• No results found

University of Groningen Idioms in the Aging Brain la Roi, Amélie

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "University of Groningen Idioms in the Aging Brain la Roi, Amélie"

Copied!
29
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

University of Groningen

Idioms in the Aging Brain

la Roi, Amélie

DOI:

10.33612/diss.168090845

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date: 2021

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

la Roi, A. (2021). Idioms in the Aging Brain. University of Groningen. https://doi.org/10.33612/diss.168090845

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

187

R

Re fe re nc es

References

Akaike, H. (1974). A New Look at the Statistical Model Identification. IEEE

Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716–723.

https://doi.org/10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705

Albert, M. S., Heller, H. S., & Milberg, W. (1988). Changes in naming ability with age.

Psychology and Aging, 3(2), 173–178.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.3.2.173

Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(4), 419–439.

Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247–264. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00059-1

Arnon, I., & Cohen Priva, U. (2013). More than Words: The Effect of Multi-word Frequency and Constituency on Phonetic Duration. Language and Speech,

56(3), 349–371. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830913484891

Arnon, I., & Priva, U. C. (2015). Time and again: The changing effect of word and multiword frequency on phonetic duration for highly frequent sequences. The

Mental Lexicon, 9(3), 377–400. https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.9.3.01arn

Arrington, C. N., Kulesz, P. A., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., & Barnes, M. A. (2014). The contribution of attentional control and working memory to reading comprehension and decoding. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(5), 325–346. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.902461

Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311

Auriacombe, S., Lechevallier, N., Amieva, H., Harston, S., Raoux, N., & Dartigues, J. F. (2006). A longitudinal study of quantitative and qualitative features of category verbal fluency in incident Alzheimer’s disease subjects: Results from the PAQUID study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21(4), 260–266. https://doi.org/10.1159/000091407

(3)

188

Bäckman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A. K., Laukka, E. J., & Small, B. J. (2005). Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis.

Neuropsychology, 19(4), 520–531.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.520

Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford University Press.

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of

Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189–208.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4

Baddeley, A., Dela Sala, S., Gray, C., Papagno, C., & Spinnler, H. (1997). Testing central executive functioning with a pencil-and-paper test. In P. Rabbitt (Ed.),

Methodology of Frontal and Executive Function (pp. 61–80). Psychology Press.

https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203344187-7

Baddeley, A., Logie, R., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Brereton, N. (1985). Components of Fluent Reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 24(1), 119–131.

Baggio, G., & Hagoort, P. (2011). The balance between memory and unification in semantics: A dynamic account of the N400. Language and Cognitive Processes,

26(9), 1338–1367. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.542671

Ball, S. S., Marsh, J. T., Schubarth, G., Brown, W. S., & Strandburg, R. (1989). Longitudinal P300 latency changes in Alzheimer’s disease. Journals of

Gerontology, 44(6), 195–200. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/44.6.M195

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(5), 857–894. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000174

Beck, S. D., & Weber, A. (2016). Bilingual and monolingual idiom processing is cut from the same cloth: The role of the L1 in literal and figurative meaning activation. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–16.

https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01350

Beck, S. D., & Weber, A. (2020). Context and Literality in Idiom Processing: Evidence from Self-Paced Reading. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-020-09719-2

(4)

189

R

Re fe re nc es

Benton, A. L. (1968). Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease.

Neuropsychologia, 6(1), 53–60.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(68)90038-9

Bobrow, S. A., & Bell, S. M. (1973). On catching on to idiomatic expressions. Memory

& Cognition, 1(3), 343–346.

Boehm, U., Van Maanen, L., Forstmann, B., & Van Rijn, H. (2014). Trial-by-trial fluctuations in CNV amplitude reflect anticipatory adjustment of response caution. NeuroImage, 96, 95–105.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.063

Boll-Avetisyan, N., Nixon, J. S., Lentz, T. O., Liu, L., Ommen, S. Van, Çöltekin, Ç., & van Rij, J. (2018). Neural response development during distributional learning.

Interspeech, September, 1432–1436.

Bopp, K. L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social

Sciences, 60(5), 223–233. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/60.5.P223

Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Keys, B. A., Carter, C. S., Cohen, J. D., Kaye, J. A., Janowsky, J. S., Taylor, S. F., Yesavage, J. A., Mumenthaler, M. S., Jagust, W. J., & Reed, B. R. (2001). Context processing in older adults: Evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: General, 130(4), 746–763.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.746

Brouwer, H., Crocker, M. W., Venhuizen, N. J., & Hoeks, J. C. J. (2017). A Neurocomputational Model of the N400 and the P600 in Language Processing.

Cognitive Science, 41, 1318–1352. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12461

Brouwer, H., & Hoeks, J. C. J. (2013). A time and place for language comprehension: Mapping the N400 and the P600 to a minimal cortical network. Frontiers in

Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00758

Brown, C., & Hagoort, P. (1993). The Processing Nature o f the N400 : Evidence from Masked Priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 34–44. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.34

Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2016). How many words do we know? Practical estimates of vocabulary size dependent on word definition, the degree of language input and the participant’s age. Frontiers in

(5)

190

Burke, D. M., & Mackay, D. G. (1997). Memory, language, and ageing. Philosophical

Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 352(1363), 1845–1856.

Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., & Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal

of Memory and Language, 30(5), 542–579.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90026-G

Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A. (2008). Language and Aging. In F. I. M. Craik (Ed.),

Handbook of Aging and Cognition (3rd ed., pp. 373–444). Psychology press.

Burkhardt, P. (2006). Inferential bridging relations reveal distinct neural mechanisms: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain and

Language, 98(2), 159–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.005

Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (2002). A practical information-theoretic approach.

Model selection and multimodel inference. (2nd ed.). Springer.

Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D. C. (2005). Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking

cognitive and cerebral aging. Oxford University Press.

Cacciari, C., & Corradini, P. (2015). Literal analysis and idiom retrieval in ambiguous idioms processing: A reading-time study. Journal of Cognitive Psychology,

27(7), 797–811. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2015.1049178

Cacciari, C., Corradini, P., & Ferlazzo, F. (2018). Cognitive and personality components underlying spoken idiom comprehension in context. An exploratory study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 659. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00659

Cacciari, C., & Tabossi, P. (1988). The Comprehension of Idioms. Journal of Memory

and Language, 27(6), 668–683.

