• No results found

A study on the news media’s verbal and visual frames, and how these influence the subjects’ perceptions of the danger

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "A study on the news media’s verbal and visual frames, and how these influence the subjects’ perceptions of the danger"

Copied!
46
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

 

 

A study on the news media’s verbal and visual frames, and how these 

influence the subjects’ perceptions of the danger.  

 

 

Oda Brandal Erlandsen  11696710  Master’s Thesis 

Graduate School of Communication  Master’s programme Communication Science 

Supervisor: Dr. R. Azrout  28th of June, 2019 

 

 

 

         

(2)

Abstract 

This research investigated if the verbal and visual frames of the media influences the        public’s danger perceptions. To answer the research question, an online survey        embedded experiment was executed, with a 2x2 full factorial design with two        between-subjects verbal frames (terrorist vs. attack) and two between-subjects visual        frames (small font vs. big font). Further, the research aimed to explore if the frames        were mediated by negative emotions, and if the relationship between verbal frames        and negative emotions was moderated by previous experience of a terrorist attack.        Neither the verbal nor the visual frames did have a significant effect on the subjects’        perceptions of the danger, which also further lead to that there could not be        determined a mediation nor interaction effect. However, previous experience of a        terrorist attack significantly moderated the relationship between the verbal frames and        emotions. This study contributes to previous studies of media effects, and        demonstrates that negative emotions play an essential role in how the media content is        perceived. In addition, if one has personally experienced a terrorist attack, the verbal        frames of the media are likely to contribute to stronger emotional reactions.  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

1

(3)

Introduction 

“The essence of framing is selection to prioritize some facts, images, or developments                          over others, thereby unconsciously promoting one particular interpretation of                  events.” (Norris, Kern and Just, 2003) 

 

In today’s globalized world, the media have become an important institution to which        millions of citizens turn to for information everyday. One influential way in which        media can shape public opinion is by framing both events and issues in particular        ways (de Vreese, 2005). Albertson and Gadarian (2014) believed that subjects are        biased information processors, they remember and agree with threatening        information. This was further studied by Soroka and McAdams (2015), who believe        one should understand the nature of news content partly as a consequence of the        asymmetry negativity bias observed in human behaviour, and that the propensity to        over-represent negativity in the news is a product of profit-maximization. Therefore,        the media frame their content towards what their audience is most likely to be reactive        towards. After all, journalists and editors are humans as well, and inhabit the same        negativity bias as their audience (Soroka and McAdams, 2015). This would explain        why there is an overrepresentation of threatening and negative information in mass        media content today.  

Public knowledge on terrorism is massively influenced by news and other        types of media. This further amplifies people’s fears and makes everyone feel as they        could be the victim of an attack (Nacos 2003; Norris et al. 2003; Robertson, 2003;        Picard 1993; Kasperson et al. 2003; Cohen 1983). Shoshani and Slone (2008) argue        that there is a symbiotic relationship between the media and terrorism, where the       

(4)

media have been referred to as “the theatre of terror”. However, while the terrorists        may be the ones writing the script and performing the drama, “the theatre of terror” is        only possible if the media provides a stage, and presents the news to a worldwide        audience. What becomes important is that terrorism is aimed at the people watching,        and not at the actual victims (Wiemann 1983). Media power is generally symbolic and        persuasive, where they have to a certain extent the potential to control the minds of        their readers (van Dijk, 1995). Furthermore, the perception of a political message, or        news in this study, is not only shaped by textual information, but by its visual        representation (Schindler, Krämer and Müller, 2017). For example, the font of the text        can have a great influence on how the reader react to the information. Big headlines        are more dramatic and contrarily to small headlines scream a message. Therefore, by        including big headlines, the media can enhance the terrorist’s message and make it not        only more visible but also more threatening. Moreover, when both visual and verbal        frames are combined, impact on the public is further strengthened and their perception        of danger is greater. 

In addition, as presented by Small, Lerner and Fischoff (2006) causal        attributions after an aggressive and tragic event, such as a terrorist attack, may be        shaped by emotions. Being presented articles and information about dramatic events        can therefore further be shaped by negative emotions, which will lead to a stronger        perception of threat. As terrorism is both dramatic and threatening (Gans, 1979),        journalists and editors have incentives to use emotionally powerful tools, visuals and        storylines in what is referred to as a competitive media environment. This is to keep        ever-shrinking news audiences, where these elements of media coverage can        strengthen the public’s perceptions of the threat (Gadarian, 2010). In addition,       

(5)

previous experiences can contribute to an increased emotional response, and how one        finally perceives the threat presented in the news. If a person has themself been close        to or been a direct victim of a terrorist attack it is likely that news concerning        terrorism will arouse strong negative emotions. 

This study intends to take the points above further, and research the extent to        which textual and visual representation of an attack can influence subjects’        perceptions of the danger. By focusing on the framing of threatening news content,        this paper intends to explore if this type of information and representation enhances        negative emotions, and how it can influence the readers’ response. Additionally, when        the audience have personally experienced a terrorist attack, if this can moderate the        relationship between the verbal frames and emotions.  

The research question will therefore be as follow: 

RQ: To what extent do the media’s frame of an attack affect subjects’ perceptions of                              the danger? 

● Does the use of the term terrorism in the news media affect subjects’                          perceptions of the danger? And does font size independently have the same                        effect?  

