• No results found

Raiding parties of male spider monkeys: Insights into human warfare?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Raiding parties of male spider monkeys: Insights into human warfare?"

Copied!
13
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Tilburg University

Raiding parties of male spider monkeys

Aureli, F.; Schaffner, C.M.; Verpooten, J.; Slater, K.; Ramos-Fernandez, G.

Published in:

American Journal of Physical Anthropology

DOI:

0.1002/ajpa.20451

Publication date:

2006

Link to publication in Tilburg University Research Portal

Citation for published version (APA):

Aureli, F., Schaffner, C. M., Verpooten, J., Slater, K., & Ramos-Fernandez, G. (2006). Raiding parties of male

spider monkeys: Insights into human warfare? American Journal of Physical Anthropology , 131(4), 486-497.

https://doi.org/0.1002/ajpa.20451

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal

Take down policy

(2)

Raiding Parties of Male Spider Monkeys: Insights into

Human Warfare?

Filippo Aureli,

1

* Colleen M. Schaffner,

2

Jan Verpooten,

1

Kathryn Slater,

2

and Gabriel Ramos-Fernandez

3

1

Research Centre in Evolutionary Anthropology and Palaeoecology, School of Biological and Earth Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool L3 3AF, United Kingdom

2

Department of Psychology, University of Chester, Chester CH1 4BJ, United Kingdom

3Centro Interdisciplinario de Investigacio´n para el Desarrollo Integral Regional, Instituto Polite´cnico Nacional,

Unidad Oaxaca, Oaxaca 71230, Mexico

KEY WORDS Ateles; coalitionary killing; fission-fusion; male bonding ABSTRACT Raids into neighboring territories may

occur for different reasons, including the increase of for-aging and mating opportunities directly or indirectly through the killing of neighboring rivals. Lethal raids have been mainly observed in humans and chimpanzees, with raiding males being reported to search purposefully for neighbors. Here we report on the first cases ever wit-nessed of raiding parties of male spider monkeys, a spe-cies expected to show such a behavioral tendency, given its similarity with humans and chimpanzees in critical socio-ecological characteristics, such as fission-fusion social dynamics and male-male bonding. Despite the high degree of arboreality of spider monkeys, all seven witnessed raids involved the males progressing single file on the ground in unusual silence. This is remarkably similar to the behavior of chimpanzees. The

circum-stances around the raids suggest that factors such as reduced mating opportunities, number of males relative to that in the neighboring community, and the strength of bonds among males could play a role in the timing of such actions. The raids did not appear to be aimed at finding food, whereas there is some indication that they may directly or indirectly increase reproductive opportu-nities. Although no killing was observed, we cannot exclude the possibility that spider monkey raids may be aimed at harming rivals if a vulnerable individual were encountered. The similarity of spider monkey raids with those of chimpanzees and humans supports the notion that lethal raiding is a convergent response to similar socio-ecological conditions. Am J Phys Anthropol 131: 486–497, 2006. VVC2006 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

Coalitionary killing of adult members of another group is an especially intense form of communal territorial ac-tivity, which occurs regularly in some social insects, but has been reported only in a few vertebrate species (reviewed in van der Dennen, 1995; Wrangham, 1999; Gros-Louis et al., 2003). Although rare, coalitionary intergroup killing has received much attention in recent years in the quest to understand the socio-ecological con-ditions for the evolutionary origin of human warfare (Goodall, 1986; Alexander, 1989; Knauft, 1991; Manson and Wrangham, 1991; Boehm, 1992; van der Dennen, 1995; Wrangham and Peterson, 1996; Otterbein, 1997; Wrangham, 1999; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Fergusson, 2001; Layton and Barton, 2001; Wilson and Wrangham, 2003). Coalitionary intergroup killing was reported in various contexts in different species of pri-mates and carnivores, such as during intergroup encoun-ters, attempted immigrations, and group takeovers (e.g., spotted hyenas: Boydston et al., 2001; wolves: Mech and Boitani, 2003; lions: Grinnell et al., 1995; African wild dogs: Creel and Creel, 2002; primates: Gros-Louis et al., 2003). Most attention, however, has been given to lethal raids of chimpanzees, not only because they are our clos-est living relatives, but also because such raids resemble simple forms of human warfare (Turney-High, 1971; Sil-litoe, 1985; Goodall, 1986; Manson and Wrangham, 1991; Chagnon, 1992; van der Dennen, 1995; Keeley, 1996; Otterbein, 1997; Maschner and Reedy-Maschner, 1998; Gat, 1999; Wrangham, 1999; Boesch and Boesch-Acher-mann, 2000; Kelly, 2000).

Lethal raiding is an unusual form of aggression be-cause it does not result from the escalation of an initial conflict (Wrangham, 1999). Lethal raids in humans and chimpanzees are characterized by the incursion of a coa-lition of males into the neighbors’ territory and the sur-prise fatal attack against vulnerable neighbors (Gat, 1999; Wrangham, 1999; Kelly, 2000). Lethal raiding dif-fers from other forms of coalitionary intergroup killing (see above) for two main reasons. First, it is an assault against neighbors rather than a defensive activity, such

Grant sponsor: Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research; Grant number: 6773; Grant sponsor: British Academy; Grant numbers: SG 32794, LRG 35389; Grant sponsor: Fondo Secto-rial CONACYT-SEMARNAT; Grant number: 0536; Grant sponsor: CONABIO; Grant number: M120; Grant sponsor: Instituto Polite´c-nico Nacional; Grant sponsor: North of England Zoological Society. FA was supported by a HEFCE Promising Researcher Fellowship during the writing of this paper.

*Correspondence to: Dr. Filippo Aureli, Research Centre in Evolu-tionary Anthropology and Palaeoecology, School of Biological and Earth Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, James Parsons Building, Byrom St., Liverpool L3 3AF, UK.

E-mail: f.aureli@ljmu.ac.uk

Received 3 June 2005; accepted 14 February 2006. DOI 10.1002/ajpa.20451

Published online 9 May 2006 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).

(3)

as when strangers are encountered by territory residents during incursions in search of food or attempted immi-grations. Second, the raiding individuals return to their own territory after the attack rather than becoming new group members, such as in group takeovers. As in humans, descriptions of chimpanzee lethal raids suggest the involvement of high coordination among raiding members and deliberate movement into the neighboring territory, apparently as a result of strategic planning (van Hooff, 1990; Boehm, 1992; Wrangham, 1999; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000).

