• No results found

Dyslectic and normally-reading children : 1. Exploration of a letter-search test for screening purposes : 2. Follow- up and further exploration in 4 weak and 4 normal readers on letter, word and number recognition

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Dyslectic and normally-reading children : 1. Exploration of a letter-search test for screening purposes : 2. Follow- up and further exploration in 4 weak and 4 normal readers on letter, word and number recognition"

Copied!
7
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Dyslectic and normally-reading children : 1. Exploration of a

letter-search test for screening purposes : 2. Follow- up and

further exploration in 4 weak and 4 normal readers on letter,

word and number recognition

Citation for published version (APA):

Legein, C. P., & Bouma, H. (1977). Dyslectic and normally-reading children : 1. Exploration of a letter-search test

for screening purposes : 2. Follow- up and further exploration in 4 weak and 4 normal readers on letter, word

and number recognition. Documenta Ophthalmologica, 42(2), 391-396. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02742254

DOI:

10.1007/BF02742254

Document status and date:

Published: 01/01/1977

Document Version:

Publisher’s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers)

Please check the document version of this publication:

• A submitted manuscript is the version of the article upon submission and before peer-review. There can be

important differences between the submitted version and the official published version of record. People

interested in the research are advised to contact the author for the final version of the publication, or visit the

DOI to the publisher's website.

• The final author version and the galley proof are versions of the publication after peer review.

• The final published version features the final layout of the paper including the volume, issue and page

numbers.

Link to publication

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal.

If the publication is distributed under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license above, please follow below link for the End User Agreement:

www.tue.nl/taverne

Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us at:

openaccess@tue.nl

(2)

Documenta Ophthalmologica 42,2 : 391 - 396, 1977

DYSLECTIC AND N O R M A L L Y - R E A D I N G CHILDREN

I. EXPLORATION O F A LETTER-SEARCH TEST FOR SCREENING PURPOSES

II. FOLLOW-UP AND F U R T H E R E X P L O R A T I O N IN 4 WEAK AND 4 N O R M A L READERS ON LETTER, WORD AND NUMBER

RECOGNITION

CH.P. LEGEIN & H. BOUMA

(Eindhoven)

INTRODUCTION

The results obtained last year from the examination of 20 dyslectic and 20 normally reading children (Bourna, Legein & Van Rens, 1974; Bouma & Legein, 1976) encouraged us to continue this study into two directions.

Firstly a follow-up study o f all 40 subjects to get an idea o f the course o f these perceptual processes. Secondly a further exploration of possible defec- tive processes in visual recognition. This paper will report b o t h on the follow-up study and on the further exploration in four dyslectic and four normally reading children selected from the above mentioned groups.

Partly summarizing last year's results we found:

1. A backwardness in reading-level for all dyslectic children by at least two years;

2. in the tachistoscopic experiments a significantly lower recognition-score for the dyslectic group with reference to embedded letters and for words especially in parafoveal presentation.

LETTER-SEARCH TEST

F o r the purpose o f bridging the gap between tachistoscopic recognition and ordinary reading we developed a search experiment in which letters e (target letters) within words or letterstrings o f 8 letters had to be marked.

As to the n u m b e r of errors, the dyslectic group missed twice as much target-letters as the control-group and b o t h groups missed more target- letters in the words than in the l e t t e r strings (Table 1).

(3)

Table 1. Percent error-scores in letter search test. Large scores: total errors; small scores: after correction for skipped lines.

w o r d s

d y s l . c o n t r .

3 6

23

14

13

strings

2 6

15

12

9

many lines occurred in which n o t a single target letter was marked. This could be an indication that they just skipped full lines probably due to an insufficient control of the eye-movement towards the new line. We made a correction for these, probably n o t inspected, lines, also indicated in Table I. The general tendency is that more target-letters are missed in the second part of the words and letterstrings; and that the first, fourth and fifth letter- position are at an advantage. Many errors are made at the last few letter- positions in the letterstrings by both groups, and also in words by the dyslectic group.

Since both groups made more errors in the word-test than in the string- test this is indicative of them using knowledge of wordforrns to a certain extent. The specificity of this knowledge has n o t yet been investigated. In conclusion this test puts attention to a probably insufficient eyecontrol on the part of dyslectic children which, together with strong parafoveal inter- ference effects influences their scores. A new version of the test might separate these effects.

FOLLOW-UP

In the tachistoscopic part of the examination there was hardly any change in the high foveal recognition scores of both groups (Table 2). In parafoveal scores, however, there was a definite improvement in particular for words in the dyslectic group.

Table 2. 1975 Average correct recognition-scores (~) of single letters /a/; embedded letters/xax/; words/wrd/. Between brackets are the 1974 scores of the same children.

f o v e a l dysl. contr. / a / 96 (96) 94 (99) /xax/ 72 (68) 9 4 (os) /wrd/ 78 (73) 100 (lOO) p a r a f o v e a l dysl. contr. 91 (m) 9 8 (ge) 28 (+o) 53 (54) S4 ( ~ ) 71 (co)

(4)

As to the reading-level of the

eight subjects

(Fig. I) we conclude that the dyslectic children especially made progress and for them a fair correlation between improved reading level and better parafoveal recognition of words is shown (Fig. 2).

