
 consistent.
 Thus,
 “strong”
 implies
 physical
 strength
 when
 it
 describes
 a
 construction
 worker,
 but
 mental
 strength
 when
 it
 applies
 to
 a
 professor
 (see
 e.g.,
 Fiske,
Neuberg,
Beattie,
&
Milberg,
1987).
Related
to
the
strategies
used
to
maintain
 expectancies
and
stereotypes,
on
a
more
general
level,
people
preferentially
search
 for
evidence
that
is
likely
to
confirm
rather
than
disconfirm
their
hypotheses
(Wason
 &
 Johnson‐Laird,
 1972;
 Snyder
 &
 Swann,
 1978),
 presumably
 because
 a
 positive
 or
 confirmatory
 instance
 of
 phenomena
 is
 experienced
 as
 particularly
 trustworthy,
 diagnostic,
and
easy
to
process
(see
e.g.,
Klayman
&
Ha,
1987,
see
also
Swann,
1990).
 The
search
for
confirmation
also
results
in
the
maintenance
of
one’s
ideas
and
a
view
 of
reality
that
is
consistent
with
prior
beliefs.


Personality differences. Finally,
there
are
personality
differences
in
the
need
 for
 consistency.
 That
 is,
 everybody
 has
 a
 need
 for
 consistency,
 but
 some
 seem
 to
 have
 this
 need
 more
 than
 others.
 People
 for
 instance
 differ
 in
 the
 extent
 to
 which
 they
 find
 predictability
 and
 structure
 important
 and
 have
 a
 chronic
 desire
 for
 consistent
knowledge
(e.g.,
Neuberg
&
Newsom,
1993;
Webster
&
Kruglanski,
1994).
 These
differences
are,
for
instance,
expressed
in
making
‘to
do
lists’,
being
organized,
 and
in
planning
things
(high
need
for
structure)
versus
tolerance
to
chaos,
going
to
 different
places
for
the
sake
of
variety
and
seeing
what
the
day
will
bring
(low
need
 for
structure).
People
with
a
high
need
for
structure,
as
compared
to
people
with
a
 low
 need
 for
 structure,
 are
 more
 likely
 to
 assign
 value
 to
 consistency
 (more
 about
 this
in
Chapter
2
and
the
General
Discussion).



Commonality in consistency theorizing 

(23)

affective
 response
 to
 inconsistencies
 could
 be
 summarized
 by
 the
 statement
 of
 Harmon‐Jones,
 who
 noted
 that
 ‘when
 knowledge
 about
 the
 environment,
 about
 oneself,
or
about
one’s
action,
beliefs,
or
attitudes
is
in
a
dissonant
relation,
the
sense
 of
being
able
to
control
and
predict
outcomes
may
be
threatened,
and
ultimately,
the
 need
to
act
effectively
would
be
undermined’
(Harmon‐Jones,
1999,
p.
95).
Based
on
 the
 perspective
 that
 need
 for
 consistency
 is
 basic
 and
 strong,
 the
 research
 in
 this
 dissertation
 answers
 the
 question
 about
 how
 people
 feel
 in
 response
 to
 the
 unexpected.
 Do
 expected
 outcomes,
 as
 a
 consequence
 of
 the
 need
 for
 consistency,
 always
feel
better
than
unexpected
outcomes,
and
what
will
happen
when
outcomes
 are
unexpected
but
positive?


 

Consistency and the unexpected 

Expectancies
 are
 an
 important
 tool
 in
 people’s
 need
 to
 be
 able
 to
 understand,
 structure,
 and
 predict
 the
 (social)
 world
 (see
 Abelson,
 et
 al.,
 1968;
 Cooper,
 2007;
 Festinger,
 1957;
 Harmon‐Jones
 &
 Mills,
 1999).
 That
 is,
 expectancies
 are
 beliefs
 about
 a
 future
 state
 of
 affairs
 that
 enable
 people
 to
 anticipate
 what
 is
 coming
and
prepare
them
for
effective
living
(Biernat,
2005;
Olson,
Roese,
&
Zanna,
 1996;
Roese
&
Sherman,
2007).
Based
on
the
need
for
consistency
perspective,
the
 answer
 to
 how
 people
 respond
 to
 the
 unexpected
 seems
 relatively
 easy:
 When
 the
 world
 is
 expectancy‐congruent,
 people
 experience
 positive
 affect
 and
 feelings
 of
 comprehension
 and
 control.
 Conversely,
 when
 expectancies
 are
 disconfirmed
 and
 the
 world
 turns
 out
 to
 be
 incongruent
 to
 predictions,
 negative
 affect
 is
 often
 experienced
(Elliot
&
Devine,
1994;
Mendes,
Blascovich,
Hunter,
Lickel,
&
Jost,
2007).
 Of
 course,
 there
 are
 individual
 differences
 and
 situational
 exceptions,
 but
 for
 most
 people
 and
 in
 most
 situations,
 incongruency,
 inconsistency,
 disconfirmation,
 and
 dissonance
are
uncomfortable
mental
states
that
elicit
negative
affect.



