• No results found

How does social distance and consumer involvement affect willingness to pay, when firms proactively disclose information?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "How does social distance and consumer involvement affect willingness to pay, when firms proactively disclose information?"

Copied!
53
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

                                                                Name:         R.M.L.  Brackel       Student  nr:     10704698    

Institution:     Amsterdam  Business  School     Study:         MSC  Business  Studies  

    Specialization:   Marketing  

    Course:     Master  Thesis       Supervisor:     J.  Demmers  MSc       Date:         21-­‐01-­‐2015          

“How  does  social  distance  and  consumer  involvement  affect  

willingness  to  pay,  when  firms  proactively  disclose  information?”  

                 

(2)

Statement  of  originality    

This  document  is  written  by  Romy  Brackel  who  declares  to  take  full  responsibility  for   the  contents  of  this  document.    

I  declare  that  the  text  and  the  work  presented  in  this  document  is  original  and  that  no   sources  other  than  those  mentioned  in  the  text  and  its  references  have  been  used  in   creating  it.    

The  Faculty  of  Economics  and  Business  is  responsible  solely  for  the  supervision  of   completion  of  the  work,  not  for  the  contents.    

                                 

(3)

Table  of  content  

Abstract                 page  4  

Introduction                 page  5-­‐7  

Contributions               page  8  

Literature  review               page  9-­‐18   Research  method               page  19-­‐24  

Results                 page  25-­‐39  

Conclusion,  discussion  and  limitations         page  40-­‐42   References                   page  43-­‐50   Appendix                 page  51-­‐53  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

(4)

Abstract  

The  purpose  of  this  research  is  to  extend  the  research  field  on  transparency  in  

marketing  in  this  so-­‐called  “age  of  transparency”.    Nowadays,  consumers  claim  to  need   for  transparency  by  the  brands,  but  more  knowledge  is  required  on  how  and  if  

transparency  actually  also  holds  positive  benefits  for  the  company  when  disclosing   information.  Companies  find  it  hard  to  make  information  available  at  such  a  fast  pace   without  running  risks  of  losing  customers  due  to  possible  disclosure  of  negative  

information.  This  study  explored  whether  companies  should  engage  in  these  marketing   transparency  efforts,  whether  brand  disclosure  compared  to  consumer  disclosure  and   non-­‐disclosure  actually  increases  consumer  involvement  and  willingness  to  pay  (H1,   H2)  and  tries  to  establish  first  findings  on  how  social  distance  could  possibly  influence   this  relationship  (H3)  with  business  to  consumer  products  in  advertising.  Literature  on   social  distance  explains  why  consumers  overvalue  what  is  socially  close  and  undervalue   what  is  socially  distant;  therefore  a  low  social  distance  is  expected  to  increase  

willingness  to  pay  and  consumer  involvement.  A  total  of  five  pre-­‐tested  advertisements   were  designed  for  the  experiment.  In  the  conditions  a  distinguish  was  made  between   transparent  and  non-­‐transparent,  brand  disclosure  and  consumer  disclosure,  high  and   low  social  distance  to  measure  the  effect  on  willingness  to  pay  and  consumer  

involvement.    

Results  show  that  transparency  actually  did  not  increase  willingness  to  pay  nor  increase   the  consumer  involvement.  Furthermore  a  low  neither  high  social  distance  increased   willingness  to  pay  or  consumer  involvement.  Overall,  this  experiment  provided  no  proof   of  positive  benefits  for  being  transparent.    

(5)

1.  Introduction  “The  Age  of  Transparency”  

Fournier  and  Avery  (2011)  call  this  “the  age  of  transparency”.  Consumers  demand   authenticity,  more  convenient  access  to  relevant,  comprehensive,  quality  and  true   information.  Nowadays,  there  are  more  educated  customers  with  higher  needs,  they   have  become  more  conscious  about  their  society  and  environment;  there  is  a  clear  shift   to  more  sustainable  products/services  in  the  market  (Bhaduri  and  Ha-­‐Brookshire,   2011).  According  to  Cohn  and  Wolfe  (2013),  customers  value  corporate  transparency.   Around  70%  considers  transparency  and  openness  in  purchase  decisions,  and  is  the   most  important  factor  following  price  and  quality.  

Originally  demand  for  transparency  started  off  in  the  financial  market  more  than  a   decade  ago  (Walter,  2004).  To  be  able  to  make  proper  investment  decisions  investors   wanted  more  transparent  information  about  the  financial  data  of  the  company  (Walter,   2004),  this  increased  need  for  information  was  caused  by  the  global  financial  crisis  of   2008  which  created  a  general  distrust  towards  companies  by  consumers  (Kirby,  2012).       The  annual  financial  reports  are  a  key  performance  indicator  for  investors,  they  base   their  analysis  on  these  reports  whether  to  invest  in  the  company  or  not  (Perrini  and   Tencati,  2006).  

The  need  for  transparency  moved  towards  a  global  concern  and  has  developed  rapidly   across  more  product  and  service  categories  over  the  years  and  it  has  serious  

implications  for  major  private  and  public  sectors  organizations  (Cohn  and  Wolfe,  2013).   For  example  in  the  media,  the  safety  of  food  in  the  supply  chain  and  working  conditions   in  apparel  industry  have  become  an  increasingly  interesting  issue  (Bhaduri  and  Ha-­‐ Brookshire,  2011;  Latvala,  2010).  Due  to  the  need  for  transparency  companies  share   more  information  than  ever  before,  but  information  transparency  may  reveal  

(6)

being  in  conflict  about  what,  when  and  how  much  they  should  actually  disclose   (Fournier  and  Avery,  2011).    

Consumers  can  attribute  claims  either  to  the  advertiser's  desire  to  sell  the  product  or  to   actual  characteristics  of  the  product  such  as  by  communicating  that  a  low  percentage   goes  to  the  farmer  who  produced  the  potato  in  the  Netherlands.  Such  inclusion  of   negative  information  leads  the  consumer  to  think  that  the  advertiser  is  telling  the  truth   and  additionally  this  enhances  the  perception  of  advertiser  credibility  (Eisend,  2006).   But  according  to  the  Information  Integration  Theory  consumers  may  perceive  

information  too  negative.  According  to  this  theory  the  information  can  be  perceived  as   important  and  unfavorable  and  this  could  be  a  main  reason  why  companies  find  it  hard   to  make  information  available  at  such  a  fast  pace  without  running  risks  of  losing  

customers  due  to  possible  negative  information.    