Wolves are the only other vertebrates for which lethal raids have been reported, but the few detailed observa-tions suggest that the main reason for incursions into neighbors’ territory is to search for food, and the killing is an accidental consequence of meeting a few resident individuals (Peterson, 1977; Mech, 1994). These descrip-tions suggest that intruding wolves, like hyenas (Hofer and East, 1993), tend to avoid residents. By contrast, raiding chimpanzees appear to search for neighbors rather than food (Goodall et al., 1979; Wilson et al., 2004), and to intentionally move toward neighbors when they locate them, usually by hearing vocalizations (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Watts and Mitani, 2001; Watts et al., 2006), giving observers the impression that killing is the goal of chimpanzee raids (Goodall, 1986; Wilson et al., 2004).

There are other characteristics relevant to lethal raids that are shared by chimpanzees and territorial human populations (Ghiglieri, 1987; Rodseth et al., 1991; Boehm, 1992; Layton and Barton, 2001; van der Dennen, 1995; Wrangham, 1999; Mitani et al., 2002; Marlowe, 2005). They both live in social systems characterized by a high degree of fission-fusion dynamics, in which mem-bers of a large community are rarely all together because they split into subgroups of flexible membership. Such a social system not only sets the stage for subgroups of several males performing raids, but creates the possibil-ity for those males to encounter vulnerable victims, ei-ther alone or in smaller subgroups (i.e., imbalance of power: Goodall, 1986; Alexander, 1989; Manson and Wrangham, 1991; Wrangham, 1999).

Another relevant shared characteristic is that males of the same community form strong bonds with one an-other and perform most territorial activities together. In male-philopatric chimpanzees, boundary patrols, incur-sions, and lethal raids are always done by males, al-though some females may join them (Nishida, 1979; Goodall, 1986; Wrangham, 1999; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Watts et al., 2006). Similarly, in terri-torial human populations, women rarely participate in warfare, and males are nearly always the perpetrators of lethal raids (Adams, 1983; Boehm, 1992; Maschner and Reedy-Maschner, 1998; Manson and Wrangham, 1991).

Several hypotheses have been put forward to explain the occurrence of lethal raids in chimpanzees, both in terms of the proximate mechanisms and the long-term functions (Wrangham, 1999; Wilson and Wrangham, 2003). They can be reduced to two main hypotheses. Le-thal raids can be viewed as an extreme form of mate competition in which males kill rival males to defend access to their own females, and possibly to gain access to females from neighboring communities (Manson and Wrangham, 1991; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000). Another possibility is that lethal raids are an extreme form of intercommunity feeding competition in which males defend or expand their territory, thus increasing

the availability of food sources for themselves, the fe-males of their community, and their offspring (Wrangham, 1999; Williams et al., 2004). Characteristics of the timing, consequences, and behavior during raids may help in discriminating between the hypotheses. For example, according to the mate competition hypothesis, a factor that could affect the timing of male raids is the low availability of mating opportunities in their own community. In addition, one of the consequences of raids could be that females visit or permanently transfer to the community of raiding males because the raiding males demonstrate the ability to conduct raids in the neighbors’ territory in defiance of the resident males (cf. Manson and Wrangham, 1991; Ghiglieri, 1987; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000). Alternatively, according to the feeding competition hypothesis, raids are expected to occur when food sources are scarce, and the raiding party would spend a considerable amount of time feeding in the neighboring territory (Wrangham, 1999). However, certain patterns may support both hypotheses. For example, a male bias in lethal raiding victims may re-duce the power of neighboring male coalitions, thus in-creasing the extent to which raiding males can protect both females and food resources in their own territory, while facilitating their access to both females and food resources in neighboring territories (Wrangham, 1999; Wilson and Wrangham, 2003).

As the debate is still open over the relative benefits achieved by male raids in terms of gaining access to other females, weakening rivals, or improving food sour-ces (Wilson and Wrangham, 2003; Williams et al., 2004), data from other species would bring a useful compara-tive perspeccompara-tive. Furthermore, detailed descriptions of nonlethal raids might provide insights into underlying factors, as a short-term increase of mating or feeding opportunities can be achieved without the killing of rivals. So far, however, lethal or nonlethal raids resem-bling those of chimpanzees have not been described in other species.

We report on the first cases ever witnessed of raiding parties of male spider monkeys, one of the few other pri-mate taxa that has been hypothesized to perform lethal raids (Manson and Wrangham, 1991; Wrangham, 1999). Like chimpanzees, spider monkeys are characterized by male philopatry, a high degree of fission-fusion dynam-ics, strong associations between males, and male cooper-ative territorial defense (Klein, 1974; Fedigan and Baxter, 1984; van Roosmalen and Klein, 1988; Syming-ton, 1988, 1990; Chapman et al., 1995; Wallace, 2001). After describing the seven raids witnessed, we discuss our observations in light of the hypotheses put forward to explain raids in chimpanzees.

METHODS Study site and subjects

The observations reported here were part of a long-term study of the behavior and ecology of two commun-ities (Eastern and Western) of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi yucatanensis) carried out in the forest sur-rounding Punta Laguna lake (208380North, 878380West,

(4)

use the successional forest on a regular basis, they spend more time in the medium forest, and return to it to sleep.

Raids were observed during 2002–2003. The two com-munities have been intensively studied since 1997. The Eastern community, to which the raiding males be-longed, was well-habituated to the human presence before our long-term study began. The Western commu-nity, on which the raids took place, was habituated dur-ing 1997. All monkeys were individually recognized.

At the beginning of our long-term study in 1997, the Eastern community consisted of 19 monkeys with only one sexually mature male and seven sexually mature females, whereas the Western community consisted of 34 monkeys with seven sexually mature males and 14 sexu-ally mature females. At the beginning of 2002, the East-ern community consisted of 22 monkeys with six sexu-ally mature males and five sexusexu-ally mature females, all with unweaned infants. The number of sexually mature males was reduced to four in the course of 2002 because of the intracommunity coalitionary killing of the third youngest in April (Valero et al., in press), and the death of the fourth youngest in September. During 2002–2003, three sexually mature females immigrated into the East-ern community. During 2002–03, the WestEast-ern commu-nity consisted of about 40 monkeys with 8–10 sexually mature males and 12–14 sexually mature females. The monthly proportion of reproductively cycling females in Figure 2 was estimated on the basis of 7.5-month preg-nancy and 18-month lactation periods. These are the lowest figures reported in the review by Chapman and Chapman (1990), and thus are conservative estimates. In the case narratives, females with infants 18–24 months old were considered likely to be lactating.