(24)

negative
expectancy
also
unpleasant?
Does
unexpected
success
feel
bad?
Are
people
 upset
when
a
product
appears
to
be
less
expensive
than
what
an
advertisement
said?
 And
 what
 about
 a
 skinhead
 that
 kindly
 helps
 an
 old
 lady
 crossing
 the
 street?
 As
 noted
 before,
 there
 are
 numerous
 examples
 where
 reality
 is
 both
 inconsistent
 and
 positive
and
the
question
is
whether
people
feel
good
about
this
outcome
because
it
 is
 positive
 or
 whether
 they
 feel
 bad
 about
 this
 outcome
 because
 it
 is
 inconsistent.
 The
 research
 in
 the
 present
 dissertation
 offers
 an
 answer
 to
 this
 question
 by
 showing
that
people
can
respond
to
the
consistency of the outcome
(negative
affect
 after
inconsistency,
positive
affect
after
consistency
–
regardless
of
the
outcome)
or
 to
the
valence of the outcome
(positive
affect
after
positive
outcomes,
negative
affect
 after
negative
outcomes
–
regardless
of
consistency).


With
 a
 variety
 of
 studies,
 we
 will
 specify
 when
 people
 respond
 to
 the
 consistency
 of
 an
 outcome
 and
 when
 to
 its
 valence.
 Specifically,
 we
 will
 argue
 and
 show
that
affective
responses
to
disconfirmation
of
expectancies
depend
on
whether
 people
are
mentally
able
and
willing
to
abandon
their
expectancies
and
accept
reality
 as
 it  is  rather
 than
 as
 they
 expected
 it  would  be.
 That
 is,
 based
 on
 the
 wealth
 of
 research
showing
that
consistency
is
very
important
to
people,
one
could
argue
that
 the
 need
 for
 consistency
 is
 relatively
 basic
 and,
 therefore,
 people’s
 primary
 and
 automatic
 reactions
 are
 likely
 to
 be
 driven
 by
 the
 congruency
 of
 the
 outcome
 (see
 also
Macrae,
Bodenhausen,
Schloerscheidt,
&
Milne,
1999).
People
use
expectancies
 to
 anticipate,
 predict,
 and
 prepare
 for
 the
 future
 and
 when
 reality
 appears
 to
 be
 different
 than
 expected,
 people
 will
 first
 experience
 the
 incongruency
 between
 expectancy
and
reality,
before
they
look
at
reality
as
it
is.
Thus,
in
order
to
see
the
 positivity
 of
 an
 outcome,
 people
 have
 to
 abandon
 their
 expectancy
 and
 shift
 their
 focus
to
the
valence
of
the
outcome
(“Although
this
is
not
what
I
expected,
I
like
it”).
 This
shift
is
likely
to
take
time,
mental
space,
and
motivation.




(25)

some
people
and
in
some
situations
consistency
is
of
course
more
important
than
for
 other
people
and
in
other
situations.
In
the
chapters
that
follow,
it
will
be
specified
 for
 which
 people,
 situations
 and
 circumstances
 consistency
 is
 more
 and
 less
 important
for
people.
When
consistency
is
important
for
people,
it
is
likely
that
the
 (in)congruency
 of
 an
 outcome
 will
 drive
 affective
 responses
 (e.g.,
 expected
 failure
 feels
good;
unexpected
success
does
not),
whereas
when
consistency
is
valued
less,
it
 is
 likely
 that
 the
 valence
 of
 the
 outcome
 (and
 not
 its
 relation
 to
 expectancies)
 will
 drive
affective
responses
(e.g.,
success
feels
good;
failure
does
not).


 

Overview of the dissertation

 

The
studies
in
the
chapters
that
follow
provide
a
systematic
and
extensive
 test
 of
 when
 and
 why
 disconfirmed
 expectancies
 elicit
 negative
 or
 positive
 affect.
 Specifically,
 in
 Chapter
 2,
 I
 start
 with
 presenting
 an
 illustration
 of
 the
 manner
 in
 which
 incongruency‐to‐outcome
 responses
 unfold.
 Subsequently,
 I
 test
 whether
 different
well‐known
social
psychological
constructs
(need
for
structure,
regulatory
 focus,
terror
management,
and
cognitive
load)
are
related
to
the
value
people
assign
 to
 consistency.
 These
 constructs
 are
 then
 used
 to
 test
 the
 notion
 that
 people
 who
 assign
value
to
consistency
respond
to
the
consistency
of
the
outcome,
regardless
of
 the
valence
of
these
outcomes,
whereas
people
who
assign
less
value
to
consistency
 respond
 to
 the
 valence
 of
 the
 outcome,
 regardless
 of
 the
 consistency
 of
 these
 outcomes
to
expectancies.
The
predictions
are
tested
in
nine
studies,
using
a
variety
 of
 expectancies
 and
 outcomes,
 ranging
 from
 taste,
 person‐perception
 and
 object
 perception
to
self‐relevant
performance.


(26)


 stereotype
is
disconfirmed
(e.g.,
a
skinhead
is
good‐tempered),
whereas
people
who
 are
not
cognitively
busy
feel
better
when
a
person
has
positive
traits
(e.g.,
a
skinhead
 is
good‐tempered)
as
compared
to
a
person
who
has
negative
traits
(i.e.,
a
skinhead
 is
aggressive),
regardless
of
whether
this
person
confirms
their
stereotype
or
not.

 In
Chapter
4,
the
generalizability
of
the
findings
is
extended
by
investigating
 the
 affective
 responses
 to
 unexpected
 windfall
 money.
 In
 two
 experiments,
 we
 disentangle
 primary
 and
 controlled
 responses
 to
 unexpected
 money
 and
 test
 the
 prediction
that
people
initially
feel
bad
when
a
product
is
cheaper
than
expected,
or
 when
they
received
more
money
than
they
anticipated.