● Is there an interaction effect when combining the independent variables?  ● Further, can it be moderated by previous experience and mediated by negative                       

emotions?          4

(6)

Theoretical framework 

The face of war and political conflict have been through a change. While in the past        war was fought in front-lines by the military, modern warfare occurs in the midst of        civilian populations (Shoshani and Slone, 2008). These changes have been paralleled        with rapid developments in the technology in the media, where these have come to        reflect the environment by increasing the vividness and accessibility of news, where        terrorism today is very much present (Carruthers, Cochrane and Pain, 2000). Cooper        (2001) argued that to label a violent act as terrorism can be regarded as a political        tool, but it is also a beneficial tool for the news media. Already in 1922, Walter        Lippmann worried about the impact the media have on the democratic society, where        he described the images that were created in the media as “pictures in our heads”. The        pictures are employed by the public to make sense of the information they are        presented. This is referred to by contemporary media as schemas, which people use as        a frame of reference to organise both existing knowledge and incoming information        (Coleman, 2010). As a consequence, an individual frame can possibly be reinforced,        challenged, but also remain unaffected. 

  To follow the news was argued by Boukes and Vliegenhart (2017) to be a        desirable behaviour, as it is crucial for what is today regarded as a functional        democracy. It evokes everything from political interest, increasing knowledge about        politics and motivates political participation. However, previous studies reveal that        the news media is dominated by negative news, and that negativity is what humans        are most interested in and also most reactive towards (Albertson and Gadarian, 2014).        How news is negatively framed can therefore have a great impact on what citizens       

(7)

decide to read and their further response to what they are exposed to (Soroka and        McAdams, 2015).  

Framing is always used when presenting information in the news media to the        public. Frames are discussed and evaluated differently in a wide range of        communication literature, and therefore it is essential for research to define and        present a proper definition. Entman (1993) argues that to frame is to select some        aspects of a perceived reality and make them more salient in a communicating text, in        such a way as to promote a particular problem definition, causal interpretation, moral        evaluation, and/or treatment recommendation for the item described. While frames        may have a range of causes and effects, they exist first of all as words, images and        symbols that appear on paper and in other media (Woods, 2014). Further, in political        and mass communication, Reese (2011) argues that frames influence how people        understand, remember, evaluate, and act upon a problem. This paper follows the        understanding of frames that is evaluated and discussed by McQuail (1987), that a        frame may refer to a characteristic of communication content. The news media is        responsible for providing news in the “pseudoenvironment”, where one relies on        experience and subjectively understands events that cannot be directly observed.        Therefore, news frames are important in relation to how events are reported, because        they reflect a process of recurring selection and emphasis in communication perceived        reality (Entman 1993; Gitlin 1980). This research will focus on the power of discourse        analysis, which studies the use of written, vocal, and/or sign of the language        (Coulthard, 2014). The study at hand will focus on understanding framing based on        visuals and words used in the media and evaluate the effect of these when        communicating to the public.  

(8)

Verbal Frames and Emotions 

By framing their content in a certain way the media is able to construct and influence        the perceptions of individuals in society (      ​Scheufele and Tewksbury, 2006      ​). Previous    research has revealed that terrorism has a negative effect on everything from        economic performance, tourism to psychological well being (Dinesen and Jæger,        2013). In 2011, Woods argued that the terrorism frame is extremely powerful and that        the salience of terrorism can affect subjects’ perceptions of the danger. Later on,        Brinson and Stohl (2012) suggested that the use of verbal frames, such as ‘terror’, not        only influences the public’s reaction to an event, but also their attitudes and how those        are processed in the formation of judgements. The use of certain words can have a        beneficial power to influence readers to react a certain way, as it can lead to different        cognitive processes. Cooper (2011) suggested that by labelling a violent act as        terrorism it can prime people’s concerns about the danger, attributions of their        behavior and assumptions on how to stop future attacks. This is because terrorism is        seemed as a threat people may have a strong emotional relation to, as it is uncertain        and can occur at any time or place. Therefore, the terrorism frame is likely to increase        the public’s perceptions of the danger, as opposed to if it was framed as an attack        (Woods, 2011). 

In addition, studies suggest that it is important to consider the role of

       

emotional responses in public opinion and political judgement in the field of political        communication (Cho et al, 2010). A wide range of research supports the existence of        a relationship between media coverage on terrorism and negative emotional        consequences (Shoshani, and Slone, 2008). Emotions are a strong contributor to        people’s reaction towards what they are exposed to; terrorism news stories are likely       

(9)

to induce negative emotions as they evoke a fear of death (Landau et al, 2004). In        fact, in a study, Slone, Shoshani and Baumgarten-Katz (2008) revealed that        participants exhibited a higher anxiety level after they viewing terrorist related media        broadcasts compared to non-terrorism-related political news. The power of emotions        is commonly used in the news to affect how citizens process information. Further        studies also concluded that terrorism has a negative psychological impact: it increases        anxiety (Gunter, 1994; Slone, 2000), anger, (Newhagen, 1998; Yukawa, Endo, &        Yoshida, 2001) and fear (Joshi & Kaschak, 1998). The study of Gadarian and        Albertson (2014) revealed that strong and negative emotions led people to pay more        attention to the news information presented to them. By using emotional appeals in        news, one can influence citizens to pay more attention to a news story and make them        willing to take action or behave a certain way based on what they read.  

In addition, the commonality of anger and anxiety can be related to the theory        of protection motivation, which argues that personally threatening stimuli is        producing automatic anxiety responses that together with high motivation for defence,        demonstrated in anger, increases the fear for potential future attacks (Shoshani, and        Slone, 2008). By taking this theory into account, negative emotions will explain the        relationship between the word terrorism in the news and the subjects’ perceptions of        the danger. As a consequence, the hypotheses will be as follows: 

 

H1: Individuals exposed to the word terrorism in news articles have higher                        perceptions of the danger compared to individuals exposed to news articles without                        the word terrorism. 