Furthermore,  due  to  technological  developments  the  number  of  customer  touch  points   skyrocketed  (Edelman,  2010).  These  developments  created  the  ability  for  companies  to   get  the  consumer  actively  involved  with  the  brand  more  easily.  This  requires  new  skills   and  capabilities  of  the  firm,  also  for  the  downsides  that  come  with  the  digital  age.     Because  of  commonly  used  social  network  sites  negative  or  positive  information  can  be   instantly  exposed  across  the  Web  and  can  have  major  consequences  for  the  brand  equity   (Fournier  and  Avery,  2011).  Therefore,  it  becomes  harder  to  conceal  information  from   consumers,  for  many  companies  managing  these  resources  became  part  of  risk  

management  (Carter  and  Rogers,  2008;  Peppers  and  Rogers,  2013).  Companies  started   to  conceal  the  information  themselves  and  even  negative  information.  Literature  by   Eisend  (2006)  on  two-­‐sided  communication  shows  that,  under  specific  circumstances,   disclosure  of  unfavorable  information  can  positively  influence  consumer  preferences.  By   disclosing  negative  information  they  might,  perceive  truthfulness  and  source  credibility,  

(7)

therefore  trigger  attention,  could  strengthen  attitudes  by  countering  mild  attacking   arguments,  and  finally  result  in  more  favorable  brand  attitudes  leading  to  higher   purchase  intentions.    This  study  defines  transparency  as  “a  situation  in  which  business   and  financial  activities  are  done  in  an  open  way  without  secrets,  so  that  people  can  trust   that  they  are  fair  and  honest”  (Cambridge  Business  Dictionary  online,  2014).    This  study   focuses  on  the  effect  of  transparency  in  marketing  communication  by  the  firm  on  

increased  willingness  to  pay.  Especially,  if  social  distance  could  possibly  influence  the   relationship  of  transparent  marketing  and  increase  willingness  to  pay.  Brands  such  as   Starbucks  are  putting  transparent  marketing  into  practice  for  a  couple  of  years  already.   Starbucks  communicates  anything  they  do  on  social  media  sites,  here  consumers  like   and  respond  more  positive  to  posts  on  Facebook  that  relate  to  people  in  their  own   country  or  community  like:  “Great  program…  love  that  you  are  investing  in  the  

community”,  as  compared  to  for  example  when  Starbucks  is  helping  farmers  in  Africa   the  Facebook  post  is  less  liked  and  has  much  fewer  positive  responses  like:  “How  kind  of   Starbucks  supporting  these  Africans,  but  what  about  the  “Create  jobs  in  the  USA  

program?!”  (Starbucks,  2015).  This  could  be  explained  by  the  social  distance,  which  is   part  of  the  construal  level  theory,  a  leading  theory  on  how  more  distant  events  

(geographically,  socially  etc.)  are  differently  evaluated  because  they  are  differentially   construed  (Trope  and  Liberman,  2003).    This  different  evaluation  is  explained  by   whether  the  affected  person  or  social  group  is  socially  close  or  distant.  The  framework   for  the  psychological  distortions  and  biases  of  consumers,  explains  the  bias  in  the  cost-­‐ benefit  analysis  i.e.  why  consumers  overvalue  what  is  socially  close  and  undervalue   what  is  socially  distant.  Which  therefore  could  imply  that  customers  are  willing  to  pay   more  when  transparent  marketing  with  a  low  social  distance  is  communicated  

(8)

2.  Contributions  

This  research  contributes  scientifically  to  a  better  understanding  of  the  underlying   psychological  process  when  consumers  experience  transparent  marketing.  Recent   studies  demonstrate  that  transparent  marketing  increases  purchase  intentions.     Measuring  ways  how  the  transparency  effect  could  become  even  more  valuable  in  the   eyes  of  the  consumer,  like  with  low  or  a  high  social  distance,  would  clarify  tactical   transparency  issues  for  the  company  even  more.    

Defining  the  effect  of  social  distance  and  consumer  involvement  contributes  on  how   messages  can  be  shaped  to  further  increase  willingness  to  pay.    When  understanding  the   effect  on  willingness  to  pay  when  consumers  encounter  transparency  in  a  high  or  low   social  distance  setting  and  whether  the  consumers  became  more  or  less  involved  with   the  product  after  being  exposed  to  a  certain  social  distance,  this  may  facilitate  effective   formulated  transparent  marketing  communication  to  increase  purchase  intentions.  It   may  help  fulfill  the  consumer  need  for  transparency  on  a  more  in-­‐depth  level,  within  the   marketing  context.  Concluding  the  results  of  this  study  should  define  in  what  ways   transparent  communication  is  most  appealing  according  to  consumer  behavior.  

 

 

 

 

 

 

(9)

3.  Literature  review  

3.1  Transparent  marketing  increases  willingness  to  pay    

A  firm  can  choose  to  proactively  disclose  information  on  different  types  of  transparency   such  as,  supply  chain,  organizational,  cost  and  technical  transparency  (Hultman  and   Axelsson,  2007).  But  the  major  issue  for  companies  when  applying  the  transparency   tactic  is  the  risk  of  losing  customers  because  the  information  is  perceived  as  too   negative.  For  example,  by  clarifying  how  many  calories  a  meal,  like  a  hamburger   contains,  might  influence  product  choice  or  willingness  to  pay  negatively.  This   consequence  is  explained  by  the  Information  Integration  Theory  (Anderson,  1971),   according  to  IIT  attitudes  are  formed  and  changed  based  on  each  piece  of  information   having  two  qualities;  value  and  weight.  The  weight  is  the  information’s  perceived   importance  of  the  information  and  the  value  of  a  bit  of  information  is  its  evaluation,   whether  it  is  favorable  or  unfavorable.  By  sharing  the  number  of  calories  in  a  hamburger   the  weight  can  be  of  high  importance  and  the  value  of  the  information  is  unfavorable.   This  is  an  example  for  why  companies  find  it  hard  to  make  information  available  at  such   a  fast  pace  without  running  risks  of  losing  customers  due  to  possible  negative  

information.  On  the  other  hand  transparency  can  turn  out  to  have  a  positive  effect  due  to   gained  knowledge  about  the  product  in  the  eye  of  the  consumer.  Recently  conducted   studies  suggest  that  the  proactive  disclosure  of  relevant  information  by  a  firm  in   marketing  communication  (transparent  marketing)  has  a  positive  effect  on  product   choice  and  consumer  willingness  to  pay  (Demmers  et  al.,  2014).    

