• No results found

The Impact of Relative Deprivation on Consumer Brand Preferences

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The Impact of Relative Deprivation on Consumer Brand Preferences"

Copied!
37
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

The Impact of Relative Deprivation on Consumer

Brand Preferences

Evangelia Pappa

(2)

Master Thesis MSc. Marketing Management

The Impact of Relative Deprivation on Consumer Brand

Preferences

By: Evangelia Pappa

S3790827

University of Groningen Faculty of Economics and Business

Department of Marketing

Completion date: 17/06/19 First supervisor: Dr. Sumaya Albalooshi

Second supervisor: M.T. van der Heide

Evangelia Pappa S3790827 Lijnbaanstraat 8

9711RV, Groningen, the Netherlands Ph: +30 6948640288

(3)

Abstract

This thesis is reviewing the impact of the feelings of relative deprivation due to financial dissatisfaction on brand preferences. Specifically, we focused our research on two brand types, namely, topdog brands and underdog brands. The literature framework considers the social comparison to be the mediator in this relationship. The central hypothesis is that consumers who are experiencing feelings of relative deprivation will sense the social comparison feeling and will have a higher preference for topdog brands as compared to underdog brands. The results showed that the impact of relative deprivation on the brand type (topdog vs. underdog) is not significant. Social comparison, the hypothesized mediator for this relationship was tested and found to be insignificant. Additionally, this paper considers possible explanations and limitations for these results and concludes with suggestions for future research. Therefore, this research contributes by providing more knowledge regarding the influence of relative deprivation on consumer behavior.

Keywords: relative deprivation, financial dissatisfaction, social comparison, topdog

(4)

Acknowledges

This thesis constitutes the final step of my Master program in Marketing Management at the University of Groningen. By taking this step, an enchanting year of studies will conclude as well. Hence, I want to take the opportunity to thank my thesis supervisor Dr. Sumaya Albalooshi, for her patience, her invaluable feedback, and her full support from the beginning of this procedure until the very end. Also, I want to thank my second supervisor, M.T. van der Heide, for taking the time to evaluate and grade my thesis. After, I would like to thank my family, Lefteris, Katerina, Barbara, and my friends, Olga and Christoforos, for constant support. Special thanks to my father Alexandros who is always on my side supporting me in every single decision that I am taking, and my brother Dimitris who is always there to remind who I am and what I am capable of. Likewise, I would like to thank the real survivor of this master, my boyfriend, John, who is always there to support me and believe in me.

Last but not least, I want to thank all the fantastic friends that I made this year, my personal “Marketing Team” who managed to make this year so fascinating for me. Notably, a big thank you to my library roommate, my friend Alberto, for his continuous help and support. It has been a very challenging and demanding year, but all of you have transformed the impossible to possible. Thank you.

(5)

TABLE OF CONTENTS

INTRODUCTION ... 6

THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK ... 8

Personal Relative Deprivation ... 8

Social Comparison ... 11

Topdog brands (vs. underdog brands) ... 13

Conceptual model ... 14

METHODOLOGY & DESIGN ... 15

Manipulation of Personal Relative Deprivation ... 16

Manipulation of Topdog brands vs. Underdog brands ... 17

Measurement of Social Comparison ... 18

RESULTS ... 19

Main Analysis ... 19

Moderated Mediation Analysis ... 21

DISCUSSION ... 23

Limitations and future research ... 24

(6)

INTRODUCTION

“A house may be large or small; as long as the neighboring houses are likewise small, it satisfies all social requirement for a residence. But let there arise next to the little house a palace, and the little house shrinks to a hut. The little house now makes it clear that its inmate has no social position at all to maintain, or but a very insignificant one; and however high it may shoot up in the course of civilization, if the neighboring palace rises in equal or even in greater measure, the occupant of the relatively little house will always find himself more uncomfortable, more dissatisfied, more cramped within his four walls”

Marx, Wage, Labour, and Capital (1942)

(7)
(8)

THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK

Personal Relative Deprivation

Personal relative deprivation (henceforth, PRD) demonstrates a condition of anger, dissatisfaction, and resentment that arises from the assumption that one is deprived of a desired and deserved outcome, compared to others who already possess it (Kim, Callan, Gheorghiu, William, and Matthews, 2017; Callan, Shead, and Olson, 2011; Bernstein and Crosby, 1980). In addition to this, Kim et al. (2017) have stated in their research that the feeling of relative deprivation results from a process where the individual engages in a comparative situation, where he or she perceives that he or she is unjustly underprivileged, among relevant others, and this leads to an angry resentment. Therefore, we can realize that the main point of view of relative deprivation is the comparisons that individuals produce in order to experience these unfavorable feelings.

(9)

theory, with the equity theory, and with the social comparison theory (Bernstein and Crosby, 1980).

One of the most notable expansions of the model of relative deprivation happened when Sweeney et al. (1990), use the relative deprivation theory to give a reason for satisfaction or dissatisfaction, among individuals, with income and pay level. In fact, in their research, they accomplished to find a direct link between income and satisfaction through relative deprivation. This link became even stronger when the individuals felt that they deserve more money than they already earned, or more money, compared to similar others (Sweeney, Mcfarlin, and Inderrieden, 1990). As a result, we can understand the significance of financial dissatisfaction on people's behavior and how this is directly associated with relative deprivation and can lead individuals to many outcomes. First of all, the dissatisfaction and entitlement that arise from PRD are directly connected, with the pay level and financial (dis)satisfaction (Sweeney, Mcfarlin, and Inderrieden, 1990). Furthermore, monetary dissatisfaction may be the result of risk-related attitudes, like the use of drugs, and other criminal outcomes, such as violent behavior (Baron, 2004). Besides that, when people feel relatively deprived due to financial constraints, an immediate need to change this disparity gap arises. For example, research has shown that people who want to improve their financial position and their degree of satisfaction engage in self-improvement actions, such as the request for better job opportunities (Olson, Roese, Meen, Robertso,1995; Zoogah, 2010). On the other hand, as Callan et al. (2011) had shown in their study, individuals who experience PRD due to monetary dissatisfaction are feeling a higher urge to gamble, as a way to gain instant rewards and narrow this disparity gap. So, individuals enter in delay discounting and prefer to acquire smaller rewards immediately, instead of waiting for more immense future rewards. This finding is very substantial, as it shows how relative deprivation due to financial constraints, results in the vulnerability of individuals.

(10)

and ambiguity. Hence, they value more extrinsic goals (vs. intrinsic), such as money, status, and appearance, as a way to reduce the feeling of relative deprivation. Besides this, Tully et al. (2015) have shown that people who feel financially deprived, purchase more material goods than experiences. In addition to this, former studies have shown that individuals with materialistic tendencies value more items that own a public meaning and consumed in public settings (Kamineni, 2005), build substantial connections with brands (Rindfleisch, Burroughs, and Wong, 2008) and that materialistic individuals identify themselves with powerful and successful brands, since they pay more attention to status (McGinnis, Gao, Jun, and Gentry, 2017). In fact, McGinnis et al. (2017) attempted to identify consumers’ motives for underdog brand preferences, and they found materialism to have a negative impact on underdog brands, with a possible explanation of the fact that people with materialistic values have an affection towards winners and not unprivileged brands. Besides that, individuals under the self-threat consume status-related products to restore their psychological condition (Sivanathan and Pettit, 2010). In the current study, we propose, that individuals, who will experience the feeling of relative deprivation, will also experience the social comparison feeling, higher materialistic tendencies, and, all of the negative emotions that we mentioned above. Consequently, they will show a higher preference for topdog brands -market leader brands- in order to mitigate these feelings and restore their adverse psychological condition.

(11)

Social Comparison

“Comparison is the thief of joy.” Theodore Roosevelt.

(12)

they basically compare themselves with similar or better-off relevant others and that upward comparisons happen faster (Wood, 1989; Ordabayeva and Pierre Chandon, 2010).