 

(10)

H2: Emotions explain the effect between the word terrorism in the news and the                            subject’s perceptions of the danger.  

 

Figure 1.  

  Verbal Frames, Emotions and Previous Experience 

Norris et al (2003) argue that the terrorism frame does not represent a distinct form of        the danger, but a frame that has been repeatedly described by the mass media as a new        social problem. It was revealed in the study of Slone, Shoshani and Baumgarten-Katz        (2008) that individuals that had high exposure to political violence showed much        greater vulnerability to exposure of media content that included the life threatening        events. As a consequence, the respondents had a higher level of anxiety related to        terror, compared to the ones that had not experienced an actual terrorist attack.        Further, Ahern, Kiehl, Sole and Byers (2006) argue that resilience is the most        common response after one has experienced an attack at the scale of a terrorist attack,        which indicates that one is prepared to recover after a trauma. This can further lead to        an automatic response to news content related to threatening information. Woods, Ten        Eyck, Kaplowitz and Shlapentokh (2008) suggested that proximity influences the        public’s negative emotions towards a terrorist attack. Therefore, if one has       

(11)

experienced terrorism, been close to a terrorist attack, or had friends or family that        were victims of it, terrorism-related news content will likely trigger anger and fear.        The hypothesis will be as follows: 

H3: Previous experience strengthens the effect between the presence of the word                        terrorism and emotions.  

 

Figure 2. 

   

Visual Frames and Emotions 

The perceptions of the messages in the news are not only shaped by the textual        information, but also by its visual representation (Schindler, Krämer and Müller,        2017); studies reveal that political and visual communication can be strongly        connected (Barnhust and Steele, 1997). Research suggests that the media offer what is        referred to as a diet of visual material chosen to attract their audience’s attention. In        addition, visual material is proven to be memorable and can be especially forceful        depending on its perceptual salience (Joffe, 2008). People are more affected by the        visuals than many would first think, and the way the media decide to visually present        their news can be an influential type of representation. Further, in the study of Machin        10

(12)

and Niblock (208) they argue that the size of the font is a form of salience, as it takes        up great space proportionally on a news page. Big fonts can be seen as being a part of        sensationalism, which means that it is a vivid and dramatic presentation of an event,        which further gives them a forceful impact on the mind of the reader (Stephenson and        Bromley, 1998). As a consequence, the size of the font, when presenting negative        information, has demonstrated to affect how people perceive the information they        receive, and further how they process it. There are, however, different views        regarding if big fonts actually signify the right values, and if one should use font size        to draw attention (Coulter and Coulter, 2005). Hence, the news media are constantly        making the headlines big, as this is often what readers are dragged towards.  

Last, it is argued that visuals are used to send people in emotive pathways        (Joffe, 2208). The font size is known as a powerful tool in relation to strengthening        the public’s reaction towards what they read. If the font is big, the message will        become more threatening and important to the reader, compared to if it is small        (Stephenson and Bromley, 1998). In the study of Schindler, Krämer and Müller        (2017), they argue that there is both effective and important with visual cues to        influence citizens to process political information, or in this study news. This is        confirmed in the study of Bayer, Sommer and Schacht (2012), where their results        reveal that large font size leads to an increase for early emotions effect, which can be        an indication of why the news media often uses big fonts in their headlines. It is not        only to draw attention to the text, but also to evoke certain types of emotions. It can        therefore be argued that when the news media present a type of layout/font, it can        have priming/framing effects on a presented message (Schindler, Krämer and Muller,        2017). Taking this into account, the hypotheses will therefore be: 

(13)

 

H4: When the size of the font is big in news articles the subjects’ perceptions of the                                  danger will be higher, compared to if it is small. 

 

H5: Emotions explain the relationship between the font size in the news and the                            subjects’ perceptions of the danger. 

 

Figure 3.  

   

 

Interaction between Verbal and Visual Frames 

By taking the theories presented in this paper into account, it is likely to believe that        there is an interaction effect if one combines the independent variables. By applying        frames to a crisis situation like a terrorist attack, it may serve as a strategy to identify        main causes and responsible agents, make moral judgments, and, finally, to suggest        policy responses to the event (Papacharissa and de Fatima Oliveira, 2008: 55).  

In addition, as already discussed by Bayer, Sommer and Schacht (2012) big        font sizes increase emotional effect, which is an indication of why big letters are       

(14)

popular and successful in the news media. It is evident that the word terrorism and big        font size separately may influence the public’s perceptions of the danger, as it can        evoke strong negative emotions. The importance of the interaction of the verbal and        visual frames is that if the font is large and includes negative and threatening        information, it is more likely to evoke a negative reaction, compared to if it is just a        big font with neutral information. With combining strong visuals with threatening        information, the message will automatically be regarded as a larger happening.        Therefore, it is likely that the combination of these has an effect as well. The last        hypothesis will therefore be:  

 

H6: There is an interaction between font size and use of the term terrorism in news                                articles on the subjects’ perception of the danger. 

  Figure 4.   

 

 

 

 

13

(15)

Methods 

Experimental design 

In order to answer the research question, an online survey embedded experiment has        been conducted. A 2x2 full factorial design with two between-subjects verbal frames        (terrorist vs. attack) and two between-subjects visual frames (small font vs. big font)        has been administered. The following variables were examined: the independent        variables verbal frames and visual frames; dependent variable subjects’ perceptions of        the danger; the moderator previous experience, and the mediator emotions.  