In
 Chapter
 5,
 the
 need
 for
 consistency
 is
 tested
 in
 regard
 to
 the
 self.
 We
 investigate
responses
to
feedback
that
is
consistent
or
inconsistent
with
the
level
of
 people’s
self‐esteem.
The
aim
of
this
chapter
is
to
integrate
our
own
view
on
people’s
 need
 for
 consistency
 with
 previous
 research
 from
 the
 area
 of
 self‐consistency.
 Specifically,
we
test
the
role
of
self‐activation
in
affective
responses
to
success
and
 failure
of
people
with
high
and
low
self‐esteem.


In
 the
 final
 Chapter
 6,
 the
 results
 of
 all
 the
 studies
 are
 summarized
 and
 further
 discussed.
 I
 will
 describe
 some
 implications
 of
 these
 findings,
 followed
 by
 more
 practical
 applications
 of
 the
 results.
 Finally,
 I
 will
 discuss
 some
 routes
 for
 future
research.



(27)
(28)

One
of
the
primary
goals
of
people
is
to
attain
meaning
and
give
 structure
 to
 the
 ever‐changing
 environment.
 As
 a
 host
 of
 classic
 and
modern
consistency
theories
in
social
psychology
have
argued
 and
 shown,
 people
 have
 a
 strong
 need
 to
 be
 able
 to
 understand,
 structure,
and
predict
the
(social)
world
(see
Abelson,
Aronson,
McGuire,
Newcomb,
 Rosenberg,
 &
 Tannenbaum,
 1968;
 Cooper,
 2007;
 Festinger,
 1957;
 Harmon‐Jones
 &
 Mills,
 1999).
 Expectancies
 are
 an
 important
 tool
 to
 achieve
 this
 goal.
 Expectancies
 can
 be
 defined
 as
 beliefs
 about
 a
 future
 state
 of
 affairs
 that
 enable
 people
 to
 anticipate
what
is
coming
and
prepare
them
for
effective
living
(Biernat,
2005;
Olson,
 Roese,
 &
 Zanna,
 1996;
 Roese
 &
 Sherman,
 2007).
 Without
 the
 help
 of
 expectancies,
 the
world
would
be
experienced
as
chaotic.
Expectancies
help
to
structure
the
world
 (“Leon
is
a
friendly
person”)
and
make
it
predictable
(“Leon
is
unlikely
to
hit
me”).


It
 feels
 pleasant
 and
 positive
 when
 expectancies
 are
 accurate.
 When
 the
 world
 is
 expectancy‐congruent,
 people
 experience
 positive
 affect
 and
 feelings
 of
 comprehension
 and
 control.
 Conversely,
 when
 expectancies
 are
 disconfirmed
 and
 the
 world
 turns
 out
 to
 be
 incongruent
 to
 predictions,
 negative
 affect
 is
 often
 experienced
(Elliot
&
Devine,
1994;
Mendes,
Blascovich,
Hunter,
Lickel,
&
Jost,
2007).
 Of
 course,
 there
 are
 individual
 differences
 (some
 people
 like
 structure
 more
 than
 others)
and
situational
exceptions
(think
of
sports
and
arts
–
excitement,
creativity),
 but
 for
 most
 people
 and
 in
 most
 situations,
 incongruency,
 inconsistency,
 disconfirmation,
and
dissonance
are
uncomfortable
mental
states
that
elicit
negative
 affect.
At
least,
that
is
what
consistency
theories
have
been
positing
for
the
past
fifty
 years
(Aronson,
1968;
Cooper,
2007;
Festinger,
1957;
Harmon‐Jones,
1999;
Heider,
 1958).
 


A paradox 

(29)

consistency‐is‐good,
 inconsistency‐is‐bad
 reasoning:
 How
 would
 one
 feel
 when
 the
 world
turns
out
to
be
inconsistent
with
predictions,
but
positive?
The
incongruency
 may
feel
uncomfortable,
but
the
outcome
may
feel
good.
The
question
is
thus,
what
 will
happen
when
a
negative
expectancy
(“Elliot
is
unfriendly”)
is
disconfirmed
in
a
 positive
way
(“Elliot
engages
is
several
friendly
acts”)?
When
will
the
(in)congruency
 drive
 the
 affective
 response
 (positive
 affect
 after
 congruency,
 negative
 affect
 after
 incongruency)
and
when
will
the
valence
of
the
outcome
drive
the
affective
response
 (positive
affect
after
a
positive
outcome,
negative
affect
after
a
negative
outcome)?
 
 We
 argue
 that
 there
 is
 a
 simple
 solution
 for
 this
 paradox:
 The
 affective
 response
 to
 disconfirmation
 of
 expectancies
 depends
 on
 whether
 people
 are
 primarily
focused
on
the
expectancy­congruency
of
the
outcome
or
on
the
valence
of
 the
outcome.
The
affective
responses
of
people
who
are
congruency‐focused
will
be
 positive
 when
 outcomes
 are
 expectancy‐congruent
 and
 negative
 when
 they
 are
 expectancy‐incongruent
 –independent
 of
 the
 valence
 of
 the
 outcome.
 The
 affective
 responses
 of
 people
 who
 are
 outcome‐focused
 will
 be
 positive
 when
 outcomes
 are
 positive
 and
 negative
 when
 they
 are
 negative
 –independent
 of
 the
 expectancy‐ congruency
of
the
outcome.