Moreover,  a  study  by  Carter  and  Curry  (2010)  on  supply  transparency  suggests  that   consumers  do  not  always  follow  classic  economic  theory  i.e.  always  choose  the  lowest   price,  whatever  the  allocation  of  money  in  the  supply  chain  may  be,  therefore  

(10)

consumers  prefer  a  higher  allocation  to  the  sympathic  agent  (for  example  the  artist  who   made  the  album)  and  therefore  often  choose  paying  a  higher  price  when  it  is  disclosed   that  more  money  would  be  allocated  to  the  sympathic  agent  in  the  supply  chain.  Hence,   there  is  strong  evidence  that  transparency  does  add  value  in  the  eye  of  the  consumer,   even  if  they  have  to  pay  a  premium  price  for  it,  which  reflects  an  increased  willingness   to  pay.    

This  holds  even  when  the  information  is  not  unambiguously  positive.  Moreover,  because   the  firm  proactively  discloses  the  information  itself,  in  comparison  to  disclosure  by   fellow  consumers,  this  leads  to  higher  perceptions  on  both  the  relevance  and  positivity   of  the  information  (Demmers  et  al.,  2014).  These  findings  suggest  that  a  firm  can  

positively  influence  willingness  to  pay  and  product  choice  of  (potential  new)  consumers   by  proactively  disclosing  information.  Also  by  voluntarily  disclosing  partially  negative   information  the  brand  is  perceived  as  being  honest  and  telling  the  truth  and  hereby   increasing  credibility  of  the  brand  in  the  eye  of  the  consumer.  According  to  the  stealing   thunder  strategy  it  is  better  to  disclose  information  yourself  and  gain  credibility  for  it   before  others  such  as  consumers  find  out,  expose  the  information  and  cause  negative   publicity.  Therefore  willingness  to  pay  would  only  increase  when  the  company  itself   instead  of  a  consumer  disclosing  the  information.    

H1:   Willingness  to  pay  is  higher  (lower)  when  a  firm  (consumer)  discloses  information.              

(11)

3.2  The  role  of  social  distance  in  explaining  consumer  behavior  

 

While  Demmers  et  al.  (2014)  explain  the  effect  of  transparent  marketing  resulting  in   increased  willingness  to  pay,  however  there  might  be  factors  that  moderate  and  mediate   this  effect.      

Social  distance  is  a  psychological  dimension  of  the  Construal  Level  Theory  that  describes   the  relation  between  psychological  distance  to  the  extent  of  how  abstract  or  concrete  an   object  is  being  thought  of  by  people.  According  to  CLT  the  closer  the  object  is  the  more   concrete  it  will  be  thought  of  (low  level  construal)  like  focusing  on  the  details.  In   contrast  to  the  more  distant  the  object  is  the  more  abstract  it  will  be  thought  of  (high   level  construal)  by  for  example  looking  at  the  overall  situation  (Trope  and  Liberman,   2011).  Opposed  to  an  objective  distance  between  two  persons,  social  distance  is  the   psychological  distance  between  two  (groups  of)  people.  Social  distance  is  defined  as   “The  perceived  or  desired  degree  of  remoteness  between  a  member  of  one  social  group  and   the  members  of  another,  as  evidenced  in  the  level  of  intimacy  tolerated  between  them”   (Oxford  Dictionaries,  2015).  

 When  framing  a  message  a  high  social  distance  reduces  the  positivity  because  a  low   social  distance  e.g.  in-­‐groups  are  perceived  as  more  positive  than  a  high  social  distance   e.g.  out-­‐groups  (Liberman,  Trope  and  Wakslak,  2007).  Assuming  consumer  prefer  a  low   social  distance  to  the  disclosed  information.  Additionally  according  to  Dhar  and  Kim   (2007)  a  construal  manipulation  does  affect  the  ability  to  process  the  message  abstract   or  concrete  and  eventually  affects  the  decision-­‐making.  Therefore  the  difference  in   construal  level,  high  or  low,  affects  people  consideration  when  purchasing  items.   This  study  will  be  looking  at  if  a  low  social  distance  does  actually  increase  consumer   involvement  and  willingness  to  pay  with  transparency.  Little  research  has  been  done  in  

(12)

the  field  of  the  persuasiveness  of  social  distance  dimension.  However,  examples  can  be   derived  from  some  studies,  although  they  are  in  a  different  context.    

For  example  Sargeant  et  al.  (2004)  studied  the  relation  between  social  distance  and   motive  to  donate  to  charity.    Results  show  that  people  intent  to  choose  a  charity  to   donate  to,  which  they  received  profit  from  in  the  past  or  believe  that  they  will  profit   from  it  in  the  future.  This  means  it  is  part  of  a  direct  and  personal  experience  referred  to   as  a  near  social  distance.  Clearly,  according  to  Sargeant  et  al.  (2004)  this  does  not  only   apply  to  themselves  it  also  applies  relatives,  loved  ones  and  friends  also  compared  to  a   low  social  distance.  Assuming  transparent  information  makes  them  better  off  because  of   gained  knowledge  about  how  the  donation  to  charity  might  profit  them  personally  or   people  closely  related  to  them  (like  people  within  their  community),  both  a  low  social   distance  then  the  low  social  distance  frame  would  be  more  persuasive  than  the  high   social  distance  frame.  Therefore  consumers  might  intent  to  choose  the  product  over   others  and  be  willing  to  donate  more.    