Observational methods

The effort by our research team (i.e., time spent fol-lowing and searching for monkeys of the two commun-ities) was about 100 hr per month in 1997 and almost tripled in 1998 and 1999, to return to about 100 hr per month from 2000 until October 2002. Effort doubled from November 2002 when we started to have two sys-tematic data-collection teams (each with at least two people) per day, one for each community. The monkeys were typically observed at a distance of 10–20 m, which allowed good-quality observations most of the time, given the forest structure and use of binoculars. Data were col-lected by well-trained field assistants, students, and established researchers. When monkey subgroups were encountered, a combination of data-collection methods (Altmann, 1974) was used: 20-min instantaneous sam-pling of individual activities and locations, focal animal continuous sampling of detailed individual behavior, and all-occurrence sampling of major events such as fission, fusion, aggression, and rare behaviors such as locomo-tion on the ground. When a raid was witnessed, a com-plete account of the entire event was recorded into a dic-taphone, while the 20-min instantaneous samples of activities and locations and focal samples were continued when visibility permitted.

Ranging patterns were quantified using the instanta-neous samples of locations collected since 1997. Ranging patterns of the two communities have fluctuated over the years, but have consistently remained in the south-eastern and southwestern portions of the forest around the lake, with a relatively small overlapping area be-tween them (Ramos-Fernandez and Ayala-Orozco, 2003;

Ramos-Fernandez et al., 2003; Fig. 1). Home ranges were estimated using the kernel method (Worton, 1989). Dur-ing 2002–2003, the period when we witnessed the raids, the territory size of the raided Western community was about 1.1 km2, with about 0.3 km2of old-growth forest.

RESULTS

We witnessed seven raiding parties both while follow-ing monkeys of the Eastern community (four cases) and while searching for monkeys in the territory of the West-ern community (three cases; Table 1). The first raid was observed on March 25, 2002, and the last on December 19, 2003. Each case was witnessed by 2–4 researchers and field assistants, who consulted one another to improve the accuracy of witnessed events. The first raid followed the longest period without reproductively cy-cling females (i.e., all females were either pregnant or lactating for 10 months) in the Eastern community since the start of our long-term study (Fig. 2). Although research effort varied during 2002–2003 (see Methods), the rates of witnessed raids were quite similar when cor-rected for effort: 0.25 per 100 hr for March–October 2002, and 0.18 for November 2002–December 2003. Interestingly, no case of raids was observed for 6 months between cases 6 and 7, while the research effort re-mained roughly the same. Here we describe in detail the seven well-observed cases (numbers corresponding to those in Table 1) and two other relevant observations.

Case 1

On March 25, 2002, at 6:05 AM, we encountered the

four oldest sexually mature males of the Eastern com-munity in an overlapping area of the ranges of the East-ern and Western communities. At 6:25 AM, they

descended to the ground and started to walk slowly and quietly into the Western community’s territory. They were very attentive, scanning and appearing to listen for sounds. At 6:44 AM, they encountered three females,

probably from the Western community (we were unable to identify them because we could not get close to them, as they avoided the raiding males), one of which had an infant. About a minute later, the female with the infant started to alarm-bark. When the males climbed up a tree the female barked louder and then fled, followed by the males. After a brief pursuit the males slowed down, and continued to move slowly into the Western community’s territory, following the three females. At 6:53 AM, one of

the two females without an infant started barking. At 7:01AM, the males seemed to follow the third female, but

gave up rapidly. At 7:05 AM, the males ate a few fruits,

and the three females left them. This was the first feed-ing bout since the males entered the neighborfeed-ing terri-tory. The males spent about 2% of their time feeding during the incursion. They fed a bit more on the way back to their territory. Three of the four males fissioned at 7:37 AM, and we followed the remaining male who

reached his own territory at 8:15AM. Case 2

(5)

started to walk silently in single file into the Western community’s territory. Unfortunately, we lost them after a few minutes. We tried to find them by going to the same location where the Eastern community males went during their first witnessed raid. At 8:01AM, we

encoun-tered a Western community subgroup. At 8:18 AM, while

we were with this subgroup, we heard loud barking from a distance. We ran toward the location of the barking, where one of the females of the Western community sub-group had moved and may have been surprised by the raiding males. We did not find any females when we arrived at the location of the barking, but we did en-counter the Eastern community males walking on the ground at 8:22 AM. We followed them. They moved

qui-etly and attentively for a short time and then climbed up a tree, where they rested and groomed one another. They also ate briefly before moving and resting again, partially on the ground. At 9:28 AM, a female, known to

have an infant and likely lactating, arrived at the same location of the raiding males. She was one of the females previously seen in the Western community subgroup, and was immediately attacked. The oldest and youngest of the raiding males grabbed her and bit her repeatedly (the third male participated little in the attack), while she vocalized loudly. She was rescued by her sexually mature son who suddenly appeared on the scene a few minutes later, running on the ground. When the oldest raiding male attacked the son, the mother defended him. The four monkeys (two raiding males and the two resi-dents) fought for about 7 min, partially on the ground right in front of the four human observers, completely ignoring them. At 9:35AM, the adult female and her son

moved away from the scene. The Eastern community males did not follow them, and headed back into their territory. The infant, who was not seen during the whole

fight, joined the mother only when the raiding males were far away. Surprisingly, no monkey was seriously in-jured. The males returned to their territory at 10:15 AM,

feeding for only 3% of the observation time during the whole raid.

Case 3

At 8:06AMon March 1, 2003, all four sexually mature

males of the Eastern community fissioned from a sub-group containing the majority of their community females and young, and started to travel on the ground toward the Western community territory, which they reached at about 8:20AM. They continued to walk on the

ground with only a few brief pauses, when they climbed about 1 m high into nearby trees. After one of these pauses at about 8:40AM, one male left and went back to

his home territory and rejoined the original subgroup. While walking on the ground, the males followed one another single file with their tails up and were extremely quiet, without producing any vocalizations or stepping on twigs that could make noise. They traveled fast, often stopping to scan the surrounding area by standing on two legs or climbing approximately 1 m into nearby small trees. Around 9:15 AM, an unidentified

monkey, probably from the Western community, ap-peared to spot the raiding males walking, and dropped to the ground (it sounded like a large branch falling down) and tried to disappear into the undergrowth. The three raiding males chased the unidentified monkey for 20–30 m on the ground toward the lake from where we heard a brief scream and nothing more. Then, the three males went up into the nearby trees, looked around for a short time, and then went back where they were before the chase started. We investigated the area where the

(6)
(7)

chase ended and where the scream came from, but we could not see the other monkey. Then, the males traveled on the ground silently and in single file, following the lake until 9:40 AM, when they finally stopped at about

1.4 km from their territory boundary. They briefly fed on fruits that were also widely available in their own terri-tory. It was the first time they had eaten since they fis-sioned from their community subgroup. Then they rested for about 1 hr. At 10:50 AM, they started to head deeper

into the neighboring territory away from the lake, mov-ing from small tree to small tree. We lost them quickly due to the dense vegetation. The raid was one of the lon-gest (about 3 hr before losing them). Remarkably, they spent most of the first 1.5 hr on the ground, virtually without feeding.