To
 illustrate,
 imagine
 you
 feel
 like
 eating
 a
 snack.
 You
 come
 home
 and
 on
 the
table
there
is
a
plate
with
pieces
of
cheese.
You
like
cheese
very
much
and
with
 the
expectancy
of
the
salty
taste
of
cheese
you
take
a
piece
and
put
it
in
your
mouth.
 After
a
few
chews
you
notice
that
it
is
not
cheese
you
are
chewing
on,
but
something
 else.
You
do
not
immediately
recognize
what
it
is
and
the
unexpected
and
unfamiliar
 substance
in
your
mouth
is
probably
an
unpleasant
experience
(congruency‐focus).
 However,
 after
 a
 few
 moments
 you
 start
 realizing
 that
 instead
 of
 the
 salty
 taste
 of
 cheese,
you
are
chewing
on
something
that
is
sweet.
You
realize
that
the
taste
is
nice
 and
that
instead
of
cheese
you
are
eating
marzipan
that
is
shaped
and
colored
to
look
 like
cheese.
Although
it
is
not
what
you
thought
is
would
be,
the
moment
you
realize
 what
it
is,
you
probably
like
the
experience
of
eating
it
(outcome‐focus).



(30)

outcome‐
or
congruency‐focused.
But
what
then
determines
one’s
focus?
Following
 earlier
consistency
theorists
(see
Abelson
et
al.,
1968;
Festinger,
1957;
but
see
also,
 Cooper,
2007;
Harmon‐Jones
&
Mills,
1999;
Swann,
1990;
Tavris
&
Aronson,
2007),
 we
 argue
 that
 the
 need
 for
 consistency
 is
 fundamental
 and,
 thus,
 the
 expectancy‐ congruency
focus
is
likely
to
be
a
relatively
basic
and
primary
response.
People
like
 the
 world
 to
 be
 predictable
 and
 controllable
 because
 this
 makes
 it
 easier
 to
 maneuver
through.
It
is
comforting
to
know
that
trees
do
not
spontaneously
change
 into
porcupines
and
tables
do
not
sometimes
turn
into
casseroles.
In
fact,
this
need
 for
consistency
is
the
basis
for
all
kinds
of
psychological
phenomena.
It
is
for
the
sake
 of
consistency
that
people
search
for
evidence
that
confirms
rather
than
disconfirms
 their
hypotheses,
expectancies,
stereotypes,
or
prejudices
(e.g.,
Klein
&
Kunda,
1992;
 Snyder
 &
 Swann,
 1978),
 that
 people
 especially
 like
 things
 they
 have
 seen
 twice
 instead
of
once
(e.g.,
Zajonc,
1968),
and
that
people
are
inclined
to
preserve
and
hold
 on
to
their
self‐views,
even
when
these
views
are
not
especially
flattering
(Aronson
 &
Carlsmith,
1962;
Swann,
1983).
People
do
not
strive
for
consistency
as
an
end
in
 itself,
but
as
a
means
of
bolstering
their
perception
that
the
world
is
predictable
and
 controllable
(see
Cooper,
2007).


(31)

(32)

is
 important
 for
 people,
 it
 is
 likely
 that
 expectancy‐congruency
 will
 be
 their
 main
 focus
 and
 the
 (in)congruency
 of
 an
 outcome
 will
 drive
 affective
 responses
 (e.g.,
 expected
 unkindness
 feels
 good;
 unexpected
 kindness
 does
 not).
 Conversely,
 when
 consistency
is
valued
less,
people
are
likely
to
be
less
congruency‐focused
and
more
 willing
and
able
to
become
outcome‐focused,
such
that
the
valence
of
the
outcome
 (and
 not
 its
 relation
 to
 expectancies)
 will
 drive
 affective
 responses
 (e.g.,
 kindness
 feels
good;
unkindness
does
not).




 In
the
present
chapter,
we
test
this
main
hypothesis
concerning
the
need
for
 consistency
 and
 the
 affective
 responses
 to
 disconfirmation
 of
 expectancies
 in
 five
 experiments
and
an
illustrative
study.
In
these
five
experiments,
we
use
a
variety
of
 stimuli.
 Specifically,
 unexpected
 food
 (illustration),
 person
 perception
 (Experiment
 2.1),
 visual
 (Experiment
 2.3),
 and
 self‐relevant
 performance
 (Experiment
 2.4
 and
 2.5)
stimuli.
Furthermore,
we
manipulate
the
value
people
assign
to
consistency
in
 different
ways.
We
do
this
not
only
to
demonstrate
the
robustness
of
our
theorizing,
 but
also
to
attest
to
the
theory‐overarching
qualities
of
the
consistency
concept:
That
 is,
theorizing
is
used
from
divergent
branches
of
social
psychology.
Specifically,
we
 use
measures
and
manipulations
of
Need
for
Structure
(Experiment
2.1a),
regulatory
 focus
 (Experiment
 2.1b),
 mortality
 salience
 (Experiment
 2.1c),
 and
 cognitive
 busyness
 (Experiment
 2.1d,
 2.3,
 2.4,
 and
 2.5)
 to
 test
 the
 general
 hypothesis
 that
 whether
 one
 likes
 or
 dislikes
 positive/negative
 outcomes
 that
 are
 congruent/incongruent
to
one’s
expectancies
is
a
matter
of
whether
one
is
outcome‐ focused
 or
 congruency‐focused
 –with
 the
 latter
 focus
 being
 the
 primary
 and
 more
 spontaneous
one.
We
will
furthermore
offer
an
illustration
of
the
manner
in
which
 incongruency‐to‐outcome
 responses
 unfold
 using
 filmed
 facial
 expressions
 to
 unexpected
food. 

