• No results found

Dyslexia: A specific recoding deficit? : an analysis of response latencies for letters and words in dyslectics and in average readers

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Dyslexia: A specific recoding deficit? : an analysis of response latencies for letters and words in dyslectics and in average readers"

Copied!
15
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Dyslexia: A specific recoding deficit? : an analysis of response

latencies for letters and words in dyslectics and in average

readers

Citation for published version (APA):

Bouma, H., & Legein, C. P. (1980). Dyslexia: A specific recoding deficit? : an analysis of response latencies for

letters and words in dyslectics and in average readers. Neuropsychologia, 18(3), 285-298.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(80)90124-4

DOI:

10.1016/0028-3932(80)90124-4

Document status and date:

Published: 01/01/1980

Document Version:

Publisher’s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers)

Please check the document version of this publication:

• A submitted manuscript is the version of the article upon submission and before peer-review. There can be

important differences between the submitted version and the official published version of record. People

interested in the research are advised to contact the author for the final version of the publication, or visit the

DOI to the publisher's website.

• The final author version and the galley proof are versions of the publication after peer review.

• The final published version features the final layout of the paper including the volume, issue and page

numbers.

Link to publication

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal.

If the publication is distributed under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license above, please follow below link for the End User Agreement:

www.tue.nl/taverne Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us at: openaccess@tue.nl

providing details and we will investigate your claim.

(2)

Neuropsychologia, Vol. 18, pp. 285 to 298.

0 Pergamon Press Ltd. 1980. Printed in Great Britain.

oozs-3932/80/0601-0285$02.00/0

DYSLEXIA:

A SPECIFIC

RECODING

DEFICIT?

AN ANALYSIS

OF RESPONSE LATENCIES

FOR LETTERS

AND WORDS IN DYSLECTICS

AND IN AVERAGE

READERS

H. BOUMA and CH. P. LEGEIN

Institute for Perception Research IPO, P.O. Box 513, Eindhoven, The Netherlands 5612 AZ

(Received 10 October 1979)

Abstract-Twenty dyslectic and 20 normal readers (11-15 yr) recognized letters and words, both foveally and parafoveally. Correct scores and response latencies were measured. Latencies of correct responses come out longer for the dyslectic group: about 100 msec more for letters and 200 msec more for words, independent of presentation. A simple sequential processing scheme is proposed in which the main source of difficulties for the dyslectics is a delay in their translation of visually recognized items into a speech code. Lower scores then result from the volatile character of visual stores.

1. INTRODUCTION

POOR

readers and normal readers are primarily distinguished by their reading abilities. Many research efforts have tried to relate poor reading to poor performance in other tasks, in the hope of finding more basic differences which might have caused the reading problems.

In studies of poor readers, many deficiencies have been reported, both visual and non- visual, such as in the analysis of complex visual configurations [l-3], in the recognition of embedded letters [4], in eye movements [S, 61, in auditory discrimination and blending [7, 81, and in recall [9-121. Unfortunately, the results are not always in harmony with each other. With other deficiencies probably still around the corner, the fruits seem too many rather than too few and bewilderment rather than understanding seems to have advanced. Notions about a diffuse minimal brain damage or a general maturational lag seem to reflect such absence of coherent theory. The present paper is mainly concerned with specific developmental dyslexia, in which poor word recognition and poor spelling are prominent in the absence of intellectual and emotional handicaps.

This paper will add one more deficiency to the above list in providing evidence that dyslectic readers are slower than average readers in the recognition of letters and words. Contrary to the spirit of general and diffuse theories, in the discussion we shall explore the hypothesis that many diverse deficiencies can be taken to derive from one specific deficiency, i.e. the translation in the brain of visual symbols into speech and vice versa.

In our view, the very specificity of dyslexia, in which the reading of words is so obviously poor in children who otherwise appear quite normal, calls for a specific explanation.

In our Institute, research on normal reading processes started from the consideration that the saccadic nature of eye movements in reading restricts useful vision to the eye pauses. For our research, this has led us to the following division of visual reading processes : (a) the control of eye saccades [13-161; (b) letter and word recognition from each single eye pause,

(3)

286 H. BOUMA and CH. P. LECEIN

both foveally and parafoveally in the reading field [17-231; and (c) the integration of infor- mation as gathered from successive eye pauses [18-201.

As from 1973 we have extended this exploration of visual reading processes to the reading of dyslectic children. We compared the recognition scores of normal and dyslectic children (age group 11-15 yr) for isolated letters, embedded letters, and words, both foveally and parafoveally. We found that both groups of children had equally high scores on isolated letters, indicating that the dyslectic children knew the letter forms. However, on embedded letters and on words dyslectic children scored lower than control children, which suggested that in dyslectic children visual interference effects were more pronounced [4]. The present paper, which deals with response latencies in the recognition of briefly presented isolated letters, embedded letters, and words, both foveally and parafoveally, leads to a somewhat different view. For an initial report, we refer to BOUMA, LEGEIN and VAN RENS [24].