Canal, P., Pesciarelli, F., Vespignani, F., Molinaro, N., & Cacciari, C. (2017). Basic composition and enriched integration in idiom processing: an EEG study.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 43(6),

928–943.

Canal, P., Pesciarelli, F., Vespignani, F., Molinaro, N., & Cacciari, C. (2015). Electrophysiological correlates of idiom comprehension: Semantic composition does not follow lexical retrieval. In V. Pirrelli, C. Marazi, & M. Ferro (Eds.), Word structure and word usage. Proceedings of the NetWordS final

(6)

191

R

Re fe re nc es

Carrol, G., & Conklin, K. (2020). Is All Formulaic Language Created Equal? Unpacking the Processing Advantage for Different Types of Formulaic Sequences.

Language and Speech, 63(1), 95–122.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830918823230

Carrol, G., & Littlemore, J. (2020). Resolving Figurative Expressions During Reading: The Role of Familiarity, Transparency, and Context. Discourse Processes, 57(7), 609–626. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2020.1729041

Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech,

Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 1142–1157.

https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/093)

Chwilla, D. J., & Kolk, H. H. J. (2002). Three-step priming in lexical decision. Memory

and Cognition, 30(2), 217–225. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195282

Clark, L. J., Gatz, M., Zheng, L., McCleary, C., MacK, W. J., & Chen, Y. L. (2009). Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent alzheimer?s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other

Dementias, 24(6), 461–468. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317509345154

Columbus, G., Sheikh, N. A., Côté-Lecaldare, M., Häuser, K., Baum, S. R., & Titone, D. A. (2015). Individual differences in executive control relate to metaphor processing: An eye movement study of sentence reading. Frontiers in Human

Neuroscience, 8, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.01057

Conklin, K., & Schmitt, N. (2008). Formulaic sequences: Are they processed more quickly than nonformulaic language by native and nonnative speakers?

Applied Linguistics, 29(1), 72–89. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amm022

Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 769–786.

https://doi.org/10.1021/cen-v020n021.p1399

Cooper, R. M. (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84–107.

Craik, F. I. M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive deficits. In F. I. M. Craik & S. E. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes. (pp. 191–211). Plenum Press. Craik, F. I. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (2011). The handbook of aging and cognition.

(7)

192

Cutting, J. C., & Bock, K. (1997). That’s the way the cookie bounces: Syntactic and semantic components of experimentally elicited idiom blends. Memory and

Cognition, 25(1), 57–71. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197285

Dagerman, K. S., MacDonald, M. C., & Harm, M. W. (2006). Aging and the use of context in ambiguity resolution: Complex changes from simple slowing.

Cognitive Science, 30(2), 311–345.

https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_46

Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual Differences in Working Memory and Reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450–466. Dave, S., Brothers, T. A., Traxler, M. J., Ferreira, F., Henderson, J. M., & Swaab, T. Y.

(2018). Electrophysiological evidence for preserved primacy of lexical prediction in aging. Neuropsychologia, 117, 135–147.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.023

Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Qué PASA? the posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1201–1209. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm155

De Deyne, S., & Storms, G. (2008). Word associations: Norms for 1,424 Dutch words in a continuous task. Behavior Research Methods, 40(1), 198–205. https://doi.org/10.37S8BRM

DeDe, G., Kemtes, K., Caplan, D., & Waters, G. (2004). The relationship between age, verbal working memory, and language comprehension. Psychology and Aging,

19(4), 601–616. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.601

Delogu, F., Brouwer, H., & Crocker, M. W. (2019). Event-related potentials index lexical retrieval (N400) and integration (P600) during language comprehension. Brain and Cognition, 135, 103569.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.007

DeLong, K. A., Groppe, D. M., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2012). Thinking ahead or not? Natural aging and anticipation during reading. Brain and Language, 121(3), 226–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.02.006

Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal

of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21.

(8)

193

R

Re fe re nc es

Diachek, E., Blank, I., Siegelman, M., Affourtit, J., & Fedorenko, E. (2020). The domain-general multiple demand (MD) network does not support core aspects of language comprehension: A large-scale fMRI investigation. Journal of

Neuroscience, 40(23), 4536–4550.

https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2036-19.2020

Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working Memory , Short-Term Memory , and General Fluid Intelligence : A Latent-Variable Approach. Journal of Experiment Psychology: General, 128(3), 309– 331.

Eyigoz, E., Mathur, S., Cecchi, G., & Naylor, M. (2020). Linguistic markers predict onset of Alzheimer’s disease. EClinicalMedicine, 100583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100583

Fanari, R., Cacciari, C., & Tabossi, P. (2010). The role of idiom length and context in spoken idiom comprehension. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(3), 321–334. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902843866

Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A Rose by Any Other Name: Long-Term Memory Structure and Sentence Processing. Journal of Memory and Language,

41(4), 469–495. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2660

Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (2005). Aging in context: Age-related changes in context use during language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 42(2), 133– 141. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00274.x

Federmeier, K. D., Kutas, M., & Schul, R. (2010). Age-related and individual differences in the use of prediction during language comprehension. Brain and

Language, 115(3), 379–390. https://doi.org/10.2217/FON.09.6.Dendritic

Federmeier, K. D., McLennan, D. B., de Ochoa, E., & Kutas, M. (2002). The impact of semantic memory organization and sentence context information on spoken language processing by younger and older adults: An ERP study.

Psychophysiology, 39(2), 133–146.

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577202001373

Federmeier, K. D., Schwartz, T. J., Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (2003). Sounds, Words, Sentences: Age-Related Changes Across Levels of Language Processing.

Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 858–872.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.858

Fedorenko, E., Gibson, E., & Rohde, D. (2006). The nature of working memory capacity in sentence comprehension: Evidence against domain-specific working memory resources. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4), 541–553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.006

(9)

194

Fernández, G., Shalom, D. E., Kliegl, R., & Sigman, M. (2014). Eye movements during reading proverbs and regular sentences: The incoming word predictability effect. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(3), 260–273. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.760745

Fjell, A. M., & Walhovd, K. B. (2010). Structural brain changes in aging: Courses, causes and cognitive consequences. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 21(3), 187– 221. https://doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2010.21.3.187

Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The Relations Among Inhibition and Interference Control Functions: A Latent-Variable Analysis. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101–135.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101

Gadsby, N., Arnott, W. L., & Copland, D. A. (2008). An Investigation of Working Memory Influences on Lexical Ambiguity Resolution. Neuropsychology, 22(2), 209–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.2.209

Galinsky, A. D., & Glucksberg, S. (2000). Inhibition of the literal: Metaphors and idioms as judgmental primes. Social Cognition, 18(1), 35–54. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2000.18.1.35

Gaudreau, G., Monetta, L., Macoir, J., Laforce, R., Poulin, S., & Hudon, C. (2013). Verbal irony comprehension in older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 27(6), 702–712.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034655

Gernsbacher, M. A., & Faust, M. E. (1991). The Mechanism of Suppression: A Component of General Comprehension Skill. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(2), 245–262.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.17.2.245

Gernsbacher, M. A., & Robertson, R. R. W. (1999). The role of suppression in figurative language comprehension. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(12), 1619– 1630. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098300712437042.Improving

Giambra, L. M., Arenberg, D., Zonderman, A. B., Kawas, C., & Costa, P. T. (1995). Adult Life Span Changes in Immediate Visual Memory and Verbal Intelligence.

Psychology and Aging, 10(1), 123–139.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.10.1.123

Gibbs, R. W. (1980). Spilling the beans on understanding and memory for idioms in conversation. Memory & Cognition, 8(2), 149–156.

(10)

195

R

Re fe re nc es

Giora, R. (1997). Understanding Figurative and Literal Language: The Graded Salience Hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(3), 183–206.

https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1997.8.3.183

Giroud, N., Lemke, U., Reich, P., Bauer, J., Widmer, S., & Meyer, M. (2018). Are you surprised to hear this? Longitudinal spectral speech exposure in older compared to middle-aged normal hearing adults. European Journal of

Neuroscience, 47(1), 58–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13772

Gollan, T. H., Salmon, D. P., Montoya, R. I., & Galasko, D. R. (2011). Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia, 49(14), 3826–3830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.041

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55(4), 468–484. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9

Grieder, M., Crinelli, R. M., Koenig, T., Wahlund, L. O., Dierks, T., & Wirth, M. (2012). Electrophysiological and behavioral correlates of stable automatic semantic retrieval in aging. Neuropsychologia, 50(1), 160–171.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.014

Grindrod, C. M., & Raizen, A. L. (2019). Age-related changes in processing speed modulate context use during idiomatic ambiguity resolution. Aging,

Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 26(6), 842–864.

https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2018.1537437

Gunning-Dixon, F. M., & Raz, N. (2000). The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: A quantitative review. Neuropsychology, 14(2), 224–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.14.2.224

Gunter, T. C., Jackson, J. L., & Mulder, G. (1992). An Electrophysiological Study of Semantic Processing in Young and Middle-Aged Academics. Psychophysiology,

29(1), 38–54.

Gunter, T. C., Jackson, J. L., & Mulder, G. (1995). Language, memory, and aging: An electrophysiological exploration of the N400 during reading of memory-demanding sentences. Psychophysiology, 32(3), 215–229.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb02951.x

Gunter, T. C., Wagner, S., & Friederici, A. D. (2003). Working memory and lexical ambiguity resolution as revealed by ERPs: A difficult case for activation theories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(5), 643–657. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2003.15.5.643

(11)

196

Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (sps) as an erp measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes,

8(4), 439–483. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969308407585

Hannon, B., & Daneman, M. (2009). Age-Related Changes in Reading Comprehension: An Individual-Differences Perspective. Experimental Aging

Research, 35(4), 432–456. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730903175808

Hasher, L., Quig, M. B., & May, C. P. (1997). Inhibitory control over no-longer-relevant information: Adult age differences. Memory and Cognition, 25(3), 286–295. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and Effortful Processes in Memory.

Journal of Experiment Psychology: General, 108(3), 356–388.

Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). WM, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 22, 193–225.

Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (1990). Generalized Additive Models. Monographs on

Statistics and Applied Probability. Chapman and Hall.

Hatta, T., Hotta, C., Kato, K., Hatta, T., Hatta, J., Fujiwara, K., & Iwahara, A. (2020). Dissociation in Age-Related Developmental Trajectories Between Phonetic Fluency and Semantic Fluency Tests: Analysis of Longitudinal Data From the Yakumo Study. The American Journal of Psychology, 133(2), 197. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.133.2.0197

Hoenig, K., & Scheef, L. (2009). Neural correlates of semantic ambiguity processing during context verification. NeuroImage, 45(3), 1009–1019.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.044

Hoffman, P. (2018). An individual differences approach to semantic cognition: Divergent effects of age on representation, retrieval and selection. Scientific

Reports, 8(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-26569-0

Holsinger, E. (2013). Representing Idioms: Syntactic and Contextual Effects on Idiom Processing. Language and Speech, 56(3), 373–394.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830913484899

Holsinger, E., & Kaiser, E. (2013). Processing (non)compositional expressions: Mistakes and recovery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory

and Cognition, 39(3), 866–878. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030410

Hsu, N. S., & Novick, J. M. (2016). Dynamic Engagement of Cognitive Control Modulates Recovery From Misinterpretation During Real-Time Language Processing. Psychological Science, 27(4), 572–582.

(12)

197

R

Re fe re nc es

Huettig, F., & Janse, E. (2016). Individual differences in working memory and processing speed predict anticipatory spoken language processing in the visual world. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1).

https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1047459

Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., McDonald-Miszczak, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Short-term longitudinal change in cognitive performance in later life.

Psychology and Aging, 7(4), 571–584.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.7.4.571

Hung, P. F., & Nippold, M. A. (2014). Idiom understanding in adulthood: Examining age-related differences. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 28(3), 208–221. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.850117

Jackendoff, R. (1995). Idioms: Structural and Psychological Perspectives. In The

boundaries of the lexicon (pp. 133–166).

Jansen, W. J., Ossenkoppele, R., Knol, D. L., Tijms, B. M., Scheltens, P., Verhey, F. R. J., Visser, P. J., Aalten, P., Aarsland, D., Alcolea, D., Alexander, M., Almdahl, I. S., Arnold, S. E., Baldeiras, I., Barthel, H., Van Berckel, B. N. M., Bibeau, K., Blennow, K., Brooks, D. J., … Zetterberg, H. (2015). Prevalence of cerebral amyloid pathology in persons without dementia: A meta-analysis. JAMA - Journal of the

American Medical Association, 313(19), 1924–1938.

https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2015.4668

January, D., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. (2009). Co-localization of Stroop and Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution in Broca’s Area. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 21(12), 2434–2444. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21179

Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten twenty system of the international federation.

Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371–375.

Juncos-Rabadán, O., Facal, D., Rodríguez, M. S., & Pereiro, A. X. (2010). Lexical knowledge and lexical retrieval in ageing: Insights from a tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(10), 1301–1334. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690961003589484

Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta

Psychologica, 86(2–3), 199–225.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(94)90003-5

Karp, A., Kåreholt, I., Qiu, C., Bellander, T., Winblad, B., & Fratiglioni, L. (2004). Relation of Education and Occupation-based Socioeconomic Status to Incident Alzheimer’s Disease. American Journal of Epidemiology, 159(2), 175–183. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwh018

(13)

198

Kathleen Pichora-Fuller, M., Schneider, B. A., & Daneman, M. (1995). How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. Journal of the Acoustical

Society of America, 97(1), 593–608. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.412282

Kaufmann, L., Koppelstaetter, F., Delazer, M., Siedentopf, C., Rhomberg, P., Golaszewski, S., Felber, S., & Ischebeck, A. (2005). Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: An event-related fMRI study.

NeuroImage, 25(3), 888–898.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.041

Kemmer, L., Coulson, S., De Ochoa, E., & Kutas, M. (2004). Syntactic processing with aging: An event-related potential study. Psychophysiology, 41(3), 372–384. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00180.x

Kemper, S., Crow, A., & Kemtes, K. (2004). Eye-Fixation Patterns of High- and Low-Span Young and Older Adults: Down the Garden Path and Back Again.

Psychology and Aging, 19(1), 157–170.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.157

Kemper, S., Greiner, L. H., Marquis, J. G., Prenovost, K., & Mitzner, T. L. (2001). Language decline across the life span: Findings from the Nun study. Psychology

and Aging, 16(2), 227–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.227

Kemper, S., Thompson, M., & Marquis, J. (2001). Longitudinal Change in Language Production : Effects of Aging and Dementia on Grammatical Complexity and Prepositional Content. Psychology and Aging, 16(4), 600–614. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.16.4.600

Kessler, R., Weber, A., & Friedrich, C. K. (2020). Activation of Literal Word Meanings in Idioms: Evidence from Eye-tracking and ERP Experiments. Language and

Speech, 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830920943625

Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2010). SUBTLEX-NL: A new measure for Dutch word frequency based on film subtitles. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 643–650. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.3.643

Konopka, A. E., & Bock, K. (2009). Lexical or syntactic control of sentence formulation? Structural generalizations from idiom production. Cognitive

Psychology, 58(1), 68–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.05.002

Kuperberg, G. R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax. Brain Research, 1146(1), 23–49.

(14)

199

R

Re fe re nc es

Kuperberg, G. R., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D., & Holcomb, P. J. (2003). Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(1), 117–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00086-7

Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463–470. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02926323

Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty Years and Counting: Finding Meaning in the N400 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential (ERP). Annual

Review of Psychology, 62(1), 621–647.

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. In Science (Vol. 207, Issue 4427, pp. 203–205). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7350657

Kutas, M., & Iragui, V. (1998). The N400 in a semantic categorization task across 6 decades. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology - Evoked

Potentials, 108(5), 456–471.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-5597(98)00023-9

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models . Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13

la Roi, A., Sprenger, S. A., & Hendriks, P. (2020). Event-related potentials reveal increased dependency on linguistic context due to cognitive aging. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 46(7), 1226–1257.

https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000777

Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem : The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition , induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211–240.

Laurent, J. P., Denhières, G., Passerieux, C., Iakimova, G., & Hardy-Baylé, M. C. (2006). On understanding idiomatic language: The salience hypothesis assessed by ERPs. Brain Research, 1068(1), 151–160.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.076

Leckey, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2020). The P3b and P600(s): Positive contributions to language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 57(7), 1–15.

(15)

200

Lee, C.-L., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Differential age effects on lexical ambiguity resolution mechanisms. Psychophysiology, 48(7), 960–972.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01158.x

Lee, C.-L., & Federmeier, K. D. (2012). Ambiguity’s aftermath: How age differences in resolving lexical ambiguity affect subsequent comprehension.

Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 869–879.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.027

Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental

Psychology, 21(1), 59–80. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151003321164627

Lenth, R. V. (2016). Least-squares means: The R package lsmeans. Journal of

Statistical Software, 69, 1–33.

Lenth, R. V. (2020). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means (1.4.5). https://cran.r-project.org/package=emmeans

Levorato, M. C., & Cacciari, C. (1992). Children’s comprehension and production of idioms : the role of context and familiarity. Journal of Child Language, 19(2), 415–433. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011478

Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A. (2008). The multidetermined nature of idiom processing. Memory and Cognition, 36(6), 1103–1121.

https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.6.1103

Light, L. L., & Capps, J. L. (1986). Comprehension of Pronouns in Young and Older Adults. Developmental Psychology, 22(4), 580–585.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.580

Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. MIT Press. Malyutina, S., Laurinavichyute, A., Terekhina, M., & Lapin, Y. (2018). No evidence for strategic nature of age-related slowing in sentence processing. Psychology and

Aging, 33(7), 1045–1059. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000302

Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2007). Lexical ambiguity in sentence comprehension.

Brain Research, 1146(1), 115–127.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.076

Matzen, L. E., & Benjamin, A. S. (2013). Older and wiser: Older adults’ episodic word memory benefits from sentence study contexts. Psychology and Aging, 28(3), 754–767. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032945

(16)

201

R

Re fe re nc es

McCoy, S. L., Tun, P. A., Cox, L. C., Colangelo, M., Stewart, R. A., & Wingfield, A. (2005).