(33)

is
 still
 unclear,
 however,
 whether
 dissonance
 always
 elicits
 negative
 affect,
 even
 when
the
dissonant
outcome
is
positive.
With
the
present
studies,
we
aim
to
further
 our
 understanding
 of
 this
 issue
 and
 provide
 support
 for
 the
 hypothesis
 that
 a
 congruency‐focus
is
a
primary,
and
an
outcome‐focus
is
a
secondary
response.
  

From incongruency to outcome: An illustration 

Before
we
turn
to
a
discussion
of
our
experiments,
we
first
want
to
illustrate
 the
manner
in
which
incongruency‐to‐outcome
responses
unfold,
using
observation
 of
 how
 people
 react
 to
 unexpected
 (but
 nice)
 food.
 Similar
 to
 the
 example
 we
 described
above,
participants
were
asked
to
eat
a
piece
of
Gouda
cheese.
In
reality,
it
 was
either
cheese
or
marzipan
that
looked
like
cheese.
We
filmed
participants’
facial
 responses
to
test
our
hypothesis
that
people
who
generally
like
marzipan
but
eat
a
 piece
 expecting
 it
 is
 cheese,
 first
 respond
 with
 a
 negative
 facial
 expression
 (confusion,
disgust),
which
in
time
turns
to
a
positive
facial
expression.
Thus,
in
this
 study
we
filmed
participants
(N =
58)
who
either
ate
a
block
of
cheese
or
a
block
of
 marzipan
that
looked
like
cheese
(see
for
pictures
of
the
marzipan
and
an
example
a
 videotaped
observation,
www.stapel.socialpsychology.nl/cheese.html).



  Procedure.  Participants
 entered
 the
 lab,
 where
 they
 were
 seated
 behind
 a
 computer
 in
 an
 individual
 cubicle.
 Participants
 read
 on
 the
 screen
 that
 they
 were
 about
to
participate
in
three
ostensibly
unrelated
studies.



(34)

who
like
cheese
and
marzipan)
and
to
check
for
possible
food
allergies.
Embedded
in
 several
filler
questions
(to
avoid
suspicion),
we
asked
participants
to
indicate
their
 preference
for
cheese
and
marzipan
(from
1
=
not at all nice to
7
=
extremely nice)
 and
 whether
 they
 were
 allergic
 to
 any
 foods,
 including
 dairy
 products
 and
 nuts.
 When
participants
indicated
that
they
were
not
allergic
to
any
of
these,
the
program
 continued
to
the
final
and
last
task,
the
“tasting”
study.


Participants
 were
 told
 that
 they
 had
 to
 taste
 a
 piece
 of
 cheese
 after
 which
 they
 would
 be
 asked
 to
 give
 their
 opinion
 about
 this
 cheese.
 We
 told
 participants
 that
they
should
take
as
much
time
as
they
needed
to
get
a
good
sense
of
the
taste
of
 the
 cheese.
 For
 half
 of
 the
 participants,
 these
 instructions
 matched
 the
 outcome:
 They
 ate
 Gouda
 cheese.
 For
 the
 other
 half
 of
 the
 participants,
 these
 instructions
 mismatched
 the
 outcome:
 They
 ate
 marzipan
 that
 looked
 like
 Gouda
 cheese.
 We
 filmed
 participants’
 facial
 responses
 while
 eating
 (with
 the
 webcam
 that
 was
 still
 running
 from
 the
 first
 part
 of
 the
 study).
 After
 participants
 tasted
 the
 cheese
 or
 marzipan,
 they
 were
 thanked
 and
 debriefed
 and
 we
 asked
 them
 for
 permission
 to
 use
the
films
for
analysis.


Coding of the facial expression. We
edited
the
films
in
such
a
way
that
they
all
 started
from
the
moment
that
participants
put
the
block
in
their
mouth
and
stopped
 20
 seconds
 later.
 Two
 independent
 coders
 coded
 for
 each
 of
 the
 20
 seconds
 the
 content
 of
 the
 response
 (“What  do  you  see?”
 choosing
 out
 of
 no  response,  disgust,  confusion and, joyfulness)
and
the
intensity
of
the
response
(“How intense is it?”
from
 1
=
hardly visible, to
5
=
extremely visible).
The
interrater
agreement
on
content
was
 79%
 and
 disagreement
 was
 solved
 trough
 discussion.
 The
 intensity
 scores
 of
 the
 coders
were
averaged.
For
an
illustration,
see
Figure
1.


Results


(35)

disgust,
 confusion
 and
 joyfulness
 (1
 =
 present,
 0
 =
 absent).
 Subsequently,
 we
 multiplied
these
variables
with
the
intensity
score
per
second
(which
thus
resulted
 in
 zero
 when
 the
 facial
 expression
 was
 absent
 and
 in
 the
 intensity
 score
 when
 the
 facial
expression
was
present)
and
we
averaged
the
disgust
and
confusion
scores
to
 create
one
negative
category.

 
 
 
 
 
   Figure 1. Examples of facial expressions to unexpected marzipan (Left: negative response at second 4. Right:  positive response at second 11).