Additionally  Park  (2010)  explored  when  psychological  distance  would  have  a  

moderating  effect  on  evaluation.  The  results  demonstrate  that  psychological  distance   only  influences  when  there  is  either  a  high  need  for  cognition  or  when  individuals   carefully  think.  Possible  explanations  as  suggested  by  Trope  and  Liberman  (2003)  are   that  it  may  be  that  psychological  distance  influences  the  likelihood  of  elaboration.  While   the  focus  on  high  social  distance  encourages  contextual  elaboration,  low  social  distance   encourages  elaboration  of  goal  relevant  information  (Petty  and  Cacioppo,  1986).       In  comparison  to  Sargeant  et  al.  (2004)  and  Park  (2010)  findings,  Nan  (2007)  found   contradicting  results  using  the  social  distrance  frame.  Nan  (2007)  used  a  high  vs.  low   social  distance  frame,  called  the  societal-­‐individual  frame,  to  translate  the  social   distance  into  a  message.  This  frame  is  based  on  compliance  (or  not)  towards  the  

(13)

community  (Nan,  2007).  An  example  of  a  high  social  distance  frame  is:  “Taking  public   transit  instead  of  driving  a  car  for  daily  commute  provides  cleaner  air  for  people  in  your   community”  (Nan,  2007,  p.  493).  The  low  social  distance  frame  is  based  on  compliance   (or  not)  towards  the  individual  (Nan,  2007).  An  example  of  the  low  social  distance  frame   is:  “Taking  public  transit  instead  of  driving  a  car  for  daily  commute  provides  cleaner  air   for  you”  (Nan,  2007,  p.  494).    The  results  of  the  study  did  not  show  that  the  low  social   distance  frame  is  more  persuasive  than  the  high  social  distance  frame.  On  the  contrary,   she  found  that  the  high  distance  frame  was  more  persuasive  than  the  low  distance  frame   when  the  social  entity  was  distant.  Moreover,  according  to  the  study  inaccurate  results   could  be  caused  by  inadequate  statistical  power  searching  for  small  effects  (Braker,   2013).  In  addition  to,  this  being  the  only  study  that  investigated  this  frame  and  therefore   more  research  is  necessary  to  make  statements  about  the  persuasiveness  of  the  societal-­‐ individual  frame.  This  study  will  continue  with  the  social  distance  frame  of  Nan  (2007)   in  a  different  context.  Although  Park  (2010)  proved  how  psychological  distance  

moderates  product  evaluations  Nan  (2007),  showed  the  exact  opposite  results  in   product  evaluations.    

The  assumption  of  this  study  is  the  opposite  of  what  has  been  found  here,  low  social   distance  (individual  frame)  will  be  more  persuasive  than  the  high  social  distance   (societal  frame).  And  that  when  consumers  are  confronted  with  a  low  social  distance   framed  message  (vs.  high  social  distance  framed  message)  they  are  willing  to  pay  a   higher  price  for  the  product  as  is  conceptualized  in  figure  1.  This  assumption  is   supported  by  Hofstede’s  Individualism-­‐Collectivism  dimension  (2001)  where  The   Netherlands  scores  high  on  individualism.  An  individualistic  country:  believes  that   nothing  is  more  important  than  the  individual  and  therefore  the  individual  framework   referred  to  as  socially  close  would  be  more  persuasive  (Triandis,  1995).    

(14)

 

   

  Figure  1.  Conceptual  model  transparent  marketing    

 

CLT  defines  several  types  of  psychological  distances;  temporal  distance,  spatial  distance,   hypothetical  distance  and  social  distance.    

According  to  Kim  Li  and  Zhang  (2008),  when  two  dimensions  of  psychological  distance   (i.e.  temporal  -­‐  and  social  distance)  are  involved  in  an  event  (i.e.  product)  they  

interactively  influence  the  evaluations  of  an  event.  Results  show  when  both  of  the  two   dimensions  are  proximal,  the  event  will  be  perceived  as  proximal  (close)  and  construed   at  low-­‐level  construals.  However,  when  either  one  of  the  dimensions  is  psychologically   distal  (far),  the  event  will  be  perceived  as  distal  and  construed  at  high-­‐level  construals.   So  evaluations  are  more  consistent  with  the  value  associated  with  low-­‐level  construals   when  both  dimensions  are  proximal  and  when  either  one  dimension  is  distal  evaluations   are  more  consistent  with  the  value  associated  with  high-­‐level  construals.    

The  theory  describes  that  when  the  object  happens  to  a  socially  close  person  or  group   the  more  concrete  it  will  be  thought  of,  while  when  the  object  happens  to  a  more  socially   distant  person  or  group,  it  will  be  thought  of  more  abstract.  

(15)

This  means  that  when  confronted  with  high-­‐level  construals,  such  as  socially  distant,   consumers  would  simultaneously  prefer  abstract  information.  While  when  consumers   are  confronted  with  low-­‐level  construals,  such  as  socially  close,  they  would  prefer   concrete  information  as  shown  in  figure  2  (Trope  and  Liberman,  2003;  Kim  and  John,   2008).    Suggesting  that  transparent  information  dimension  makes  the  message  more   proximal  because  this  information  is  considered  more  concrete  than  abstract  thus   would  be  more  consistent  with  low-­‐level  construals  and  the  transparent  information   communication  should  be  more  effective  when  aligned  to  the  low  level  construal   characters.                        

  Table  2.  Distinguishing  high-­‐level  and  low-­‐level  construals  (Trope  and  Liberman,  2003,  p.  405)    

  Figure  2.  High  and  Low  level  construal  characters      

It  is  necessary  to  further  explore  the  influence  of  social  distance  on  purchase  intentions   made  with  transparent  marketing.  Especially,  which  message  context  is  better  to  deliver   to  increase  purchase  intentions.  This  may  offer  implications  for  marketers,  they  can   benefit  from  considering  for  whom  and  how  to  write  marketing  context,  abstract  vs.   concrete,  in  designing  transparent  marketing  communications  (Park,  2010).      

 

 

High-­‐level  construals   Low-­‐level  construals  

Abstract   Concrete  

Simple   Complex  

Structured,  coherent   Unstructured,  incoherent  

Decontextualized   Contextualized  

Primary,  core   Secondary,  surface  

Superordinate   Subordinate  

(16)

3.3  The  role  of  consumer  involvement  in  explaining  consumer  

behavior  

The  level  of  consumer  involvement  in  explaining  consumer  behavior  is  playing  an   important  role;  the  level  of  consumer  involvement  can  be  explained  as  the  crucial  factor   influencing  buying  decisions  (Bennet  et  al.  2005).  The  most  accepted  definition  of   involvement  by  researchers  is  that  they  agree  that;  the  level  of  involvement  is  related  to   the  perceived  personal  relevance  of  a  certain  product  for  the  consumer  (Homburg  and   Giering,  2001;  Knox  et  al.,  1994).  In  other  words,  the  level  of  involvement,  high  or  low,   indicates  how  important  a  product  and  the  consequences  of  its  purchase  are  for  the   individual.  Petty  and  Cacioppo  (1981)  explore  how  involvement  is  a  potential  moderator   in  consumer  behavior.  The  results  of  the  study  suggested  that  the  content  factor  of  the   message  is  more  influential  than  for  example  source  characteristics  in  high  involvement   conditions.  

