• No results found

Individual differences in learning rate are reflected in feedback related brain processes

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Individual differences in learning rate are reflected in feedback related brain processes"

Copied!
2
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

University of Groningen

Individual differences in learning rate are reflected in feedback related brain processes van den Berg, Berry

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Publication date: 2020

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

van den Berg, B. (2020). Individual differences in learning rate are reflected in feedback related brain processes.

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

Data

Modelled data Data

I

NDIVIDUAL

DIFFERENCES

IN

LEARNING

RATE

ARE

REFLECTED

IN

FEEDBACK

RELATED

BRAIN

PROCESSES

VIRTUAL Cognitive Neuroscience Society 2020

berry.van.den.berg@rug.nl

Here, participants chose on each trial either a face or a house, which was

fol-lowed by receiving either a zero (0) gain (+) or a loss (-) of different

magni-tudes (0:8)

On each set of 20 trials either the face or house was the set-winner and was more likely to yield net gains.

Summary

Feedback processing was marked

by amplitude modulations induced by both magnitude and valence in the earlyl atency range with distinct topo-graphical effects. Specifically,

va-lence showed a classical negative polarity feedback related negativity (FRN). Magnitude showed a frontal postive deflection for larger

out-comes

Participants learned over the course of 20 trials to choose the stimulus that yielded

higher net-gains. There was substantial variability in how well participants were able

to do so.

ERP amplitudes in the late latency range were modulated by feedbackn-1. High learn-ing rates were characterized by an LPC

that was stronger modulated by previous feedback information as opposed to low learning rates.

The ability to use and integrate feedback information over time is key to our ability to learn and decision making. Although it is fairly well established how the brain processes outcomes on a single trial, it is less well studied how

these processes depend on encountered information on previous trials.

In sum, this study provides a

novel and important set of

findings providing more

in-sight into how the brain

dy-namically integrates feedback

information over multiple trials

to guide decision making in an

uncertain world.

+ + + + +

+8

duration (ms)

1000-1500 until resp.

(max 1200)

300

700-900

500

Fixation Fixation Feedback Highlight choice Choice Cue

2000-2500

Berry

van den Berg, Timothy Sondej, Celina Pütz, Marty G Woldorff & Monicque M Lorist

University of Groningen; Duke University

P

ROCESSING

OF

FEEDBACK

I

NTEGRATION

OF

F

EEDBACK

ACROSS

TRIALS

[-7; -8] 0 [-5; -6] [-3; -4] [-1; -2] [+1; +2] [+3; +4] [+5; +6] [+7; +8] feedback -2 2 6 10 14 18 -200 0 200 400 600 800 -2 2 6 10 14 18 time (ms) amplitude ( μV)

H

OW

DO

WE

USE

FEEDBACK

?

0.00 0.05 0.10

individual learning rate β trial

trial number 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 5 10 15 20 p(choosing set−winner)

learning rate < median learning rate > median average

250 to 350

Loss minus Gain

250 to 350 250 to 350 Gain: [+5:+8] minus Gain [+1:+4] Loss: [-8:-5] minus Loss [-4:-1] amplitude (μV) -3 3 500 to 600

Loss minus Gain

Gain: [+5:+8] minus Gain [+1:+4] Loss: [-8:-5] minus Loss[-4:-1] 500 to 600 500 to 600 amplitude (μV) -3 3 feedbackn 7.5 10.0 12.5 15.0 17.5 −8 −6 −4 −2 0 +2 +4 +6 +8 feedback n feedbackn-1 2 amplitude (µV) 14 −8 −6 −4 −20 +2 +4 +6 +8 −8−6−4−2 0 +2+4+6+8 4 amplitude (µV)20 −8 −6 −4 −20 +2 +4 +6 +8 −8−6−4−2 0 +2+4+6+8 feedbackn-1 feedback n amplitude (µ V) amplitude (µ V) feedbackn 6 8 10 12 −8 −6 −4 −2 0 +2 +4 +6 +8 −8−6 −4 −20 +2 +4 +6 +8 −8−6−4−2 0 +2+4+6+8 −8 −6 −4 −20 +2 +4 +6 +8 −8−6−4−2 0 +2+4+6+8 −8−6−4−2 0 2 4 6 8 −8−6−4−2 0 2 4 6 8 −8 −6 −4 −20 +2 +4 +6 +8 feedback n

learning rate < medianamplitude (μV)

3 20 Data feedback n feedbackn-1 Modelled data feedbackn-1

learning rate > median

amplitude (μV)m 3 20 −8−6−4−2 0 +2+4+6+8 −8−6−4−2 0 +2+4+6+8 −8 −6 −4 −20 +2 +4 +6 +8 −8−6−4−2 0 +2+4+6+8 −8−6−4−2 0 +2+4+6+8 −8 −6 −4 −20 +2 +4 +6 +8 Late latency range

Early latency range

Processes in the late latency range

(500-600ms) were modulated by both cur-rent feedback contents, and also by the

feedback on the previous trial, indicating an integrative role. Strikingly, this integration

was even further modulated by the individu-al participants’ learning rate. As such, the processes that are marked by the LPC sub-serve a dynamic updating role that is highly susceptible to prior information.

Feedback processing was characterized by amplitudes in the early latency range

(250-350ms) being modulated by the magni-tude and valence on the current trial. In this early time range we found minimal influence of the feedback of the previous trial,

sug-gesting a feedback registration mechanism, that is not modulated by prior information

(i.e. expectation).

Modelled data

Both magnitude and valence modu-lated amplitudes in the later latency range. These modulations had si-miliar scalp topographies (sugges-tive of a modulation of the Late Pos-tive Complex [LPC]), suggesting a similiar neuro-cognitive process by both factors is involved in this later time period. feedback onset Early processing (250-350ms) Late processing(500-600ms) feedbackn-1

learning rate < median learning rate > median

ERP amplitudes in the early latency range were slightly modulated by feedbackn-1 but not learning rate.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

The present study showed that satisfaction of students' basic needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness has an incremental value over and above their personality traits

The two studies differ in the analytical approach, in that in Chapter 3 we used an Independent Component Analysis approach to investigate brain’s networks

Door dementie kunnen mensen veel dingen niet meer die ze vroeger wel konden en het vormt dus een uitdaging om mensen zoveel mogelijk hun oude rollen te laten behouden, zich autonoom

Tot 10 miljoen vissen is er sprake geweest van een toename van de visstand... Extra

This leaflet summarises a report, Parental Involvement in Early Learning - A review of research, policy and good practice, researched and prepared by Margaret Kernan of

There are two possible conclusions concerning the effect of self- explanations, (1) self-explanations were able to enhance learning gains but were not able to enhance

Adding the results of the multiple choice test and the word recognition task a t-test showed that both experimental and control group had an improvement in test scores between pre-

Our review distinguishes itself from the above reviews by focusing on the aspect of generating feedback in programming learning tools, closely exam- ining the different types