• No results found

Media logic for elite versus anti-elite parties in Dutch talkshows during the campaign for the general elections of 2017

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Media logic for elite versus anti-elite parties in Dutch talkshows during the campaign for the general elections of 2017"

Copied!
38
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Media logic for elite versus anti-elite parties in Dutch

talkshows during the campaign for the general

elections of 2017

Anique Gijsberts (10353445) Master’s Thesis

Graduate School of Communication

Master’s programme Communication Science University of Amsterdam

Supervisor: Dr. Marjolein Moorman June 30, 2017

(2)

Abstract

An often heard complaint of anti-elite politicians is that they are not treated the same as elite

parties. This research tests whether this is a legitimate accusation or not.That’s why the

following question is central: How is media logic visible for elite vs. anti-elite parties in the

run up to the Dutch general elections of 2017 in the major talkshows? Media logic is divided into ‘control over form’ and ‘control over content’. Control over form includes the variables ‘reaction time’, ‘interrupting comments’ and ‘question type’, and ’control over content’ includes personalisation, horse race and negativity. A quantitative content analysis is

conducted in which comments made by talkshow hosts to politicians are coded (N=615).

From the results, no clear conclusion can be drawn, but some differences in media logic for

anti-elite parties and elite parties are found. This lays a foundation for further research to find

out why media logic is different for anti-elite parties and elite parties.

Keywords

(3)

Introduction

In the run up to the Dutch general elections of 2017, the hosts of several talkshows on

television were criticised by a vast number of anti-elite parties. For instance, Jan Roos, leader

of Voor Nederland (VNL), said after his interview with Pauw & Jinek that ‘if you are a right

wing politician, you have a problem in the Netherlands’, Geert Wilders from the Partij voor

de Vrijheid (PVV) never showed up in Nieuwsuur because ‘he didn’t like it’, and Thierry

Baudet of Forum voor Democratie (FvD) and Tunahan Kuzu, leader of DENK, both criticized

the Dutch media multiple times for being biased. In addition to this, an often heard complaint

is that parties which don’t belong to the party elite of old, (often) big, established parties, are

not treated the same as other parties. One has to only think of the recent discussions on the

media and the content of the media as ‘fake’, ‘left wing hate media’1 and being ‘a mouthpiece’ of the ‘elite’2

.

This mistrust is shared by a lot of followers of the anti-elite parties, and goes often

hand in hand with a low trust in the government (Lee, 2010). On Twitter, Facebook and other

public platforms there is a lot of support for this anti-media rhetoric. The discussion further

fuelled by the ongoing flow of anti-media and anti-establishment messages of politicians

through Twitter and in interviews, but can that mistrust be substantiated? We already know

that the media have an influence on what is considered important by the public and the way

different issues are framed (McCombs & Shaw, 1972). Is there scientific prove that

legitimizes this distrust in the media by anti-establishment politicians, or does their claim

against the media just turn out to be an easy way for a party to put itself in the underdog

position? One way to see if the trust issues with the media can be substantiated, is to find out

1 From http://www.telegraaf.nl/binnenland/27788708/__Wilders_haalt_uit_naar__haatmedia___.html

2

From http://www.independent.co.uk/news/uk/politics/len-mccluskey-jeremy-corbyn-media-bias-corporate-mouthpiece-establishment-labour-conference-latest-a7329196.html

(4)

if the anti-elite parties have a point; are anti-elite parties actually treated differently by the

media?

A way to test this, would be to look at media logic. This is part of the process of

mediatisation; a process in which politics becomes more dependent on the rules of the media

(Strömbäck, 2008). In the final stage of mediatisation, media logic determines how issues are

portrayed in the media. This can be looked at from the side of the politician, in which he or

she adjusts to the demands of media logic, but also from the side of the media. In this thesis, I

will look at this side of media logic, also called media interventionism. This describes the

ways in which media can intervene, without defining if this happens consciously or

subconsciously. Media interventionism is divided into ‘control over form’ and ‘control over content’ (Voltmer & Brants, 2011). Media logic through control over form happens by

influencing the form of, in this case, an interview in a talkshows. This happens by influencing

reaction time, the type of comment or by interruptions. Media logic through control over

content happens by influencing the content of a conversation, by asking things that fit within

media logic. To see if elite parties are treated differently than anti-elite parties, I will compare

how media logic is applied for both party types.

This is relevant, as television still is an important medium for people for finding

information about politics. Although the media landscape is changing rapidly and online

media make television seem somewhat outdated, television still has a substantial share in the

media landscape and media routines of lots of its citizens. It is the most important source for

news according to Papathanassopoulos et al. (2013), but a development can be seen in which

especially young people are adopting other media in their routine of news gathering.

However, also for young people television is still an important medium to gain knowledge of

(5)

Looking back at the declining trust in the media and anti-media rhetoric becoming

more present in the current political discourse and the public sphere, it is important to see if

the accusations of anti-elite parties are legitimate. Although this research does not aim to

assess whether the content that is produced by the media can be trusted, it aims to say

something about the presence or absence of differences in attention for different party types.

To be able to say something about what drives this media logic, one first needs to have

a clear overview of the presence of media logic in the media. The research aims to make a

contribution to this understanding. This is important, as a clear overview of how media logic

works for different party types lays a foundation for further research in understanding what

effects explain media logic and what journalistic choices lie behind it. Therefore, in this

thesis, a division will be made between parties with a strong anti-establishment rhetoric and

parties that belong to the establishment.This has been done for a lot of news programs

(Voltmer & Brants, 2011), but not for political interviews in talkshows.

As parties are especially visible during election time when they are campaigning, this

research focuses on the way politicians are approached by interviewers in talkshow interviews

in the three weeks prior to Dutch general elections of 2017 and whether the presence of media

logic is different for those parties. I will look at interviews with politicians in the commercial,

late night talkshow RTL Late Night and Pauw en Jinek, a talkshow on the Dutch public

broadcasting channel in which two important late night talk show hosts of the public

broadcaster host a show that is specially produced for the elections, a few weeks before the

elections. That’s why the following question is central:

How is media logic visible for elite vs. anti-elite parties in the run up to the Dutch general elections of 2017 in the major talkshows?

(6)

Theoretical framework

The aim of this theoretical framework is to give an oversight of what is already known in the

literature about the different factors in the main research question; media logic and

differences between parties. This theoretical framework is structured as follows: first I will

give a common definition of media logic. Then I will elaborate on how media logic can be

studied. Within this framework, I explain the process of media interventionism by dividing it

into two parts; influence on form and influence on content. Finally, I explain how anti-elite

parties are defined. This section ends with two specific research questions.

Media logic

The process in which media and politics interact is a highly studied relationship in which the

logic that the media applies, or the way media present political reality, constantly changes.