Around  a  decade  ago  researchers  were  exploring  the  psychology  underlying  consumer   involvement,  as  companies  shifted  to  viewing  customers  as  co  creators  of  value  instead   of  just  recipients  (Firat  et  al.,  1995;  Holbrook  and  Hirschman,1982;  Prahalad  and  

Ramaswamy  2000,  2002;  Vargo  and  Lusch  2004).  Now  companies  are  actively  involving   the  customers  in  marketing,  testing  and  design  of  their  products  and  services  (Lengnick-­‐ Hall  1996).  The  results  of  Norton  et  al.  (2010),  demonstrate  how  involving  customers  in   the  creation  of  Ikea  boxes,  can  change  a  low  involved  customer  into  a  high  involved   customer  leading  to  a  higher  willingness  to  pay.    

       

(17)

The  results  of  Park  (2010)  on  the  moderating  role  of  psychological  distance  suggest  that   socially  close  vs.  distant  only  influences  under  the  condition  of  thoughtful  thinking.   Which  leads  us  to  the  elaboration  likelihood  model  by  Petty  and  Cacioppo  (1986),  they   provide  a  well-­‐documented  model  to  explain  this  underlying  psychological  process.   There  are  two  distinct  routes  to  attitude  change:  the  central  –  and  the  peripheral  route.   The  central  route  views  attitudes  change  as  resulting  from  a  cognitive  consideration  of   information  that  is  core  to  what  people  feel.  The  peripheral  route,  views  attitudes   change  because  the  attitude  object  has  been  associated  with  either  positive  or  negative   cues  or  a  simple  decision  rule  to  evaluate  a  communication  is  used,  so  influence  on   attitude  without  issue-­‐relevant  thought  occurring.    

By  analyzing  through  the  elaboration  likelihood  model  whether  the  person  is  motivated   and  has  the  ability  to  think  about  the  communication  to  which  he  or  she  is  exposed,  one   could  conclude  whether  they  process  the  communication  centrally  or  peripherally.     According  to  Trope  and  Liberman  (2011)  people  who  are  confronted  with  low  level   construals  people  are  more  likely  to  follow  the  peripheral  route  to  attitude  change,  by   focusing  on  the  present  with  great  detail  and  think  of  the  advertisement  more  

concretely.  Whereas  when  people  are  engaged  in  high  level  construals  they  are  more   likely  to  follow  the  central  route  to  attitude  change,  by  looking  at  the  bigger  picture  and   think  of  the  advertisement  more  abstractly.  

Furthermore,  Apsler  and  Sears  (1968)  designed  a  method  to  manipulate  involvement   through  manipulating  the  personal  relevance.  In  this  experiment  the  first  group  of   participants  were  told  that  a  persuasive  plan  had  personal  implications  for  them  or   people  in  their  community,  which  can  be  compared  to  a  low  social  distance.  The  second   group  of  participants  was  told  that  the  persuasive  plan  had  no  direct  implications  to   them  or  people  in  their  community  in  the  near  future,  referred  to  a  high  social  distance.    

(18)

Results  show  that  first  group  of  people  were  more  highly  involved  as  compared  to  the   second  group  being  low  involved.    

Therefore  consumers  may  become  more  involved  through  concrete  transparent   information  communicated  with  low  social  distance  information  compared  to  as  more   abstract  and  high  social  distant  information.    By  manipulating  the  personal  relevance,  a   high  vs.  low  social  distance,  we  can  measure  the  increase  or  decrease  in  involvement   with  the  product.  

Also  manipulating  the  social  distance  to  a  low  distance  with  the  sympathic  agent  will   lead  to  a  higher  involvement  and  eventually  willingness  to  pay.  As  the  results  of  Carter   and  Curry  (2010)  suggest,  the  consumer  is  higher  involved  with  the  product  when  the   sympathic  agent  is  disclosed.  The  respondents  preferred  a  higher  allocation  to  the   sympathic  agent  and  were  willing  to  pay  a  higher  price  for  the  product  when  more   money  would  be  allocated  to  this  agent.    

   

H2:   The  effect  of  disclosure  source  (H1)  is  mediated  by  consumer  involvement,  such  that   the  recipient  of  the  information  is  more  (less)  involved  when  information  is  proactively   disclosed  by  the  firm  (disclosed  by  a  fellow  consumer).    

 

H3:   The  effect  of  disclosure  source  on  consumer  involvement  (H2)  is  moderated  by   social  distance,  such  that  the  recipient  of  the  disclosed  information  is  more  (less)  involved   when  social  distance  between  the  recipient  of  the  information  and  the  beneficent  in  the   disclosed  information  is  low  (high).  

 

 

(19)

4.  Research  method  

4.1  Method  

The  main  objective  of  this  research  is  to  explore  the  role  of  social  distance  under  the   condition  of  firms  proactively  disclosing  information  in  their  marketing  communications   using  a  deductive  approach.  By  having  explored  secondary  data  on  the  effects  of  social   distance  and  consumer  involvement  on  consumer  behavior,  propositions  were  derived   from  them.    

Accurate  secondary  data,  which  was  already  easily  available  and  therefore  cost-­‐effective,   was  to  find  existing  knowledge  on  the  topic  before  conducting  the  experiment.  

Primary  data  is  gathered  through  a  consumer  experiment  to  test  if  the  effects  of   transparent  marketing  on  WTP  were  mediated  by  consumer  involvement  and  

moderated  by  social  distance.  A  consumer  experiment  is  one  method  of  assessing  the   effect  of  social  distance  and  consumer  involvement.  Firstly,  by  having  manipulated  the   social  distance  (socially  close  vs.  socially  distant,  the  independent  variable)  one  is  able   to  measure  the  moderating  effect  on  the  causal  relationship  of  transparent  marketing   increasing  WTP  (the  dependent  variable)  as  is  shown  in  figure  1  (Saunders  et  al.,  2008).     Secondly,  by  looking  at  the  effect  of  transparent  marketing  on  the  level  of  involvement   one  can  test  a  possible  mediating  effect  in  the  IV-­‐DV  relationship  as  is  shown  in  figure  1.   High  involvement  may  have  further  increased  the  WTP.      