The true experiment was carried out by an online questionnaire software

       

named Qualtrics. The respondents were randomly assigned to one of four conditions        through a randomization procedure that Qualtrics offers. The manipulation involved        participants  being exposed to a news article within four conditions. These four        conditions combined the independent variables verbal frames (attack vs. terrorist        attack) and visual frames (small font vs. big font). The respondents received either a        news article that included a small font - attack framing, big font - attack framing,        small font - terrorist attack framing, or big font - terrorist attack framing.  

 

Sampling  

A convenience sample was conducted, where the respondents were recruited through        social media platforms such as Facebook, Whatsapp and Linkedin, and through the        word of mouth. The respondents were required to be above the age of 18, and needed        to have enough English qualification to fill in an English survey questionnaire. This        study intended to collect respondents from all over the world and people of all        generations. To access the questionnaire the participants received an anonymous       

(16)

online link, and the survey was estimated to take around 5 minutes to complete. The        online questionnaire was accessible from the 14th of May 2019 till the 28th of May        2019, and had in total         ​N​=208 valid respondents. Prior to this, 50 respondents had to be        removed as they did not finish the survey completely. In addition, it was observed that        the respondents were not equally distributed between the conditions, which is due to        the randomization algorithm in Qualtrics (see table 5).  

 

Table 5.​ Diagram of the number of participants in each condition.    

 

Most of the respondents were female (68,8 %), followed by male (31,3%). The        age of the respondents ranged from 19 to 88, where the average age was M= 31,75        (SD=12,78). 32 different nationalities participated in the survey, which made the        sample quite diverse. However, the majority of the respondents were from Northern        Europe. Most of the respondents came from Norway (56,3 %), and other countries        such as the Netherlands (7,2 %) and Sweden (5,8 %). The highest level of education        most of the respondents had received was a Bachelor’s Degree (51,4 %), followed by        Master’s Degree (36,1 %) and High School Diploma (7,7 %). In terms of their current       

(17)

employment status, most respondents were employed full-time (50 %), followed by        students (33,7 %) and employed part-time (6,3%). 

 

Procedure 

After receiving an anonymous link through an online media platform, respondents        gained an access to the online questionnaire. First, they were provided with        information about the topic and aim of the experiment. However, the information was        not too detailed in order to avoid priming the respondents. Further, information about        the consent of participating was presented, where the participants had to agree on        being sufficiently informed and if they wanted to proceed to the experiment. They        additionally received some demographic questions such as gender, age, nationality,        level of education and current employment status. 

Furthermore, the respondents were exposed to the stimuli, where they received        one of the conditions determined in the experimental design. After receiving the        stimuli the participants were asked to what degree they have felt angry, anxious or        frightened when reading the news about an attack. Further, questions concerning risk        judgements, information seeking and on how to stop future attacks followed. One of        the last steps was the manipulation check, where the respondents were asked if they        remembered the title of the BBC article they read and if they considered the headline        as big or small. Last, in order to measure the moderator previous experience, the        respondents were asked if they or a friend or/and a family member had been close to        or witnessed a terrorist attack, or if they just had experienced it through the news        media. Last, the participants were thanked for their participation, and finally, the       

(18)

researcher revealed the aim of the study. They were also informed that the BBC        article provided in their stimuli was not real, and that it was only used in this research.  

 

Measures 

Independent Variables and Manipulation 

The independent variables and stimuli in this study were two verbal frames (terrorism        vs. attack) and two visual frames (big font vs. small font). As the sample was        international it was decided to use a manipulated article from the BBC as the stimulus        for each condition. This was to make sure that the respondents had to some extent        knowledge about the newspaper, and because the BBC is additionally known to be a        reliable newspaper (Jackson, 2015).  

 

Dependent Variable 

The dependent variable was subjects’ perceptions of the danger, adapted from the        study of Woods (2011). However, by subjects’ perceptions of the danger, this paper        intends to measure it through three dependent variables. These are if the frames in the        media influence their attribution of behaviour (information seeking), increase their        concern about the danger, and influence their assumptions on how to stop future        attacks, which is similar to the variables in Woods (2011) study. In addition, this        study includes the variable attribution of behaviour (information seeking) from the        study of Boyle et al. (2004).  

First, when the media apply frames to a crisis situation such as a terrorist        attack, it can serve as a strategy to make moral judgements and to suggest policy        responses to the event that occurred (Winter, 2008). When reading about an attack it       

(19)

influences the public’s assumptions and thoughts on how to stop future attacks. This        can be by agreeing to policies such as having stricter national border controls, make        the police more present in the streets and that the authority should increase the        surveillance to stop future attacks. These were therefore measured on a Seven-point        Likert scale, where the respondents could agree on statements regarding these policies        on a scale from (1) strongly agree to (7) strongly disagree. A factor analysis was        conducted, and the results revealed that all the items loaded on one component, where        the factors explain 73,15% of the variance in the original variables. The reliability        scale was good, Cronbach’s alpha = .82(​M​=4,69, ​SD​=1,65).  

Second, the uncertainty reduction theory argues that individuals are motivated        to seek more information and learn more about terrorism to reduce the uncertainty        about what happened (Boyle et al, 2005). To measure the information seeking        variable the respondents were asked if they search for more information about the        attack when reading about it in the news and if they further search for information        about attacks in general. This was measured on a Five-point Likert scale from (1)        always to (5) never. In addition, it measured if they needed to do their own research to        feel well informed on a Seven-point Likert scale from (1) strongly agree to (7)        strongly disagree. A factor analysis was conducted, which revealed that all the items        loaded on one component, where they together explain 72,15% of the variance in the        original variables. The reliability scale was acceptable, Cronbach’s alpha =        .75(​M​=3,10, ​SD​=1,29).  