Media first acted according to partisan logic, as a mouthpiece for political parties, but this role

changed to a public logic where as a result of depillarisation, professionalisation of the media

gained momentum and public broadcasters dominated the television landscape. The last phase

is a media logic that is characterized by commercial channels, the decline of newspapers and a

poll-driven style of reporting (Brants and Van Praag, 2006). This media logic is also seen as

the last of four phases of mediatisation, a societal process in which politics becomes more

dependent on media and in the end completely adjusts to the logic of the media (Strömbäck,

2008; 2011). Media logic is defined by Strömbäck (2008) as “the news values and the

storytelling techniques the media make use of to take advantage of their own medium and its

format, and to be competitive in the ongoing struggle to capture people’s attention”

(Strömbäck, 2008; pp. 233). In this context, media logic has been studied in various ways.

First, research looks at the way politicians adapt to media logic, by following the laws of the

(7)

logic only. For instance, research to soundbites looks at the way politicians use one-liners that

enhance the chance of being covered in the media (Esser, 2013), by making short statements

that fit in the short and simple style of reporting, or by addressing issues in a way that is

interesting for the media. Although Hallin (1992) concludes that this proves a more

journalist-driven style of reporting, it still looks at the side of media logic in which the politician adjusts

him or herself to the logic of the media. Research on how media coverage of politics is

influenced by media logic without taking into account the intention of a politician is more

scarce. This process is described by Strömbäck (2008) ‘media interventionism’.

In this research, I will look at the media interventionism side of media logic. By

looking at this side of media logic, one can really say something about the influence that

journalists have on the process of information gathering, regardless of how politicians act.

A way to explain media interventionism is to look closer at the journalistic role

conceptions. Plaisance and Skewes (2003) sum up three main role conceptions of journalists:

the disseminator; which describes the role of getting information to a public that is as broad as

possible and as quick as possible. The adversarial role conception stands for the journalist as

an adversarial of public people like business people and politicians. The interpretive function

stands for an analyzing style of reporting in which the journalists gives context and

clarifications for complex problems like new laws and (international) developments. Beam,

Weaver and Brownlee (2009) add the populist mobilizer role to this list, which stands for a

function in which ‘normal people’ get the chance to express an opinion and to be involved in

decision-making processes. Those different role conceptions are not mutually exclusive and

can form an explanation for the presence (or absence) of journalistic voice and input

(Mellado, 2015).

As stated earlier, this research will look at media logic from the perspective of media

(8)

control over time and control over content (Voltmer & Brants, 2011). In the next section, the

division between both types of control will be explained further.

Influence on form

Interviewers can apply media logic by influencing the form of an interview with politicians in

various ways. The different intervention techniques that are studied in this research are type

of comment, interrupting comments and the time a politician gets to answer to the comment.

Firstly, journalists can influence the amount of pressure they put on a politician to

explain his or her answers by adjusting his or her comments. For instance, the type of

comment the interviewer uses determines how much room a politician has to answer to a comment. If it is a closed question, a yes or no answer is required which makes it much more

difficult for a politician to prove his or her point and keeps the power of the conversation on

the side of the journalist (Voltmer & Brants, 2011).

Another indicator of influence on form is whether the politician is interrupted or not,

or as Stamper and Brants (2011) call it; turn-taking. They looked at the process of turn-taking,

described as “the main mechanism through which the exchange between interviewer and

interviewee is organized” (pp. 133). This can happen in a natural way (‘natural turn-taking’)

or through interruption. Within interruptions, the division between successful and

unsuccessful interruptions can be made. An interruption is successful when the interviewer

manages to take over control over the interview. When the politician retains control, the

interruption is considered unsuccessful. In this research, only successful interruptions are

counted, as this has the consequence of taking control, in contrast to unsuccessful turn-taking.

The last indicator of influence on the form of an interview is the time a politician gets

to answer to a comment the interviewer made. When a politician is given more time to

(9)

her electorate to hear about and can frame an issue in a way that is beneficial for him, her or

the party (Voltmer & Brants, 2011).

Influence on content

Besides controlling the form of an interview, interviewers can also control the content of the

comments made in an interview. According to Strömbäck, for instance, (news) media apply

their logic by using a game frame and/or horse race coverage. Zeh and Hopman (2013) add to

this, next to horse race coverage, with personalisation, visualisation and negative coverage. In

addition to this research, Takens et al. (2013) conclude that the combination of three

indicators, namely negative coverage, personalisation and contest coverage explain one single

media logic (also underlined by Broersma, Den Herder & Schohaus, 2013). In this research,

personalisation, contest coverage and negativity are studied as a part of media logic. Focusing

on (one of) those aspects fit into media logic for different reasons. One of the explanations is

that it corresponds with some of the traditional news values of Galtung and Ruge (1965).

They argue that stories get more attention when they fit into one of the news value categories.

The fact that journalism takes news values into account, says something about the shift from a

descriptive style of journalism to an interpretative style of journalism (Brants & Van Praag,

2006; Voltmer & Brants, 2011), in which there is a place for the interpretations of the

journalist (consciously or subconsciously). By following an interpretative style of journalism,

journalists follow the media logic by focusing on mass audiences. That’s why media logic

often goes hand in hand with a more populist way of reporting in talkshows (Blumler &

Kavanagh, 1999; Blumler, 2001) where the opinions of ‘normal’ men and women are voiced

and people can join the show in the audience.

The usage of personalisation, negative coverage and horse race coverage is a way to

(10)

negative effect is found for negative coverage of political news on political trust

(Kleinnijenhuis et al., 2006), and critics of media logic argue that media logic can have a

negative effect on political interest, political knowledge and an informed electorate (Esser,

2013).

Personalisation is seen as part of media logic because it indicates a more entertainment-driven style of reporting. By focussing (partly) on the person behind the

politician, issues that the politician stands for receive less attention. Personalisation is

researched in different ways within the context of media logic. Some focus more on “the

visibility of top-ranked politicians versus other lower ranked politicians” (Zeh & Hopman,

2013, pp. 229) and the “proportion of news stories with prime minister candidates” (Zeh &

Hopman, 2013, pp. 231). This research looks at a more substantive aspect of personalisation

like Van Santen & Van Zoonen (2009; 2010) do. They argue that personalisation is more than

looking at an increased attention for politicians instead of the political parties they represent.

They distinguish different types of personalisation of which three types can be seen in the

political communication field, looking at from the perspective of the media: an increased

focus on professional qualities of the politician (individualisation), an increased focus on

private persona of a politician (privatisation) and an increased focus on personal emotions of a

politician (emotionalisation).