Social comparison leads to a plethora of consequences. For example, social comparison with a successful referent can lead individuals to feel more jealousy (Salovey and Rodin, 1984), or more envy (Smith, 2004). Furthermore, social comparison can lead to miserable psychological conditions. Ellaway et al. (2004), have associated social comparison with higher anxiety, higher depression, and lower-self-esteem. Additionally, research has shown that social comparison leads individuals to be less empathic towards others (Yip and Kelly, 2013). However, this is not always the case. Research has also identified positive aspects of social comparisons, such as a hope for better self-enhance (Collins, 1996).

(13)

Topdog brands (vs. underdog brands)

Brands are widely accepted as one of the most valuable intangible assets of a company’s long-term success. From a consumer’s perspective, research has shown that people build meaningful relationships with their brands (Fournier, 1998) and that brands offer the opportunity to the consumer to show who she or he is, or who she or he would like to be (Park and John, 2010). From this perspective, consumers use the brands to “construct or sustain their self-concept” (Freling and Forbes, 2005). As such, people do not buy products only for their utilitarian motives but also for their symbolic purposes. Topdog brands are market leaders’ in their product category. They are companies with a plethora of resources that possess strong brands in the market. On the other hand, underdogs, lack the resources of the topdogs and are trying to acquire the position of topdogs in the market.

Underdog brands have gained much importance the recent years in the scientific domain, and there have been many studies that highlight their impact on consumer behavior. Specifically, there are a plethora of studies that argue that consumers prefer underdog brands, out of empathy or as a way of anti-consumption to keep the “weak player in the game” of the market (McGinnis and Gentry, 2009). Moreover, individuals who were primed with feelings of compassion demonstrate a higher preference for the underdog brand (Staton, Paharia, and Oveis, 2012). By contrast, topdog brands have not been investigated to the same extent. Research suggests that people often distance themselves from losers, and associate with winners, known as the tendency, to bask in reflected glory or BIRG (Cialdini, Borden, Thorne, Walker, Freeman, and Sloan, 1976; End, Dietz-Uhler, Harrick, and Jacquemotte, 2002), which can be linked with preferences for topdog brands.

(14)

a characteristic that is associated with underdog brands. As such, we argue that the social comparison theory will increase even more their preferences for market-leading brands, which provide status and safety - the topdog brands.

Hypothesis 2: Consumers experiencing high levels of personal relative deprivation (vs. low levels) will show a higher preference for topdog brands (vs. underdog brands), which is mediated by the social comparison experience (Mediation effect).

Conceptual model

Figure 1: The conceptual model

(15)

METHODOLOGY & DESIGN

(16)

Manipulation of Personal Relative Deprivation

(17)

Manipulation of Topdog brands vs. Underdog brands

(18)

Measurement of Social Comparison

(19)

RESULTS

Main Analysis

A 2 (Personal Relative Deprivation: low vs. high) x 2 (Brand Type: topdog vs. underdog), between-subjects factorial analysis of variance was performed to test our main hypothesis. The statistical analysis was conducted twice to check if there were differences using two dependent variables, namely Brand Attitude and Behavior Intention. First, we conducted the analysis for the dependent variable of Brand Attitude. Contrary to the predictions, the results for the main effects were not significant. Specifically, the main effect of PRD was not significant (F (3,194) = 1.380, p = .242, η2p = .007) and the same was found for the main effect of brand type (Topdog vs Underdog) (F (3,194) = .456, p = .500, η2p = .002). Moreover, the interaction effect of PRD and brand type failed to reach significance on the dependent variable of Brand Attitude (F (3,194) = .120, p = .729, η2p = .001).

Furthermore, the same condition was held for the main effects of PRD and brand type for the dependent variable of Behavior Intention. Specifically, the main effect of PRD was F (3,194) = .010, p = .919, η2p = .000 and the main effect of brand type was F (3,194) = .070, p = .791, η2p = .000. In addition to this, the interaction effect of PRD and brand type failed to reach significance (F (3,194) = .045, p = .833, η2p = .000). Thus, H1 was not supported.