Third, when reading about attacks it can influence the attributes of the        subjects’ risk judgements. By searching for more information one increases the        knowledge about what happened and receive an overview of the threat. By becoming       

(20)

more informed it may influence citizens to be more worried. This will further make        them become more concerned that an attack will happen in their country in the near        future, feel concerned about themselves or a friend/family member being a victim of        future attacks. In addition, it was asked if they have experienced an attack that shaken        their own sense of personal safety and security. The measures were influenced by the        variables from the study of Huddy, Feldman, Taber and Lahav (2005). This was        measured on a Five-point Likert scale from (1) Very often to (5) never. A factor        analysis was conducted, which further revealed that all the items loaded on one        component, where they together explain 66,91% of the variance in the original        variables. The reliability scale was acceptable, Cronbach’s alpha = .75(​M​=3,47,        SD​=1,05).  

 

Moderator and Mediator 

The moderator in this study is previous experience of terrorism. As argued in the        study of Woods, Ten Eyck, Kaplowitz and Shlapentokh (2008) do proximity        influence the public’s negative emotions towards a terrorist attack. At the end of the        questionnaire in this experiment, the respondents were asked if they themselves,        and/or a friend or family member had been a direct victim, been in close vicinity or        been in the same city as a terrorist attack, or if they just read about it in the news. This        was coded into No experience (0), Self (1), Family and/or friends (2), and self and        family and/or friends (3). The descriptives revealed that 42,8 % did not have any        experience at all, 23,1% had self experienced or been close to a terrorist attack, 29,3%        had family/friends that had experienced a terrorist attack, and 4,8% had both        themselves and had family/friends who experienced a terrorist attack. In addition, the       

(21)

mediator in this study is emotions, where it measured negative related emotions such        as anger, anxiety and fear. These were influenced by the study of Vasilopoulos,        Marcus, Valentino and Foucault (2018) and Albertson and Gadarian (2014). The        respondents were asked how often, if ever, they have felt angry, anxious and        frightened when reading about attacks in the news on a Five-point Likert scale        measured from (1) very strongly to (5) not at all. A factor analysis was conducted,        which determined that all the items loaded on one component, where together they        explained 72,36% of the variance in the original variables. The reliability scale was        good, Cronbach’s alpha = .81(​M​=3,04, ​SD​=1,14). 

 

Results 

Randomization Check 

In order to reveal if it was a difference in the four conditions (terror vs. attack and        small font vs. big font), it was important to execute a randomization check. This was        tested with chi-square analyses and ANOVA on demographics such as gender, age,        level of education, employment status and on the moderator previous experience.        First, the chi-square analysis demonstrated that gender was equally distributed        between the conditions (​p​=. 524). An analysis of variance was executed on age, which        revealed that it was not significant and was therefore equally distributed between the        conditions (   ​p =.597). An ANOVA demonstrated that the participants were distributed        evenly in terms of the level of education between the four conditions (​p​=.451).  

Furthermore, a chi-square analysis test on current employment status,        revealed that there was not a significant difference between the participants in the four        conditions (​p​= .648). Last, the chi-square tested on the previous experience revealed       

(22)

that it was not significant (​p​=.740), which indicates that the moderator was also        equally distributed between the four conditions. This demonstrates that the        randomization worked for all the demographics and the moderator in this experiment,        and it was consequently not necessary to control for any of the variables.  

 

Manipulation Check 

A manipulation check was carried out to explore if the stimuli worked in the manner        they were supposed to. There were two questions, one asked if the name of the title        was “Deadly terrorist attack in London” or “Deadly attack in London” and the second        asked if the respondents considered the headline of the article as big or small. In order        to determine if the stimuli worked, a chi-square test to reveal if the respondents        remembered the manipulation they were presented. The chi-square revealed that 141        participants answered one or both of the questions wrong.  

However, the results demonstrated that out of the respondents who were        exposed to the small font stimuli, 40 considered the headline to be big, and of the        respondents who received the big font article, 14 of them consider the headline to be        small. The results revealed that there was a significant moderate association       ​Φ=.49, p​=.000. In addition, out of the respondents exposed to the attack frame, 12 believe        that they read about a terrorist attack, and of the respondents exposed to the terror        frame, 32 believed that it was an attack. The cross-tabulation revealed that there was a        significant strong association     Φ=​ ​.59, ​p​= .000, as the majority of the respondents        answered the questions correct for the specific manipulation. As a consequence, it can        be argued that the manipulation was successful as the relationships were significant        for the conditions.  

(23)

 

Hypothesis 1 

The first hypothesis suggested that individuals exposed to the word terrorism in news        articles have higher perceptions of the danger compared to individuals exposed to        news articles without the word terrorism. A one-way ANOVA was conducted to test        the hypothesis. 

The results revealed that the assumptions for the one-way ANOVA have been        met, when testing the verbal frames on seeking for more information. The equal        variance in the population has been met, Levene's (3, 204)=.630,      ​p​=.596.  Additionally, there was not a significant difference between the verbal frames if the        respondents would seek more information,         ​F​(1, 104) = .01, ​p = .923. Further, the        one-way ANOVA tested on the influence of the verbal frames on the respondents        assumptions on how to stop future attacks, revealed that the equal variance in the        population has been met, Levene's (3, 204) = .195,       ​p​=.900. There was, however, not a        significant difference between the verbal frames on the assumptions on how to stop        future attacks,   ​F​(1, 41)= 2.30, ​p​= .131. Last, when executing a one-way ANOVA to        test the influence of the verbal frames on risk judgments, it revealed that the equal        variance in the population was met, Levene’s (3, 204)=1.20,       ​p​=.313. Hence, it was not          a significant difference between the verbal frames on the respondents risk judgements,        F​(1, 1)= .132, ​p​= .716. In conclusion, H1 could not be confirmed (See table 11 for        descriptives). 