Another indicator of influence on the content of an interview is horse race coverage, a

type of coverage about the contest between two or more leading parties. With horse race

coverage, the focus is more on the game element of elections (who is winning or losing) at the

cost of substantial questions and content. It is part of the game frame (Aalberg, Strömbäck &

De Vreese, 2011), which refers to “news stories that portray politics as a game and are

centered around: who is winning or losing elections, in the battle for public opinion, in

(11)

approval or disapproval from interest groups or particular constituencies or publics; or that

speculate about electoral or policy outcomes or potential coalitions” (Aalberg, Strömbäck &

De Vreese, 2011, pp. 172).

Closely related to contest coverage is negative coverage, as contest coverage can be

brought in a positive way or a negative way. Negativity is part of media logic as it is

“eye-catching, adds drama, stimulates interest and is easy to understand even by uninformed audiences” (Lengauer et al., 2012, pp. 182). Takens et al. (2013) define negativity it as the mean of the positive or negative directions of contest coverage. Subsequently, with the rise in

contest coverage in several countries, also the amount of negative coverage has increased. In

the United States and Germany, Farnsworth and Lichter (2006) found a rise in negativism in

election news coverage. This also counts for the Netherlands: from 1990 until 2003

Kleinnijenhuis et al. (2006) found an increase in negative campaign coverage, with an

exception in 2006.

Party types

As mentioned in the research question, the aim of this research is to find out if there is a

difference in the presence of media logic in interviews with politicians of anti-elite parties

compared to politicians of elite parties. In the Netherlands as well as in the international

politics, it is a well-known tactic of anti-elite politicians to blame journalists for treating them

differently than established parties. In this thesis, I test whether this is a legitimate accusation

or not.

Anti-elite politics (or anti-establishment politics) is defined as “a rhetorical appeal

based on opposition to those who wield power within the state” (Barr, 2009, pp. 44). The

emphasis is on the flaws of the current system or established political structure. The clearest

(12)

party PVV and socialist populist party SP (Bos & Brants, 2014). Relatively new parties that

were never represented in parliament (Otjes & Louwerse, 2015) and use an anti-establishment

rhetoric, like Forum voor Democratie (FvD) and Artikel13, are also considered anti-elite. In

the method section, an overview of every party in the research population that is considered

anti-elite is provided (see Table 2).

Except for Bos et al. (2010), who conclude that quality television programs pay more

attention to populist elements than to mainstream leaders (Bos et al., 2010), the

interventionism side of media logic in relation to anti-elite parties, as discussed earlier in this

paragraph, did not get that much attention in literature. Taking that into account, research on

the question if media logic is different for elite parties and anti-elite parties is missing. This

will be tested in this thesis. As a result that clear theory that supports the research question is

missing, no clear hypotheses could be formed. Therefore, two research questions are

formulated together that contain all the concepts that are discussed in this theoretical section.

The results of these questions together form a clear answer on the main question of this thesis:

RQ1: Is there a difference in the way interviewers exercise control over the form of the interview with anti-elite parties versus elite parties?

RQ2: Is there a difference in the way interviewers exercise control over the content of the interview with anti-elite parties versus elite parties?

Method

For this thesis, a quantitative content analysis was conducted of all comments made by

interviewers to politicians in two of the most frequently watched talkshows in the

3 On June 6, 2017, judges decided that Artikel1 could no longer use it’s name due to trademark problems. However, the party campaigned under the name Artikel1 and therefore this is the name that will be used in this thesis. For more information refer to

(13)

Netherlands, ‘Pauw & Jinek’ and ‘RTL Late Night’. With a combination of information and

entertainment, talkshows provide a platform for a broad public that has different interests. Not

only is there more time for questions and debate than, for instance, in the news, the goal of

providing both information as well as entertainment gives an interviewer the chance to ask

questions or make comments about more than only the political beliefs and plans of the

politicians (personal life, predictions about the outcomes of the elections). The interviews that

are studied mainly focussed on the campaigns and elections, so you can really say something

about the media logic within the context of the elections. In talkshows there is a lot of time for

interviewers to ask questions and for interviewees to answer them which, together with the

goal of providing both information as well as entertainment, gives an interviewer the

opportunity to ask questions about more than political beliefs (e.g. personal life, predictions

about the outcome of the elections). Together with the assumption that talkshows use a more

populist rhetoric by including the voice of normal people in the audience as well as at the

table (Blumler & Kavanagh, 1999; Blumler, 2001), this would indicate that talkshows are a

good study object when you want to study signs of media logic for different party types.

Both talkshows were frequently watched: Pauw en Jinek scored above one million

viewers almost every day, while both talkshow hosts in their own talkshows score 690.000

(Pauw) and 745.000 (Jinek) every day. RTL Late Night is a bit under that, but also scores

between 400.000 and 500.000 on average (Stichting Kijkonderzoek, 2017). In table 1, an

overview of the guests at both talkshows and the dates of broadcast is given.

Table 1 – Overview of politicians in RTL Late Night and Pauw & Jinek

Show

Date of

broadcast Politician (party) Anti-elite or elite party Pauw & Jinek 27-02-2017 Emile Roemer (SP)

Halbe Zijlstra (VVD) Henk Krol (50PLUS)

Anti-elite Elite Anti-elite Pauw & Jinek 28-02-2017 Alexander Pechtold (D66)

Jan Roos (VNL)

Elite Anti-elite

(14)

Table 1 – Overview of politicians in RTL Late Night and Pauw & Jinek - Continued

Show Date of broadcast

Politician (Party) Anti-elite or elite party

Pauw & Jinek 01-03-2017 Fleur Agema (PVV) Mona Keijzer (CDA)

Anti-elite Elite RTL Late Night 01-03-2017 Marianne Thieme (PvdD) Elite Pauw & Jinek 02-03-2017 Lodewijk Asscher (PvdA)

Sybrand Buma (CDA)

Elite Elite RTL Late Night 02-03-2017 Henk Krol (50PLUS)

Leonie Sazias (50PLUS)

Anti-elite Anti-elite Pauw & Jinek 03-03-2017 Kees van der Staaij (SGP)

Edith Schippers (VVD) Jesse Klaver (GroenLinks)

Elite Elite Elite Pauw & Jinek 06-03-2017 Mark Rutte (VVD)

Jan Marijnissen (SP)

Elite Anti-elite RTL Late Night 06-03-2017 Jesse Klaver (GroenLinks) Elite Pauw & Jinek 07-03-2017 Klaas Dijkhoff (VVD)

Kadija Arib (PvdA)

Elite Elite RTL Late Night 07-03-2017 Emile Roemer (SP) Anti-elite Pauw & Jinek 08-03-2017 Sylvana Simons (Artikel1)

Marianne Thieme (PvdD) Sharon Dijksma (PvdA)

Anti-elite Elite Elite RTL Late Night 08-03-2017 Sybrand Buma (CDA)

Wouter Bos (PvdA)