(36)

any
 positive
 or
 negative
 facial
 expressions,
 whereas
 participants
 in
 the
 marzipan
 condition
 initially
 respond
 with
 a
 negative
 facial
 expression
 (congruency‐focus),
 which
is
then
followed
by
a
positive
facial
expression
(outcome‐focus).
Thus,
these
 results
nicely
illustrate
the
logic
of
our
general
model:
People
do
not
like
unexpected
 outcomes,
even
when
they
are
pleasant.
It
takes
time
to
appreciate
the
pleasantness
 of
the
unexpected.
 
 Figure 2. Mean intensity of positive and negative facial expressions as a function of food (expected cheese or  unexpected marzipan). 

Experiments 2.1a­2.1d: Person Perception

  
 In
the
first
series
of
experiments,
we
set
out
to
test
our
hypothesis
using
a
 general
 person
 perception
 paradigm.
 In
 all
 these
 studies,
 we
 induced
 a
 positive
 or
 negative
 expectancy
 about
 a
 target
 and
 then
 presented
 respondents
 with
 information
that
either
confirmed
or
disconfirmed
this
expectancy.
In
each
study,
we
 use
well‐known
social
psychological
phenomena
that
have
been
shown
to
influence
 the
need
for
consistency.


(37)

Experiment 2.1a: Personal Need for Structure 

  In
this
experiment,
we
test
our
main
hypothesis
using
a
chronic
measure
of
 one’s
 willingness
 and
 ability
 to
 abandon
 expectancies
 and
 focus
 on
 the
 outcome.
 People
differ
in
the
extent
to
which
they
find
predictability
and
structure
important
 and
have
a
chronic
desire
for
consistent
knowledge
(e.g.,
Neuberg
&
Newsom,
1993;
 Webster
 &
 Kruglanski,
 1994).
 In
 the
 present
 experiment,
 we
 tap
 these
 individual
 differences
in
the
Personal Need for Structure
(PNS,
Thompson,
Naccarato,
&
Parker,
 1989)
 and
 hypothesize
 that
 people
 who
 have
 a
 high
 PNS
 are
 more
 likely
 to
 be
 congruency‐focused,
such
that
expectancy‐congruent
outcomes
elicit
positive
affect
 and
expectancy‐incongruent
outcomes
elicit
negative
affect.
People
who
have
a
low
 PNS
should
be
more
focused
on
actual
rather
than
expected
reality,
such
that
positive
 outcomes
 elicit
 positive
 affect
 and
 negative
 outcomes
 elicit
 negative
 affect,
 regardless
of
congruency.


Method 

Participants
 were
 undergraduate
 students
 (N  
 =
 80,
 Mage
 =
 19
 years),
 who
 were
randomly
assigned
to
one
of
the
four
conditions
of
a
2
(Expectancy:
positive,
 negative)
 x
 2
 (Target:
 positive,
 negative)
 between‐subjects
 design,
 with
 Personal
 Need
for
Structure
as
a
continuous
moderator
variable.


Procedure.  Participants
 received
 a
 set
 of
 two
 seemingly
 unrelated
 studies.
 After
 an
 instruction,
 participants
 filled
 out
 the
 Personal
 Need
 for
 Structure
 scale
 (Thompson
 et
 al.,
 1989;
 α
 =
 .80).
 After
 this,
 the
 second
 study
 followed,
 which
 was
 framed
as
a
person
perception
study.
In
the
instructions,
we
induced
the
expectancy:
 We
 told
 participants
 in
 the
 positive
 expectancy
 condition
 that
 they
 were
 going
 to
 read
 about
 a
 kind  man,
 named
 Paul.
 In
 the
 negative
 expectancy
 condition,
 we
 told
 participants
 that
 Paul
 was
 unkind.
 Subsequently,
 participants
 read
 a
 scenario
 that
 was
congruent
or
incongruent
to
their
expectancy,
about
kind or
unkind
Paul.2
After
 reading
 the
 scenario,
 participants
 were
 asked
 to
 rate
 their
 current
 feelings
 on
 a
 9‐

2
We
pretested
the
two
scenarios
(“What
is
your
impression
of
Paul”,
from
1
=
not at all kind,
to
9
=


extremely kind),
which
showed
that
the
kind
scenario
of
Paul
was
judged
extremely
kind
(M =
8.2)
and
the


(38)

point
rating
scale
(“Right
now,
how
do
you
feel?”
from
1
=
not at all,
to
9
=
extremely),
 using
a
mood
questionnaire
developed
by
Förster
and
colleagues
(Förster,
Higgins,
&
 Werth,
2004;
happy, content, discouraged, disappointed, calm, relaxed, tense, worried;
 α
 =
 .94).
 Finally,
 participants
 were
 asked
 to
 judge
 Paul
 on
 a
 9‐point
 rating
 scale
 (“What
is
your
impression
of
Paul?”,
from
1
=
not at all kind,
to
9
=
extremely kind). 

