• No results found

The influence of the exaggeration of vocal emotional expressions on the subjective emotion experience

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The influence of the exaggeration of vocal emotional expressions on the subjective emotion experience"

Copied!
29
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

The Influence of the Exaggeration of Vocal Emotional Expressions on the

Subjective Emotion Experience

Name: Sebastiaan Sarton

Student card no.: 10515275

Supervisor: Disa Sauter

2nd Assessor: Lisanne Pauw Department: Social Psychology

(2)

2 Abstract

Studies about emotion regulation have shown mixed results regarding how people experience

emotions subjectively when they exaggerate their emotional responses. Although there are clear

differences between the results of studies using physiological measures and studies using self-report

measures, contradictory findings of studies using self-report measures might also have originated

from limitations of their particular research designs. The present study tried to replicate the findings

of an earlier exaggeration study that used self-report measures, but clearly improved on the

limitations of said study. Additionally, the present study explored the question whether the

exaggeration of vocal emotional expressions is able to influence the subjective emotion experience.

Based on the results of earlier studies it was hypothesized that people would experience increased

arousal and increased emotional intensity when they exaggerate their vocal emotional response to

stimuli eliciting positive emotions, but not to stimuli eliciting negative emotions. Participants were

asked to exaggerate their vocal response to film clips eliciting amusement and disgust (exaggeration

condition), or just watched the film clips as they would naturally (control condition). Afterwards

they had to fill in two self-report questionnaires that measured arousal and emotional intensity. No

differences between the exaggeration and control conditions were found for amusement and disgust.

(3)

3 Introduction

Emotion regulation is an important aspect of interpersonal communication and social functioning.

In the case of interpersonal conflicts, emotional suppression is often an appropriate strategy to avoid

escalation and to work towards solving the conflict at hand. Another form of emotion regulation is

emotional exaggeration, which can be an appropriate strategy to use when you, for example, want to

show your sympathy towards your co-worker who has gotten a flat tire while driving to work.

Emotion regulation has been defined by Gross (1998b) as “the processes by which

individuals influence which emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience

and express these emotions.” (p. 275). A lot of research has been done in regard to the regulation of

emotions which encompasses several subfields of psychology (e.g., Gross, 1998b; Philippot &

Feldman, 2004). Some experiments have focused specifically on the subjective emotion experience

when people modulate their emotional response by the means of suppression, reappraisal or

exaggeration. The present study will explore the question whether people experience more arousal

and increased emotional intensity when they exaggerate their emotional response to stimuli eliciting

discrete emotions. The present study will also try to extend the findings of earlier studies by

assessing the role of vocal emotional expressions, instead of asking participants to modulate their

facial emotional response which is typically done in exaggeration research (e.g. Strack, Martin &

Stepper, 1988; Demaree, Schmeichel, Robinson, & Everhart, 2004).

Some interesting results have been found regarding how people experience their emotions

subjectively when they regulate their emotions by exaggerating their emotional responses. Higher

levels of arousal seem to be a feature of emotion regulation in general. Studies using physiological

measures have consistently found that people experience more arousal when exaggerating

(Demaree et al., 2004; Demaree, Schmeichel, Robinson, Pu, Everhart, & Berntson, 2006;

Schmeichel, Demaree, Robinson, & Pu, 2006) and suppressing (e.g. Gross & Levenson, 1993,

1997; Gross, 1998a; Richards & Gross, 1999) their responses to emotion eliciting stimuli.

(4)

4

exaggerating and suppressing emotional responses on experienced emotional intensity, by the

means of physiological measures (Jackson, Malmstadt, Larson, & Davidson, 2000). Participants

had to suppress, maintain or enhance (exaggerate) their emotional response to high arousal eliciting,

highly unpleasant picture slides. Higher startle eye blink magnitude and facial muscle (corrugator

supercilii) activity was found in the enhancement condition. According to the authors, this indicated

that the exaggeration of an emotional response led to an even more unpleasant experience while

viewing the picture slides, compared to the neutral condition. Additionally, participants showed

lower scores on the two measures in the suppression condition, compared to the neutral condition.

For this matter, suppression and exaggeration seem to be opposites of each other in terms of their

influence on emotional intensity: suppression caused lower levels of experienced negative affect,

while exaggeration caused higher levels of experienced negative affect, in comparison to the neutral

condition. These results are interesting, because they do give an indication about the effect of

emotion exaggeration on how emotions are being experienced subjectively. Additionally, their study

shows the difference between exaggerating emotional responses and suppressing emotional

responses in how they shape the subjective emotion experience.

However, it could be argued that physiological measures are not able to capture what a

person feels while experiencing an emotion. Although physiological measures indicate that they

would, do people really feel more aroused, and do people really feel increased emotional intensity

when exaggerating their emotional response? Emotional experiences contain a subjective

component that can probably be measured more effectively with self-reports (Wallbott & Scherer,

1989). Additionally, physiological measures are only able to assess general positive or negative

emotional intensity, when looking at the Jackson et al. (2000) study. Physiological measures are as

far as is known not capable to show if people also experience an increased intensity of a specific

discrete emotion. According to Gross and Levenson (1995) “a discrete emotional response has

occurred when a participant reports feeling a single emotion more intensely than 15 other positive

(5)

5

stimuli, they developed a self-report questionnaire which measures the intensity of 16 discrete

emotional states. In addition to that, they also developed stimuli (film clips cut from existing films,

both commercial and non-commercial) which were able to elicit discrete emotional states in at least

78 percent of their sample. Their self-report questionnaire and their stimuli have been used in

several studies about emotion regulation.