2. METHODS Subjects

Twenty weak readers, among whom two girls, had been selected earlier by the staff of a remedial teaching school as to obvious reading difficulties in the absence of intellectual and emotional handicaps. At the time of the experiment, their ages were between 1 I and 15 yr and their reading levels according to the Tanghe test [25] were I-4 yr below normal. An earlier opthalmological examination had revealed no eye defects.

The control group of 20 normal readers, among whom nine girls, of the same age (11-l 5 yr) was selected as to average reading level from grades 46 of a normal primary school. Ail 40 subjects had also participated in earlier experiments reported in this journal [4].

Stimuli

There were 23 different lower-case letters (all except x, y, q, which are very infrequent in Dutch) and 48 well known Dutch words. There were three types of stimuli: (1) isolated letters, presented one by one in random order, symbolically indicated as /a/; (2) letters embedded between two letters x at normal typewriter spacing, indicated as /xax/; (3) words, indicated as /wrd/, all of regular spelling and equally distributed over three word lengths (3-4-5 letters). For other details on stimuli, tachistoscope (100 msec) and presentation, we refer to our earlier paper [4].

Presentation

Eccentricity of presentation of the isolated and embedded test letters was at q = 0” (foveal) or at /q/ = 1” (parafoveal), either right (p > 0’) or left (p? < 0”) of fixation. One degree visual angle corresponds to four letter positions. Word stimuli were presented such that the word was centred at the fovea (v = 0”) or, for parafoveal stimuli, that the letter closest to the fovea was at /T/ = 1”. For each type of stimulus, parafoveal stimuli were presented randomly right or left of fixation in equal numbers. Each letter stimulus was presented once foveally, once left and once right of fixation. Parafoveal word stimuli were divided into two lists, each list containing each word just once. The words presented left of fixation on one list were presented right of fixation in the other list. In the experiments, the two parafoveal word lists were separated by letter lists and by a fovea1 word list. Each list started with a few extra stimuli to get the subjects used to the task. Pauses were inserted once or twice in a session.

Responses; voice switch

The subjects responded orally with one letter or one word, directly after stimulus presentation. They were not pressed, in order to let them operate as normally as possible. Responses “illegible”, “nothing seen” etc. were allowed. A subject’s response operated a voice switch and the time interval between the onset of the stimulus and the triggering of the voice switch was measured electronically. All responses were recorded on tape, together with audible clicks of the microswitch which started each stimulus. Careful evaluation of the sound tracks enabled us to skip latencies unreliable because of heavy breathing, occasional noise from out- side, etc. Recent recordings have shown that operation of the voice switch systematically depends on the initial sounds produced, differences being of the order of 30 msec [27]. Consequently, the uncertainty in separate latencies is less than 50 msec.

(4)

DYSLEXIA: A SPECIFIC RECODING DEFICIT? 287

3. RESULTS* 3.1. Group averages

Correct scores for the fovea1 and parafoveal stimuli are given in Fig. la as averages of the

individual results for the two groups of subjects. The results are largely similar to the data gathered from the same subjects a year earlier [4], but the scores are now generally higher. For embedded letters and for words, the better recognition in the right visual field as com- pared with the left equally confirms our earlier findings and is consistent with earlier literature [27I. In our earlier experiments different stimuli were used left and right of fixation whereas here they were the same.

Response Iatencies as averaged over the groups are given in Fig. 1 b and Table 1. For

correct letter recognition, the dyslectic group is slower by some 120 msec both for isolated letters and for embedded letters, and both for fovea1 and for parafoveal presentation. For correct word recognition, the dyslectic group is slower by some 220 msec both in fovea1 and in parafoveal presentation. Response latencies of correct responses do not show a significant left-right difference. Average latencies of incorrect responses are generally less reliable because of the smaller number and the greater spread of the data.

Differences between dyslectics and controls for incorrect responses are 220 msec for

4 eccentricity t

4 eccentricity t

FIG. I. Fractions of correct recognitions and corresponding latencies averaged for the dyslectic subjects D and the control subjects C for three retinal positions. Recognition concerns isolated

letters la/, embedded letters jxax/ and well-known short words /wrd/.

(5)

Table 1. Group averages of correct scores and response latencies for correct and incorrect responses, together with their between-subjects standard deviations (between brackets). The last column indicates group differences, which depend on type of response rather than on recognition difficulty Dyslectics Controls Score (%) Latency (ms) Score (%> Latency (ms) Dysl.-Contr. Score (%) Latency (ms)

0”

c Correct responses Fovea1 Ial lxaxl lwrdl F 96 (5) 910 (260) 95 (7) 770 (I 20) +1 140 E 72 (23) 940 (240) 90 (11) 830 (170) -18 110 89 (15) 900 (190) 99 (2) 680 (100) -10 220 Parafoveal /a/ 93 (6) 840 (140) 95 (6) 740 (110) -2 100 a ixaxi 43 (12) 1040 (120) 54 (15) 920 (130) -11 120 lwrdl 57 (12) 940 (150) 75 (11) 730 (90) -18 210 E z Incorrect responses Parafoveal /xax/ 1380 (230) 1160 (250) 220 lwrdl 1240 (250) 1000 (210) 240

(6)

DYSLEXIA : A SPECIFIC RECODING DEFICIT? 289

parafoveal embedded letters and 240 msec for parafoveal words. For both groups of sub- jects, incorrect responses are on average substantially slower than correct responses. the

differences being some 300 msec.