 

Hypothesis 2 

(24)

The second hypothesis claimed that when people read about terrorism in the news        negative emotions will more likely arouse, compared to if they read about an attack.        The emotions will further lead to higher perceptions of the danger. Model 4 in the        PROCESS macro by Hayes (2013) was executed to test the mediation effect. This was        performed to explore if emotions had a mediated effect on the relationship between        the verbal frames and the subjects’ perceptions of the danger. A bootstrapping        procedure with a sample of 5000 to analyse the indirect effect of 95% bias-corrected        bootstrap confidence interval. This analysis tested the verbal frames as the        independent variable, emotions as the mediator, and information seeking, risk        judgements and assumptions on how to stop future attacks as dependent variables.        The PROCESS test was therefore executed three times to test each of the dependent        variables.  

As expected after the results revealed in the previous analysis, the verbal        frames did not have a significant effect on risk judgements, also called path c’(​b​=-.13,        p=.​895), and did additionally not have an effect on emotions, path a(​b​=-.26, ​p​=.522).        Although not hypothesised, emotions had a significant effect on risk judgements, path        b(​b​=.48, ​t​(205)=7,83, ​p​=.000). In addition, the verbal frames did not have a        significant indirect effect on risk judgements, as the bias-corrected bootstrap        confidence interval for the indirect effect included a zero (Indirect effect=-.12, 95%        BCBI[-0.55 0.25]). Second, the verbal frames did not have a direct effect on        information seeking, path c’      ​(b=-.​00​, p=​.991​)​. In addition, the results for a and c’-path        did not reveal a significant mediation effect on information seeking and assumptions        on how to stop future attacks. However, both analyses demonstrated that the b-path        was significant (see figure 7 and 8). Further, the verbal frames did not have a       

(25)

significant indirect effect on information seeking(Indirect effect=-.06, 95%        BCBI[-0.28 0.13]) and assumptions on how to stop future attacks(Indirect effect=-.13,        95% BCBI[-1,90 0.29), because the bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for        the indirect effect included a zero. As a consequence, the second hypothesis could not        be confirmed. 

 

 

Figure 6.   ​Path model: Mediation effect on the relationship between the verbal frames and risk        judgements.  

 

Figure 7.   ​Path model: Mediation effect on the relationship between the verbal frames and information        seeking.  

 

(26)

 

Figure 8.   ​Path model: Mediation effect on the relationship between the verbal frames and assumptions        on how to stop future attacks. 

 

Hypothesis 3 

The third hypothesis suggested that if friends and/or family of the respondents, or they        themselves had experienced or been close to a terrorist attack, it will strengthen the        effect between emotions and the word terrorism​.       ​In order to test this moderated        mediation effect, model 7 of the SPSS macro PROCESS by Hayes (2013) was run.        This was executed to test if previous experience of terror could moderate the effect        between  the  verbal frames    and emotions. PROCESS conducts an OLS        regression-based path analysis, which estimates both the direct and indirect effects        (Hayes, 2013) A sample of 5000 with a bootstrapping procedure was determined to        analyse the indirect effect with a 95% confidence interval. As the moderator was a        nominal variable, a dummy variable created, which was coded into No (0) and self,        friends/family and both (1) in order to execute the analysis.  

The results of the analysis revealed that there was an interaction effect

       

between the verbal frames and previous experience on emotions, as the results        revealed are significant (​b​= 1,94, ​p​= .018). In addition, the moderated mediation was       

(27)

statistically significant because the bootstrap confidence interval did not include a        zero (index of moderated mediation= .10, boot SE= .49, 95% BCBI [0.17 2.06]). In        conclusion,  H3 could be confirmed, previous experience of a terrorist attack        moderates the relationship between verbal frames and emotions.  

   

Hypothesis 4 

The fourth hypothesis argued that if the size of the font is big the subjects’ will have        higher perceptions of the danger, compared to if the font size of the article is small.        As the subjects’ perceptions of the danger contains three variables, it was conducted a        one-way ANOVA to explore if there is an effect of the visual frames on information        seeking, risk judgements and assumptions on how to stop future attacks.  

The results revealed that the assumptions for the one-way ANOVA have been        met, when testing the visual frames on seeking for more information. The equal        variance in the population has been met, Levene’s       ​F​(3, 204) = .63, ​p​= .596. Hence, it        was not a significant difference between the visual frames if the respondents would        seek more information,     ​F​(1,19) = 1,71, ​p​= .193. In addition, when conducting an        one-way ANOVA analysis on the effect of the visual frames on respondents        assumptions on how to stop future attack, it revealed that the equal variance in the        population was met, Levene’s (3,204) = .20,       ​p​= .900. It was, however, not a        significant difference between the visual frames on the assumptions on how to stop        future attacks, F(1, 4) = .258,       ​p​= .612. Last, the one-way ANOVA tested on the        influence of the visual frames on respondents risk judgements, revealed that the equal        variance in the population was met, Levene’s (3,204) = 1,20,       ​p​= .313. Hence, it was         

(28)

not a significant difference between the visual frames on the respondents risk        judgements, ​F​(1, 13)= 1,47, ​p​= .227. In conclusion, hypothesis 4 could not be        confirmed (see table 11 for descriptives). 

     

Hypothesis 5 

The fifth hypothesis expected that when subjects’ are exposed to articles with big        fonts in the news, the negative emotions will be stronger, compared to if they are        exposed to articles with small fonts. The negative emotions will further lead to higher        perceptions of the danger. To test the mediation effect, model 4 in PROCESS macro        by Hayes (2013) was executed. This analysis was run to explore if emotions had a        mediated effect on the relationship between the visual frames and the subjects’        perceptions of the danger. This test included a bootstrapping procedure with a sample        of 5000 to analyse the indirect effect of 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence        interval. This analysis tested the visual frames as the independent variable, emotions        as the mediator, and information seeking, risk judgements and assumptions on how to        stop future attacks as the dependent variables. 