Case 4

At 1:35PMon March 24, 2003, we encountered the four

sexually mature males of the Eastern community while searching for monkeys in the Western community terri-tory. The males walked quietly in single file on the ground for some time, always with the oldest male in the lead. At 2:10 PM, they encountered three adult

fe-males and two juvenile fefe-males of the Western commu-nity. Two of the adult females had an infant: one was very young, and the other was likely weaned (this was the same female that the raiding males attacked severely in case 2; by now, her infant was old enough to be weaned and thus she was likely reproductively cy-cling). The raiding males briefly chased the adult female without an infant, but then crept up on the adult female with the weaned infant, who was several meters away from the first adult female and seemed not to have noticed the males yet. The oldest two males immediately chased the adult female with the weaned infant, but

they appeared not to put effort in catching or harming her, as they waited for her to get away from them every time they went within arm’s reach of her. The chase con-tinued for about 10 min, until the oldest male chased the female off into the distance. Her infant was left behind with the other Western community females, who then also moved in the same direction as the chased female and the oldest raiding male. We stayed with the three remaining raiding males, who moved a little back toward their territory without going to the ground again. They spent time resting and socializing (especially the two old-est of these three males). At about 4:00 PM, they again

moved slowly toward their territory with some feeding and resting until 4:30 PM, when the two oldest males

chased the third male away and then returned to their own territory.

Case 5

While searching for monkeys quite deep in the West-ern community’s territory, we encountered the four sexu-ally mature males of the Eastern community at 8:20 AM

on May 12, 2003. The males were on the ground, travel-ing silently in typical stravel-ingle file, with the oldest male in the lead. We followed them as they traveled along the lake, away from their own territory. At 8:45 AM, there

was much confusion as the raiding males came across a subgroup of Western community monkeys. Several females scattered as soon as they saw the raiding males. Suddenly, a sexually mature male of the Western com-munity came crashing through the forest, followed by another sexually mature male and a sexually mature female. The first male screamed and ran around as if he was looking for something (perhaps the raiding males). We did not see the raiding males after the arrival of the three Western community monkeys. From 9:00 AM, the

(8)

two sexually mature males from the Western community alarm-called and screamed for nearly 2 hr, constantly climbing trees and looking out across the lake.

Case 6

At 6:40AMon May 20, 2003, we encountered the four

sexually mature males of the Eastern community well within their own territory. They soon started to move rapidly from tree to tree in the direction of the Western community’s territory. At 7:50 AM, they dropped to the

ground and walked silently in single file, with the oldest male in the lead. One of the males fissioned shortly thereafter and returned to his own territory. At 8:15AM,

the raiding males encountered two sexually mature females of the Western community, one of whom had a young infant. The two females appeared very afraid, emitting distress vocalizations and scanning. The raiding males just sat on the ground, quite peacefully, until the females relaxed. After about 10 min, the two females left, and the males continued to travel on the ground in a different direction. We followed the males, but soon lost them. We found them again at 9:10 AM, when we

heard screaming ahead, and an unidentified sexually mature female came crashing through the forest, pur-sued by the three raiding males. The males chased her to the edge of the lake, and then all four monkeys jumped into the lake. There was further screaming from the female and a lot of splashing by all the monkeys. It was difficult to determine whether the males were trying to attack the female or just trying to get out of the lake. After about 5 min, the males climbed back into the trees, but the female remained in the water. The three males wrapped their arms around each other (a typical behav-ior during coalitionary aggression), shook branches, and threatened the female, which appeared to prevent her from leaving the water. The female attempted to leave several times and then half-swam, half-waded across a little bay and vanished before the males could reach her by land. The males then rested for a short time by the lake, and traveled along the lake to return to their terri-tory, having gone entirely around the lake. Unfortu-nately, at about 10:10 AM, we lost them due to poor

visi-bility and difficult terrain, but by then they were out of the Western community’s territory.

In cases 4–6, the raiding males spent virtually no time feeding while traveling into Western community terri-tory, with less than 5% of the time spent feeding in total in the neighboring territory, mostly on their way back.

Case 7

At 12:57 PM on December 19, 2003, we encountered

the four sexually mature males of the Eastern commu-nity deep inside the Western commucommu-nity’s territory. They were walking silently single file on the ground, but when they saw us, they climbed into nearby trees. They con-tinued their incursion, moving from tree to tree, and going farther into the neighboring territory along the lake. They did not descend to the ground again, and con-tinued along the lake until they reached the outer boundary of the neighboring territory, which was not ad-jacent to their own territory (Fig. 1). After about 1 hr, we lost them when they went into an area of dense vege-tation. During the witnessed part of the raid, the males mainly traveled and spent almost no time feeding.

An additional case

At 5:15 PM on February 25, 2003, we encountered

three sexually mature males of the Eastern community in the Western community’s territory. They were not on the ground, but it was dusk and they might have been settling down to sleep there, as it was late to be travel-ing back to their own territory. We did not consider this case with the others because we were with the males only a few minutes. However, it is interesting to report that the following day, a Western community female spent a few hours with the Eastern community males within their territory.

A further case by males from another community

On September 1, 2003, we observed three males be-longing to an unknown community walking silently in single file on the ground in the Western community’s ter-ritory. As soon as they saw us, the males ran away and disappeared. The three males (who could easily be recog-nized due to specific markings) had been seen with other unknown monkeys participating in an aggressive inter-community encounter with members of the Western com-munity 6 days earlier.

DISCUSSION

All seven raids involved 3–4 sexually mature males walking silently on the ground in single file for up to 1.5 hr, without spending much time feeding or resting. The males moved up to 2 km into the neighboring terri-tory, entirely within the neighbors’ highly used area of old-growth forest (Fig. 1). A whole raid may last for over 3 hr (Table 1; it could easily be longer than we wit-nessed, as sometimes we encountered raiding males al-ready in the neighboring territory, or lost them due to the difficulty in following them on the ground).