As to differences between the types of stimuli, each group reported correct letters and correct words equally fast, whereas correct embedded letters were slower by some 50- 200 msec. As expected, correct responses to parafoveal stimuli are somewhat slower than to fovea1 stimuli, with the exception of isolated letters which are reported about equally fast foveally and parafoveally.

For isolated letters, ceiling effects prevent a possibly more difficult parafoveal recognition from becoming manifest. However, correct response latencies of isolated letters do not differ significantly for fovea1 and parafoveal presentation. Since the fovea1 letters came first in the session, we have considered whether the responses to fovea1 stimuli might perhaps have suffered because the subjects had not yet got used to the task. Two arguments would seem to contradict this : Firstly, each list started with several extra stimuli to get the subjects used to the task, which is indeed an easy one. Secondly, we found no latency differences between the first and second halves of the stimuli for each condition. HAUN [29] found a small effect of eccentricity on recognition latency of isolated letters.

3.2. Certain individual results

Table 1 gives standard deviations of latency distributions of individual averages. These distributions are approximately normal. Generally, the dyslectic subjects differ more among each other than the control subjects. There appears to be overlap between subjects in the dyslectic and the control groups. Distributions of correct response fractions (scores) in Table 1 are generally not normal because of ceiling effects.

3.3. Certain relations between variables

The experiments as described have many variables. Independent variables are type of stimulus (isolated letters, embedded letters, words), retinal location (foveal, parafoveal), and type of subject (dyslectics, normal readers). Dependent variables are type of response (correct, incorrect) and response latency, Conditions for an analysis of variance are not met. We mention here the most noteworthy relations, for which we shall use correlation coeffi- cients as an index.

Relations between correct-recognition scores for difSerent tasks. In correspondence with the earlier results [4], there is for both groups a general, moderately positive relation between correct response fractions for different stimuli and for different retinal positions. For fovea1 presentations in particular, ceiling effects prevent a useful interpretation of such relations. Generally, the correlation coefficients are of the order of +0.45 for the dyslectics and r = +0.50 for the controls. Parafoveal embedded letter scores and parafoveal word scores correlate r = +0.53 in the dyslectics and r = +0.61 in the controls, as compared with

r = +0.44 (dyslectics) and r = 0.50 (controls) in the experiments carried out a year earlier with the same subjects [4]. In summary, there is evidence of a weak common component in correct recognition of embedded letters and of words.

Relations between correct-response Iatencies for dzyerent tasks. Both for the group of dyslectics and for the group of normal readers, there is a general, significantly positive relation between latencies of correct responses for the various tasks. As an example, Fig. 2(a) plots latencies for fovea1 embedded letters vs fovea1 words (dyslectics r = +0.73, controls

(7)

H. BOUMA and CH. P. LEGEIN . . __._ . l

Q

=o

to

00 0 .q

O

c

co

0 0 08 . K . D . : > 0 0

+ latency /xax/ taveal

.

i

t latency lxoxl pamfaveal

FIG. 2. Correlation diagram for response latencies of embedded letters /xax/ and of words /wrd/, both foveally (2a) and parafoveally (2b).

(8)

DYSLEXIA : A SPECIFIC RECODING DEFICIT? 1.0, ooc 8 ^ : Oo 80a9 4 b 00 0 F . . . 3 c zo. l . m. 0 0.5 P - . ? . IJ P . . ti t On'

1

a0 0.5 1.0 ., .5r

- word response latency (parafoveal)

FIG. 3. Correlation diagram for response latencies and response scores, both for parafoveal words.

291

FIG. 4. Relation of response latency for fovea1 word responses and reading level according to the Tanghe test. Although the correlation coefficient is not significant, it will be seen that there

are no quick responders among those with low reading level (~3.5).

r = +0.79). Figure 5(b) gives similar results for parafoveal presentation (dyslectics r =

+0.61, controls r = +0.88). In the correlation matrix relating all “correct” latencies, correlation coefficients are 0.50 < r d +0.90 for the dyslectics and 0.44 < r < 0.88 for the controls. Generally, these relations support the notion of a common component in the speed of responding correctly to visually presented alphabetic material.