As expected after the results determined in the previous analysis, the visual        frames did not have a direct effect on risk judgements, path c’ (​b​= .12, ​p​= .335), and        did additionally not have an effect on emotions, path a (      ​b​= -.31, ​p​=442). Although not          hypothesised, emotions did have a significant effect on risk judgements, path b        (​b​=.16, ​p​=.000). In addition, the visual frames did not have a significant indirect        effect on risk judgements, as the bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the       

(29)

indirect effect did include a zero (Indirect effect= .15, 95% BCBI [-0.23 0.54]). In        addition, the results for a and c’-path did not reveal a significant direct effect on        information seeking and assumptions on how to stop future attacks. However, both        analyses demonstrated that the b-path was significant (see figure 10 and 11). Further,        the visual frames did not have a significant indirect effect on information seeking        (Indirect effect = .07, 95% BCBI [-0.12 0.27]) and assumptions on how to stop future        attacks (Indirect effect = .160, 95% BCBI [-0.24 0.65]), because the bias-corrected        bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect included a zero. In conclusion, the        fifth hypothesis could not be confirmed. 

 

 

Figure 9.   ​Path model: Mediation effect on the relationship between the visual frames and risk        judgements.  

 

(30)

Figure 10.   ​Path model: Mediation effect on the relationship between the visual frames and information        seeking.  

 

Figure 11.   ​Path model: Mediation effect on the relationship between the visual frames and assumptions        on how to stop future attacks. 

 

Hypothesis 6 

The last hypothesis claimed that there is an interaction effect between visual frames        and verbal frames on the subjects’ perceptions of the danger. An analysis of variance        was conducted to explore if the hypothesis could be confirmed. Equally to the other        hypotheses tested, the independent variables were tested on information seeking, risk        judgements and assumptions on how to stop future attacks. First, the results revealed        that it was not a statistically significant interaction between the effects of the visual        frames and the verbal frames on information seeking,       ​F​(1,8) =.159, ​p​= .691. Further,          the second analysis revealed that it was not a statistically significant interaction        between the effects of the visual frames and the verbal frames on risk judgements,        F​(1, 0)=.031, ​p​=.861. Last, the analysis of variance additionally revealed that there        was not a significant interaction effect between the visual frames and the verbal        frames on assumptions on how to stop future attacks       ​F​(1, 25) = 1,43, ​p​=.233. In        conclusion, the sixth and last hypothesis could also not be confirmed.  

(31)

 

Table 11. ​Mean values of Dependent Variables by Condition.  

 

Discussion 

In a media landscape with an increased focus on negativity in the news, it has become        interesting to research the effect of negative and threatening frames on the media        audiences (Soroka and McAdams, 2015). This study aimed to examine the impact of        the visual and verbal frames in the media on the subjects’ perceptions of the danger.        The research included the following dependent variables: - risk judgements, -        assumptions on how to stop future attacks, and - information seeking. It also used        negative emotions as the mediator variable and previous experience of a terrorist        attack as the moderator variable. The results revealed that there was a significant        moderated mediation effect, where previous experience strengthened the effect        between the presence of the word terrorism and emotions. However, the other        hypotheses tested did not reveal any statistically significant results. 

   

(32)

 

Verbal Frames, Emotions and Previous Experience 

The results in the first hypothesis revealed that people exposed to the attack frame had        higher perceptions of the danger, compared to the subjects’ exposed to the terror        frame. This contradicts what the first hypothesis suggested and what a large amount        of previous literature suggest. By labelling a violent act as terrorism rather than a        criminal act, or in this study attack, it primes the subjects’ perceptions of the danger        (Cooper, 2011). Hence, as this study revealed, the attack frame lead slightly to higher        perceptions of the danger, compared to the terrorist attack frame. This may be due to        an attack can be regarded to be more uncertain than a terrorist attack, as terrorists        often have a certain goal with their violence. This is interesting in relation to the risk        perception research, where it is suggested that danger which is perceived as new,        evokes greater levels perceived threat, compared to old and familiar hazards        (Fischhoff et al. 1978, Covello 1992, Slovic 2004, Sjöberg 2005). When an attack is        presented in the news it may indicate that it is a new and more uncertain type of        danger, as it is not confirmed what the motivation for the violence was. This may        influence the violent act to be regarded as more threatening than a terrorist attack.        Consequently, terrorism has had an increased media coverage within the last decade        (Powell, 2011), and therefore may not be regarded as that uncertain and threatening        for today’s news audience.  

This correspond with the study of Woods (2011), where his results revealed        that by manipulating a term by labelling it as terrorism, did not have an effect on        people’s worry. He suggested that the reason for this could possibly have been        because the respondents conceptualize it as terrorism, even though it was not.       

(33)

However, in this research paper more people who were exposed to the terror stimuli        labeled the article they read as an attack, rather than the other direction. For future        research, it may be interesting to change the manipulation into a criminal act, rather        than an attack. Or narrow the focus by studying the difference between how the media        label radical Islamic terrorism compared to extreme right terrorism and the effects of        it, as this is something that has become evident in the media the last years.  