The  combination  of  the  transparency  advertisement  experiment  and  the  5-­‐item  scale   increased  the  reliability  of  findings.  To  increase  construct  validity  of  the  findings  a   pretest  was  executed  on  social  distance,  source  disclosure,  and  positivity  of  information   and  clarity  of  advertisements  used  in  general.  The  pretest  made  it  possible  to  make  sure   that  the  5  advertisement  conditions  used  would  represent  an  accurate  image  to  what   they  were  supposed  to  measure.  

(20)

4.2  Veylinx  

Veylinx  is  an  online  bidding  platform  used  to  gather  data  on  consumer’s  actual  

willingness  to  pay  in  a  convenient  way.  The  participants  of  the  experiment  are  existing   members  of  Veylinx,  the  database  is  build  up  from  people  voluntarily  subscribing  the   online  bidding  platform  for  several  reasons;  general  interest,  willing  to  help  others  and   winning  gifts  after  joining  the  online  bidding.  All  existing  members,  around  3200,  were   used  to  randomly  select  1000  individuals  for  the  experiment;  done  by  the  consumer   research  company  Veylinx.  The  database  yields  sufficient  variance  and  therefore  a   presents  a  representative  sample.  Furthermore,  by  collecting  around  950  participants   for  the  experiment  the  data  has  a  high  probability  to  be  quantified  and  therefore  develop   more  generalizable  findings.  These  Veylinx  members  were  approached  by  e-­‐mail  on  the   basis  of  simple  random  sampling  within  the  group  members  of  Veylinx,  the  response   rate  was  around  95%.    

The  choice  for  these  respondents  is  appropriate  to  the  extent  that  the  online  bidding   platform  provides  the  best  opportunity  measuring  the  willingness  to  pay.  Veylinx   measures  what  people  do,  not  what  people  say,  it  will  reveal  what  they  really  want  to   pay  for  the  product,  this  is  not  measuring  intentions  and  opinions  it  is  actual  behavior.   Veylinx  uses  Vickrey  auction  to  measure  this  actual  behavior,  here  bidders  submit  their   bid  in  euros  and  the  highest  bidder  pays  the  second  highest  bid  (Ausubel  and  Milgrom,   2006).  The  bidders  cannot  see  the  bid  of  other  people.  So  eventually  consumers  

participate  in  the  auction  by  placing  one  bid  in  euros,  subsequently  they  answer  a  few   questions  about  their  involvement  with  the  product.  

 

 

(21)

4.3  Stimuli  

4.3.1.  The  Product    

The  product  that  was  auctioned  online  at  Veylinx  was  a  luxury  brand  chocolate  bar.   Since  2005,  Tony’s  Chocolonely,  Dutch  chocolate  bar  company,  was  the  first  to  introduce   a  so-­‐called  ‘slave-­‐free  chocolate  bar’  with  a  fair-­‐trade  logo,  which  they  later  on  changed   to  ‘the  road  to  100%  slave-­‐free  chocolate’  due  to  legal  issues.  Tony’s  Chocolonely  buys   directly  buying  from  cocoa  farmer  and  they  establish  a  long-­‐term  relationship  with   them,  besides  they  source  Fair  Trade  ingredients  and  organic  products  as  much  as   possible.  The  chocolate  bar  chosen  was  a  large  milk  chocolate  bar  because  this  is  the   most  popular  flavor  and  therefore  more  recognizable  and  appealing  to  a  larger  group  of   respondents.  According  to  Beulens  et  al.  (2005),  modern  consumers  demand  products   that  are  healthy,  safe  and  consistent  high  quality,  demanding  guarantees  for  food   characteristics.  Consumers  expect  transparency  and  effective  response  if  a  problem   arises,  as  a  result  businesses  are  forced  to  comply  with  the  governmental  and  consumer   demands.  Because  of  information  exchange  throughout  the  food  supply  chain  the  origin   and  added  value  of  food  products  can  be  communicated  and,  thus,  become  more  

transparent  (Beulens  et  al.,  2005).  This  fair-­‐trade  Tony’s  Chocolonely  bar  was  chosen   because  it  allowed  disclosure  of  information  on  the  fixed  percentage  that  goes  to  the   sympathic  agents,  who  are  the  producers  of  the  product  in  this  situation  the  farmers  in   Africa  and  the  Netherlands.  Tony’s  Chocolonely’s  organic  milk  is  produced  by  farmers  in   the  Netherlands  (West-­‐Friesland)  and  cacao  is  produced  by  farmers  in  West-­‐Africa   (Ghana,  Ivory  coast)  (Tony’s  Chocolonely  annual  report,  2013).  Usually  this  information   is  not  shared  on  the  packaging  of  food  and  beverages  likewise  with  the  chocolate  bar   from  Tony’s  Chocolonely.    

(22)

4.3.2.  The  experiment  conditions    

The  experiment  will  be  performing  five  different  conditions  of  ads  in  total,  where  social   distance  is  manipulated  to  a  high  and  a  low  social  condition.  And  brand  disclosure  is   manipulated  to  a  brand  disclosure  and  a  Veylinx  consumer  disclosure  condition.  The  last   condition  is  the  non-­‐transparent  condition,  which  is  the  control  group.  So,  in  total  the   ads  are  separated  into  the  five  following  conditions,  first  the  brand  disclosure  with  high   social  distance,  secondly  brand  disclosure  with  low  social  distance,  thirdly  consumer   disclosure  with  high  social  distance  and  fourthly  consumer  disclosure  with  low  social   distance  towards  the  respondent  and  finally  the  control  group.    

 

The  first  hypothesis,  if  willingness  to  pay  is  higher  when  a  firm  discloses  information   will  be  measured  through  a  non-­‐transparent  advertisement  condition,  which  is  the   control  group,  comparing  to  the  advertisements  with  disclosed  information.  Veylinx  is   used  to  measure  the  differences  in  willingness  to  pay  between  both  (non)  transparent   conditions  presented  by  the  brand  compared  to  the  consumer  to  test  if  transparency  by   the  brand  is  valued  more  than  when  transparency  is  presented  by  a  customer.    