Hearing loss and perceptual effort : Downstream effects on older adults’ memory for speech. 58A(1), 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000151

Meulman, N., Wieling, M., Sprenger, S. A., Stowe, L. A., & Schmid, M. S. (2015). Age Effects in L2 Grammar processing as revealed by ERPs and How (Not) to Study Them. PLoS ONE, 10(12), 1–27.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143328

Meyer, A. M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2010). Event-related potentials reveal the effects of aging on meaning selection and revision. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 673–686. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00983.x

Missonnier, P., Deiber, M. P., Gold, G., Herrmann, F. R., Millet, P., Michon, A., Fazio-Costa, L., Ibañez, V., & Giannakopoulos, P. (2007). Working memory load-related electroencephalographic parameters can differentiate progressive from stable mild cognitive impairment. Neuroscience, 150(2), 346–356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.09.009

Missonnier, P., Gold, G., Fazio-Costa, L., Michel, J. P., Mulligan, R., Michon, A., Ibáñez, V., & Giannakopoulos, P. (2005). Early event-related potential changes during working memory activation predict rapid decline in mild cognitive impairment. Journals of Gerontology - Series A Biological Sciences and Medical

Sciences, 60(5), 660–666. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/60.5.660

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis.

Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Miyake, A., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1994). Working memory constraints on the resolution of lexical ambiguity: Maintaining multiple interpretations in neutral contexts. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 175–202.

Mohades, S. G., Struys, E., Van Schuerbeek, P., Baeken, C., Van De Craen, P., & Luypaert, R. (2014). Age of second language acquisition affects nonverbal conflict processing in children: An fMRI study. Brain and Behavior, 4(5), 626– 642. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.246

Mohan, R., & Weber, C. (2019). Neural activity reveals effects of aging on inhibitory processes during word retrieval. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 26(5), 660–687. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2018.1519105

(17)

202

Molfese, D. L., Molfese, V. J., Key, S., Modglin, A., Kelley, S., & Terrell, S. (2002). Reading and cognitive abilities: Longitudinal studies of brain and behavior changes in young children. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 99–119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-002-0008-7

Molinaro, N., Canal, P., Vespignani, F., Pesciarelli, F., & Cacciari, C. (2013). Are complex function words processed as semantically empty strings? A reading time and ERP study of collocational complex prepositions. Language and

Cognitive Processes, 28(6), 762–788.

https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.665465

Molinaro, N., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Electrophysiological evidence of interaction between contextual expectation and semantic integration during the processing of collocations. Biological Psychology, 83(3), 176–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.12.006

Molinaro, N., Conrad, M., Barber, H. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). On the functional nature of the N400: Contrasting effects related to visual word recognition and contextual semantic integration. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 1–7.

Morrone, I., Declercq, C., Novella, J. L., & Besche, C. (2010). Aging and inhibition processes: The case of metaphor treatment. Psychology and Aging, 25(3), 697– 701. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019578

Murphy, D. R., Craik, F. I. M., Li, K. Z. H., & Schneider, B. A. (2000). Comparing the Effects of Aging and Background Noise on Short-Term Memory Performance.

Psychology and Aging, 15(2), 323–334.

https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.15.2.323

Ness, T., & Meltzer-Asscher, A. (2018). Predictive Preupdating and Working Memory Capacity: Evidence from Eventrelated Potentials. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 30(12), 1916–1938. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn

Newcombe, F. (1969). Missile wounds of the brain: A study of psychological deficits. Oxford University Press.

Nieuwland, M. S., & Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2006). Individual differences and contextual bias in pronoun resolution: Evidence from ERPs. Brain Research, 1118(1), 155–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.022

Nixon, J. S., van Rij, J., Mok, P., Baayen, R. H., & Chen, Y. (2016). The temporal dynamics of perceptual uncertainty: Eye movement evidence from Cantonese segment and tone perception. Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 103–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.005

(18)

203

R

Re fe re nc es

Nordmann, E., & Jambazova, A. A. (2017). Normative data for idiomatic expressions.

Behavior Research Methods, 49(1), 198–215.

https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0705-5

Novick, J. M., Hussey, E., Teubner-Rhodes, S., Harbison, J. I., & Bunting, M. F. (2014). Clearing the garden-path: Improving sentence processing through cognitive control training. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(2), 186–217. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.758297

Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(5), 292–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005

Nyberg, L., Rönnlund, M., Dixon, R. A., Maitland, S. B., Bäckman, L., Wahlin, Å., & Nilsson, L. G. (2003). Selective adult age differences in an age-invariant multifactor model of declarative memory. Psychology and Aging, 18(1), 149– 160. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.1.149

Olichney, J. M., Taylor, J. R., Gatherwright, J., Salmon, D. P., Bressler, A. J., Kutas, M., & Iragui-Madoz, V. J. (2008). Patients with MCI and N400 or P600 abnormalities are at very high risk for conversion to dementia. Neurology, 70(19 Part 2), 1763–1770. https://doi.org/10.1038/jid.2014.371

Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brain Potentials Elicited by Garden-Path Sentences: Evidence of the Application of Verb Information During Parsing These two parsing models make distinct claims concerning.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4),

786–803.

Palladino, P., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Pazzaglia, F. (2001). Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 29(2), 344–354. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194929

Papagno, C., Lucchelli, F., Muggia, S., & Rizzo, S. (2003). Idiom comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease: The role of the central executive. Brain, 126(11), 2419– 2430. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg243

Papagno, C., Tabossi, P., Colombo, M. R., & Zampetti, P. (2004). Idiom comprehension in aphasic patients. Brain and Language, 89(1), 226–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00398-5

Papaliagkas, V. T., Kimiskidis, V. K., Tsolaki, M. N., & Anogianakis, G. (2011). Cognitive event-related potentials: Longitudinal changes in mild cognitive impairment.

Clinical Neurophysiology, 122(7), 1322–1326.

(19)

204

Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span.

Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299–320.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299

Paulmann, S., Ghareeb-Ali, Z., & Felser, C. (2015). Neurophysiological markers of phrasal verb processing: Evidence from L1 and L2 speakers. In R. R. Heredia & A. B. Cieslicka (Eds.), Bilingual Figurative Language Processing (pp. 245–267).

Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139342100.013

Pawley, A., & Syder, F. H. (1983). Two puzzles for linguistic theory: Nativelike selection and nativelike fluency. In J. C. Richards & R. W. Schmidt (Eds.),

Language and Communication (pp. 191–225). Longman.

Payne, B. R., & Federmeier, K. D. (2018). Contextual constraints on lexico-semantic processing in aging: Evidence from single-word event-related brain potentials.