Relations between correct scores and correct response latencies for the same tasks. The

results indicate a relation between correct response fractions and latencies of correct responses in the sense that the higher the correct recognition score, the qricker these correct responses came. Here too, ceiling effects in response scores prevent a useful interpretation

(9)

292 H. BOUMA and CH. P. LEGEIN

in a number of instances. Figure 3 plots parafoveal word score vs latency of correct responses for each subject (dyslectics r = - 0.49, controls r = - 0.46). Thus, to a moderate extent, the quick responders have the higher scores and there is no such thing as a speed-accuracy trade-off, in which the quick responders would have had the lower accuracies. In our Institute, we have recently found many instances in which response latencies of correct responses substantially increased when recognition became more difficult as judged from lower correct scores [30]. The correlations mentioned here, however, concern relations between different subjects carrying out the same task, whereas BOUWHUIS et al. [30] were primarily concerned with different tasks carried out by the same subjects.

Relations with the Tanghe reading test. Interestingly, both for the dyslectics and the controls, correlations between correct scores or latencies on the one hand and Tanghe read- ing level on the other are generally not significantly different from zero, although they are usually in the expected direction (positive for word scores, negative for correct response latencies). Thus, both correct recognition scores and latencies fail to classify as clear deter- miners of reading level. As can be seen in Fig. 4, there is nevertheless a relation in the sense that all very weak readers (level < 3.5) come out with long response latencies.

4. DISCUSSION

4.1. Group diflerences between dyslectics and controls

A number of such differences have been found. Summarized, we corroborated earlier results that dyslectic subjects have lower scores for embedded letters and for words both in fovea1 and in parafoveal presentation. Scores for isolated letters, however, are equal and close to IOO’A. Ceiling effects may have prevented a possible underlying difference in recognition difficulty from becoming manifest, but the results again clearly show that our dyslectic children know the letter forms.

As to latencies, the dyslectics are generally slower than the controls to an average value of 120 msec for letter recognitions and 220 msec for word recognitions. It is striking and theoretically important that the difference depends on the type of recognition only and not on the mode of presentation. For words, it does not even depend on the correctness of the response. The particular value of the difference is not so important because it depends on the selection of the widely differing subjects. Generally, incorrect responses take 300- 400 msec longer than correct responses in both groups. Such an effect is also well known from experiments with adult subjects in a number of tasks [30]. PERFETTI and HOGABOAM [31, 321 have reported that in visual word reading and search tasks, poor readers need more

time for words, values being not too different from the present ones. 4.2. A simple sequential processing scheme

We shall now try to use the present results for a better understanding of dyslexia as a specific reading weakness. For this purpose, we propose a simple scheme of sequential information processing in overt letter and word recognition. The notion “sequential” is taken to imply a sequence of processes, which, however, may overlap in time, and in which the later ones are dependent on the earlier ones, but not vice versa. Each of these processes takes a certain amount of time, partly to be specified. The scheme is depicted in Fig. 5 and relates to a group of closely connected theories on processes involved in recognition, known as logogen theory [33, 341, multicomponent theory [35] and letter-confusion theory [21-231, to which the reader is referred. The scheme should be considered primarily as an aid for

(10)

DYSLEXIA : A SPECIFIC RECODING DEFICIT ? 293 Print ,+, PRINT RECOGNITION

4

I

Speech SPEECH RECOGNITION

FIG. 5. Sequential processing scheme for allocating findings from visual recognition experi- ments of letters and words. In dyslectics, the visual-speech recoding is assumed to be delayed.

explaining results, for distinguishing main effects from side effects, and for guiding research into the nature and possible remedy of dyslexia. No claim is made to true independence of stages and even less to full comprehension of the dyslexia syndrome.

We shall discuss the scheme of Fig. 5 first in relation to the present results and next to literature on dyslexia.

The visual quality of the stimulus influences the time needed for visual analysis. Thus, isolated letters need less analysis time than embedded letters and fovea1 words need less time than parafoveal words. The corresponding differences in response latencies that have been found are therefore attributed to the process of visual analysis.

Latency differences between correct and incorrect recognitions are attributed to the selection phase. In cases of correct recognition, activation of the correct unit will usually have surpassed activation of alternative candidates. Thus, the selection is relatively easy and straightforward. In cases of incorrect recognition, a decision between the activated incorrect unit and at least one other unit (here the correct unit) is required. The latency differences of 300-400 msec between correct and incorrect recognitions are thus attributed to the extra time necessary for selecting between units which visually resemble each other.

The visual-speech recoding phase is proposed as the most likely candidate for the dif- ferences between dyslectic and control subjects. The idea is that dyslectic subjects need somewhat more time for recoding a visually recognized letter into its letter name than the control subjects, and even more time for recoding a visual word into its sound (name). The primary reason for attributing this difference to the visual-speech recoding is that the latency difference between dyslectics and controls depends on the item to be recognized (letters vs words), but not on recognition difficulty (isolated vs embedded letters, fovea1 vs parafoveal). If the time difference were attributed to the visual activation phase, the latency difference between dyslectics and controls would be expected to increase with recognition difficulty.

KLAPP

et al.