In  addition  to  the  advertisements  shown  on  the  online  bidding  platform.  Furthermore  it   is  important  to  measure  information  transparency  with  negative  information  revealed,   because  disclosing  positive  information  is  easy  for  the  company  and  has  a  high  chance  of   already  being  perceived  more  valuable  and  liked  by  the  customer  as  compared  to  

disclosing  something  negative.  Therefore  the  information  transparency  should  be   perceived  negative  to  be  able  to  measure  a  strong  transparency  effect.  The  Revised   Product  Involvement  Inventory  (RPII)  is  used  to  measure  hypothesis  2,  if  effect  of   disclosure  source  (H1)  is  mediated  by  consumer  involvement,  such  that  the  recipient  of   the  information  is  more  (less)  involved  when  information  is  proactively  disclosed  by  the  

(23)

firm  (disclosed  by  a  fellow  consumer).  This  5-­‐item  scale  measure  has  a  limited  amount   of  questions  and  is  derived  from  the  original  PII,  therefore  respondents  are  less  likely  to   be  irritated  due  to  the  length  of  the  interview  compared  to  the  PII,  which  consists  of  20-­‐ items  (Zaichkowsky,  1985;  McQuarrie  and  Munson,  1992).  The  items  are  scored  on  7-­‐ point  scales  on  the  following  constructs:  

1.  Unimportant  –  important.  

2.  Means  a  lot  to  me  –  means  nothing  to  me.   3.  Matters  to  me  –  does  not  matter  

4.  Significant  –  insignificant  

5.  Of  no  concern  to  me  –  of  concern  to  me  

The  5-­‐item  scale  will  appear  after  the  bidding  took  place  to  be  able  to  find  out  the   difference  between  the  levels  of  involvement  between  all  three  conditions.  

This  questionnaire  works  with  a  defined  schedule  of  questions,  which  are  tested   scientifically,  which  makes  the  research  more  reliable  and  credible.    The  defined  set  of   questions  from  which  the  interviewer  should  not  deviate,  makes  it  easy  to  standardize   the  research  method  (Saunders  et  al.,  2008).    

To  be  able  to  measure  the  third  hypothesis,  if  the  effect  of  disclosure  source  on  

consumer  involvement  (H2)  is  moderated  by  social  distance,  such  that  the  recipient  of   the  disclosed  information  is  more  (less)  involved  when  social  distance  between  the   recipient  of  the  information  and  the  beneficent  in  the  disclosed  information  is  low   (high),  we  must  first  develop  an  easy  and  applicable  message  frame  for  both  high  and   low  social  distance.    

The  transparent  advertisement  communication  will  be  primarily  based  upon  the  

message  context  as  is  used  in  the  Nan  (2007)  study.  The  first  transparent  condition  ad  is   socially  close  presented  by  the  brand  with  the  following  text:  “5%  of  the  profit  goes  to  the  

(24)

farmer  in  West-­‐Friesland”.    The  second  transparent  condition  ad  is  socially  close  

presented  by  the  customer:  “I  love  the  taste  of  the  chocolate,  in  the  meanwhile  5%  of  the  

profit  goes  to  the  farmer  in  West-­‐Friesland”.  

The  third  transparent  condition  ad  is  socially  distant  presented  by  the  brand:  “5%  of  the  

profit  goes  to  the  farmer  in  West-­‐Africa”.  

The  fourth  transparent  condition  ad  is  socially  distant  presented  by  a  customer:  “I  love  

the  taste  of  the  chocolate,  in  the  meanwhile  5%  of  the  profit  goes  to  the  farmer  in  West-­‐ Africa”.  The  advertisements  used  in  the  experiment  can  be  found  in  appendix  1.    

                                 

(25)

5.  Results    

5.1  Pre-­‐test  

A  pre-­‐test  was  undertaken  to  find  out  if  the  5  conditions  used  would  represent  an   accurate  image  to  what  they  were  supposed  to  measure.  The  pre-­‐test  helps  to  identify   problems  with  the  advertisements  used  that  otherwise  would  have  led  to  biased   answers.  The  survey  was  conducted  through  online  survey  tool  Qualtrics.    

Thirty-­‐four  respondents  were  asked  to  indicate  if  the  information  on  the  allocation  of   the  percentage  to  the  sympathic  agent  was  too  low  or  too  high  on  a  6-­‐point  scale.  This   made  it  possible  to  measure  if  the  disclosed  information  was  perceived  as  something   negative  or  positive.    The  respondents  were  also  asked  to  indicate  whether  the  

advertisement  message  was  clear  to  them  on  a  5-­‐point  Likert  scale,  from  totally  disagree   to  totally  agree.  Furthermore  they  were  asked  to  answer  if  the  disclosed  information   was  given  by  the  brand  or  not  by  the  brand.  So  whether  it  was  brand  disclosure  or   disclosure  by  a  consumer,  this  determinates  the  brand  disclosure  source.  Finally  the   respondents  were  asked  to  indicate  the  social  distance,  by  several  scale  questions  from   1  to  7  (i.e.  1=  feels  close  to  me/  7=  feels  far  from  me  and  I  identify  myself  the  most  with   and  the  least  with  scales)  throughout  the  pre-­‐test.  Respondents  could  indicate  whether   the  West-­‐Friesian  Dutch  farmer  or  the  West-­‐African  farmer  was  more  socially  close  to   them.    

A  one-­‐way  ANOVA  was  conducted  to  determine  the  significance  in  outcomes  on  social   distance  scales.  The  findings  reveal  there  was  a  statistically  significant  effect  of  social   distance  between  the  respondent  and  the  Dutch  farmer  and  the  respondent  and  the   African  farmer.  People  identified  themselves  significantly  more  with  the  Dutch  farmer   than  the  African  farmer,  F(1,  66)  =  90,5,  p  <.05  (table  1).  Post  hoc  comparisons  using  the   Tukey  HSD  test  indicated  that  the  mean  score  for  the  low  social  distance  condition  (M  =  

(26)

2.38,  SD  =  1.25)  was  significantly  different  than  the  high  social  distance  condition  (M  =   5.29,  SD  =  1.26).  Concluding  the  results  showed  that  the  respondents  felt  a  low  social   distance  with  the  Dutch  sympathic  agent  condition  and  a  high  social  distance  with  the   African  sympathic  agent  condition.    

 

Table  1.  One-­‐way  ANOVA  of  relevance  means  per  social  distance  between   identification  with  two  farmers  

  Sum  of  Squares   df   Mean  Square   F   Sig.  

Between  Groups   144.132   1   144.132   90.521   .000  

Within  Groups   105.088   66   1.592      

Total   249.221   67        

 

Overall  the  transparent  information  provided  was  perceived  negative  in  all  transparent   information  conditions,  this  was  analyzed  through  frequencies  run  on  answers  

categorized  in  groups  of  positive  and  negative  answers  to  the  question  in  table  2  and  3.    