In line with the previous results, the inspection of the means for the dependent variable of Brand Attitude showed no significant differences between the groups. In detail, the means of topdog brand type in low PRD condition (M = 5.43, SD = 1.50) are slightly lower for the high PRD condition (M = 5.56, SD = 1.04). A related situation was also held for the underdog brand type. Particularly, in low PRD condition M = 5.48, SD = 1.15, and, in high PRD condition M = 5.74, SD = .95. However, the total means of PRD indicate a slight difference for low levels of PRD (M = 5.46, SD = 1.30) and high levels of PRD (M = 5.65, SD = .99)

(20)

These results also indicate that our main hypothesis is not supported. Different levels of PRD did not influence the participants to brand attitude towards topdog or underdog brand types. The results can be visually seen in the graphs below.

Figure 2: Levels of Brand Attitude

(21)

Figure 3: Levels of Behavioral Intentions

Moderated Mediation Analysis

Since our first hypothesis is rejected, and there is no main effect of the relative deprivation on the topdog brand preferences, we suspected that our second hypothesis will be rejected. However, we conducted the moderated mediation analysis using Andrew Hayes’ PROCESS model 15 to confirm our assumption and to test if there are other non-predicted effects. First, we tested the hypothesis for the dependent variable of the Brand Attitude. The Personal Relative Deprivation was the independent variable in our model, the suggested mediator was Social Comparison, and the moderator was the Brand Type (topdog vs. underdog).

(22)

Moreover, the model summary of the Brand Attitude as the dependent variable was not significant (F (5,192) = .7844, p > 0.001, R2 = .0200). Also, the main effects of PRD, social comparison, and brand type were found to be not significant (β = .1969, t (196) = 1.1863, p = .2370, 95% CI [ -.1305, .5243]; β = .0922, t (196) = 1.4360, p = .1526, 95% CI [ -. 0344, .2187]; β = -.0822, t (196) = .4916, p = .6235, 95% CI [ -.4122, .2477]). Furthermore, the interaction effect of the PRD and the brand type was not significant, (β = -.1352, t (196) = .4071, p = .6844, 95% CI [ -.7904, .5199]), and the same condition was held for the interaction effect of Social Comparison on brand type (β = -.0039, t (196) = .0301, p = .9761, 95%, CI [-.2571, .2494]).

Lastly, we conducted a moderated mediation again for the dependent variable of the Behavior Intentions. The results indicated no significance. Specifically, the main effect of PRD on Behavior Intentions was β = .0247, t (196) = .1121, p = 9109, 95%, CI [ -.4107, 4602], and the main effect of brand type on Behavioral Intentions was, β = .1289, t (196) = .5795, p = 5630, 95%, CI [ -.3099, .5677]. However, the main effect of social comparison on Behavior Intention found to be significant with β = 2125, t (196) = 2.4901, p = .0136, 95%, CI [ .0442, .3809]. But the interaction effects of PRD on brand type, (β = .0480, t (196) = .1087, p = 9136, 95%, CI [ -.8234, .9194] and, of social comparison on brand type, failed to reached significance β = -.0647, t (196) = .3790, p = .7051, 95%, CI [ -.4015, .2721]).

(23)

DISCUSSION

The goal of this research was to investigate how participants that are possessed with relative deprivation feelings respond to different brand types, namely topdog and underdog brands. Topdog brands are market leader brands, which symbolize status and success. On the other hand, underdog brands are small brands that express passion, and consumers are feeling sympathy towards them. Regarding our study, we hypothesized that participants who are facing the feelings of personal relative deprivation would have a higher preference for topdog brands. Our assumption arose from the fact that the individuals who experience relative deprivation, tend to be more materialistic and as a result, favor positively status products. Moreover, since topdog brands are market leader brands that signal status, participants will have a superior inclination to them, as a way to reduce the conflicting emotions that were experiencing. In addition to this, we hypothesized that social comparison is the mechanism that motivates respondents to behave in this specific way since it is a fundamental mechanism for the feelings of relative deprivation.