Although multimale incursions into neighboring groups were reported in other monkey species (Sicotte and Mac-intosh, 2004), there are striking similarities between what we observed in spider monkeys and the behavior involved in boundary patrols and raids described in several chim-panzee populations (Table 2; Goodall, 1986; Nishida et al., 1999; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Watts and Mitani, 2001; Mitani et al., 2002; Wilson et al., 2004; Watts et al., 2006). The incursions were remarkably deep, given the 1.1-km2territory of the neighboring community

(Fig. 1), which is toward the lower end of territory sizes reported for other spider monkey communities (1–4 km2: Ramos-Fernandez and Ayala-Orozco, 2003), and much

TABLE 2. Characteristics of raids in chimpanzees and spider monkeys1

Characteristics of raids Chimpanzees

Spider monkeys Sexually mature males

always involved U U

Females may join in U

Walking on ground U U

Walking in single file U U

Silent, minimizing noise U U

Virtually no feeding U U

Pausing to scan and listen U U

Killing of neighbors U

1

(9)

smaller than chimpanzee territories (5–38 km2: Nishida

and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, 1987; Herbinger et al., 2001; Watts and Mitani, 2001). Although the seven cases described in detail here involved the same 3–4 males, it is unlikely that raiding parties are a peculiar feature of the Eastern community males, as we observed three males belonging to an unknown community walking silently in single file on the ground in the Western community’s territory.

Incursions into adjacent territories were reported from the four longest-term study sites with neighboring com-munities of chimpanzees (reviewed in Wrangham, 1999; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Mitani et al., 2002; Wilson and Wrangham, 2003), and lethal raids were witnessed or inferred in three of them: Gombe (Goodall et al., 1979; Wilson et al., 2004), Mahale (Nishida et al., 1985), and Kibale (Muller, 2002; Watts et al., 2006). At the fourth site, Taı¨ in Ivory Coast, chim-panzees are usually in larger subgroups and are more rarely alone than at other sites (Boesch, 1996), suggest-ing that the conditions for an imbalance of power between aggressors and victims, which facilitates coali-tionary killing, are less likely to occur (Wrangham, 1999; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000).

A similar argument was put forward to explain the ab-sence of reports of lethal raids in bonobos (Wrangham, 1999), who share many characteristics with chimpan-zees. This argument, however, may apply only to the study site of Wamba, where subgroups tend to be large (Kuroda, 1979; Kano, 1992); at Lomako, subgroup sizes are within the range found in chimpanzees (White, 1996; Hohmann and Fruth, 2002). Another explanation for the absence of lethal raids in bonobos is based on differences in intergroup interactions between the two Pan species, as bonobos appear to be less antagonistic (Idani, 1990; Kano, 1992; White, 1996). Alternatively, lethal raiding may not have been observed in bonobos because it is an event that arises from rare conditions. After all, it took 15 years, thousands of observations, and many intercom-munity encounters before witnessing the first case in Gombe chimpanzees (Stanford, 1998). However, the fact that unlike chimpanzees, boundary patrols and incur-sions into neighboring territories have not been observed in bonobos (Kano, 1992; Hohmann and Fruth, 2002) makes it unlikely that coalitionary killing during raids will be observed.

No lethal raids have been reported in spider monkeys. One hypothesized reason for the lack of evidence in this genus could be that lethal raids do not occur because the highly arboreal lifestyle of spider monkeys (Campbell et al., 2005) reduces the effectiveness of coalitionary aggression (Wrangham, 1999). As for bonobos, an alter-native explanation is that no sufficiently long-term study on fully habituated animals has been carried out on Ateles (Manson and Wrangham, 1991). This is especially true as raids are conducted on the ground, and high habituation to humans would certainly facilitate the ob-servation of such risky behavior for a highly arboreal species. Furthermore, the research effort on spider mon-keys is minuscule compared to that on chimpanzees. Thus, it is no surprise that raiding, although not lethal, was first witnessed in our Eastern community, likely the longest continuously studied and best-habituated com-munity of spider monkeys.

Four of the seven witnessed raids resulted in coalitio-nary aggression against small subgroups of 1–3 neigh-boring monkeys. While in three cases, aggression was

limited to (sometimes prolonged) chases, in case 2, a likely lactating female was severely attacked. Although no killing resulted from any of the raids, one may won-der what would have happened to the female victim in case 2 if her son had not rushed to her rescue. In case 5, at least two males and a female from the Western com-munity arrived on the scene, crashing through the forest when some females scattered after spotting the raiding males on the ground. Their sudden and noisy arrival resulted in the disappearance of the raiding males. Such rescue actions were also reported in chimpanzees, in which individuals from the resident community came to the aid of vulnerable victims, scaring intruders away (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Muller, 2002; Watts et al., 2006).

As expected if lethal raids are aimed at reducing the power of neighboring male coalitions (see introduction), all but one of the 11 victims of lethal raids in chimpan-zees were males (Wilson et al., 2004). This male bias may have some implications for interpreting our obser-vations. Killing may not have occurred in the seven raids we witnessed because the Eastern community males never surprised a vulnerable resident male. West-ern community males were seen only in two raids (cases 2 and 5), when they came to rescue females who first encountered the raiding males.

The Eastern community males are certainly capable of coalitionary killing, as they killed a male from their own community a week after the first witnessed raid (Valero et al., in press). Thus, they may perform lethal raids under the appropriate circumstances, i.e., if they find an isolated and vulnerable male victim. However, these cir-cumstances may be less common than for chimpanzees. Given that spider monkeys live in smaller territories (see above) and at higher densities (6–87 individuals/ km2

: Ramos-Fernandez et al., 2003) than chimpanzees (1–5 individuals/km2: Nishida and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa,

1987; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000), other resi-dents could more easily hear victims’ distress vocaliza-tions and come promptly to their rescue. It is also possi-ble that, as in bonobos, relationships between males of different communities are less antagonistic in spider monkeys than in chimpanzees. No interaction other than antagonism has been reported between chimpanzee males of different communities, apart from the initial phases when a community splits into two. In contrast, in the first part of our long-term project, we witnessed Western com-munity males not only interacting aggressively, but also exchanging friendly behavior with Eastern community males during extended visits in Eastern community terri-tory (Ramos-Fernandez, unpublished data).

(10)

ground to avoid being detected, see Campbell, 2003; see also Milton’s case 25 in Byrne and Whiten, 1990, p. 23). Females and juveniles were never seen walking on the ground in the same manner.