According to the results of physiological measures, one could expect that people would also

self-report feeling increased arousal and feeling discrete emotions more intensely, while

exaggerating their emotional response. However, the results of self-report measures have not been

as clear cut, and have sometimes been inconsistent with the findings of physiological measures.

Several studies have found that exaggerating facial expressions does not lead to more self-reported

emotional intensity and self-reported arousal in response to negative affective stimuli, such as

disgust-eliciting stimuli (Demaree et al., 2004; Demaree, Schmeichel, Robinson, Pu, Everhart, &

Berntson, 2006; Schmeichel, et al., 2006). Also, a decrease in self-reported emotional intensity has

not been found in response to stimuli eliciting disgust and sadness in suppression studies (Gross &

levenson, 1993, 1997). Schmeichel et al. (2006) concluded that the exaggeration as well as the

suppression of facial expressions might be ineffective at changing the subjective experience of

negative affective states, as well as the intensity of negative discrete emotions. Gross and Levenson

(1997) have provided a plausible reason for these findings that has yet to be tested. Because people

seem to control their negative emotions to a much larger degree than positive emotions (Wallbott &

Scherer, 1989), they might have had more experiences of disjunctions between their emotional

experience and their emotional expressions in the case of negative emotions. This might lead people

to infer their subjective emotion experience less from their negative emotional expressions,

compared to their positive emotional expressions which are being controlled to a much lesser

extent. However, this possible explanation seems to ignore the fact that results regarding negative

emotions have been found with physiological measures. At present it is unclear where the

(6)

6 emotion experience stem from.

In the case of positive discrete emotions such as amusement it has been found that

exaggerating or suppressing emotional expressions does lead to increases or decreases in

self-reported emotional intensity, respectively (Strack et al., 1988; Gross & Levenson, 1997). Another

study included both positive and negative discrete emotions (amusement and disgust) to explore

exaggeration as an emotion regulation strategy (Demaree et al., 2004). While watching film clips

eliciting amusement or disgust, participants were asked to either exaggerate their emotional

reaction, or to relax and watch the film clips naturally. According to their self-report measures, the

film clips did not elicit the intended discrete emotions, but did elicit general positive and negative

affective states. Their study did not find an increase in self-reported emotional intensity in the

exaggeration condition, contradicting earlier findings. Additionally they also did not find an

increase in self-reported arousal in the exaggeration condition, which contradicts earlier findings

because heightened arousal seems to be a prominent feature of emotion regulation. These results

were found in both positive and negative affective states. Their lack of results can possibly be

explained due to the fact that their study was seriously underpowered: the between participants

design did only use 13 participants per condition. Also, no other study than the Demaree et al.

(2004) study has looked at self-reported arousal when people exaggerate positive emotions.

Because limitations of their research design are probably the reason for the lack of results, it is still

possible that people do experience more arousal and higher emotional intensity when exaggerating

positive emotions.

An important point to address is that most research in the area of the subjective experience

of emotion regulation has been focused on facial expressions in response to emotion eliciting

stimuli. In some studies participants' facial expressions have been used as the sole measure of

subjective experience and concurrently as a manipulation check (Gross & Levenson, 1993; Gross &

Levenson, 1997; Gross, 1998a; Richards & Gross, 1999). In addition to that, participants in some

(7)

7

Demaree, Robinson, Pu, & Allen, 2006; Demaree, Schmeichel, Robinson, Pu, Everhart, &

Berntson, 2006; Schmeichel et al., 2006), after which their facial expressions were also rated.

However, there has not been a lot of research about the subjective experience of emotion regulation

that takes other nonverbal communicative channels into account.

Facial expressions are an important, but of course not the only nonverbal communicative

channel from which emotional signals can be interpreted. Another important channel concerning

nonverbal emotion communication is the voice. The voice communicates verbal as well as

nonverbal information (Frank, Maroulis & Griffin, 2013). In addition to verbal information, the

voice also communicates acoustic properties, such as loudness and pitch. It has been found that both

vocal emotional expressions and facial emotional expressions are able to convey information about

the senders' emotion experience (Zuckerman, Larrance, Spiegel & Klorman, 1980). This should

enable an observer to assess experienced emotional intensity while only paying attention to the

vocal channel. Being able to convey information from the vocal emotional channel exclusively is a

requirement for the research paradigm employed by the present study. Strictly speaking, in similar

suppression and exaggeration studies facial emotional intensity displayed by the participants had to

be rated as a manipulation check.

Additionally it could be possible that the exaggeration of vocal expressions does also lead to

people experiencing their emotions more intensely. Gross and Levenson (1993) expected that the

facial feedback hypothesis (Buck, 1980) might be one of the driving forces behind the finding that

people experience their emotions as less intense when they suppress them. Support has also been

found for a similar vocal feedback hypothesis (Hatfield, Hsee, Costello, Schalekamp Weisman, &

Denney, 1995). In their study participants had to reproduce sound patterns, of which the voice

quality, the rhythm, the intonation and the pausing corresponded to feelings of joy, love, sadness,

anger and fear. They developed the sound patterns with a group of 20 communication experts. The

stimuli were validated, finding that the emotions corresponding to the sound patterns were being

(8)

8

experimental condition were abstract and therefore left the participants blind to the purpose of the

study. A control condition was included in which the participants had to reproduce a neutral drone

sound. The results indicated that the participants experienced more positive affect when reproducing

joy and love sound patterns and more negative affect when reproducing sadness, anger and fear

sound patterns compared to the control condition. These findings on vocal emotional expressions

suggest that the exaggeration of said expressions would be able to shape the subjective emotion

experience, at least when people are being exposed to positive emotional stimuli. However, based

on previous exaggeration and suppression research, it is unlikely that the same would be the case

when people are being exposed to negative emotional stimuli.