Specifically, the hypotheses were as follows: H1: Consumers experiencing high levels of Personal Relative Deprivation (vs. low levels) will show a higher preference for topdog brands (vs. underdog brands) and H2: Consumers experiencing high levels of Personal Relative Deprivation (vs. low levels) will indicate a higher preference for topdog brands (vs. underdog brands), which is mediated by the social comparison experience.

(24)

Limitations and future research

Manipulation of Personal Relative Deprivation

(25)

Manipulation of the topdog vs. underdogs

(26)

Measurement of Social Comparison

After the manipulation of personal relative deprivation, either high or low, respondents were presented with the social comparison scale, in order to test our second hypothesis. It is widely accepted that people have a deep-rooted tendency to compare themselves with others. In addition to this, participants who feel deprived engage in social comparison, since it is a prerequisite for relative deprivation feelings (Kim, Callan, Gheorghiu, William, and Matthews, 2017). However, the results of our survey did not prove this relationship.

(27)

CONCLUSION

The current research investigated the impact of relative deprivation due to financial dissatisfaction on specific brand types, namely topdogs and underdogs. Our hypothesis stated that individuals who feel relative deprived would have a higher preference for topdog brands as a way to restore their feelings. In addition to this, we hypothesized that the underlying mechanism for this relationship is the social comparison feeling, which constitutes a prerequisite for relative deprivation feelings. As such, individuals who feel relative deprived compare themselves with better-off others, and their inclination towards topdog brands will be even higher, as a way to rebuild their status and surpass their negative feelings. To test these hypotheses, a survey was conducted. The participants were gathered through Amazon’s Mechanical Trunk, and they were United States residents.

(28)

REFERENCES

Baron, S. W. (2004). General Strain, Street Youth And Crime: A Test Of Agnews Revised Theory. Criminology,42(2), 457-484.

Bernstein, M. and Crosby, F. (1980). An empirical examination of relative deprivation theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16(5), pp.442-456. Callan, M. J., Shead, N. W., & Olson, J. M. (2011). Personal relative deprivation, delay discounting, and gambling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(5), 955–973.

Chipp, K., Kleyn, N., & Manzi, T. (2011). Catch Up and Keep Up: Relative Deprivation and Conspicuous Consumption in an Emerging Market. Journal of International Consumer Marketing,23(2), 117-134.

Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thorne, A., Walker, M. R., Freeman, S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Basking in reflected glory: Three (football) field studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(3), 366-375.

Collins, R. L. (1996). For better or worse: The impact of upward social comparison on self-evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 51–69.

Eibner, C., Sturm, R., & Gresenz, C. R. (2004). Does relative deprivation predict the need for mental health services? Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics, 7(4), 167–175.

Ellaway, A., McKay, L., Macintyre, S., Kearns, A., & Hiscock, R. (2004). Are social comparisons of homes and cars related to psychosocial health?. International Journal of Epidemiology, 33(5), 1065-1071.

(29)

Festinger, L. (1954). A Theory of Social Comparison Processes. Human Relations, 7(2), pp.117-140.

Fournier, S. (1998). Consumers and their brands: Developing relationship theory in consumer research. Journal of consumer research, 24(4), 343-373.

Freling, T. H., & Forbes, L. P. (2005). An examination of brand personality through methodological triangulation. Journal of brand management, 13(2), 148-162. Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison: Development of a scale of social comparison orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,76(1), 129-142.

Helgeson, V. S., & Mickelson, K. D. (1995). Motives for Social Comparison. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,21(11), 1200-1209. Hoch, S. J., & Deighton, J. (1989). Managing what consumers learn from experience. Journal of Marketing, 53(2), 1-20.

Kamineni, R. (2005). Influence of materialism, gender and nationality on consumer brand perceptions. Journal of Targeting, Measurement and Analysis for Marketing,14(1), 25-32.