Habituation to observers is unlikely to have played a role in the timing of the first witnessed raid, as the East-ern community monkeys were habituated before our long-term study began in 1997. In addition, the timing of the first raid cannot be attributed to a lower amount of effort by our research team before March 2002, as the effort was similar or even higher before that date (see Methods). Although we may have missed cases, the increased effort from November 2002 influenced the number of observed raids, as 5 of 7 cases were observed after this date. However, the rates of witnessed raids were quite similar when corrected for effort. Thus, research effort did seem to influence the witnessing of cases after the first raid, but there was no actual change in overall frequency of the monkeys’ behavior.

A factor that could have affected the timing of the East-ern community raids is the number of sexually mature males relative to that of the neighboring community, a factor central to the imbalance of power hypothesis (Wrangham, 1999). There is observational and experimen-tal evidence supporting the importance of this factor in chimpanzees. Parties with more males are more likely to patrol (Mitani and Watts, 2005) and to approach extra-community males (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Watts and Mitani, 2001), and experimental playback of loud calls of an extragroup male elicits stronger coopera-tive responses from larger parties of male chimpanzees (Wilson et al., 2001; Herbinger and Boesch, 2004). In the case of spider monkeys, the number of sexually mature males in the Eastern community increased from one at the beginning of our long-term study to six at the time of the first witnessed raid, whereas the number of males in the Western community did not change as much. This rel-ative change would have therefore increased the relrel-ative power of the Eastern community males.

The larger number of males in the Eastern community is also likely to have increased mating competition within the community, which could have prompted raids in order to increase mating opportunities. Such competi-tion could have been the cause of the intragroup male coalitionary attacks in the Eastern community observed during the month before the first witnessed raid. These attacks had a lethal outcome, as one of the six males was eventually killed by an intracommunity male coali-tion a week after the first raid (Valero et al., in press). Although the loss of one male reduced the relative power of the Eastern community, the intracommunity coalitio-nary attacks against a common enemy might have strengthened the bonds between the other males, thereby facilitating the initiation of raids by increasing coordina-tion and trust for such risky behavior.

The timing of our first witnessed raid could support the mate competition hypothesis, as it followed the lon-gest period without reproductively cycling females in the Eastern community since the start of our study. How-ever, during the following 21 months in which we observed the other six raids, there was no clear relation-ship between their timing and the availability of cycling females in their own community (Fig. 2). Thus, although decreased mating opportunities within a community may prompt males to carry out raids in order to increase mat-ing, it would appear that once raiding is triggered, it continues for several months.

When Eastern community males encountered Western community females during five raids (probably six, but in case 3, the encountered monkey could not be sexed), they behaved toward them in ways that are in accord-ance with increasing mating opportunities. As reported in chimpanzees (Goodall et al., 1979; Nishida and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, 1985; Goodall, 1986; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Watts et al., 2006), the raiding males did not physically attack females without young or with old offspring who were or could soon become potential mating partners, whereas they severely attacked a likely lactating female (case 2). In some cases, the Eastern community males chased females without any serious attempt to catch them. The chases were sim-ilar to the aggressive behavior that male spider monkeys typically direct to females of their own community (Fedigan and Baxter, 1984; Campbell, 2003; Slater et al., 2005). During one raid (case 4), after a chase of about 10 min, the older Eastern community male quietly disap-peared with a neighboring female. The possibility that females visit or permanently transfer to the community of raiding males is probably not the primary reason for chimpanzee raids (Williams et al., 2004), but one obser-vation suggests that it may be more important in spider monkeys. In the additional case in Results, we reported that the day after the Eastern community males were seen in the neighboring territory at dusk, one Western community female spent a few hours with them within their territory.

Our observations do not bear much support for the feeding competition hypothesis, as the males spent virtu-ally no time feeding during the incursions. The little feeding they did in the neighboring territory was mainly done after they encountered neighboring monkeys and on food sources also available in their own territory, sug-gesting that foraging was not the main reason for the raids (as in chimpanzees: Wilson et al., 2004). Spider monkey raids may have long-term effects in the expan-sion of their own territory and the increase of food sources by reducing the power of the neighboring male coalition, but at the moment, we do not have evidence for these effects. The observation of the severe attack on a lactating female (case 2) could, however, fit the feeding competition hypothesis if the female is viewed as a feed-ing competitor rather than a matfeed-ing partner (cf. Wil-liams et al., 2004).

(11)

Our observations, although in favor of the explanation of lethal raids as a convergent response rather than due to common ancestry, are not fully supportive of the cur-rent interpretation of their evolution. As there is no evi-dence for cooperative hunting in spider monkeys, the similarity of spider monkey raids with those of chimpan-zees and humans could challenge the proposal of a link between group hunting and lethal raids based on the occurrence of both in humans and chimpanzees, but of neither in bonobos (Goodall et al., 1979; van Hooff, 1990; van der Dennen, 1995; Wrangham, 1999).

CONCLUSIONS

We cannot draw any firm conclusions from only seven raids. However, we noted some intriguing similarities with chimpanzees, suggested factors affecting the timing and possible functions of raids, and discussed their possi-ble relevance to the understanding of human warfare. First, like chimpanzees, spider monkey males progress in single file on the ground, and are unusually silent during raids. The similarity, which is especially remark-able given that spider monkeys are otherwise almost entirely arboreal (Campbell et al., 2005), suggests that such characteristics are critical for the success of raids.

Second, the circumstances around the first witnessed raid suggest that several factors could play a role in the timing of such actions. One possible factor is that the reduced mating opportunity in their own community may make males take risks to search for other females. Another factor is that spider monkey raids also seem to depend on an imbalance of power (Manson and Wrang-ham, 1991; WrangWrang-ham, 1999), as indicated by the fact that Eastern community males started to raid after their number increased relative to the number in the Western community. A critical factor is possibly the strength of bonds among males to assure trust and coordination dur-ing the risky raids, but these are difficult to operation-ally define and thus measure.

Third, the behavior of raiding males and resident mon-keys during encounters can give us some indication about the function of spider monkey raids. Like in chim-panzees, raiders appear to be searching for neighbors, not for food. Some of our observations are in agreement with the possibility that raids may directly or indirectly increase reproductive opportunities, but mating was not actually witnessed. Although no killing was observed, raiders might harm or kill male rivals who are suffi-ciently vulnerable. However, encountering such targets may be less likely than in chimpanzees, due to the higher probability that other residents come to their res-cue. In addition, even given such low-cost opportunities, spider monkeys may have lower motivation for lethal attacks than chimpanzees, due to the possibly less an-tagonistic nature of the relationships between males of neighboring communities.