The present study explored the same hypothesis regarding self-reported emotional intensity

as the Demaree et al. (2004) study, but did improve on its limitations concerning the design and the

materials. The present study used a mixed design, with at least 25 participants per condition, in

which every participant has been exposed to two of the four conditions. The participants would

either receive instructions to watch the amusement and the disgust film clips as they naturally

would, or to exaggerate their emotional response to both film clips. Demaree et al. (2004) did

clearly not use film clips from the earlier mentioned Gross and Levenson (1995) set, which might

explain why they were not able to elicit discrete emotions. The present study used two film clips of

this validated set. The method of the present study differed also from the Demaree at al. (2004)

study because participants were asked to exaggerate their vocal emotional expressions instead of

their facial emotional expressions.

It was expected that people experience amusement more intensely and feel more aroused

when they exaggerate their vocal response to stimuli eliciting amusement, compared to people who

did not modulate their emotional response. In the case of disgust, it was expected that there would

be no difference between the exaggeration and control conditions in regard to self-reported

(9)

9 Method

Participants

A total of 52 participants (14 men, 38 women) participated in the study. The sample consisted of 8

undergraduate students to fulfil a requirement of an introductory psychology course, and 44

participants participated for a financial reward (5 Euro for 30 minutes). The participants were 18-57

years old (M = 24.6, SD = 9.4). It was important that all participants spoke Dutch, because this is

the language in which the experiment was conducted. Dutch was the native language for 44

participants. Eight participants indicated that Dutch was not their native language, but they were

proficient enough in Dutch to be able to participate. This study used a 2 (happiness vs. disgust) x 2

(no-exaggeration vs exaggeration) mixed design. Participants were randomly assigned to either two

exaggeration (experimental) conditions or to the two no-exaggeration (control) conditions.

Materials

The stimuli that have been used were two film clips, which have been validated by Gross

and Levenson (1995) as being able to elicit discrete emotions. To elicit amusement, a two and a half

minute clip of the comedy “When Harry met Sally” was used in which people have a funny

discussion about orgasms in a café. The film clip was able to elicit discrete amusement in 93.1

percent of their sample. The mean intensity rating of this film on a 9-point scale (0 – 8) was 5.54.

To elicit disgust, a 30 second clip of the commercial film “Pink Flamingos” was used, in which a

person eats dog faeces. The film clip was able to elicit discrete disgust in 84.9 percent of their

sample. The mean intensity rating of this film on the same 9-point scale was 6.45. The film clips

were cut according to instructions provided by Gross (n.d.) on the website of Stanford University.

Vocal emotional expressions. As a manipulation check, the recorded vocal emotional

responses of the participants were rated by three independent judges who were blind to conditions.

(10)

10

random order. The judges were asked to indicate for each vocal response whether they thought that

the recording corresponds to either amusement or disgust, or both. Likert scales were used to rate

vocal emotional intensity (1 = no emotion to 7 = high intense emotion). This has been done to make

sure that participants in the exaggeration conditions did indeed show more vocal emotional

intensity.

The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Watson, Clark & Tellegen, 1988) has

been used as a control measure to make sure that there were no differences between the

exaggeration and control conditions regarding the baseline affective state, before the actual

manipulation started. The PANAS measures the intensity of 20 affective states, of which ten

affective states are positive and ten are negative. An example of a positive affective state is

“inspired”. And an example of a negative affective state is “tension”. Participants were asked to rate

to what extent they experienced the affective states at the present moment on a 5-point Likert scale

(1 = very slightly or not at all, 5 = extremely). The validity and reliability of the Dutch translation

of the PANAS are considered to be sufficient by the test commission (COTAN) of the Dutch

psychology institute NIP (Egberink, Janssen & Vermeulen, 2014).

The Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) was used to measure self-reported arousal. The SAM

has been developed by Bradley and Lang (1994) to measure three aspects of emotional experience

on a Likert-type scale, namely; emotional valence, arousal and dominance. The SAM depicts five

illustrations on each aspect of emotional experience; from happy to sad (1-5), from aroused to

relaxed (1-5) and from submissive to dominant (1-5). Dominance has not been measured, because it

is not a relevant factor in the present study. General emotional valence has not been measured,

because this study used another measure to assess the intensity of discrete emotions. Only the

arousal question of the SAM was used in the present research. The SAM is a picture based

questionnaire, but in the present study it was accompanied with the sentence; “Choose the picture

that best depicts how active you feel after watching the film clip”. This has been done to further

(11)

11

question, rather than being accompanied with the other two SAM questions. The SAM has often

been used to measure participants' reactions to affective stimuli (e.g. Demaree et al., 2004;

Demaree, Robinson, Pu, & Allen, 2006; Bradley, Cuthbert, & Lang, 1996).

The Discrete Emotions Questionnaire (DEQ) has been used to measure the intensity of 16

different emotions elicited by video clips (e.g. Gross & Levenson, 1995; Demaree et al., 2004;

Schmeichel et al., 2006). In the present research the DEQ has been used to measure differences

regarding the intensity of experienced discrete emotions, between the experimental and control

conditions. All 16 DEQ emotions, including amusement and disgust, have been measured. The

participants rated their emotional state on a 9-point Likert scale from 0 (you did not even feel the

slightest bit of the emotion) to 8 (the most you have ever felt in your life). As there is no Dutch

version of the DEQ, it has been translated into Dutch for this study. The Dutch translation has been

added in appendix IV.