Elite Elite Pauw & Jinek 09-03-2017 Jesse Klaver (GroenLinks)

Jeanine Hennis (VVD) Emile Roemer (SP)

Elite Elite Anti-elite RTL Late Night 09-03-2017 Alexander Pechtold (D66) Elite Pauw & Jinek 10-03-2017 Ahmed Aboutaleb (PvdA)

Thierry Baudet (FvD) Gert-Jan Segers (CU)

Elite Anti-elite Elite RTL Late Night 10-03-2017 Jesse Klaver (GroenLinks) Elite Pauw & Jinek 13-03-2017 Alexander Pechtold (D66)

Sybrand Buma (CDA)

Elite Elite RTL Late Night 13-03-2017 Mark Rutte (VVD) Elite

(15)

Table 1 – Overview of politicians in RTL Late Night and Pauw & Jinek - Continued

Show Date of broadcast

Politician (Party) Anti-elite or elite party

Pauw & Jinek 14-03-2017 (15-03-2017)

Mark Rutte (VVD) Emile Roemer (SP) Alexander Pechtold (D66) Jesse Klaver (GroenLinks) Lodewijk Asscher (PvdA) Gert Jan Segers (CU) Kees van der Staaij (SGP) Jan Roos (VNL)

Sylvana Simons (Artikel1) Marianne Thieme (PvdD) Elite Anti-elite Elite Elite Elite Elite Elite Anti-elite Anti-elite Elite RTL Late Night 14-03-2017 Jan Marijnissen (SP)

Lillian Marijnissen (SP)

Anti-elite Anti-elite

Table 1 - Overview of political guests at RTL Late Night and Pauw & Jinek

Sampling

For this thesis, broadcasts with politicians in it, in the last three weeks prior to the Dutch

general elections of 2017, were included. The research units are comments made by the

interviewer, which include both questions asked by the interviewer as well as remarks made

by the interviewer.

Because the Pauw & Jinek talkshow was specially produced for the general elections

of 2017 in the Netherlands and started broadcasting two weeks before the election, every

broadcast of this show was included which is a total of twelve broadcasts of one hour each.

From RTL Late Night, every broadcast with a political interview in the context of the

elections was included, which is a total of nine broadcasts. As the time frame allowed this

whole population to be coded, no sample was drawn from the data. The data were collected

by using a codebook (see appendix) while watching the broadcasts on the website of the

Dutch public broadcaster4 and the website of the commercial broadcaster RTL5.

4

www.npo.nl

5

(16)

In this thesis, the research units are comments made by the interviewer, among which

are both questions asked by an interviewer as well as remarks made by an interviewer to

provoke a reaction of a politician. The research units will be referred to as comments. In total,

615 comments were coded.

Variables

The following variables were included in the codebook. First, descriptive variables like date,

show and political party of the politician were coded. The concept of media logic is measured

with two main categories of coding questions: influence on form and influence on content.

Influence on form consists of reaction time, type of comment and the presence of interrupting

comments. Influence on content consists of personalisation, horse race and negativity. The

coding instructions can be found in the appendix.

Type of comment

Three types of comments are distinguished: closed questions (1), limited choice questions (2),

open questions (3) and remarks (4). A closed question is a question on which a predefined

number of answers is available, like yes or no or ‘do you prefer X or Y’? In a limited choice

question, a few answers within a limited frame are possible. Those are questions like ‘how much?’, ‘why?’, ‘how long?’ (Voltmer & Brants, 2011). Open questions are questions where any answer is possible and the interviewee is not limited to a set range of possible answers,

like ‘what do you think about X?’ (Voltmer & Brants, 2011). The last category was used for

comments that could be identified as remarks. In total, 615 comments were made. Most of the

questions asked were closed questions (42,9%), followed by ‘remarks’ (28,7%), limited

(17)

Reaction time

The time that a politician gets to answer a question was coded in seconds, using a stopwatch.

If the politician was interrupted by anyone other than the interviewer (other guest at the table,

someone from the public), and after that continued with his or her reaction, this was counted

as one answer. The average reaction time of politicians was 29,19 seconds (SD = 28,96), with

a range between 0,35 seconds and 321,83 seconds.

Looking further at the division between parties, on average politicians from D66 took

the longest time to react (M = 37,28, SD = 25,77), and PVV got the least (M = 18,06, SD =

18,00). Politicians from both incumbent parties from the past cabinet, PvdA and VVD, were

invited the most: both parties were represented in the population with five politicians. On

average, GroenLinks received the most comments (n=72) and ChristenUnie (CU) the least

(n=14).

Interrupting comments

When the interviewer made a comment which ended the reaction time of the politician in an

unnatural way, by interruption, this variable was coded positive. In total 9,4% of the questions

ended with an interruption.

Personalisation

For this variable, first, a dummy variable is coded that indicates whether or not the question

contains signs of personalisation. If this is the case, three types of personalisation are

distinguished: individualisation (1), privatisation (2) and emotionalisation (3).

Individualisation is a focus on the professional or political qualities of the politicians, like

trustworthiness, integrity and competencies. Privatisation is a focus on the private life of the

politician, like his or her children, religious beliefs, relationships and hobbies.

(18)

and feelings (Van Santen and Van Zoonen, 2009; 2010). 16,0% of the questions showed signs

of personalisation in general. Within this number, the largest category was privatisation

(45,4%), followed by individualisation (30,9%) and emotionalisation (23,7%).

Horse race

For this variable, first, a dummy variable is coded that indicates whether or not the question

shows signs of horse race coverage. If this is the case, four types of horse race coverage are

distinguished: who is winning or losing the elections (1), opinion polls (2), approval or

disapproval from interest groups (3), speculations about electoral outcomes or coalitions (4).

In total, 15,3% of the questions showed signs of horse race coverage. Most of those were

speculations about electoral outcomes or coalitions (59,6%), followed by opinion polls

(36,2%) and ‘who is winning or losing the elections’ (4,3%). The category ‘approval or

disapproval from interest groups’ was included in the codebook (see appendix) but did not

appear in the population.

Negativity

A question was considered negative when the horse race or personalisation was present in a

negative way. Of the 615 questions, 181 were questions with signs of horse race and/or

personalisation. Of those, 11,6% was negative.

Elite parties vs. anti-elite parties

In this research, as explained in the theoretical framework, every party with a strong populist

and/or anti-establishment message is considered an anti-elite party. From parties of which

politicians were a guest in the coded talkshows, FvD, PVV, SP, VNL and 50PLUS fit this

description. This resulted in a division of parties that are presented in Table 2 (a more detailed

(19)

71,38% were comments made to politicians of elite parties, 28,62% were comments made to

politicians of anti-elite parties.