Results 

We
computed
the
mean
of
the
items
measuring
Personal
Need
for
Structure
 and
standardized
this
measure
so
that
M
=
0
and
SD
=
1.
We
performed
a
regression
 analysis
 with
 Personal
 Need
 for
 Structure,
 expectancy,
 target
 and
 the
 interactions
 between
 these
 variables
 on
 affect.
 The
 analysis
 revealed
 the
 predicted
 three‐way
 interaction
between
expectancy,
target
and
Personal
Need
for
Structure
on
affect,
B =
 2.31,
 t(72)
 =
 5.48,
 p  <
 .001.
 Following
 Aiken
 and
 West
 (1991),
 we
 selected
 data
 points
 for
 estimating
 regression
 lines
 at
 one
 standard
 deviation
 above
 the
 mean
 (labeled
 high
 need
 for
 structure)
 and
 at
 one
 standard
 deviation
 below
 the
 mean
 (labeled
low
need
for
structure).
Table
2.1a
depicts
means
of
affect
as
a
function
of
 expectancy,
 target
 and
 label
 for
 high
 need
 for
 structure
 (+1
 SD)
 and
 low
 need
 for
 structure
(‐1
SD).




 
 High
need
for
Structure
 Low
Need
for


Structure
 
 Outcome
 Positive
 Negative
 Positive
 Negative
 Expectancy
 Positive
 7.42
 6.358.096.37


 Negative
 5.72
 8.378.656.21

Table  2.1a.  Mean  affective  response  as  a  function  of  expectancy,  target  and  Personal  Need  for  Structure  (Experiment 2.1a). Note: higher values indicate a more positive affective response. 

(39)

Further
 analyses
 indicated
 that,
 as
 expected,
 people
 with
 a
 high
 need
 for
 structure
responded
more
positively
to
congruency
than
to
incongruency,
regardless
 of
 the
 outcome
 (Mpositive
 expectancy,
 positive
 target
 =
 7.42,
 Mpositive
 expectancy,
 negative
 target
=
 6.35,


F(1,72)
 =
 9.93,
 p  <
 .01,
 η2
=
 .06.
 Mnegative
 expectancy,
 negative
 target
 =
 8.37,
 Mnegative
 expectancy,
 positive
 target
=
 5.72,
 F(1,72)
 =
 40.12,
 p  <
 .001,
 η2
=
 .25).
 People
 with
 a
 low
 need
 for
 structure
responded
more
positively
to
positive
outcomes
than
to
negative
outcome,
 regardless
of
congruency
(Mpositive
expectancy,
positive
target
=
8.09,
Mpositive
expectancy,
negative
target
 =
6.37,
F(1,72)
=
16.61,
p <
.001,
η2
=
.10.
Mnegative
 expectancy,
 positive
 target
=
8.65,
Mnegative
 expectancy,
negative
target
=
6.21,
F(1,72)
=
30.20,
p <
.001,
η2
=
.18).



In
sum,
the
affective
responses
of
people
with
a
high
need
for
structure
were
 more
 positive
 after
 congruent
 than
 after
 incongruent
 outcomes,
 whereas
 the
 affective
responses
of
people
with
a
low
need
for
structure
were
more
positive
after
 positive
than
after
negative
outcomes.



The
 judgment
 results
 of
 this
 and
 the
 following
 person
 perception
 studies
 will
be
discussed
in
the
discussion
at
the
end
of
this
section.


Experiment 2.1b: Regulatory focus 

(40)

logic.
In
Experiment
2.1b
we
thus
manipulate
regulatory
focus
and
hypothesize
that
 people
with
a
prevention
focus
are
more
likely
to
be
congruency‐focused,
such
that
 expectancy‐congruent
 outcomes
 elicit
 positive
 affect
 and
 expectancy‐incongruent
 outcomes
elicit
negative
affect.
People
with
a
promotion
focus,
however,
should
be
 more
 likely
 to
 have
 an
 outcome‐focus,
 such
 that
 positive
 outcomes
 elicit
 positive
 affect
and
negative
outcomes
elicit
negative
affect,
regardless
of
congruency.



Method 

Participants
 were
 undergraduate
 students
 (N
 =
 91,
 Mage
 =
 19
 years)
 who
 were
randomly
assigned
to
one
of
the
eight
conditions
of
a
2
(Expectancy:
positive,
 negative)
 x
 2
 (Target:
 positive,
 negative)
 x
 2
 (Regulatory
 focus:
 promotion,
 prevention)
between‐subjects
design.


(41)

Lockwood
 and
 colleagues
 (2002),
 on
 a
 7‐point
 rating
 scale
 (“I
 am
 more
 concerned
 with
 reaching
 success
 than
 preventing
 failure”,
 from
 1
 =
 strongly  disagree,
 to
 7
 =
 strongly agree),
which
served
as
a
manipulation
check.


Results

An
 ANOVA
 with
 regulatory
 focus
 on
 the
 manipulation
 check
 item
 showed
 that
the
regulatory
focus
manipulation
was
successful
(Mprevention =
5.04,
 Mpromotion
=
 5.78),
F(1,83)
=
13.47,
p <
.001.



We
conducted
a
2
(Expectancy)
x
2
(Target)
x
2
(Regulatory
focus)
ANOVA
 on
 the
 affect
 scores,
 which
 showed
 the
 expected
 three‐way
 interaction
 between
 expectancy,
 target
 and
 regulatory
 focus
 F(1,83)
 =
 12.08,
 p  <
 .01,
 η2
=
 .11.
 We
 also
 found
a
main
effect
of
target,
F(1,83)
=
9.74,
p
<
.01,
η2
=
.09,
a
two‐way
interaction
 between
 expectancy
 and
 target,
 F(1,83)
 =
 4.85,
 p
 <
 .05,
 η2
=
 .04,
 and
 a
 marginally
 significant
two‐way
interaction
between
target
and
regulatory
focus
F(1,83)
=
2.74,
p
 =
 .10,
 η2
=
 .02
 (Other
 effects,
 Fs
 <
 1.00).
 To
 interpret
 the
 three‐way
 interaction
 between
 expectancy,
 target
 and
 regulatory
 focus
 on
 affect,
 we
 first
 looked
 at
 the
 prevention‐focus
conditions
and
then
at
the
promotion‐focus
conditions
(see
Table
 2.1b).