FURTHER EXPLORATION

Further exploration was done o n foveal recognition of common words (frequency of usage in printed Dutch about 1 : I0,000) of larger lengths (l = 6, 7 and 8). Six words of each length were presented (exposure-time 100 msec). In the dyslectic group the scores for these long words are de- finitely lower than for the shorter words, whereas for the normal groups the scores remained perfect. The scores for word lengths l = 3 - 5 of these subjects have also been included (Table 3).

Table 3. Correct foveal word recognition scores for various wordlengths.

dysl. contr.

f = 3

77

100

f = 4

77

100

~=s

8 0

100

f : 6

42

100

f=7

38

100

f=8

33

96

Forty, randomly composed, digit strings (length l = 1, 2, 3, 4) were foveaUy presented (100 msec exposure time). Table 4 gives the results for both groups and it is obvious that, as from l = 3, dyslectics have far more diffi- culties than children which read normally. We tested numers of two digits parafoveally (eccentricity = 1 ~ and found that both groups had rather high scores: dyslectics 8270 and controls 9470. A foveal recognition experiment of numbers was also done using both a normal ( I 0 0 msec) and a prolonged presentation time (500 msec). Table 4 shows an increase of correct scores. This probably indicates that the short-term memory capacity is n o t an essential limiting factor in these experiments.

(5)

7

"1- 6 Z ~ s @

~ 4

"10 P 3

2

grade / / / / / / / " "s

/

//ysexc

1 , i r l L , i I i I 9 I 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 yrs. a g e

Fig. 1. Follow-up of reading level 1974 (small symbols)-1975 (large symbols).

1-0

%- +1 n ~O i--

0

3

"5

dvslexic /

~ . - l l l

control

o

g o

" O I I I I I I

1

2

3

4

5

6 grade 7

reading level (TANGHE)

Fig. 2. Follow-up of parafoveal word-recognition and of reading level 1974 (small symboIs)-1975 (large symbols),

(6)

Table 4. Correct foveal number scores for various lengths (1) at two stimulus durations. f - - 1

'f= 2

f : 3

1 / = 4

/=5

dysl.

lOO ms soo ms 9 5 9 5 6 8 9 1 3 8 7 0 - 3 0

contr.

Ioo m s soo ms 1 0 0 - I O 0 - 9 5 1 0 0 9 8 9 2 - 6 5

Nevertheless the short term memory function (rehearsal) seems worse in the dyslectic group as is indicated by testing this function as well by audi- tory presentation of digit-strings (l = 3,4,5) with a pronounciating speed of 2 numbers per second. Table 5 shows the results and indicates a low score for the dyslectic group in repeating long digit-strings (l = 5). In conclusion the lower scores ,obtained in visual presentation of. numbers then seem due to perceptual factors rather than to short-term memory dysfunction.

Table 5. Auditory presentation of digit-strings. Correct scores for various lengths.

dysl. contr.

I = 3

100

100

f : 4

80

88

f - 5

42

80

CONCLUSION

As to the research on dyslexia the conclusion then seems to be that eye control, perception and recognition of letters and words, and storage pro- cesses should be studied n o t just in isolation b u t also in their mutual de- pendence on one another. This conclusion links up with the n o t i o n that dyslexia stems from many different adverse factors.

(7)

the resulting difficulties are moreover n o t confined to the reading of text, but are clearly present in the recognition of numbers. Indeed, dyslexia is a syndrome. We have hopes that the understanding of the underlaying phe- n o m e n a of dyslexia may proceed equally fast as the understanding of normal reading processes, which, in the literature as well as at IPO (Institute for Perception Research), is a subject of renewed interest.

REFERENCES

Bouma, H., Legein, Ch.P. & Van Rens, A.L.M. Visual recognition by dyslexic children. IPO Annual Progress Report 9: 104-109 (1974).

Bouma, H. & Legein, Ch.P. Foveal and parafoveal recognition of letters and words by dyslexics and by average readers. Neuropsych. (in press) 1976.

Authors' address:

Institute for Perception Research, IPO P.O. Box 513

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Column 1 shows the effect of financial structure on systemic risk excluding the control variables, column 2 controls for banking sector size (the total assets held by deposit

The majority of deaf signers are, or have been, members of student signing communities at school and boarding school outside the village, where communication between students

It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http s ://dare.uva.nl) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository).. Slipping through

33 A Procrustean assessment of the European eel stock 27 44 The fractal geometry of the European eel stock 37 55 Long-term trends in the glasseels immigrating at Den Oever,

Tot slot dank ik mijn ouders, Erik en Marianne, mijn zussen Rozemarijn en Wieteke en mijn beste vriend Tim voor hun steun, de nodige ontspanning en de interesse in mijn

are higher for a number of nicotinamide biomimetics than for natural coenzymes, but in order to obtain better insight into the overall catalytic cycle (relevant to exploitation of