Table 2 - Division of political parties in elite and anti-elite parties

Elite parties Anti-elite parties

CDA D66 GroenLinks PvdA PvdD SGP VVD Artikel1 FvD VNL PVV SP 50PLUS Reliability

To test the reliability of the codebook, an intercoder reliability test (ICR) was conducted. To

do this, a guest coder coded the dependent variables for a sample (n=100) of the whole

population (N=615). The coded data of both coders are compared by reporting the scores for

percent agreement, Kappa and Krippendorff’s Alpha. The results can be found in Table 3.

As explained by De Swert (2012) a Krippendorff’s Alpha of > 0.8 is desired. However, as the variables are complex variables with a specific description, a value of > 0.6 is also acceptable (De Swert, 2012, pp. 5). The variables ‘personalisation’ and ‘type of personalisation’ have a Krippendorff’s Alpha between 0.6 and 0.7. A possible explanation is that personalisation is not as clearly noticeable as for instance horse race. The lower score can be low because this variable was only coded when a comment showed signs of personalisation and horse race. Because this variable is related to personalisation and horse race, the possibility of errors is higher than for other variables that are not related.

(20)

Table 3 - Intercoder reliability

Variable Description Level of measurement

Percent

agreement Kappa

Krippendorff’s Alpha

Party Party of the politician

Nominal 100% 0,953 0,953

Comment type Type of comment Nominal 84% 0,779 0,779

Personalisation Are there signs of personalisation in the comment? Nominal 85% 0,613 0,614 Type of personalisation Which type of personalisation? Nominal 78% 0,599 0,603

Horse race Are there signs of horse race in the comment?

Nominal 96% 0,811 0,811

Type of horse race

Which type of horse race?

Nominal 87% 0,750 0,755

Negativity Is the comment with signs of horse race or personality negative?

Nominal 93% 0,668 0,688

Analysis6

To analyse the data, some t-tests and chi square tests were conducted to compare the average

on every variable between elite and anti-elite parties. The descriptives of every variable can

be found in table 4 and table 5. In the results section, the most notable results are discussed.

Results

RQ1: Is there a difference in the way interviewers exercise control over the form of the interview with anti-elite parties vs. elite parties?

6 Because every political interview of both talkshows within three weeks prior to the Dutch general elections of 2017 is coded, no sample is drawn from the population. Therefore, statistical tests are not necessary, as every difference that is found between the data represents the entire population. However, to clarify the results that are found and to give an idea of the strength of the correlations, the statistical tests are still conducted and described in the result section.

(21)

The three variables used to code control over form are speaking time, type of comments and

number of interruptions. In Table 4, general descriptives of each variable can be found as well

as the differences between elite and anti-elite parties, including the statistical tests.

On average the answers of politicians of elite parties were longer compared to the answers of

anti-elite parties. However, this difference is not significant. A chi-square test for type of

comment does not indicate any dependency between the variable ‘type of comment’ as a whole and party type, χ2 (3) = 5,58, p = 0,134. Looking at the different comment types, the

differences between number remarks and party type and number of open questions and party

type appeared to be significant. There were more remarks made to anti-elite parties than to

elite parties, but more open questions asked to elite-parties than to anti-elite parties. Although

the percentages differ slightly between the closed questions that elite parties got and the

closed questions that anti-elite parties got, this difference is not very big and appeared to be

not significant. This also was the case for limited choice questions, as can be seen in Table 4.

Generally can be concluded that in this research, elite parties are asked more open questions

but fewer remarks than anti-elite parties.

The last indicator of control over form is the presence of an interrupting comment. On

average, 9,4% of the answers of politicians ended with an interrupting comment from the

interviewer. Although elite parties were interrupted more than anti-elite parties, this

(22)

Table 4 - Control over form Party Number of comments Number of politicians Number of comments per politician (M) Reaction time (total seconds) Reaction time (M (SD)) Interrupting comments (% within party) Closed questions (% within party) Limited choice questions (% within party) Open questions (% within party) Remarks (% within party) Artikel1 17 1 17 360,21 21,19 (16,92) 0% 52,9% 17,6% 0% 29,4% CDA 74 2 37 1979,55 26,75 (23,44) 6,8% 46,6% 24,7% 5,5% 23,3% CU 14 1 14 360,58 25,76 (26,89) 7,1% 64,3% 0% 7,1% 28,6% D66 49 1 49 1826,94 37,28 (25,77) 8,2% 42,9% 24,5% 12,2% 20,4% FvD 16 1 16 524,30 32,77 (21,08) 12,5% 56,3% 12,5% 0,0 31,3% GL 72 1 72 1865,73 25,91 (20,27) 6,9% 43,7% 22,5% 9,9% 23,9% PvdA 79 5 15,8 2396,60 30,34 (39,55) 15,2% 32,9% 26,6% 6,3% 34,2% PvdD 24 1 24 603,87 25,16 (17,20) 25,0% 33,3% 16,7% 8,3% 41,7% PVV 23 1 23 415,37 18,06 (18,00) 0% 60,9% 13,0% 0% 26,1% SGP 24 1 24 545,79 22,74 (13,31) 4,2% 62,5% 8,3% 0% 29,2% SP 66 3 22 2117,87 32,09 (28,70) 7,6% 43,9% 22,7% 6,1% 27,3% VNL 21 1 21 464,66 22,13 (14,36) 9,5% 23,8% 38,1% 0% 38,1% VVD 103 5 20,6 3555,34 34,52 (40,67) 8,7% 41,7% 28,2% 3,9% 26,2% 50PLUS 33 2 16,5 932,97 28,27 (19,60) 18,2% 30,3% 24,2% 0% 45,5%

Elite parties 439 17 25,8 13494,61 29,92 (31,00)A(1) 9,8%A(2) 42,8%A(3) 23,3%A(4) 6,6%A(5) 27,2%A(6)

Anti-elite parties 176 9 19,6 4451,37 27,36 (23,12)A 8,5%A 43,2%A 22,2%A 2,3%B 32,4%B Total 615 26 371,9 17946,78 29,19 (28,96) 9,4% 42,9% 23,0% 5,4% 28,7% 1 t (613) = 0,99, p = 0,218, 95% CI = [-2,52, 7,63] 2 t (613) = 0,49, p = 0,327 95% CI = [-0,04, 0,06] 3 t (613) = -0,87, p = 0,873, 95% CI = [-0,09, 0,08] 4 t (613) = 0,29, p = 0,564, 95% CI = [-0,06, 0,08] 5 t (613) = -0,27, p < 0,001, 95% CI = [0,00, 0,08] 6 t (613) = -1,25, p = 0,018, 95% CI = [-0,12, 0,03]

(23)

RQ2: Is there a difference in the way interviewers exercise control over the content of the interview with anti-elite parties vs elite parties?