Procedure

After being welcomed by the experimenter, the participants were placed in a room with a computer

which they used to fill in the questionnaires and to watch the film clips (stimuli). A camera has been

present in the room that was used to record the vocal responses of the participants to the stimuli.

The participants were told that the recordings would be used to check whether the participants have

been taking part in the experiment seriously. When the participants agreed and signed the informed

consent, the experimenter left the room.

The stimuli and the questionnaires were presented in Qualtrics. Firstly the participants were

asked to report demographic information about their age and gender and filled in the PANAS next.

After that the two film clips eliciting amusement and disgust followed in a random order. The

following instructions accompanied the film clips for participants in the control conditions: “We

will now be showing you a short film clip. It is important to us that you watch the film clip carefully, but if you find the film clip too distressing, click on the stop-button and call for the experimenter”.

(12)

12

Participants who have been assigned to the experimental conditions received the following

instructions: “We will now be showing you a short film clip. It is important to us that you watch the

film clip carefully, but if you find the film clip too distressing, click on the stop-button and call for the experimenter. If you have any feelings as you watch the film clip, please try your best to

exaggerate these feelings. In other words, exaggerate your vocal reaction to the film clip, so that if somebody watched the videotape of you watching the film clip, they would know exactly what you were feeling”. These instructions have been taken from the Gross and Levenson (1997) study and

have been edited to fit the present study about exaggeration, rather than the original suppression

study. For the actual experiment, these instructions have been translated to Dutch and can be found

in appendix IV. The vocal reactions to the film clips in each condition were recorded with a video

camera. After each film clip the participants filled in two short questionnaires (the Self-Assessment

Manikin and the Discrete Emotions Questionnaire). Subsequently, the participants did also fill in

the neuroticism scale of the NEO-PI-R and the Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (BFNE-2),

to be able to compare the results of this study to the study of Skene (2015). The NEO-PI-R and

BFNE-2 questionnaires have however not been used in the present study.

After finishing the experiment the participants were thanked by the experimenter. Before the

participants left they received a debriefing from the experimenter, which asked for their permission

to use their vocal recordings for analysis. When the debriefing was signed the participants received

(13)

13 Results

Firstly, one participant has been excluded, because the audio recording of this person was deleted

from the camera by accident. Another participant has been excluded because the camera recordings

indicated this person did not participate seriously. Further data inspection revealed that this person

did also not fill in the questionnaires appropriately. This left 50 participants (13 men, 37 women), of

18-58 years old (M = 24.8, SD = 9.5). Twenty-five participants remained in the control condition

and 25 participants remained in the exaggeration condition. The main analyses have been preceded

by three manipulation checks.

The baseline affective state. The first manipulation check tested for significant differences in

the baseline affective state of the participants between the control and exaggeration conditions. The

ten positive subscales and the ten negative subscales of the PANAS were merged to form variables

of baseline positive and negative affect respectively. An independent t-test revealed no differences

regarding the baseline positive affective state between the control (M = 3.30, SD = .61) and

exaggeration (M = 3.06, SD = .74) conditions, t(48) = 1.25, p > .2, 95% CI [-.15, .63]. The data of

the baseline negative affective state was not normally distributed, as shown by the fact that the

Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was significant, D(50) = .23, p < .001. Therefore a Mann-Whitney test

was used as a nonparametric equivalent of the independent t-test. The Mann-Whitney test revealed

no differences regarding the baseline negative affective state between the control (M = 1.36) and

exaggeration (M = 1.23) conditions, U = 254.00, z = -1.14, p > .2. In addition to the reported means,

also the medians of the negative PANAS scale and all other research variables can be found in

appendix III, because they should be reported alongside nonparametric tests (Field, 2009).

Vocal emotional intensity ratings. The audio clips that were rated by the three independent

judges were used as a manipulation check to make sure that participants in the exaggeration

condition did indeed show more vocal emotional intensity compared to controls. The four ratings

(all audio clips of the participants' vocal amusement and disgust expressions were rated twice) were

(14)

14

was used. High correlations were found between the two ratings of amusement, rs = .72, p < .001

and the two ratings of disgust, rs = .89, p < .001. Therefore the ratings were merged to form one

vocal intensity variable for each emotion. The merged variables were not normally distributed in the

case of amusement, D(50) = .15, p = .005, or disgust D(50) = .26, p < .001. A Mann-Whitney test

revealed that participants in the exaggeration condition (M = 3.10) showed more vocal emotional

intensity compared to the control condition (M = 2.14), U = 199.00, z = -2.23, p = .026, R2 = .10 in

the case of amusement. Another Mann-Whitney test revealed that also in the case of disgust

participants showed more vocal emotional intensity in the exaggeration condition (M = 3.62)

compared to the control condition (M = 1.38), U = 85.00, z = -4.65, p < .001, R2 = .43. These results

showed that participants in the exaggeration condition did indeed exaggerate their emotions more

than participants in the control condition.