[36], SPOEHR and SMITH [37], and SPOEHR [38] have developed notions of how the recoding from a visual into a speech code may be envisaged. The envisaged influence of

(11)

294 H. BOUMA and CH. P. LEGEIN

length is well applicable to dyslexic children, who have particular difficulty in dealing with long words.

Finally, it is proposed that the articulation phase is equally developed in dyslectics and controls. The basic evidence for this is that dyslectic children are normal speakers and listeners. In a pilot experiment in which both dyslectic and control children repeated spoken words, which included long and difficult ones, we found no obvious extra delays among the dyslectic subjects. This is in strong contrast to the reading of long words, which dyslectics find particularly difficult [4, 391. This makes it unlikely that slow speaking is the source of the long latencies in visual letter and word recognition. On the same grounds, a general slowness of dyslectic children in all kinds of recognition seems unsuitable for explaining the specific delays observed in the experiments.

Summarizing this part of the discussion, we propose that dyslectic children’s visual information processing and speaking are just as efficient as those of control children, but their recoding into speech is deficient. Why, then, should their recognition scores be lower ? The explanation proposed here takes a lead from the finding that low recognition scores and long latencies tend to go together. Also it is of interest to note that latencies are often as long as l-2 sec. During this period, the information has to reside somewhere in the visual or the response system. On the assumption that storage of the response as such is possible only after that response has become available internally, the preceding phases of the recognition process should have their own storage facilities. If we take the first storage facility after the retina to retain rather unprocessed visual information for some 0.25 set, we clearly need other forms of visual storage before the information can be carried to a more lasting response form. These storage facilities are assumed to be volatile, such that each extra delay automatically leads to decreased performance (lower probability of correct responses).

The picture as sketched above is a simplistic one, but it has the advantage of clearly assigning experimental results to possible processing stages and it lends itself to further experimental tests.

In general terms, such an approach has recently been advocated by a number of researchers in the field [40, 411. A view somewhat similar to the one put forward here has been advo- cated by JORM [8] (1979), who attributes the primary difficulty to auditory short-term memory problems rather than to “phonemic translation”. However, TORCESEN [42] has found that lower short-term memory scores are open to easy improvement by training.

It .seem~ relevant to consider briefly the relation between word recognition and letter recognition. The longer latencies for embedded letters as compared with those for words might be taken to suggest that word recognition occurs earlier than recognition of the constituent letters and thus cannot be based on the latter. We think that this conclusion is premature. During actual word recognition, neither an explicit selection between alternative letters nor a recoding of individual letters into their names is necessary. Thus, in our view, word recognition can very well be based on the activation, without selection and recoding, of a number of the word’s constituent letters. For a quantitative theory on this the reader is referred to BOUWHUIS and BOUMA [21-231.

We now turn to word recognition and reading. Correlations of reading level with word recognition scores were positive though small, and with word recognition latency negative though small. Thus, the reading level of the children cannot be deduced from their word recognition in any simple way. On the other hand, in our data a low fovea1 word score implies a low reading level and a quick fovea1 word response excludes a low reading level. It seems then that other component skills of reading (see [40]) sometimes make up for short- comings in word recognition, for example an adapted eye movement strategy, a good con- centration, or a better utilization of context. It can be maintained, therefore, that for adequate

(12)

DYSLEXIA : A SPECIFIC RECODING DEFICIT ? 295

reading, word recognition skills are a necessity, in addition to other skills. It should be added that the simple reading test we used was not designed to reflect the intricacies of the com- ponent skills ; as our insight deepens the need for more sophisticated reading tests increases.

The above discussion has been based on group averages. We wish to stress again that the distribution of results of dyslectic and control subjects overlap and that there are marked differences between individual dyslectic subjects. The framework sketched above is clearly suited for individual results as well.

4.3. Confusing issues in dyslexia research

UnspeciJic and specljic theories. Many authors have noted that the literature on dyslexia

is rather confused, both as to experimental results and to type of theories 12,411. A number of factors contribute to the confusion. A first difficulty is the distinction between poor read- ing in general and specific developmental dyslexia. Throughout the literature, authors have been struck by the very specific nature of the reading difficulties of so many subjects, mainly difficulties of visual word recognition and of spelling, in the absence of any general intel- lectural, emotional or social handicap. However, in selecting subjects for research purposes, it is rather difficult to exclude other weak readers, and so it is often difficult to compare results found by different authors.

A second difficulty resides in the type of explanation. Some authors favour a neurological explanation (minimal brain damage, dysfunction of the left hemisphere, dysfunction of the parietal lobe); others prefer a functional explanation (disturbed visual processing, disturbed phonemic recoding); still others lean towards a developmental explanation (general matura- tional lag). The types of theories are not mutually exclusive, but the different goals of the authors and the different degrees in which theoretical notions have been worked out make a comparison or combination difficult.

Thirdly there is the quite general difference between statistically significant results and substantial results. Since the difficulty of visual word recognition is quite substantial, weak but statistically significant correlations cannot be convincing as explanatory factors unless they are part of a more comprehensive theory.