Kim, H., Callan, M. J., Gheorghiu, A. I., & Matthews, W. J. (2017). Social comparison, personal relative deprivation, and materialism. British Journal of Social Psychology, 56(2), 373-392.

Levin, I. P., Schnittjer, S. K., & Thee, S. L. (1988). Information framing effects in social and personal decisions. Journal of experimental social psychology, 24(6), 520-529.

Luttmer, E. (2005). Neighbors as Negatives: Relative Earnings and Well-Being. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 120(3), pp.963-1002.

(30)

McGinnis, L. P., & Gentry, J. W. (2009). Underdog consumption: An exploration into meanings and motives. Journal of Business Research, 62(2), 191–199.

McGinnis, L. P., Gao, T., Jun, S., & Gentry, J. (2017). Motivational bases for consumers’ underdog affection in commerce. Journal of Service Management,28(3), 563-592.

Mishra, S. and Novakowski, D. (2016). Personal relative deprivation and risk: An examination of individual differences in personality, attitudes, and behavioral outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 90, pp.22-26

Moeini-Jazani, M., Albalooshi, S., & Muller Seljeseth, I. Self-Affirmation Reduces Delay Discounting of the Financially Deprived. Frontiers in Psychology (Forthcoming).

Olson, J. M., Herman, C. P., & Zanna, M. P. (1986). Relative deprivation and social comparison: The Ontario Symposium, volume 4. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.

Olson, J. M., Roesesc, N. J., Meen, J., & Robertson, D. J. (1995). The Preconditions and Consequences of Relative Deprivation: Two Field Studies 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25(11), 944-964.

Ordabayeva, N., & Chandon, P. (2010). Getting ahead of the Joneses: When equality increases conspicuous consumption among bottom-tier consumers. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 27-41.

Paharia, N., Keinan, A., Avery, J., & Schor, J. B. (2010). The underdog effect: The marketing of disadvantage and determination through brand biography. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(5), 775-790.

(31)

Pham-Kanter, G. (2009). Social comparisons and health: Can having richer friends and neighbors make you sick?. Social science & medicine, 69(3), 335-344.

Rindfleisch, A., Burroughs, J. E., & Wong, N. (2008). The safety of objects: Materialism, existential insecurity, and brand connection. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(1), 1-16.

Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1984). Some antecedents and consequences of social-comparison jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,47(4), 780-792.

Sivanathan, N., & Pettit, N. C. (2010). Protecting the self through consumption: Status goods as affirmational commodities. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,46(3), 564-570.

Smith, R. H. (2004). Envy and its transmutations. The social life of emotions, 43-63.

Staton, M., Paharia, N., & Oveis, C. (2012). Emotional marketing: How pride and compassion impact preferences for underdog and top dog brands. Advances in Consumer Research, 40, 1045–1046.

Sweeney, P. D., Mcfarlin, D. B., & Inderrieden, E. J. (1990). Research Notes: Using Relative Deprivation Theory to Explain Satisfaction With Income and Pay Level: A Multistudy Examination. Academy of Management Journal,33(2), 423-436. Tougas, F., & Beaton, A. M. (2008). Personal relative deprivation: A look at the grievous consequences of grievance. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(4), 1753-1766.

Tully, S. M., Hershfield, H. E., & Meyvis, T. (2015). Seeking lasting enjoyment with limited money: Financial constraints increase preference for material goods over experiences. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(1), 59-75.

(32)

Walker, L., & Mann, L. (1987). Unemployment, relative deprivation, and social protest. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13(2), 275-283.

Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin,90(2), 245-271

Wilson, S. R., & Benner, L. A. (1971). The effects of self-esteem and situation upon comparison choices during ability evaluation. Sociometry, 381-397.

Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin,106(2), 231-248.

Yip, J. J., & Kelly, A. E. (2013). Upward and downward social comparisons can decrease prosocial behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,43(3), 591-602. Zhang, H., Tian, Y., Lei, B., Yu, S. and Liu, M. (2015). Personal Relative Deprivation Boosts Materialism. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 37(5), pp.247-259.