Discreteness of the elicited emotions. To check whether the amusement and disgust film

clips elicited discrete emotions, the means of the 16 DEQ emotions had to be compared (see

appendix II for the means of the 16 DEQ emotions in all four conditions). The data of the DEQ was

not normally distributed (as shown in appendix I) and therefore it was not possible to compare the

means of the 16 emotions with an ANOVA with post-hoc comparisons. Instead a Wilcoxon

signed-rank test was used to compare the means of the target variables to the means of the emotion with the

second highest mean in each condition. In the control-amusement condition participants

experienced amusement (M = 6.44) more intensely than the second highest emotion, interest (M =

4.68), T = 50.50, z = -2.68, p = .007, R2 = .14. In the control-disgust condition participants did not

significantly experience disgust (M = 7.20) more intensely than the second highest emotion,

confusion (M = 6.08), T = 74.00, z = -1.71, p = .087, R2 = .06. In the exaggeration-amusement

condition participants experienced amusement (M = 6.76) more intensely than the second highest

emotion, interest (M = 4.80), T = 33.50, z = -3.20, p = .001, R2 = .20. Also, in the

exaggeration-disgust condition participants experienced exaggeration-disgust (M = 7.72) more intensely than the second

(15)

15

that the film clips did successfully elicit discrete emotions in three of the four conditions instead of

general positive and negative affective states. Although the mean of disgust was not significantly

higher than the mean of confusion in the control condition, the difference could be classified as a

statistical trend. The main analysis of self-reported emotional intensity therefore still compared

disgust in the control condition with disgust in the exaggeration condition, instead of comparing

general negative affective states.

Self-Reported Arousal. The SAM arousal scores were compared between conditions for both

emotions, to check whether the participants reported feeling more aroused while exaggerating their

vocal emotional response. The SAM data was not normally distributed in the case of amusement,

D(50) = .26, p < .001, or in the case of disgust, D(50) = .20, p < .001. Mann-Whitney tests revealed

that the participants did not report more arousal in the exaggeration condition compared to the

control condition in case of amusement, U = 265.00, z = -1.00, p > .3, or in the case of disgust, U =

282.00, z = -.62, p >.5. The results did not confirm the hypothesis that people would report feeling

more aroused while exaggerating their vocal emotional response to amusement-eliciting stimuli.

The results did however confirm the hypothesis that people would not report feeling more aroused

when exaggerating their vocal emotional response to disgust-eliciting stimuli.

Table 1. Means of Self-reported Arousal in the Case of Amusement and Disgust in Both the Control and Exaggeration Conditions.

Amusement Means Disgust Means

Control condition 2.96 2.80

Exaggeration condition 3.12 2.96

Self-Reported Emotional Intensity. The DEQ scores of amusement and disgust were

compared between conditions to check whether both discrete emotions were experienced more

(16)

16

amusement, D(50) = .19, p < .001, or in the case of disgust, D(50) = .20, p < .001. Mann-Whitney

tests revealed that the participants did not report experiencing emotions more intensely in the

exaggeration condition compared to the control condition in the case of amusement, U = 289.00, z =

-.46, p > .6, or in the case of disgust, U = 284.00, z = -.58, p > .5. The results did not support the

hypothesis that people would experience stimuli eliciting amusement more intensely in the

exaggeration condition, compared to the control condition. On the other hand, the results did

confirm, as expected, that there was no difference in self-reported emotional intensity of disgust

between the exaggeration and control conditions.

Table 2. Means of Self-reported Emotional Intensity in the Case of Amusement and Disgust in Both the Control and Exaggeration Conditions.

Amusement Means Disgust Means

Control condition 6.40 7.16

Exaggeration condition 6.76 7.72

Exploratory Analysis – Do Self-Reported Emotional Intensity and Observed Emotional Intensity Ratings Relate? Following the hypothesis it is expected that there should be a relation

between how intense people reported feeling and how intense they were rated sounding for

amusement, but not for disgust. Although no significant differences were found between the

exaggeration and control conditions for amusement and disgust, it was checked whether

self-reported emotional intensity and the vocal intensity ratings by the independent judges correlated

positively. Spearman's correlation coefficient was used because the variables were not normally

distributed, as shown before. The analysis revealed a statistical trend, but no significant correlation

between self-reported emotional intensity and how intense people were rated sounding for

(17)

17 Discussion

The purpose of this study was to replicate the findings of Demaree et al. (2004), and to test whether

the exaggeration of vocal expressions increases emotional intensity in a similar way as facial

expressions have been hypothesized to do. The present study did find support for earlier findings

that the exaggeration of emotional expressions in response to stimuli eliciting negative emotions

does not change the subjective experience. Participants who exaggerated their emotional response

to the disgust-eliciting film clip did not report feeling more aroused and did not experience

increased emotional intensity, compared to the control condition. However, the hypothesis was not

supported with regard to the subjective emotion experience when people exaggerated their

emotional response to stimuli eliciting positive emotions. Participants who exaggerated their vocal

emotional response to the amusement-eliciting film clip did not report feeling more aroused, and

did also not experience increased emotional intensity compared to the control condition.

The present study did successfully improve on the limitations of the Demaree et al. (2004)

study. This study did include at least 25 participants per condition, and one of the between-subjects

variables was made into a within-subjects factor, resulting in more statistical power to test the

hypothesis. Moreover, the film clips used to elicit amusement and disgust did successfully elicit

discrete emotions in three of the four conditions, rather than more general positive or negative

affective states. In the remaining condition, the difference between disgust and the second highest

intense emotion can still be classified as a statistical trend. Additionally, the exaggeration

manipulation has been successful, because participants in the exaggeration condition did show more

vocal intensity compared to participants in the control condition, as has been determined by the

three independent judges who were blind to conditions.