Brain Research, 1687, 117-128.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2018.02.021

Payne, B. R., & Silcox, J. W. (2019). Chapter Seven - Aging, context processing, and comprehension. In K. D. B. T.-P. of L. and M. Federmeier (Ed.), The Psychology

of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 71, pp. 215–264). Academic Press.

https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2019.07.001

Peterson, R. R., Dell, G. S., Burgess, C., & Eberhard, K. M. (2001). Dissociation between Syntactic and Semantic Processing during Idiom Comprehension. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 27(5), 1223–1237.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.27.5.1223

Porretta, V., Kyröläinen, A. J., van Rij, J., & Järvikivi, J. (2017). Visual world paradigm data: From preprocessing to nonlinear time-course analysis. International

Conference on Intelligent Decision Technologies, 268–277.

Proverbio, A. M., Crotti, N., Zani, A., & Adorni, R. (2009). The role of left and right hemispheres in the comprehension of idiomatic language: An electrical neuroimaging study. BMC Neuroscience, 10(1), 116.

https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-10-116

R Core Team. (2018). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.r-project.org

Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P., Shaoul, C., Milin, P., & Baayen, R. H. (2014). The myth of cognitive decline: Non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning. Topics in Cognitive

(20)

205

R

Re fe re nc es

Ramscar, M., Sun, C. C., Hendrix, P., & Baayen, R. H. (2017). The Mismeasurement of Mind: Life-Span Changes in Paired-Associate-Learning Scores Reflect the “Cost” of Learning, Not Cognitive Decline. Psychological Science, 28(8), 1171– 1179. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617706393

Rapp, A. M., & Wild, B. (2011). Nonliteral language in alzheimer dementia: A review.

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(2), 207–218.

https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710001682

Rassiga, C., Lucchelli, F., Crippa, F., & Papagno, C. (2009). Ambiguous idiom comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental

Neuropsychology, 31(4), 402–411.

https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390802220019

Rayner, K., Reichle, E. D., Stroud, M. J., Williams, C. C., & Pollatsek, A. (2006). The effect of word frequency, word predictability, and font difficulty on the eye movements of young and older readers. Psychology and Aging, 21(3), 448–465. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.448

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., Dahle, C., Gerstorf, D., & Acker, J. D. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral

Cortex, 15(11), 1676–1689. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi044

Regel, S., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Isnʼt It Ironic - An Electrophysiological Exploration of Figurative Language Processing. Journal of

Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(2), 277–293.

Regel, S., Meyer, L., & Gunter, T. C. (2014). Distinguishing neurocognitive processes reflected by P600 effects: Evidence from ERPs and neural oscillations. PLoS

ONE, 9(5), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096840

Risse, S., & Kliegl, R. (2011). Adult Age Differences in the Perceptual Span During Reading. Psychology and Aging, 26(2), 451–460.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021616

Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). “Oops!”: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured andnormal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758. https://doi.org/10.1080/01619565609536671

Rodrigue, K. M., & Raz, N. (2004). Shrinkage of the Entorhinal Cortex over Five Years Predicts Memory Performance in Healthy Adults. Journal of Neuroscience,

(21)

206

Rommers, J., Dijkstra, T., & Bastiaansen, M. (2013). Context-dependent semantic processing in the human brain: Evidence from idiom comprehension. Journal

of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(5), 762–776.

Rönnlund, M., Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L., & Nilsson, L. G. (2005). Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychology and Aging,

20(1), 3–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.3

Salisbury, D. F. (2004). Semantic memory and verbal working memory correlates of N400 to subordinate homographs. Brain and Cognition, 55(2), 396–399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.057.Semantic

Salthouse, T. A. (1994). The Nature of the Influence of Speed on Adult Age Differences in Cognition. Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 240–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.6.1486

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The Processing-Speed Theory of Adult Age Differences in Cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 403–428.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403

Salthouse, T. A. (2000). Aging and measures of processing speed. Biological

Psychology, 54(1–3), 35–54.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00052-1

Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin?

Neurobiology of Aging, 30(4), 507–514.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023

Salthouse, T. A., Kausler, D., & Saults, J. S. (1988). Investigation of student status, background variables, and feasibility of standard tasks in cognitive aging research. Psychology and Aging, 3(1), 29–37. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.3.1.29

Schaie, K. W. (1983). The Seattle Longitudinal Study: A 21-year exploration of psychometric intelligence in adulthood. Longitudinal Studies of Adult

Psychological Development, 64(135), 484–493.

Schaie, K. W. (2013). Longitudinal studies. In Developmental Influences on Adult

Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.

https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof

Shain, C., Blank, I. A., van Schijndel, M., Schuler, W., & Fedorenko, E. (2020). fMRI reveals language-specific predictive coding during naturalistic sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 138(March 2019), 107307.

(22)

207

R

Re fe re nc es

Shao, Z., Janse, E., Visser, K., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Frontiers

in Psychology, 5, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772

Sheldon, S., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., & Schneider, B. A. (2008). Priming and sentence context support listening to noise-vocoded speech by younger and older adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(1), 489–499. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2783762

Shimamura, A. P., Jurica, P. J., Mangels, J. A., Gershberg, F. B., & Knight, R. T. (1995). Susceptibility to Memory Interference Effects following Frontal Lobe Damage: Findings from Tests of Paired-Associate Learning. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 7(2), 144–152.

Singh-Manoux, A., Kivimaki, M., Glymour, M. M., Elbaz, A., Berr, C., Ebmeier, K. P., Ferrie, J. E., & Dugravot, A. (2012). Timing of onset of cognitive decline: Results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ (Online), 344(7840), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d7622

Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Conklin, K., Caffarra, S., Kaan, E., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2017). Representation and processing of multi-word expressions in the brain.

Brain and Language, 175, 111–122.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2017.10.004

Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Conklin, K., & Schmitt, N. (2011). Adding more fuel to the fire: An eye-tracking study of idiom processing by native and non-native speakers. Second Language Research, 27(2), 251–272.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658310382068

Sliwinski, M., & Buschke, H. (1999). Cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships among age, cognition, and processing speed. Psychology and Aging, 14(1), 18– 33. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.14.1.18

Snider, N., & Arnon, I. (2012). A unified lexicon and grammar ? Compositional and non-compositional phrases in the lexicon. In Frequency effects in language (pp. 127–163).