Table  2.  Frequencies  of  5%  to  African  sympathic  agent  is  perceived  positive  or   negative  

  Frequency   Percent   Valid  Percent   Cumulative   Percent  

Valid   Positive   8   23.5   23.5   23.5  

Negative   26   76.5   76.5   100.0  

Total   34   100.0   100.0    

(27)

Table  3.  Frequencies  of  5%  to  Dutch  sympathic  agent  is  perceived  positive  or   negative  

  Frequency   Percent   Valid  Percent   Cumulative   Percent  

Valid   Positive   12   35.3   35.3   35.3  

Negative   23   67.7   67.7   100.0  

Total   34   100.0   100.0    

 

All  advertisement  conditions  were  clear  and  understood  by  the  respondents  as  is  shown   in  the  descriptive  analysis  table  4,  a  mean  of  above  4  on  a  Likert-­‐scale  from  1  (totally   disagree)  to  5  (totally  agree).  A  one-­‐way  ANOVA  was  conducted  to  determine  the  

significance  in  outcomes  between  the  clearness  of  different  advertisement.  The  findings   reveal  there  was  no  statistically  significant  effect  between  any  of  the  advertisements   F(2,  98)  =  .086,  p  >.05  (table  5).    This  means  they  agree  with  the  statements  that  all  of   the  different  conditions  of  advertisements  shown  are  clear  to  them.  

Table  4.  Descriptives  of  comprehensibility  of  advertisement  

  N   Minimum   Maximum   Mean   Std.  Deviation  

Clear     non  transparent     34   1   5   4.09   .866   Clear  consumer   disclosure   34   1   5   4.12   .913   Clear  brand   disclosure   34   1   5   4.15   .821   Valid  N  (listwise)   34            

(28)

 

Furthermore  respondents  were  asked  to  look  if  the  advertisement  was  communicated   by  the  brand  or  the  consumer.  Disclosure  by  the  brand  was  much  more  clear  to  the   respondent,  with  percentage  values  of  88.2  and  97.1,  as  compared  when  it  was  

communicated  by  the  consumer  in  table  6,  still  64.7%  understood  that  the  information   was  given  by  the  consumer  instead  of  the  brand  itself.  The  data  was  analyzed  using  chi-­‐ square  goodness  of  fit  test,  the  difference  in  these  proportions  between  disclosure  by   brand  as  compared  to  disclosure  by  customer  is  significant  X²  (1,  N=101)  =  13.35  P<.05.   Overall  the  pre-­‐test  led  to  the  results  that  the  advertisements  proposed  where  valid  and   therefore  measures  what  needed  to  be  measured.      

 

Table  6.  Descriptives  of  source  disclosure  (consumer)    

The  information  is  given  by  Tony's  Chocolonely?  (No)  

  Frequency   Percent   Valid  Percent   Cumulative  

Percent   Valid   Yes   12   35.3   35.3   35.3   No   22   64.7   64.7   100.0   Total   34   100.0   100.0      

 

 

 

Table  5.  One-­‐way  ANOVA  of  relevance  of  means  between  comprehensibility  of   conditions/ads    

  Sum  of  

Squares  

df   Mean  Square   F   Sig.  

Between   Groups  

.129   2   .065   .086   .918  

Within  Groups   73.673   98   .752      

(29)

5.2  The  experiment  

The  actual  experiment  was  conducted  with  Veylinx  on  Friday  the  14th  of  November  

2014.  Around  a  1000  voluntary  subscribers  received  e-­‐mail  to  join  the  online  bidding  for   the  Tony’s  Chocolonely  bar  and  during  that  day  a  total  of  950  respondents  made  a  bid   and  answered  the  consumer  involvement  questions  after  the  bidding.  This  includes  the   192  respondents  who  bid  zero  eurocents  for  the  product.  To  following  headings  present   the  results  obtained  from  the  experiment.    

 

5.3  Demographics  

The  experiment  consisted  of  five  conditions;  each  condition  was  presented  to  around   190  respondents,  which  makes  a  total  of  950  respondents.  From  the  total  of  950   respondents  462  were  female  and  488  were  male  respondents.    Of  which  the  age   differed  between  114  years  old  (this  may  have  been  a  typo  by  a  respondent)  and  16   years  old,  with  an  average  age  of  41  years  old.  Additionally  a  total  of  154  of  the  

respondents  indicated  to  be  a  full-­‐time  student  at  that  moment  in  time.  Moreover  483   respondents  were  higher  educated  (HBO  and  WO  degree  according  to  the  Dutch   education  system).    Overall  the  average  bids  for  one  chocolate  bar  measured  in  SPSS   were  143,50  eurocents  (table  7a,  7b).  The  highest  bid  was  153.42  eurocents  for  the   transparent  condition  with  low  social  distance.  

         

(30)

Table  7a.  Average  bid  amount  per  treatment  in  SPSS  

Bid  amount      

Condition   Mean   N   Std.  Deviation  

Control   134.30   187   112.398   BrandHighS   142.87   195   121.928   BrandLowS   153.42   180   113.379   ConsLowS   136.22   198   113.957   ConsHighS   151.38   190   107.172   Total   143.50   950   113.939    

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

However, a few researchers have suggested that a team members’ perception of having high status that is seen as legitimate by other team members, can have wide-ranging effects

Taking Africa’s positive economic and welfare developments into consideration it is interesting to research whether increased Chinese involvement has contributed to

The Effect of Thematic Frames on Attribution of Responsibility in the European Multi-level Government: The Moderating Role of the Scale Frame and Political Sophistication

MalekGhaini, “Effect of friction stir welding speed on the microstructure and mechanical properties of a duplex stainless steel,” Materials Science and

Specifically, the aim of this study was to expand the literature on the topic of trivial product attributes, by investigating consumers’ willingness to pay, including the

Nederland past echter een lagere vrijstelling voor buitenlandse belasting op grond van de objectvrijstelling toe in de situatie dat een activum vanuit een Nederlands hoofdhuis

Een opvallend resultaat is dat als een onderneming meer bezittingen heeft (hogere total assets), de beloning lager zou zijn. Verschillen kunnen mogelijk verklaard

In general, the studies on the acoustics of discrete basic emotions (e.g., Banse and Scherer [12], Murray and Arnott [123]) seem to provide a consistent view, except for a