 
 Prevention
Focus
 Promotion
Focus



 Outcome
 Positive
 Negative
 Positive
 Negative
 Expectancy
 Positive
 7.43
 6.007.036.33


 Negative
 6.30
 7.147.256.04

Table  2.1b.  Mean  affective  response  as  a  function  of  expectancy,  target  and  regulatory  focus  (Experiment  2.1b). Note: higher values indicate a more positive affective response. 

(42)

In
the prevention focus conditions,
we
found
the
predicted
interaction
effect
 between
expectancy
and
target,
F(1,83)
=
14.64,
p <
.001,
η2
=
.14.
When
expectancy
 and
 target
 were
 congruent,
 the
 affective
 response
 was
 more
 positive
 than
 when
 expectancy
and
target
were
incongruent,
regardless
of
the
outcome
(Mpositive
expectancy,
 positive
 target
=
7.43,
Mpositive
 expectancy,
 negative
 target
=
6.00,
F(1,83)
=
12.95,
p <
.01,
η2
=
.11.


Mnegative
expectancy,
positive
target
=
6.30,
Mnegative
expectancy,
negative
target
=
7.14,
F(1,83)
=
4.31,
p <
 .05,
η2
=
.04).



In
 the
 promotion  focus  conditions,
 we
 found
 the
 predicted
 main
 effect
 of
 target,
 F(1,83)
 =
 9.96,
 p  <
 .01,
 η2
=
 .10.
 The
 affective
 response
 was
 more
 positive
 when
the
target
was
positive,
regardless
of
congruency.
(Mpositive
expectancy,
positive
target
=
 7.03,
Mpositive
 expectancy,
 negative
 target
=
6.33,
F(1,83)
=
3.23,
p =
.08,
η2
=
.03,
and
Mnegative
 expectancy,
positive
target
=
7.25,
Mnegative
expectancy,
negative
target
=
6.04,
F (1,83)
=
8.82,
p <
.01,
η2
=
 .08).



Thus,
these
results
show
that,
as
expected,
the
affective
responses
of
people
 with
a
prevention
focus
were
more
positive
after
congruent
than
after
incongruent
 outcomes,
 whereas
 the
 affective
 responses
 of
 people
 with
 a
 promotion
 focus
 were
 more
positive
after
positive
than
after
negative
outcomes.


Experiment 2.1c: Terror Management 

(43)

that
 expectancy‐congruent
 outcomes
 elicit
 positive
 affect
 and
 expectancy‐ incongruent
outcomes
elicit
negative
affect.
When
mortality
is
not
salient,
however,
 people
 should
 be
 outcome‐focused,
 such
 that
 positive
 outcomes
 elicit
 positive
 and
 negative
outcomes
elicit
negative
affect,
regardless
of
congruency.



Method 

To
 increase
 the
 generalizability
 of
 our
 findings,
 we
 used
 a
 somewhat
 different
 method
 from
 the
 one
 used
 in
 Experiments
 2.1a
 and
 2.1b.
 First,
 we
 constructed
a
different
affect
measure,
which
was
an
adaptation
of
the
dissonance
as
 psychological
 discomfort
 measure
 of
 Elliot
 and
 Devine
 (1994;
 happy,  energetic,  uncomfortable, uneasy, calm, optimistic, annoyed, dejected,
α
=
.91).
Second,
we
used
a
 different
 scenario,
 which
 described
 a
 woman
 named
 Maria
 as
 either
 an
 intelligent
 woman
(positive
target)
or
as
an
unintelligent
woman
(negative
target).
Participants
 were
 undergraduate
 students
 (N  =
 101,
 Mage
 =
 19
 years)
 who
 were
 randomly
 assigned
 to
 the
 conditions
 of
 a
 2
 (Expectancy:
 positive,
 negative)
 x
 2
 (Target:
 positive,
negative)
x
2
(Mortality
salience:
yes,
no)
between‐subjects
design.


Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Bewegend war unser Abschied von ihm, wenige Tage, ja wenige Standen vor seinem Tode im Hospiz in Wesel.. Bewegend war unser Abschied von ihm auf der

Terwijl Belle gaandeweg haar belangstelling voor de politiek verliest, zal Constant zich na zijn kennismaking met de ambitieuze Madame de Staël opmaken voor een actieve

dosering en optimaal teeltklimaat. Gezondere planten zijn minder gevoelig voor ziekten en plagen. ) Ziekten als meeldauw zijn met een goede klimaatregeling (beperken

How to design a mechanism that will be best in securing compliance, by all EU Member States, with common standards in the field of the rule of law and human

Testing 32 relative strength trading strategies (buying past winners and selling past losers) di ffering in number of ex post months of returns and following monthly returns, the

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of

It is not necessary that all terms from two domains be matched, we only have to define the mappings for terms used to link information from two domains, with respect to some

This study therefore ap- plies a method proposed by Bratti and Miranda (2011), who use maximum simulated likelihood to introduce an estimator for models where a count variable