Spaan, P. E. J. (2012). Veroudering en de ziekte van Alzheimer. Tijdschrift Voor

Neuropsychologie, 7(1), 3–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03082329

Speranza, F., Daneman, M., & Schneider, B. A. (2000). How aging affects the reading of words in noisy backgrounds. Psychology and Aging, 15(2), 253–258. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.15.2.253

(23)

208

Spotorno, N., Cheylus, A., Van Der Henst, J. B., & Noveck, I. A. (2013). What’s behind a P600? Integration Operations during Irony Processing. PLoS ONE, 8(6), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066839

Sprenger, S. A., la Roi, A., & van Rij, J. (2019). The development of idiom knowledge across the lifespan. Frontiers in Communication, 4, 29.

https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2019.00029

Sprenger, S. A., Levelt, W. J. M., & Kempen, G. (2006). Lexical access during the production of idiomatic phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(2), 161– 184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.11.001

St Clair, D., Blackburn, I., Blackwood, D., & Tyrer, G. (1988). Measuring the course of Alzheimer’s disease. A longitudinal study of neuropsychological function and changes in P3 event-related potential. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152(1), 48– 54. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.152.1.48

Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., & Wingfield, A. (1994). Older adults can inhibit high-probability competitors in speech recognition. Aging, Neuropsychology, and

Cognition, 1(2), 152–157.

https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09289919408251456

Stites, M. C., Federmeier, K. D., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2013). Cross-age comparisons reveal multiple strategies for lexical ambiguity resolution during natural reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and

Cognition, 39(6), 1823–1841. https://doi.org/10.1038/jid.2014.371

Strandburg, R. J., Marsh, J. T., Brown, W. S., Asarnow, R. F., Guthrie, D., & Higa, J. (1993). Event-related potentials in high-functioning adult autistics: Linguistic and nonlinguistic visual information processing tasks. Neuropsychologia,

31(5), 413–434. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(93)90058-8

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of

Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.

https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651

Swinney, D. A., & Cutler, A. (1979). The access and processing of idiomatic expressions. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(5), 523–534. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90284-6

Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., Sedivy, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of Visual and Linguistic Information in Spoken Language Comprehension.

(24)

209

R

Re fe re nc es

Thompson, J. J., Blair, M. R., & Henrey, A. J. (2014). Over the hill at 24: Persistent age-related cognitive-motor decline in reaction times in an ecologically valid video game task begins in early adulthood. PLoS ONE, 9(4), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094215

Thornton, R., & Light, L. L. (2006). Language production and comprehension in normal aging. In J. E. Birren & K. Warner Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the

Psychology of Aging (pp. 261–287). Elsevier.

https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012101264-9/50015-X

Titone, D. A., & Connine, C. M. (1994). Comprehension of Idiomatic Expressions: Effects of Predictability and Literality. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1126–1138.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1126

Titone, D. A., & Connine, C. M. (1999). On the compositional and noncompositional nature of idiomatic expressions. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(12), 1655–1674. Titone, D. A., Lovseth, K., Kasparian, K., & Tiv, M. (2019). Are Figurative

Interpretations of Idioms Directly Retrieved, Compositionally Built, or Both? Evidence from Eye Movement Measures of Reading. Canadian Journal of

Experimental Psychology. doi: 10.1037/cep0000175

Tombaugh, T. N., Kozak, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming.

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(2), 167–177.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00095-4

Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is Working Memory Capacity Task Dependent?

Journal of Memory and Language, 28(2), 127–154.

Underwood, G., Schmitt, N., & Galpin, A. (2004). The eyes have it: An eye-movement study into the processing of formulaic sequences. In N. Schmitt (Ed.),

Formulaic sequences (pp. 153–172). John Benjamins.

Unsworth, N., Fukuda, K., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2014). Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval.

Cognitive Psychology, 71, 1–26.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.01.003

Van Den Noort, M., Bosch, P., Haverkort, M., & Hugdahl, K. (2008). A standard computerized version of the reading span test in different languages. European

Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24(1), 35–42.

(25)

210

Van der Linden, M., Hupet, M., Feyereisen, P., Schelstraete, M.-A., Bestgen, Y., Bruyer, R., Lories, G., El Ahmadi, A., & Seron, X. (1999). Cognitive Mediators of Age-Related Differences in Language Comprehension and Verbal Memory Performance. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 6(1), 32–55. https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.6.1.32.791

Van Ginkel, W., & Dijkstra, T. (2019). The tug of war between an idiom’s figurative and literal meanings: Evidence from native and bilingual speakers.

Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23, 131–174. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918001219

Van Hooren, S. A. H., Valentijn, A. M., Bosma, H., Ponds, R. W. H. M., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., & Jolles, J. (2007). Cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64-81: A cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and education. Aging,

Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14(1), 40–54.

https://doi.org/10.1080/138255890969483

Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J. (2012). Prediction during language comprehension: Benefits, costs, and ERP components. International Journal of

Psychophysiology, 83(2), 176–190.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.015

Van Petten, C., Weckerly, J., McIsaac, H. K., & Kutas, M. (1997). Working memory capacity dissociates lexical and sentential context effects. Psychological

Science, 8(3), 238–242. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00418.x

van Rij, J., Hendriks, P., van Rijn, H., Baayen, R. H., & Wood, S. N. (2019). Analyzing the Time Course of Pupillometric Data. Trends in Hearing, 23, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/2331216519832483

van Rij, J., Wieling, M., Baayen, R. H., & van Rijn, H. (2017). itsadug: Interpreting Time

Series and Autocorrelated Data Using GAMMs. (R package version 2.3).

Van Zuijen, T. L., Plakas, A., Maassen, B. A. M., Been, P., Maurits, N. M., Krikhaar, E., van Driel, J., & van der Leij, A. (2012). Temporal auditory processing at 17 months of age is associated with preliterate language comprehension and later word reading fluency: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 528(1), 31–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.08.058

Veivo, O., Porretta, V., Hyönä, J., & Järvikivi, J. (2018). Spoken second language words activate native language orthographic information in late second language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(5), 1011–1032.