Finally, the similarity of spider monkey raids with those of chimpanzees and humans suggests that a broad comparative perspective may help in elucidating the fac-tors and conditions underlying warfare. Such perspective should not only incorporate all cases of lethal raids, but would also benefit from the inclusion of well-detailed cases of nonlethal raids, as shown by our observations. In addition, it would be beneficial if the comparative per-spective is broadened to integrate cases of intergroup coalitionary killing in species with strong male-male bonding, but with a low degree of fission-fusion social

dy-namics, such as capuchin monkeys (Gros-Louis et al., 2003).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We are grateful for logistic support from the Punta La-guna community and Pronatura Peninsula de Yucatan. We thank Luisa Rebecchini for providing insights from her observations, Eulogio Canul, Macedonio Canul, Augusto Canul, and Juan Canul for valuable assistance in the field, and Laura Vick and David Taub for sharing the management of the long-term project. We are also thankful to Luigi Boitani, Paolo Ciucci, Marion East, Kay Holekamp, Robert Layton, David Mech, Frank Mar-lowe, Katie Milton, John Mitani, Michael Wilson, David Watts, the editor, and two anonymous reviewers for sharing information, helpful discussions, and comments on the manuscript.

LITERATURE CITED

Adams DB. 1983. Why there are so few women warriors. Behav Sci Res 18:196–212.

Alexander RD. 1989. Evolution of the human psyche. In: Mel-lars P, Stringer C, editors. The human revolution: behavioral and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. p 455–513.

Altmann J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–267.

Boehm C. 1992. Segmentary ‘‘warfare’’ and the management of conflict: comparison of East African chimpanzees and patrilin-eal-patrilocal humans. In: Harcourt AH, de Waal FBM, edi-tors. Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p 137–173.

Boesch C. 1996. Social grouping in Taı¨ chimpanzees. In: McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T, editors. Great ape soci-eties. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p 101–113. Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H. 2000. The chimpanzees of the

Taı¨ forest: behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Boydston EE, Morelli TL, Holekamp KE. 2001. Sex differences in territorial behavior exhibited by the spotted hyena (Hyae-nidae, Crocuta crocuta). Ethology 107:369–385.

Byrne RW, Whiten A. 1990. Tactical deception in primates: the 1990 database. Primate Rep 27. p 1–101.

Campbell CJ. 2003. Female-directed aggression in free-ranging Ateles geoffroyi. Int J Primatol 24:223–237.

Campbell CJ, Aureli F, Chapman CA, Ramos-Fernandez G, Mat-thews K, Russo SE, Suarez S, Vick L. 2005. Terrestrial behav-ior of spider monkeys (Ateles spp.): a comparative study. Int J Primatol 26:1039–1051.

Chagnon NA. 1992. Yanomamo¨: the last days of Eden. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.

Chapman CA, Chapman LJ. 1990. Reproductive biology of cap-tive and free-ranging spider monkeys. Zoo Biol 9:1–9. Chapman CA, Wrangham RW, Chapman LJ. 1995. Ecological

constraints on group size: an analysis of spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroubs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 36:59–70. Creel S, Creel NM. 2002. The African wild dog: behavior,

ecol-ogy, and conservation. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Fedigan LM, Baxter MJ. 1984. Sex differences and social

orga-nization in free-ranging spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). Pri-mates 25:279–294.

Ferguson RB. 2001. Materialist, cultural and biological theories on why Yanomami make war. Anthropol Theor 1:99–116. Gat A. 1999. The pattern of fighting in simple, small-scale,

pre-state societies. J Anthropol Res 55:563–583.

Ghiglieri MP. 1987. Sociobiology of the great apes and the homi-nid ancestor. J Hum Evol 16:319–357.

(12)

Goodall J, Bandora A, Bergmann E, Busse C, Matama H, Mpongo E, Pierce A, Riss D. 1979. Intercommunity interac-tions in the chimpanzee population of the Gombe National Park. In: Hamburg DA, McCown ER, editors. The great apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings Publishing Co. p 13– 54.

Grinnell J, Packer C, Pusey AE. 1995. Cooperation in male lions: kinship, reciprocity or mutualism? Anim Behav 49:95– 105.

Gros-Louis J, Perry S, Manson JH. 2003. Violent coalitionary attacks and intraspecific killing in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Primates 44:341–346.

Herbinger I, Boesch C. 2004. Is it worth a fight? The roles of resource holding potential and resource value in territorial defence among wild male chimpanzees. Folia Primatol [Suppl] (Basel) 75:272.

Herbinger I, Boesch C, Rothe H. 2001. Territory character-istics among three neighboring chimpanzee communities in the Taı¨ National Park, Coˆte d’Ivoire. Int J Primatol 22:143– 167.

Hofer H, East ML. 1993. The commuting system of Serengeti spotted hyaenas: how a predator copes with migratory prey. II. Intrusions pressure and commuters’ space use. Anim Behav 46:559–574.

Hohmann G, Fruth B. 2002. Dynamics in social organization of bonobos (Pan paniscus). In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, editors. Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bono-bos. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p 138–150. Idani G. 1990. Relations between unit-groups of bonobos at

Wamba, Zaire: encounters and temporary fusions. Afr Stud Monogr 11:153–186.

Kano T. 1992. The last ape: pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Keeley LH. 1996. War before civilization. New York: Oxford Uni-versity Press.

Kelly RC. 2000. Warless societies and the origin of war. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

Klein L. 1974. Agonistic behavior in neotropical primates. In: Holloway R, editor. Primate aggression, territoriality, and xenophobia: a comparative perspective. New York: Academic Press. p 77–122.

Knauft BM. 1991. Violence and sociality in human evolution. Curr Anthropol 32:391–428.

Kuroda S. 1979. Grouping of the pygmy chimpanzees. Primates 20:161–183.

Layton R, Barton RA. 2001. Warfare and human social evolu-tion. In: Fewster KJ, Zvelebil M, editors. Ethnoarcheology and hunter-gatherers: pictures at an exhibition. Oxford: Ar-chaeopress. p 13–24.

Manson JH. 1991. Reply. Curr Anthropol 32:385–387.

Manson JH, Wrangham RW. 1991. Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. Curr Anthropol 32:369–390. Marchant LF, McGrew WC. 1991. Commentary on Mason and

Wrangham’s article. Curr Anthropol 32:380–381.

Marlowe FW. 2005. Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evol Anthropol 14:54–67.

Maschner HDG, Reedy-Maschner KL. 1998. Raid, retreat, defend (repeat): the archaeology and ethnohistory of warfare on the North Pacific rim. J Anthropol Archaeol 17:19–51. Mech LD. 1994. Buffer zones of territories of gray wolves as

regions of intraspecific strife. J Mammal 75:199–202.