Overall, the present study has been a robust test of the hypothesis, but still found similar

results as Demaree et al. (2004) with regard to experienced emotional intensity and arousal. It could

be argued that this might be due to the fact that the present study asked participants to exaggerate

(18)

18

Following this argument, one could doubt that vocal feedback is as effective in changing the

subjective emotion experience in the same degree as facial feedback has shown to do in past

experiments (e.g. Strack, et al. 1988). These doubts may be partly justified as there has not been a

lot of research about the vocal feedback hypothesis. Furthermore, one experiment compared the

effects of facial, bodily and vocal feedback on the subjective emotion experience and found the

effects of vocal feedback to be the least consistent (Flack, 2006). The participants in the study at

issue were asked to produce facial muscle configurations, bodily postures and read text with vocal

qualities that correspond to multiple types of emotions. The expectation was that people would also

report feeling the corresponding emotions more intensely than any other emotion after producing

the expression. For a large extent this was true for facial and bodily feedback, in the case of both

positive and negative emotions. In the case of vocal feedback there was only an effect for

happiness, while it had inconsistent effects for the other emotions. However, it has to be taken into

account that the manipulation of the study at issue relied mainly on speech prosody instead of the

nonverbal vocalizations used by the present research. Their results might therefore not be entirely

suitable to explain the results of the present study.

On the other hand, while most emotion regulation studies about subjective emotion

experience have assumed facial feedback to be a possible working mechanism (e.g. Gross &

Levenson, 1993; Demaree et al., 2004), none of them have tried to quantify the influence it has on

the subjective emotion experience. Moreover, the proposed effect of facial feedback on the

subjective emotion experience has been non-existent in the case of negative emotions (e.g.

Schmeichel, 2006). It is not clear whether there is a difference between the influence of facial and

vocal feedback on the subjective emotion experience. There have not been enough findings in this

area of research to indicate that the reliance on vocal emotion expressions has negatively impacted

the present study. Furthermore, a similar study that has been running simultaneously with the

present study did also not find any differences between the control and exaggeration conditions, in

(19)

19

issue used the exact same method and materials, did also look at self-reported emotional intensity

and arousal, but used different participants from the same subject pool of the University of

Amsterdam. The fact that the results of both studies are comparable makes it less likely that the

usage of vocal emotional expressions was the culprit causing the lack of results in the present study.

To further assess the effectiveness of the manipulation, it is interesting to look at the results

of the exploratory analysis. Although the means of amusement and disgust were higher in the

exaggeration conditions compared to the control conditions, albeit non-significant, it is not entirely

clear whether this happened purely due to chance. The results of the exploratory analysis might

shed some light on this issue. The analysis revealed a weak positive correlation for amusement

between self-reported emotional intensity and how intense participants were rated sounding by the

independent judges. This relation was non-significant, but can be classified as a statistical trend.

Even if this relation were significant, an increase in actual vocal emotional intensity would only be

able to explain seven percent of the increase in self-reported emotional intensity. A possible

explanation therefore is that the manipulation might have worked to a small degree for amusement,

but has not been effective enough to cause significant differences between the control and

exaggeration conditions in the main analysis. However, the fact that the means of amusement and

disgust in the exaggeration condition turned out to be a bit higher for both emotions should

probably be attributed mainly to chance.

A possible limitation regarding the discreteness of the elicited emotions is that

embarrassment (one of the 16 DEQ emotions) has not been translated into Dutch correctly. In the

questionnaire that was used for the experiment, embarrassment has been translated as “verlegen”,

which practically means feeling shy. It should have been translated to Dutch as “in verlegenheid

gebracht”, which comes much closer to the definition of embarrassment. When translated right, this

might have impacted the results of the DEQ questionnaire, because both film clips contain elements

that might have also elicited embarrassment. It is however unlikely that it would undo the finding

(20)

20

The hypothesized effect of vocal expressions on the subjective emotion experience could not

be demonstrated, because of the lack of findings of the present study. There are a few issues about

vocal emotional expressions that should be discussed though. Two issues became clear when editing

the video recordings of the participants to separate the audio channel from the video. Although these

issues are not supported by quantitative data and therefore reflect the subjective opinion of the

author, they might prove useful to take into account, or to further explore in future studies about

vocal emotional expressions. Firstly, while looking at the videotapes of the participants it was clear

that vocal emotional expressions were always accompanied with a facial emotional expression.

Furthermore, the facial emotional expressions overall tended to be more intense when the vocal

emotional expressions also increased in intensity. Although the facial expressions have not been

rated, so there is no data to back up this claim, it could be questioned whether the current research

paradigm is suited to compare the influence of vocal emotional expressions to that of facial

emotional expressions. The hypothesis of this study expected that vocal emotional expressions

would lead to similar results, but there was also a possibility that their influence on the subjective

experience would be different from the influence of facial emotional expressions. While vocal and

facial emotional expressions were assumed to have similar qualities, they might also differ in some

aspects regarding they influence the subjective emotion experience. When vocal and facial

expressions constantly occur together it is difficult to isolate a distinct effect of one of these types of

expressions on the subjective emotion experience. This might be true to a greater extend for vocal

emotional expressions than for facial emotional expressions though. In some cases participants did

show facial expressions, but did not vocalize their emotions. On the contrary, participants did

always show facial expressions when they vocalized their emotions.

Secondly, one possible difference between vocal and facial expressions occurs when

emotional expressions reside on the lower end of the intensity scale. Facial emotional expressions

with low intensity seem to be much easier to interpret when compared to vocal emotional

(21)

21

becomes apparent what emotion they are experiencing when paying attention to their facial

expressions. Most of the times the participants showed clear amusement or disgust facial

expressions, but vocalized only a soft sigh or even made no sound at all. Although the present

research confirms that vocal expressions are also able to convey information about the senders’

emotional experience, as was also found by Zuckerman et al. (1980), emotional information

conveyed exclusively by the vocal channel could possibly be less frequent and less clear.

Schmeichel et al. (2006) concluded that the exaggeration and suppression of negative

emotions might not be effective at changing the subjective emotion experience. Demaree et al.