(33)

APPENDICES

Appendix A

Insert Income for Low Personal Relative Deprivation Condition

(34)
(35)

Appendix B

Low Personal Relative Deprivation Condition

“Our online calculator compared your information and income with a large, representative database of individuals who have a similar profile as you do. Based on the information you provided, our calculator identified you as an individual who is financially adequate, relative to others; that is, someone who, relatively, has adequate and sufficient financial resources (i.e., money). We would like you to take a few minutes to reflect and write on how it feels to be in a relatively adequate financial position and to know that, on average, you have sufficient money to use at your will or when required in daily life, relative to those who are financially more constrained. Consider carefully and vividly how your life is with a relatively adequate amount of money and what the consequences of having sufficient money to live a stable life are.”

High Personal Relative Deprivation Condition

(36)

Appendix C

Underdog Brand Biography

Delight is a relatively small and new corn flakes brand. Even though they had smaller marketing and distribution budgets, the founders always believed that their dedication and passion would help them overcome the odds and bring their high-quality product to market. Though still relatively less know compared to powerful competitors, they are regarded by critics to be a high-quality corn flakes brand.

Topdog Brand Biography

Delight is a corn flakes brand that is well-resourced and has done well in the cereal industry. The founders have significant experience in the food industry and are known to maintain quality in every step of the production process. As a well-known and powerful brand, Delight is currently the market leader and has been able to maintain its leading position in the market.

Appendix D

(37)

Appendix E

Using a seven-point Likert scale participants indicate how much agree or disagree to the following items:

1) I often compare how my loved ones (boy or girlfriend, family members, etc.) are doing with how others are doing

2) I always pay a lot of attention to how I do things compared with how other do things 3) If I want to find out how well I have done something, I compare what I have done with how others have done

4) I often compare how I am doing socially (e.g. social skills, popularity) with other people

5) I am not the type of person who compares often with others (reversed)

6) I often compare myself with others with respect to what I have accomplished in life 7) I often like to talk with others about mutual opinions and experiences

8) I often try to find out what others think who face similar problems as I face 9) I always like to know what others in a similar situation would do

10) If I want to learn more about something, I try to find out what others think about it 11) I never consider my situation in life relative to that of other people (reversed)

Appendix F

- Based on your honest judgement, how attentive were you when completing this survey? Not attentive at all - Extremely attentive

- What is your favorite TV sport? Most people like to watch television programs. Recently, sports television programs have seen a major increase in ratings. Many sports start with the letter 'B'. However, we ask that from the list below you select a sport that does not start with the letter 'B' but starts with the letter 'H'. 1 = Baseball, 2 = Basketball, 3 = Soccer, 4 = Bowling, 5 = Hockey, 6 = Other

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

We found indications for the relative deprivation hypothesis: conditioned on changes in the income of adolescents ’ family, moving to a wealthier neighborhood was related to

This can explain the significant effect for store atmosphere and the lack of a direct effect for hedonic shopping value and brand heritage function on sharing content on

To summarize, even though in some cases CSR advertising backfired and instead of making good image and good reputation, it failed the company’s financial objectives

Een directe associatie tussen keramiek en oven zou enkel met enige zekerheid in de afvalkuilen te be- palen, zijn ware het niet dat hier slechts een drietal kleine

Mit Blick auf den Paternalismusbegriff ist für die hier im Fokus stehende Spannung zwischen Toleranz und Paternalismus deshalb entscheidend, dass sich die im

Therefore the domain bounds are restricted to positive values (using the environment variable discussed in Section 3.2), while making use of the updated constraint

In the COPE-active group as well as in the control group, none of the patient characteristics measured at baseline were correlated with change in daily physical activity over 7

van die jaar onderdruk bulle dit deur passiwiteit wat al. Hieronder sluit ek diegene in wat aan 'n enkele akademiese verenisina behoort of aan 'n spesifieke