The variables that were used to code control over content are personalisation, horse race and

negativity. General descriptives of each variable can be found in Table 5. On average, more

comments made to politicians from anti-elite parties contained signs of personalisation

(29,5%) than comments that were made to politicians from elite parties (10,5%). Although the

majority of the comments did not show signs of personalisation, this difference appeared to be

significant. This is also the case for personalisation; more anti-elite parties than elite parties

received personalised comments. Within the personalised comments asked to elite and

anti-elite parties, anti-anti-elite parties only scored higher on privatisation (60,8%) than anti-elite parties

(28,3%). This is a significant difference, which says that politicians from anti-elite parties are

asked more about their private life than politicians from elite parties.On the other types of

personalisation elite parties scored higher; 34,8% of the comments to elite parties contained

signs of individualisation versus 27,5% for the anti-elite parties, but this difference is not

significant. The difference between party type and emotionalisation on the other hand,

appeared to be significant: 37,0% of the comments made to elite parties contained signs of

emotionalisation versus 11,8% for the anti-elite parties, which says that elite parties are asked

more about their emotions and feelings in the talkshows interviews than anti-elite parties.

Elite parties and anti-elite parties did not significantly differ in the number of

questions that showed signs of horse race. When looking at the different types of horse race

coverage, chi-square is not significant, which means there is no clear relationship between

party type and the horse race types, χ2 (2) = 5,58, p = 0,134. On a more detailed level,

comments made to politicians from elite parties more often include speculations about

electoral outcomes or coalitions (63,6%) than is the case for comments made to politicians

(24)

more comments about who is winning or losing (4,3%) than anti-elite parties (4,2%), but this

difference was not significant as well. Anti-elite parties scored higher on comments that

referred to expressions of public opinion differed a lot: 31,8% of the horse race questions

asked to elite parties were about opinion polls, compared to 46,4% of the horse race questions

asked to anti-elite parties.

The third indicator for influence on content is negativity, which was coded for the

comments that contained signs of personalisation or horse race coverage, or both. Elite parties

were approached less in a negative way; 9,3% of the comments that were coded as

personalisation or horse race, were negative. For the anti-elite parties, this percentage was

(25)

Table 5 - Control over content Party Number of comments Number of politicians Personalisation (% within party) Individualisation (% of personalisation) Privatisation (% of personalisation Emotionalisation (% of personalisation) Horse race (% within party) Who is winning / losing elections (% of horse race) Opinion polls (% of horse race) Speculations about electoral outcomes or coalitions (% of horse race) Negativity (% of sum personalisation and horse race)

Artikel1 17 1 11,8% 50% 0% 50% 23,5% 0% 25% 75% 16,7% CDA 74 2 8,1% 33,3% 33,3% 33,3% 24,3% 5,6% 0% 94,4% 4,2% CU 14 1 0% 0% 0% 0% 14,3% 0% 50% 50% 0% D66 49 1 16,3% 37,5% 50% 12,5% 14,3% 0% 0% 100% 13,3% FvD 16 1 43,8% 42,9% 14,3% 42,9% 25% 0% 50% 50% 25% GL 72 1 14,3% 30% 20% 50% 9,7% 14,3% 42,9% 42,9% 12,5% PvdA 79 5 13,9% 36,4% 0% 63,6% 20,3% 0% 62,5% 37,5% 14,8% PvdD 24 1 0% 0% 0% 0% 8,3% 0% 50% 50% 0% PVV 23 1 43,5% 10% 80% 10% 8,7% 0% 100% 0% 0% SGP 24 1 12,5% 33,3% 66,7% 0 4,2% 0% 0% 100% 0% SP 66 3 18,2% 33,3% 66,7% 0% 16,7% 0% 36,4% 63,6% 0% VNL 21 1 28,6% 16,7% 66,7% 16,7% 14,3% 33,3% 0% 66,7% 42,9% VVD 103 5 7,8% 37,5% 37,5% 25% 12,6% 7,7% 46,2% 46,2% 5,6% 50PLUS 33 2 45,5% 28,6% 71,4% 0% 12,1% 0% 100% 0% 29,4%

Elite parties 439 17 10,5%A(1) 34,8%A(2) 28,3%A(4) 37%A(4) 15,0%A(5) 4,5%A(6) 31,8%A(7) 63,6%A(8) 9,3%A(9)

Anti-elite parties 176 9 29,5% B 27,5%A 60,8%B 11,8%B 15,9%A 3,6%A 46%A 50%A 15,1%B Total 615 26 16,0% 30,9% 45,4% 23,7% 15,3% 4,3% 36,2% 59,6% 11,6% 1 t (613) = -6,00, p < 0,001, 95% CI = [-0,25, -0,13] 2 t (95) = 0,77, p = 0,131, 95% CI = [0,11, 0,26] 3 t (95) = -3,36, p = 0,028, 95% CI = [-0,52, -0,13] 4 t (95) = 3,02, p < 0,001, 95% CI = [0,09, 0,42] 5 t (613) = -0,27, p = 0,588, 95% CI = [-0,07, 0,05] 6 t (92) = 0,21, p = 0,671, 95% CI = [-0,08, 0,10] 7 t (92) = -1,35, p = 0,055, 95% CI = [-0,36, 0,07] 8 t (92) = 1,23, p = 0,141, 95% CI = [-0,08, 0,36] 9 t (179) = -1,20, p = 0,018, 95% CI = [-0,15, 0,04]

(26)

Conclusion & Discussion

The aim of this thesis was to find an answer on the following question: How is media logic

visible for elite vs. anti-elite parties in the run up to the Dutch general elections of 2017 in the major talkshows? By conducting a quantitative content analysis on comments made by

interviewers of two popular Dutch talkshows, in political interviews during three weeks prior

to the elections, it was investigated to what extent media logic differed for elite parties and

anti-elite parties. This was done by looking at the indicators reaction time, interrupting

comment and comment type, which are part of control over form, and the indicators

personalisation, horse race and negativity, which are part of control over content. In this

paragraph, the conclusion and implications following from the results are given and some

limitations and suggestions for further research are discussed.

RQ1: Is there a difference in the way interviewers exercise control over the form of the interview with anti-elite parties vs. elite parties?

From the results, no clear conclusion can be drawn when looking at the ‘influence on form’ as

a construct. Looking closer to individual variables, answers of elite parties were longer than

answers of anti-elite parties. No clear difference was found for interrupting comments.

The clearest difference between anti-elite parties and elite parties can be found in the

type of comments. Although elite parties received more open questions, they received fewer

remarks than anti-elite parties. This is ambiguous: when an interviewer uses remarks or open

questions, the politician gets a chance to choose his or her own line of reasoning instead of the

interviewers, allowing the politician to choose its own frame. On the other hand, closed

questions limit the choice of the politician to choose their own line of reasoning, and so do

limited choice questions (to a lesser extent), which can indicate the presence of a media logic

(27)

RQ2: Is there a difference in the way interviewers exercise control over the content of the interview with anti-elite parties vs elite parties?