(2004), Skene (2015) and the present research show that the subjective emotion experience is also

not really being influenced when people exaggerate their response to stimuli eliciting positive

emotions. As far as is known now, it is not clear what causes underlie the lack of findings in the

studies at issue. Despite the absence of results there are still areas worth exploring. It could for

example be interesting to determine whether the suppression of vocal emotional responses to

stimuli eliciting positive emotions might reduce the experienced emotional intensity in a similar

research paradigm. Additionally, it would be interesting to check whether the modulation of vocal

emotional expressions lead to similar results regarding physiological measures as has been found in

the case of facial emotional expressions (e.g. Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997; Demaree et al., 2004).

In both cases it might prove useful to take into account that vocal expressions possibly occur

together with facial expressions most of the time, and that it could be difficult to attribute the found

effects to vocal expressions exclusively.

Although an effect of the exaggeration of positive emotional expressions has not been found,

and not a lot can be said about the role of vocal emotional expressions in emotion regulation

research, there is still room to take the current research design into interesting directions. Hopefully

this study will be helpful for future research to uncover more insights about the subjective

(22)

22 Literature

Bradley, M.M., Cuthbert, B.N., & Lang, P.J. (1996). Picture media and emotion: effects of a

sustained affective context. Psychophysiology, 33, 662 – 670.

Bradley, M.M., & Lang, P.J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the

semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49 –

59.

Buck, R. (1980). Nonverbal behavior and the theory of emotion: the facial feedback hypothesis.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(5), 811 – 824.

Demaree, H.A., Robinson, J.L., Pu, J., & Allen, J.J.B. (2006). Strategies actually employed during

response-focused emotion regulation research: Affective and physiological consequences.

Cognition and Emotion, 20(8), 1248 – 1260.

Demaree, H.A., Schmeichel, B.J., Robinson, J.L., & Everhart, D.E. (2004). Behavioral, affective

and physiological effects of negative and positive emotional exaggeration. Cognition and

Emotion, 18(8), 1079 – 1097.

Demaree, H.A., Schmeichel, B.J., Robinson, J.L., Pu, J., Everhart, D.E., & Berntson, G.G. (2006).

Up- and down-regulating facial disgust: Affective, vagal, sympathetic, and respiratory

consequences. Biological Psychology, 71, 90 – 99.

Egberink, I.J.L., Janssen, N.A.M., & Vermeulen, C.S.M. (2015, May 7). COTAN review 1999,

PANAS. Retrieved from www.cotandocumentatie.nl.

Flack, W. (2006). Peripheral feedback effects of facial expressions, bodily postures, and vocal

expressions on emotional feelings. Cognition and Emotion, 20(2), 177 – 195.

Frank, M.G., Maroulis, A., & Griffin, D.J. (2013). The voice. In Matsumoto, D., Frank, M.G., &

Hwang, H.S. (2013). Nonverbal communication: science and applications. Thousand Oaks:

SAGE Publications, Inc.

Gross, J.J. (n.d.). Instructions for editing Gross and Levenson (1995) film clips. Retrieved May 15th

(23)

23

Gross, J.J. (1998a). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences

for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

74(1), 224 – 237.

Gross, J.J. (1998b). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Review of

General Psychology, 2(3), 271 – 299.

Gross, J.J., & Levenson, R.W. (1993). Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and

expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(6), 970 – 986.

Gross, J.J., & Levenson, R.W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion,

9(1), 87 – 108.

Gross, J.J., & Levenson, R.W. (1997). Hiding feelings: the acute effects of inhibiting negative and

positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 95 – 103.

Hatfield, E., Hsee, C.K., Costello, J., Schalekamp Weisman, M., & Denney, C. (1995). The impact

of vocal feedback on emotional experience and expression. Journal of Social Behavior and

Personality, 10(2), 293 – 312.

Jackson, D.C., Malmstadt, J.R., Larson, C.L., & Davidson, R.J. (2000). Suppression and

enhancement of emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology, 37, 515 –

522.

Philippot, P., & Feldman, R.S. (2004). The regulation of emotion. London: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates, Publishers.

Richards, J.M., & Gross, J.J. (1999). Composure at any cost? The cognitive consequences of

emotion suppression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(8), 1033 – 1044.

Schmeichel, B.J., Demaree, H.A., Robinson, J.L., & Pu, J. (2006). Ego depletion by response

exaggeration. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 95 – 102.

Skene, C.S. (2015). The influence of exaggerating facial expressions on subjective emotional

(24)

24

Strack, F., Martin, L.L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the

human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 54, 768 – 777.

Wallbott, H. G., & Scherer, K. 1989. Assessing emotion by questionnaire. In Plutchik, R. &

Kellermand, H. (1989), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience: The measurement of

emotions, 4, 55 – 82. New York: Academic Press.

Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of

positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 54(6), 1063 – 1070.

Zuckerman, M., Larrance, D.T., Spiegel, N.H., & Klorman, R. (1980). Controlling nonverbal

displays: facial expressions and tone of voice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,

(25)

25

Appendix I – Tables of Normality Tests

Table 1. Normality Tests of the Vocal Intensity Ratings by the Independent Judges.

Condition D df p

Judge 1 – Amusement .22 50 < .0005

Judge 1 – Disgust .27 50 < .0005

Judge 2 and 3 – Amusement .21 50 < .0005

Judge 2 and 3 – Disgust .30 50 < .0005

Table 2. Normality Tests of the DEQ Questionnaire in All Four Conditions

Condition D df p

Control – Amusement .27 414 < .0005

Control – Disgust .31 414 < .0005

Exaggeration – Amusement .28 414 < .0005

(26)

26

Appendix II – Means and Medians of the 16 DEQ Emotions

Table 1. Means (M) and Medians (Mdn) of the 16 DEQ Emotions in All Four Conditions.