However, the main difficulty is probably distinguishing between causes and effects. In our society, printed text is such an important source of information that subjects with poor reading ability will almost certainly develop a multitude of secondary relative deficits, such as in knowledge, in perceptual skills, and sometimes in emotional stability. Little imagina- tion is needed to perceive the development of many weak correlations, but once they have been found, it is difficult to give any specific explanation of their source. For these reasons, it seems a relatively safe strategy to direct research efforts to the strong effects themselves or to closely related effects, and almost without exception these have been found in the field of visual language material.

In trying to attribute the main difficulty and cause of dyslexia to poor visual-speech recoding, it remains to explain why in many other tasks certain deficiencies have been observed. Recently, VELLUTINO [44] has discussed the issue at some length and therefore we shall only touch upon a few of these questions here.

If there are no primary deficiencies in visual analysis, the relative difficulty of dyslectics in dealing with complex configurations [ 1, 21 may then result from having missed a certain transfer of the skill of visual word analysis to complex visual processing in general, or from a hidden use of names when carrying out the tasks. Perhaps it is as well to remember that any children with a primary deficiency in visual analysis will most likely also find it difficult

(13)

296 H. BOUMA and CH. P. LEGEIN

to analyse printed words. This would give rise to the contention of a positive correlation between the two skills, but would not allow the conclusion that dyslexia is generally caused by problems in visual analysis. VELLUTINO

et al.

[43, 441 reject the hypothesis of a visual deficiency on the grounds of copying experiments in which dyslectic children performed just as well as controls. This is indeed strong evidence, and it will be interesting to see the approach extended for more complex visual configurations.

Quite different evidence against a primary visual deficiency explanation can be derived from the consistent reports that dyslexia is virtually unknown in Japan [45]. The Kanji symbols, which are adapted from Chinese, directly represent meaning, not mediated by sound. In the two Kana scripts which do represent sound (syllables), there is one-to-one correspondence between sign and sound, much more regular than in most alphabetic languages (see also [46]). Despite the complexity of the visual configurations, Kanji symbols are more easily learned by young children than Kana [47].

In alphabetic languages visual analysis of print is by definition deficient in dyslectic sub- jects. If we put the primary difficulty in the visual-to-speech recoding phase, the deficient letter and word recognition could be attributed to (a) the disappearance of visual informa- tion from volatile visual stores before it can be put into the more stable speech code, and (b) the interference of recoding difficulty with a proper development of internal visual word units. This might be expected to affect mainly long and infrequent words. In the experiments reported here, even the low frequency words were short and well known.

In our view, deviating scanning patterns of the eyes should be considered as secondary to reading difficulties.

Apart from visual deficiencies, auditory deficiencies have also been reported in dyslectics, usually involving an inability to make fine discriminations between phonemes embedded in words [2, 31. As VALTIN [41] has pointed out, the awareness of phonemes is increased by proper spelling experience, and these auditory deficiencies may therefore be secondary to the spelling difficulties of dyslectics. Along the same lines, WALLACH et

al.

[48] have found that children who experience no difficulty in discriminating between words with small differences in just one vowel, may well have difficulties in naming the vowels explicitly. Again, if certain primary auditory or speech deficiencies exist, these may lead to reading difficulties [49] and thus inflate general correlation coefficients between the two.

VELLUTINO [44] in his review comes to the conclusion that the cause of dyslexia is either to be found in deficient phonemic recoding, or in deficient general language abilities. We think the latter notion unattractive, since it conflicts with the normal listening and speaking capabilities of dyslectics. Certain secondary language deficiencies may occur, however, inasmuch as they normally develop concurrently with reading. PERFETTI and HOCABOAM [3 11, too, have advocated a position in which they stress the verbal part of reading difficulty, just as we do.

Acknowledgements-Thanks are due to Mr. A. L. M. van Rens for his enthusiastic assistance throughout the investigation and to Mr. A. van Vroenhoven and Mr. J. Hupperetz, directors of the schools involved, as well as to their staffs for their interest and their fine cooperation, Dr. D. G. Bouwhuis helped us by his comments on an earlier draft and by advising on statistical problems.

REFERENCES

1. MALMQUIST, E. Factors Relating to Reading Disabilities in the First Grade of the Elementary School. Almquist & Wiksell, Stockholm, 1958.

(14)

DYSLEXIA : A SPECIFIC RECODING DEFICIT? 297 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22.

VERNON, M. D. Varieties of deficiency in the reading process. Hurv. Educ. Rev. 47, 39G10, 1977. BOUMA, H. and LEGEIN, Ch. P. Fovea] and parafoveal recognition of letters and words by dyslectics and by average readers. Neuropsychologia 15, 69-80, 1977.

PAVLIDIS, G. Th. The dyslkctic’s erratic eye movements: case studies. Dyslexia Rev. 1, 22-27, 1978. ADLER-GRINBERG, D. and STARK, L. Eye movements, scanpaths, and dyslexia. Am. J. Optom. Physio. Optic. 55, 557-570, 1978.