From the results, also no clear conclusion can be drawn when looking at ‘influence on content’ as a construct. On a more detailed level, especially the difference in personalisation between elite and anti-elite parties appeared to be important. Anti-elite parties scored higher

on privatisation than elite parties. This indicates that those parties are asked more about their

private life than elite parties. Furthermore, elite parties scored significantly higher on

emotionalisation, which indicates that they received more comments about their personal

emotions and feelings. Another finding is that anti-elite parties are approached in a more

negative way than elite parties. For both party types, no clear difference was found in

comments that contained signs of horse race.

Conclusion

Due to the sometimes contradictory and often not clear results, no clear conclusion can be

drawn in terms of a difference in media logic between anti-elite parties and elite parties.

Although elite parties scored higher on variables that indicate the absence of a media logic in

the way interviewers approach politicians (a longer reaction time, more open questions, less

personalisation and horse race), the majority of the results appeared to be not significant. In

the comparison between the two talkshows, it stood out that elite politicians received more

open questions but fewer remarks. From these results can be concluded that there are some

differences in how indicators of media logic are visible for elite versus anti-elite parties in the

run up to the Dutch general elections of 2017 in the major talkshows. However, based on the

results one cannot conclude that control over content or control over form is significantly

(28)

Discussion

An important limitation of this research is that it only focused on two talkshows. As explained

earlier, television is still one the most important source for news (Papathanassopoulos et al.,

2013). However, if you want to get a full understanding of the coverage of anti-elite and elite

parties and also want to include the younger generation and the important possibilities for

publicity that the current media landscape offers, it is important to also look at the way

politicians are covered on other popular forms of media on the internet like YouTube, news

websites and Snapchat. To get a broader view of the representation of anti-elite parties in the

Dutch media landscape it would be interesting to conduct this research with a sample that is

more representative for the entire Dutch media landscape. Also, as a suggestion for future

research, a longitudinal study in which the representation of anti-elite parties versus elite

parties in different time periods and different elections is researched, can give more insight

into how media logic is visible in interviews with elite and anti-elite parties. Going back to

the role conceptions of journalists (Plaisance & Skewes, 2003; Weaver & Brownlee, 2009;

Mellado, 2015) and looking at the results, not one role conception stood out. As stated in the

theoretical framework, the role conceptions are not mutually exclusive. The emphasis on the

private life and emotions of politicians fits within the audience approach role conception, but

other results, like the influence on type of comments, says something about power relations,

in which the open questions and remarks give a politician more freedom. This indicates

leaning towards the loyal-facilitator role rather than the watchdog role. However, it does not

explain why the results for elite parties and anti-elite parties were so contradictory in this area.

A big question that remains unanswered is why some indicators of media logic differ

for both party types. It appeared that the politicians of anti-elite parties who blame the media

for treating ‘them’ differently are partly right, but in this thesis also results are found that

(29)

research is needed to give a clear answer on this. A good way to start would be to look at the

role conceptions, as discussed in the theory (Plaisance & Skewes, 2003; Weaver & Brownlee,

2009; Mellado, 2015). If the motivations and role conceptions of journalists are more clear, it

can help to explain the difference that is found in how media logic is visible for anti-elite

parties and elite parties and provide more understanding about this subject. This thesis can be

(30)

References

Aalberg, T., Strömbäck, J., & De Vreese, C. H. (2012). The framing of politics as strategy and

game: A review of concepts, operationalizations and key findings. Journalism, 13(2),

162-178.

Barr, R. R. (2009). Populists, outsiders and anti-establishment politics. Party Politics, 15(1),

29-48.

Beam, R. A., Weaver, D. H., & Brownlee, B. J. (2009). Changes in professionalism of US

journalists in the turbulent twenty-first century. Journalism & Mass Communication

Quarterly, 86(2), 277-298.

Blumler, J. G. (2001). The third age of political communication. Journal of public affairs,

1(3), 201-209

Blumler, J. G., & Kavanagh, D. (1999). The third age of political communication: Influences

and features. Political communication, 16(3), 209-230.

Brants, K., & Van Praag, P. (2006). Signs of media logic half a century of political

communication in the Netherlands. Javnost-the public, 13(1), 25-40.

Bos, L., & Brants, K. (2014). Populist rhetoric in politics and media: A longitudinal study of

(31)

Bos, L., Van der Brug, W., & De Vreese, C. (2010). Media coverage of right-wing populist

leaders. Communications, 35(2), 141-163.

Broersma, M., Den Herder, B., & Schohaus, B. (2013). A question of power: The changing

dynamics between journalists and sources. Journalism Practice, 7(4), 388-395.

Chaffee, S., & Frank, S. (1996). How Americans get political information: Print versus

broadcast news. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 546(1),

48-58.

De Swert, K. (2012). Calculating inter-coder reliability in media content analysis using Krippendorff’s Alpha. Center for Politics and Communication, 1-15.

Esser, F. (2013). Mediatization as a challenge: Media logic versus political logic. In

Democracy in the Age of Globalization and Mediatization (pp. 155-176). Palgrave Macmillan UK.

Farnsworth, S. J., & Lichter, S. R. (2006). The 2004 New Hampshire Democratic primary and

network news. Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 11(1), 53-63.

Galtung, J., & Ruge, M. H. (1965). The structure of foreign news: The presentation of the

Congo, Cuba and Cyprus crises in four Norwegian newspapers. Journal of peace research,

2(1), 64-90.

Hallin, D. C. (1992). Sound bite news: Television coverage of elections, 1968–1988. Journal

(32)

Kleinnijenhuis, J., Van Hoof, A. M., & Oegema, D. (2006). Negative news and the sleeper

effect of distrust. Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 11(2), 86-104.

Lee, T. T. (2010). Why they don’t trust the media: An examination of factors predicting trust.

American Behavioral Scientist, 54(1), 8-21.

Lengauer, G., Esser, F., & Berganza, R. (2012). Negativity in political news: A review of

concepts, operationalizations and key findings. Journalism, 13(2), 179-202.

Mellado, C. (2015). Professional roles in news content: Six dimensions of journalistic role

performance. Journalism Studies, 16(4), 596-614.

Mazzoleni, G. (2003). The media and the growth of neo-populism in contemporary

democracies. The media and neo-populism: A contemporary comparative analysis, 1-21.

McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. L. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public

opinion quarterly, 36(2), 176-187.

Otjes, S., & Louwerse, T. (2015). Populists in parliament: Comparing left-wing and

right-wing populism in the Netherlands. Political Studies, 63(1), 60-79.