Mech LD, Boitani L. 2003. Wolf social ecology. In: Mech LD, Boitani L, editors. Wolves: behavior, ecology and conservation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p 1–34.

Mitani JC, Watts DP. 2005. Correlates of territorial boundary patrol behaviour in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 70:1079– 1086.

Mitani JC, Watts DP, Muller MN. 2002. Recent developments in the study of wild chimpanzee behavior. Evol Anthropol 11:9– 25.

Muller MN. 2002. Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chim-panzees. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, editors. Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cam-bridge: Cambridge University Press. p 112–123.

Nishida T. 1979. The social structure of the chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. In: Hamburg DA, McCown ER, editors. The great apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings Pub-lishing Co. p 73–121.

Nishida T, Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M. 1985. Responses to a stranger mother-son pair in the wild chimpanzee: a case report. Prima-tes 26:1–13.

Nishida T, Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M. 1987. Chimpanzees and bono-bos: cooperative relationships among males. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT, editors. Primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p 165–177.

Nishida T, Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M, Hasegawa T, Takahata Y. 1985. Group extinction and female transfer in wild chimpan-zees in the Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Z Tierpsychol 67:284–301.

Nishida T, Kano T, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nakamura M. 1999. Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthro-pol Sci 107:141–188.

Otterbein KF. 1997. The origin of war. Crit Rev 11:251–277. Peterson RO. 1977. Wolf ecology and prey relationships on Isle

Royale. Washington, DC: National Park Service, monograph series no. 11.

Power M. 1991. The egalitarians—human and chimpanzee: an anthropological view of social organization. Cambridge: Cam-bridge University Press.

Ramos-Fernandez G, Ayala-Orozco B. 2003. Population size and habitat use of spider monkeys at Punta Laguna, Mexico. In: Marsh LK, editor. Primates in fragments: ecology and conser-vation. New York: Plenum Press. p 191–209.

Ramos-Fernandez G, Vick LG, Aureli F, Schaffner C, Taub DM. 2003. Behavioral ecology and conservation status of spider monkeys in the Otoch Ma’ax Yetel Kooh protected area. Neo-trop Primates 11:155–158.

Rodseth L, Wrangham RW, Smuts BB, Harrigan A. 1991. The human community as a primate society. Curr Anthropol 32: 221–254.

Sicotte P, Macintosh AJ. 2004. Inter-group encounters and male incursions in Colobus vellerosus in central Ghana. Behaviour 141:533–553.

Sillitoe P. 1985. War, primitive. In: Kuper A, Kuper J, editors. The social science encyclopedia. London: Routledge. p 890–891. Slater K, Schaffner CM, Aureli F. 2005. Female-directed

aggres-sion in wild spider monkeys: male display and female mate choice. Primate Rep 72:89–90.

Stanford CB. 1998. The social behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos. Curr Anthropol 39:399–420.

Sussman RW. 1999. The myth of man the hunter, man the killer and the evolution of human morality (evolutionary and reli-gious perspectives on morality). Zygon 34:453–472.

Symington MM. 1988. Demography, ranging patterns, and activ-ity budgets of black spider monkeys (Ateles paniscus chamek) in the Manu National Park, Peru. Am J Primatol 15:45–67. Symington MM. 1990. Fission-fusion social organization in

Ateles and Pan. Int J Primatol 11:47–61.

Turney-High HH. 1971. Primitive war: its practice and concepts. Columbia, SC: University of South Carolina Press.

Valero A, Schaffner CM, Vick LG, Aureli F, Ramos-Fernandez G. In press. Intragroup lethal aggression in wild spider mon-keys. Am J Primatol .

van der Dennen JMG. 1995. The origin of war: the evolution of a male-coalitional reproductive strategy. Groningen: Origin Press. van Hooff JARAM. 1990. Intergroup competition and conflict in

animals and man. In: van der Dennen JMG, Falger VSE, edi-tors. Sociobiology and conflict: evolutionary perspectives on competition, cooperation, violence and warfare. London: Chap-man and Hall. p 23–54.

van Roosmalen MGM, Klein LL. 1988. The spider monkeys, ge-nus Ateles. In: Mittermeier RA, Coimbra-Filho AF, de Fonseca GAB, editors. Ecology and behavior of neotropical primates. Washington, DC: World Wildlife Fund. p 455–537.

(13)

Watts DP, Mitani JC. 2001. Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138:299–327. Watts DP, Muller M, Amsler SJ, Mbabazi G, Mitani JC. 2006.

Lethal intergroup aggression by chimpanzees in the Kibale national Park, Uganda. Am J Primatol 68:161–180.

White F. 1996. Comparative socioecology of Pan paniscus. In: McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T, editors. Great ape soci-eties. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p 29–41. Williams JM, Oehlert GW, Carlis JV, Pusey AE. 2004. Why do

male chimpanzees defend a group range? Anim Behav 68: 523–532.

Wilson ML, Wrangham RW. 2003. Intergroup relations in chim-panzees. Annu Rev Anthropol 32:363–392.

Wilson ML, Hauser M, Wrangham RW. 2001. Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Anim Behav 61:1203– 1216.

Wilson ML, Wallauer WR, Pusey AE. 2004. New cases of inter-group violence among chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Int J Primatol 25:523–549.

Worton BJ. 1989. Kernel methods for estimating the utilization distribution in home-range studies. Ecology 70:164–168. Wrangham RW. 1999. Evolution of coalitionary killing. Yrbk

Phys Anthropol 42:1–30.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

This hypothesis differs from H1, since the second hypothesis does not look at the performance of companies that have female board members in a given year, but tries to find if

Through linear regression and mediation analyses, it was figured out that functional as well as emotional reasons showed not enough statistical power to support the

In Chapter 7 we define a model for the administration of RBAC standard in a distributed system, formal safety and availability requirements, and we propose administrative procedures

The user would then have the choice of a more user friendly biometric based pairing method and a more robust alternative method, 73% of our subjects would like to have both

La nouvelle se répand comme une traînée de poudre, et la fièvre monte dans la ville : tout le monde veut voir ces deux boxeurs noirs venus des États-Unis pour un combat inédit en

With regard to restrictions applied in accordance with paragraph 2 (c) or paragraph 4 (d) of this Article, the selection of a representative period for any goods and the

Organizational coupling Coupling; Organizational performance; Innovation performance; Network innovation; Collaborative innovation; 49 Strategic alliances related

Only in three cases is the relationship between libertarian attitudes and voting for pirate parties stronger than the relationship between political trust and voting for them: in