(26)

211

R

Re fe re nc es

Verhaeghen, P. (2003). Aging and vocabulary scores: A meta-analysis. Psychology

and Aging, 18(2), 332–339. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.332

Vespignani, F., Canal, P., Molinaro, N., Fonda, S., & Cacciari, C. (2010). Predictive mechanisms in idiom comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1682–1700. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21293

von der Malsburg, T., Kliegl, R., & Vasishth, S. (2015). Determinants of Scanpath Regularity in Reading. Cognitive Science, 39(7), 1675–1703. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12208

Vonk, J. M., Higby, E., Nikolaev, A., Cahana-Amitay, D., Spiro, A., Albert, M. L., & Obler, L. K. (2020). Demographic Effects on Longitudinal Semantic Processing, Working Memory, and Cognitive Speed. The Journal of Gerontology: Series B,

75(9), 1850-1862..

Vos, S. H., Gunter, T. C., Schriefers, H., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Syntactic parsing and working memory: The effects of syntactic complexity, reading span, and concurrent load. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(1), 65–103. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000085

Vulchanova, M., Milburn, E., Vulchanov, V., & Baggio, G. (2019). Boon or Burden? The Role of Compositional Meaning in Figurative Language Processing and Acquisition. Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 28(2), 359–387. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10849-019-09282-7

Walhovd, K. B., Fjell, A. M., Reinvang, I., Lundervold, A., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., & Dale, A. M. (2004). Size does matter in the long run: Hippocampal and cortical volume predict recall across weeks. Neurology, 63(7), 1193–1197. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.WNL.0000140489.33249.95

Waters, G., & Caplan, D. (2005). The relationship between age, processing speed, working memory capacity, and language comprehension. Memory, 13(3–4), 403–413. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210344000459

Wehbe, L., Blank, I. A., Shain, C., Futrell, R., Levy, R., von der Malsburg, T., Smith, N., Gibson, E., & Fedorenko, E. (2020). Incremental language comprehension

difficulty predicts activity in the language network but not the multiple demand network. 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.15.043844

Weinreich, U. (1969). Problems in the analysis of idioms. Substance and Structure of

Language, 23–81.

West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120(2), 272–292.

(27)

212

West, R. L. (2004). The Effects of Aging on Controlled Attention and Conflict Processing in the Stroop Task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(1), 103– 113. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892904322755593

Westbury, C., & Titone, D. A. (2011). Idiom Literality Judgments in Younger and Older Adults: Age-Related Effects in Resolving Semantic Interference.

Psychology and Aging, 26(2), 467–474. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022438

Wieling, M. (2018). Analyzing dynamic phonetic data using generalized additive mixed modeling: a tutorial focusing on articulatory differences between L1 and L2 speakers of English. Journal of Phonetics, 70, 86–116.

Wieling, M., Montemagni, S., Nerbonne, J., & Baayen, R. H. (2014). Lexical differences between Tuscan dialects and standard Italian: Accounting for geographic and sociodemographic variation using generalized additive mixed modeling.

Language, 90(3), 669–692. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2014.0064

Wieling, M., Nerbonne, J., & Baayen, R. H. (2011). Quantitative social dialectology: Explaining linguistic variation geographically and socially. PLoS ONE, 6(9). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023613

Wlotko, E. W., & Federmeier, K. D. (2012). Age-related changes in the impact of contextual strength on multiple aspects of sentence comprehension.

Psychophysiology, 49(6), 770–785.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01366.x

Wlotko, E. W., Lee, C.-L., & Federmeier, K. D. (2010). Language of the Aging Brain: Event-Related Potential Studies of Comprehension in Older Adults. Linguistics

and Language Compass, 4(8), 623–638.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00224.x

Wlotko, E. W., Lee, C.-L., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Comprehension in Older Adults.

Language and Linguistics Compass, 4(8), 623–638.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00224.x.Language

Wood, S. (2017). Generalized Additive Models: An Introduction With R. (2nd ed.). Chapman and Hall/CRC Press.

Woodward, S. H., Ford, J. M., & Hammett, S. C. (1993). N4 to spoken sentences in young and older subjects. Electroencephalography and Clinical

Neurophysiology, 87(5), 306–320.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(93)90184-W

World Health Organization. (2018a). Ageing and health [Fact sheet]. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ageing-and-health

(28)

213

R

Re fe re nc es

World Health Organization. (2018b). Global Health Estimates 2016: Deaths by Cause,

Age, Sex, by Country and by Region, 2000-2016.

Wulff, D., De Deyne, S., Jones, M. N., Mata, R., & The Aging Lexicon Consortium. (2019). New Perspectives on the Aging Lexicon. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,

23(8), 686–698.

Ye, Z., & Zhou, X. (2008). Involvement of cognitive control in sentence comprehension: Evidence from ERPs. Brain Research, 1203, 103–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.090

Yoon, C., May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (2000). Aging, circadian arousal patterns, and cognition. In D. C. Park & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 151–171). Psychology press.

Zempleni, M. Z., Renken, R., Hoeks, J. C. J., Hoogduin, J. M., & Stowe, L. A. (2007). Semantic ambiguity processing in sentence context: Evidence from event-related fMRI. NeuroImage, 34(3), 1270–1279.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.048

Zurrón, M., Lindín, M., Galdo-Alvarez, S., & Díaz, F. (2014). Age-related effects on event-related brain potentials in a congruence/incongruence judgment color-word Stroop task. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00128

(29)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Also, findings from the individual fixed effects model could mean that both acceptance of homosexuality and level of education are partially confounded by levels of cognitive

Subsequently, a second model was estimated where we added the effect of bullying victimization and parental rejection on the intercept and slope, the effect of sexual orientation

We studied the extent to which social integration within the adolescent peer context could explain mental health differences between heterosexual and sexual minority

In order to come to a better-informed evaluation of the empirical validity of the minority stress framework for explaining mental health differences between sexual minority

Deze vertraging in de verwerking van idiomen bij ouderen kon echter niet verklaard worden door de achteruitgang die we vonden in hun algemene cognitieve vaardigheden, zoals

SVO Berend viel voor Johanna's zwoele blik, want ze bracht hem het hoofd op hol/snel/bad met haar ogen. 80

Reference and cognition: Experimental and computational cognitive modeling studies on reference processing in Dutch and Italian.. One Model to Rule

Age-related decline in cognitive abilities does not directly entail age-related decline in language processing (Chapters 2, 3, and 5, this thesis)9. Language experience serves as