Papathanassopoulos, S., Coen, S., Curran, J., Aalberg, T., Rowe, D., Jones, P., ... & Tiffen, R.

(2013). Online threat, but television is still dominant: A comparative study of 11 nations’

(33)

Plaisance, P. L., & Skewes, E. A. (2003). Personal and professional dimensions of news

work: Exploring the link between journalists' values and roles. Journalism & Mass

Communication Quarterly, 80(4), 833-848.

Stamper, J., & Brants, K. (2011). A changing culture of political television journalism. In

Political Communication in Postmodern Democracy (pp. 111-125). Palgrave Macmillan UK.

Stichting Kijkonderzoek (2017). TV Kijkcijfers. Retrieved on 22 June 2017 from

https://kijkonderzoek.nl/kijkcijfers

Strömbäck, J. (2008). Four phases of mediatization: An analysis of the mediatization of

politics. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 13(3), 228-246.

Strömbäck, J. (2011). Mediatization and perceptions of the media's political influence.

Journalism Studies, 12(4), 423-439.

Takens, J., Van Atteveldt, W., van Hoof, A., & Kleinnijenhuis, J. (2013). Media logic in

election campaign coverage. European Journal of Communication, 28(3), 277-293.

Van Santen, R., & Van Zoonen, L. (2009). Stand van de wetenschap: Popularisering en

personalisering in politieke communicatie. Tijdschrift voor Communicatiewetenschap, 37(2),

155.

Van Santen, R., & Van Zoonen, L. (2010). The personal in political television biographies.

(34)

Voltmer, K., & Brants, K. (2011). A question of control: Journalists and politicians in

political broadcast interviews. In Political Communication in Postmodern Democracy (pp.

126-145). Palgrave Macmillan UK.

Zeh, R., & Hopmann, D. N. (2013). Indicating mediatization? Two decades of election

(35)

Appendix - Coding scheme

In this coding scheme, the unit of analysis is a comment that is made by an interviewer to a politician. Comments that are made by other guests at the table should not be included in this research.

A. General information A1. Date

Use the following notation: DD/MM/YY E.g.: 02/02/2017

A2. Show

-

Pauw & Jinek (1)

-

Nieuwsuur (2)

A3. Party

What is the political party of the politician?

-

Artikel1 (1)

-

CDA (2)

-

CU (3)

-

DENK (4)

-

D66 (5)

-

FvD (6)

-

GeenPeil (7)

-

GroenLinks (8)

-

NIEUWE WEGEN (9)

-

PvdA (10)

-

PvdD (11)

-

PVV (12)

-

SGP (13)

-

SP (14)

(36)

-

VVD (15)

-

VNL (16)

-

50PLUS (17)

-

Other (18)

B. Form of Comment

B1. Speaking time of politician vs. journalist

The length of the time a politician gets to answer to the comment, measured in seconds. This is the amount of seconds until the next comment is made by the interviewer, a video begins or an other part of the broadcast begins. The time that interruptions, like comments of others at the table and words spoken by the interviewer takes, should not be counted.

[Seconds]

B2. Type of comment

Closed question: In contrast, closed questions limit the range of possible answers, often to two clearcut alternatives. (…) Another variant of the closed question, ‘Do you prefer X or Y?’, equally limits the range of responses to distinct options” (Voltmer & Brants, 2011, pp. 134).

Limited choice question: “A type of question that provides more freedom, but still exerts limitations on the choices the interviewees can make to construct their own narrative. Examples of these are enquiries about ‘how much?’, ‘how long?’, ‘when?’ or ‘why?’” (Voltmer & Brants, 2011, pp. 134).

Open question: A question where any answer is possible. “Typical examples are ‘What do you think about X?’” (Voltmer & Brants, 2011, pp. 134).

Remark: Anything other than a question.

-

Closed question (1)

-

Limited choice question (2)

-

Open question (3)

(37)

B3. Did the reaction end with an interrupting comment?

This is the case when the reaction of the politician does not end in a natural way (when he or she is finished talking), but by an interruption. This is only the case when the politicians gets clearly interrupted and can’t finish a sentence.

- Yes (1) - No (0)

C. Content of comment

C1. Personalisation

Personalisation (Van Santen and Van Zoonen, 2009; 2010):

-

Individualistation: focus on professional/political qualities of a politician, like individual competences, integrity and trustworthiness

-

Privatisation: focus on private persona of a politician, like children, relationships, hobby’s etc.

-

Emotionalisation: focus on personal emotions of a politician, like personal experiences and feelings

-

Yes (1)

-

No (0)

C1A. If yes, which kind of personalisation?

-

Individualisation (1)

-

Privatisation (2)

-

Emotionalisation (3)

-

Other (4)

C2. Horse race

Horse race: When a comment contains an “expressions of public opinion (polls, vox pops); approval or disapproval from interest groups or particular constituencies or publics; or that speculate about electoral or policy outcomes or potential coalitions.”

(38)

- “Who is winning or losing elections, in the battle for public opinion, in legislative debates, or in politics in general”

- “Expressions of public opinion (polls, vox pops)”

- “Approval or disapproval from interest groups or particular constituencies or publics” - “Speculations about electoral or policy outcomes or potential coalitions” (Aalberg,

Strömbäck and De Vreese, 2011, pp. 172).

-

Yes (1)

-

No (0)

C2A: If yes; which type of horse race?

-

Who is winning or losing (1)

-

Opinion polls (2)

-

Approval or disapproval from interest groups (3)

-

Speculations about electoral outcomes or coalitions (4)

C3. Negativity

Negativity: When the individualisation or horse race frame is mentioned in a negative context, about losing instead of winning, making mistakes, talking about fights, lies etc.

-

Yes (1)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

We have also computed probabilities that an athlete from a certain weight category with certain world ranking position reaches a specific tournament round in a certain type

However, un- certainties regarding the range of the proton beam going through heterogeneous tissues, the interplay effect between the motion of the scanning beam and respiratory

begrotingstekort  en  de

Based on the fine grid and on the BenO Baseline schematisation &#34; beno13_5 &#34; , WAQUA subdomain models are created for the three River Rhine branches (Waal , Ijssel , Neder-Rijn

VGrtraagde Ako Gstic sc Tcrugvoering (V.. Die verskil tussen die me t ings van die normale spraakaspekt e en die van die spraakaspekte wat tydens V. op die

We begin with Figure 1 where we plot the mean rating given to the elite in both studies by experimental condition (top set of subgraphs) as well as the difference in

But, in the specific case of Awo, the Yoruba and Nigeria, the local dynamics that predetermine and condition the continued political life of a late hero can be explicated by

Since the Brit- ish were interested only in controlling the island and considered the well-established Franco-Mauritian elite a valuable asset, they allowed them to stay