VALTIN, R. Legasthenie-Theorien und Untersuchungen. Beltz, Basel, 1970.

JORM, A. F. The cognitive and neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: a theoretical framework and review. Cognition 7, 19-33, 1979.

VELLUTINO, F. R., PRUZEK, R. M., STEGER, J. A. and MESHORLAM, U. Immediate visual recall in poor and normal readers as a function or orthographic-linguistic familiarity. Cortex 9, 368-384, 1973. VELLUTINO, F. R., STEGER, J. A., DESETTO, L. and PHILLIPS, F. Immediate and delayed recognition of visual stimuli in poor and normal readers. J. exp. Child Psycho/. 19, 223-232, 1975.

JORM, A. F. Parietal lobe function in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologiu 15, 841-844, 1977. TORGESON, J. K., Reading performance of disabled children on serial memory tasks: a selective review of recent research. Read. Res. Quart. 14, 57-87, 197811979.

BOUMA, H. and DE VOOGD, A. H. On the control of eye saccades in reading. Vis. Res. 14,273-284,1974. ANDRIESSEN, J. J. and DE VOOGD, A. H. Analysis of eye movement patterns in silent reading. ZPO Ann. Prog. Rep. 8, 29-34, 1973.

RAYNER, K. Eye movements in reading and information processing. Psycho/. Bull. 85, 618-660, 1978. LEVY SCHOEN, A. and O’REGAN, K. The control of eye movements in reading. In Processing of Visible Language, I, P. A. KOLERS, M. WROLSTAD and H. BOUMA (Editors). Plenum Press, New York, 1979. BOUMA, H. Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nafure, London 226, 177-178, 1970. BOUMA, H. Visual search and reading: eye movements and functional visual field. In Attention and Performance VII, J. REQUIN (Editor), pp. 115-147. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1978.

BOUMA, H. Visual interference in the parafoveal recognition of initial and final letters of words. Vis. Res. 13, 767-782, 1973.

SCHIEPERS, C. W. J. Global attributes in visual word recognition-l and 2. Vis. Res. 16, 1343-1349 and 1445-1454, 1976.

BOUWHUIS, D. G. and BOUMA, H., Visual recognition of three-letter words as derived from the recogni- tion of the constituent letters. Percept. Psychophys. 25, 12-22, 1979.

BOUWHUIS, D. G., A model for the visual recognition of words of three letters. In Attention and Per- formance VII, J. REQUIN (Editor), pp. 631-644. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1978.

23. BOUWHUIS, D. G. Visual word recognition. Thesis Nijmegen, 1979.

24. BOUMA, H., LEGEIN, CH. P. and VAN RENS, A. L. M., Visual recognition by dyslectic children: response

25 26 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38.

latencies for letters and words, IPO Ann. Prog. Rep. 12, 87-91, 1977.

TANGHE, M., DE B. V. L. Zndividuele Leestests. Broeders van Liefde, Tessenderloo, 1971.

LINSCHOTEN, J. De la Court’s frequentietelling van Nederlandse woorden. Report 6301. Psychological Lab., Utrecht, 1963.

SCHR~DER, U. A controlled voice switch. ZPO Ann. Prog. Rep. 12, 137-139, 1977.

MCKEEVER, W. F. and HULING, M. D. Lateral dominance in tachistoscopic word recognitions of children at two levels of ability. Q. J. exp. Psycho/. 22, 600-604, 1970.

HAUN, F. Functional dissociation of the hemispheres using fovea1 input. Neuropsychologiu 16, 725-733, 1978.

BOUWHUIS, D. G., SCHIEPERS, C. J. W., SCHRGDER, U. 0. and TIMMERS, H. Temporal structure of visual word recognition responses. ZPO Ann. Prog. Rep. 13, 83-93, 1978.

PERFETTI, C. A. and HOGABOAM, T. The relationship between simple word decoding and reading com- prehension skill. J. Educ. Psychol. 67, 461469, 1975.

HOGABOAM, T. W. and PERFETTI, C. A. Reading skill and the role of verbal experience in decoding. J. Educ. Psycho!. 70,717-729, 1978.

MORTON, J. Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychol. Rev. 76, 165-178, 1969.

MORTON, J. Some experiments on facilitation in word and picture recognition and their relevance for the evolution of a theoretical position. In Processing of Visible Language 1, P. A. KOLERS, M. WROLSTAD and H. BOUMA (Editors). Plenum Press, New York, 1979.

RUMELHART, D. E. and SIPLE, P. Process of recognizing tachistoscopically presented words. Psychol. Rev. 81.99-118. 1974.

KLAPP, S. T., ANDERSON, W. G. and BERRIAN, R. W. Implicit speech in reading, reconsidered. J. Exp. Psychol. 100, 368-374, 1973.

SPOEHR, K. T. and SMITH, E. E. The role of orthographic and phonotactic rules in perceiving letter patterns. J. exp. Psychol., Hum. Percep. Perform. 1, 21-34, 1975.