Control conditions Exaggeration Conditions

Amusement M (Mdn) Disgust M (Mdn) Amusement M (Mdn) Disgust M (Mdn) Amusement 6.40 (7)a 2.16 (1) 6.76 (7)a 1.92 (1) Anger 1.60 (1) 2.16 (1) 1.52 (1) 1.64 (1) Arousal 3.52 (3.5) 1.68 (1) 3.00 (2) 1.36 (1) Confusion 2.60 (1) 6.08 (6) b 2.24 (1) 5.12 (6)b Contempt 1.68 (1) 4.64 (4.5) 1.64 (1) 4.12 (4) Contentment 4.40 (5) 1.60 (1) 4.52 (5) 1.20 (1) Disgust 1.80 (1) 7.16 (8)a 1.40 (1) 7.72 (8)a Embarrassment 3.56 (3) 2.48 (1) 3.40 (3) 1.84 (1) Fear 1.24 (1) 2.08 (1) 1.04 (1) 1.36 (1) Happiness 3.96 (4) 1.64 (1) 3.36 (3) 1.08 (1) Interest 4.68 (5)b 2.16 (1) 4.68 (5)b 2.16 (1) Pain 1.28 (1) 2.12 (1) 1.04 (1) 1.24 (1) Relief 2.12 (1) 1.40 (1) 2.20 (1) 1.36 (1) Sadness 1.28 (1) 2.12 (1) 1.00 (1) 1.28 (1) Surprise 4.32 (5) 5.32 (5) 3.64 (4) 4.68 (5) Tension 3.04 (2.5) 2.56 (2) 2.92 (2.5) 2.40 (2) a

(27)

27

Appendix III – Means and Medians of the Research Variables

Table 1. Means (M) and Medians (Mdn) of the Research Variables in the Control and Exaggeration Conditions. Control Condition M (Mdn) Exaggeration Condition M (Mdn) PANAS Positive 3.30 (3.4) 3.06 (3.2) PANAS Negative 1.36 (1.2) 1.23 (1.1)

Judged Intensity Amusement 2.14 (2) 3.10 (2.5)

Judged Intensity Disgust 1.38 (1) 3.62 (3.5)

Amusement Arousal (SAM) 2.96 (3) 3.12 (3)

Disgust Arousal (SAM) 2.80 (3) 2.96 (3)

Amusement Intensity (DEQ) 6.40 (7) 6.76 (7)

(28)

28

Appendix IV – Translations of the Materials and Instructions

Table 1. Translation of the Instructions.

Control Condition – English Control Condition – Dutch

“We will now be showing you a short film clip. It is important to us that you watch the film clip carefully, but if you find the film clip too distressing, click on the stop-button and call for the experimenter.”

“We laten u een korte film clip zien. Het is belangrijk dat u de film clip zorgvuldig bekijkt. Als u de film te verontrustend vindt, klik dan op stop en roep de proefleider.”

Addition to Exaggeration Condition – English Addition to Exaggeration Condition – Dutch

“If you have any feelings as you watch the film clip, please try your best to exaggerate these feelings. In other words, exaggerate your vocal reaction to the film clip, so that if somebody watched the videotape of you watching the film clip, they would know exactly what you were feeling.”

“Als u emoties voelt tijdens deze film clip, doe dan uw best om met uw stem deze emoties te overdrijven. Met andere woorden, zorg ervoor dat iemand die later naar een opname van uw stem zou luisteren precies weet hoe u zich voelde.”

Table 2. Translation of the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) Instruction.

English Dutch

“Choose the picture that best depicts how active you feel after watching the film clip.”

“Kies het plaatje dat het beste aangeeft hoe actief u zichzelf voelt na het zien van de film clip.”

(29)

29

Table 3. Translation of the Discrete Emotions Questionnaire (DEQ).

English Dutch English Dutch

Amusement Plezier Fear Angst

Anger Boosheid Happiness Geluk

Arousal Opwinding Interest Interesse

Confusion Verwarring Pain Pijn

Contempt Minachting Relief Opluchting

Contentment Tevreden Sadness Verdriet

Disgust Walging Surprise Verrassing

Embarrassment* Verlegen* Tension Spanning

*The translation of “embarrassment” to “verlegen” was not entirely correct. The translation should have been: “In verlegenheid gebracht”.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Following our findings in MeDALL showing that asthma, eczema and rhinitis in children constitute an allergic multimorbidity cluster suggesting common pathways [ 5 , 6 ], we under-

A similar temperature dependence of the growth rate as in the present work was also observed for AgInSbTe PCMs, where the Arrhenius dependence of viscosity was found at

“Moet Verenso het debat stimuleren? Stelling nemen? Als vereniging van specialisten ouderengeneeskunde worste­ len we met die vraag. Ik probeer in elk geval de dialoog open te

Daarnaast zal nog exploratief gekeken worden naar de voorspellende waarde van hechting in het beloop van maintenance behavior gedurende de HmT relatiecursus, de samenhang tussen

actual audit fees paid by firms to the external auditor and the normal audit fees estimated denotes by equation (2), ABNPAFEE the abnormal positive audit fee is an indicator

Met!HAC! 291,00! 291,26! 0,26387! Zonder!HAC! 298,92! 298,92! 0,00049!

43 Die aanpak is dan ook te zien als een tijdelijke overbrugging op de weg naar een duurzame energievoorziening – maar wel een overbrugging die gunsti- ger is voor de

These studies show that preparation processes not only affects subsequent behavior in response to imminent target stimuli, but they also influence brain activity elicited