SPOEHR, K. T. Phonological encoding in visual word recognition. J. verb. Learn. verb. Behav. 17, 127- 141, 1978.

(15)

298 H. BOUMA and CH. P. LE~EIN 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49.

BOUMA, H., LEGEIN, CH. P. and VAN RENS, A. L. M. Visual recognition by dyslectic children. Further exploration of letter, word and number recognition in 4 weak and 4 normal readers. IPO Ann. Prog. Rep. 10 72-78, 1975.

FR’~DERIKSEN, J. R. A chronometric study of component skills in reading. Bolt, Beranek and Newman. Rep ort 3757.

VALTIN, R. Dyslexia: deficit in reading or deficit in research? Read. Res. Quart. 14,201-221, 1978/1979. TORGESON, J. K. Memorization processes in reading of disabled children. J. educ. Psychol. 69, 571-578, 1977.

VELLUTINO, F. R., SMITH, H., STEGER, J. A. and KAMAN, M. Reading disability: age differences and the perceptual deficit hypothesis. Child Dev. 46, 487-493, 1975.

VELLUTINO, F. R. Alternative conceptualizations of dyslexia. Evidence in support of a verbal deficit hypothesis. Harv. educ. Rev. 47, 334-354, 1977.

MAKITA, K. The rarity of reading disability in Japanese children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 38, 599-614, 1968.

GLEITMAN, L. R. and ROZIN, P. The structure and acquisition of reading I & II. In Toward a Psychology

of Reading, A. S. REBER and D. L. SCARBOROUGH (Editors). Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1977.

STEINBERG, D. D. and YAMADA, J. Are whole word kanji easier to learn than syllable kana? Read. Res. Quart. 14, S&99, 1978/1979.

WALLACH, L., WALLACH, M. A., DOZIER, M. G. and KAPLAN, N. E. Poor children learning to read do not have trouble with auditory discrimination but do have trouble with phoneme recognition. J. educ. Psychol. 69, 36-39, 1977.

LYLE, J. G. Certain antenatal, perinatal and developmental variables and reading retardation in middle- class boys. Child Dev. 41,481, 1970. (Cited from VELLUTINO [44].)

20 dyslexiques et 20 lecteurs normaux Bg@s de 11 .3 15 an* devaient

reconnaftre des lettres et de* mats en vision .3 la fois fov6ale

et parafov6ale. On mesurait les scores de rGponse correcte et les I%-

ponses de latence. Les latences de r6ponses correctes @taient plus

longues pour le groupe dyslexique d'environ 100 msec. pour les lettres

et de 200 msec. pour les mots, quelle que soit la pr6sentation. On propose un schPme de traitement skquentiel simple selon lequel

la *curce principale des difficult&s pour les dyslexiques est un

retard de la traduction des items reconnus visuellement, en un code

verbal. Les scores inferieurs proviennent alors du caractSre fugace

du store visuel.

Zusammenfassung:

20 dyslektische und 20 normale Leser im Alter van 11 bis 15 Jahren erkannten WBrter und Buchstaben fovea1 und parafoveal. Es wurden die Scores fi.ir zutreffende Antworten und die Latenzen der Reakfionen gemessen.

Die Latenzen der korrekten Antworten waren bei der dyslektischen Gruppe 18nger: etwa 100 msec mehr fiir Buchstaben und 200 msec mehr fur Wtjrter, unabhgngig van der Art der Darbietung.

Ein einfaches Schema der sequentiellen Verarbeitung erd vorgeschlagen, in dem die Hauptursache fCir die Probleme der dyslektiscben Personen eine Verzegerung in der Ubertragung visuell erkannter Items in das Sprachsystem ist. Die geringeren Leistungen sind daher die Folge der flfichtigeo Natur der visuellen Speicherung.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

To sum up, conduction velocity probably does play a role in latencies, but we did not add it as a constraint because it cannot explain the discrepancies between timing data

significantly from the neutral control condition. Both CV and CVC primes yielded significant priming effects for CV and CVC targets when compared to the neutral control

Also, the underestimation of future contextual factors results in purchasing food that is eventually not consumed (Griffin &amp; Ross, 1991, Evans, 2011a). Based on

velocities gives a- systematic motion of the clusters which nearly coincides with that of the stars of highest velocity.4 It may be of significance that the mean antapex of the

Appendix 9: Distribution of return on assets during economic downturn (left side = publicly owned companies, right side = privately owned companies).. Most of the research

Er is in het huidige onderzoek gecorrigeerd voor SES, maar dat er geen verschil is gevonden in het totaal aantal verbalisaties tussen de groep met psychosociale problemen en

laevis from the USA ( McCoid &amp; Fritts, 1980 ), Wales ( Measey, 1998a ), Chile ( Lobos &amp; Measey, 2002 ), and South Africa ( Vogt et al., 2017 ) with data collected during

The first scientific study that was published on ostrich skin quality only appeared in 1996 (Mellett et al., 1996). Most research reports published since then focussed