In addition, when including emotions, it revealed that despite that the fact that        the direct and indirect effects did not have a significant influence on the subjects’        perceptions of the danger, respondents with negative emotions may have higher risk        judgements, information seeking and assumptions on how to stop future attacks.        However, as the results of this analysis do not reveal causality, it can also indicate that        higher danger perceptions lead to more negative emotions. This indicates that        negative emotions and people’s danger perceptions may have an effect on each other,        but that the framing of news events cannot be significantly demonstrated to evoke        negative emotions and influence the subjects’ threat perceptions. It is, however,        important for future research to continue to study the relationship between news        content on negative emotions, especially with the negativity bias that is evident within        the news content today (Soroka and Mcadams, 2015). 

Further, previous experience did determine a significant moderated mediation        effect, and therefore moderate the relationship between the verbal frames and the        negative emotions. It is an interesting finding, as this has not been sufficiently studied        in previous literature. However, it was revealed in the study of Slone, Shoshani and        Baumgarten-Katz (2008) that individuals with a high exposure to political violence        showed much greater vulnerability to exposure of media content that included life       

(34)

threatening events. Therefore, this is confirmed in this study, as previous experience        of a terrorist attack should be a factor that should be taken into further consideration        when researching the effect of terrorist-related news content. As previous experience        strenghtens the effect of verbal frames on negative emotions, further research should        dive into which kind of emotions arise from it, and look more specifically at how this        can further affect the respondents reactions towards terrorism-related news content.        Lastly, psychological trauma is an interesting moderator for future research on the        influence of threatening information on the audience of the news media. 

 

Visual Frames and Emotions 

Regarding the visual frames, the results revealed that the respondents had higher        perceptions of the danger when the font of the article was big, compared to when it        was small. This correlates with what previous research has revealed, that big fonts can        be argued to be a part of sensationalism, which indicates that it is both a vivid and        dramatic presentation of an event. That this gives a forceful impact on the mind of the        media audience (Stephenson and Bromley, 1998). However, a great amount of the        respondents failed the manipulation check, which may be the reason why the results        were not significant for the direct effect. Hence, it is interesting if the respondents still        were unconsciously manipulated by the stimuli, despite thinking that they were        exposed to something else than what their manipulation intended to. When taking the        manipulation check into account, the threatening information may have made the        participants in the small font condition think that the font was big, as the information        they read seemed both vivid and dramatic. 

(35)

Further, it is interesting for future research to study the effect of the font, as it        has become more evident that the media uses this as a visual tool to draw attention.        However, there is a lot of research on visual images and audio, but there is a lack of        studies that focuses on the power of the font size and visual appearance of text. The        media have a huge influence on the public’s perceptions of the world, and therefore it        is essential to study the tools the news media apply to influence their readers.  

It is also interesting to relate the visual frames with emotions, as studies        suggest that there is a strong link between visual information processing and the        emotions of individuals. Research demonstrate that emotions likely play a role in        individuals’ level of concern about terrorism (Fahmy, Cho, Wanta and Song, 2006).        This can be related to the results of this study, where emotions had a significant role        in the subjects’ perceptions of the danger. However, there were no significant direct        effect or indirect effect of visual frames on emotions and risk judgements, information        seeking and the assumptions on how to stop future attacks. It may still be interesting        for future studies to take this further, as negative news has become more evident in        the media today. 

Last, the results were the same for the interaction effect, that when one is        combining the visual and verbal frames the results did not reveal any statistically        significant results. However, if future studies can demonstrate a main effect for each        of the independent variables, it is likely that the combination of the variables will        increase the subjects’ perceptions of the danger. A screaming threatening message can        be argued as having a greater influence, than just a screaming neutral message or a        threatening message alone. 

 

(36)

    Limitations 

Some limitations have to be taken into account regarding this study. First, five out of        six hypotheses could not be confirmed. This may be due to the participants not pay        sufficient attention or remember what they saw in the manipulation or that the        questions asked were not clearly formulated. For future studies, it is important to be        cautious and clear when posing questions in an online survey.  

Further, this study included one type of media, which was the British news        outlet the BBC. This can be regarded as a limitation because most of the respondents        were from Norway or other Northern European countries, which may have influenced        the respondents’ reaction towards the content they read. Moreover, as it was an online        questionnaire and the news articles were manipulated by the researcher, some of the        participants may have recognised that the articles were not real. In addition, because        this study had an international sample, the stimuli and the questionnaire were in        English, which may have had an impact on the respondents’ answers, as English is not        the first language of most of the participants. This was, however, because this paper        executed a convenience sample, and can therefore not be argued to represent the        general public. Therefore, the results have to be considered with caution as the        respondents are not in their natural habitat. For future studies to focus on one        nationality, instead of having an international sample, or if possible, do a field        experiment 

  Conclusion 

(37)

The media have great power in being the main provider of distributing information to        the public in today’s society. It is essential for the future of democracy that audiences        become more cautious and aware as to how the media frame their news content, since        as this study showed, framing can impact their reaction and opinion regarding news        information. This study sought to contribute to the research on media framing and        negativity bias, and how it can influence the subjects’ perceptions of the danger        presented. The findings revealed that previous experience can strengthen the        relationship between the verbal frames of the media and the negative emotions.        However, the majority of hypotheses tested did not result in any significant findings,        this is still something that should be studied further, as verbal frames such as        terrorism and visual frames such as font size are very much present in the news media        today. This study demonstrated that to some extent the presentation of news content in        the media can have an effect on the news audience. Additionally, previous experience        of a terrorist attack moderated the relationship between the verbal frames of the media        and emotions. This leads us to conclude that the way in which the media decides to        frame their content, both visually and verbally, can prompt different cognitive        processes, and as a result shape the public’s perceptions on what they read. 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

36

(38)

 

 

Reference List: 

 

Bayer, M., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2012). Font size matters—emotion and attention in cortical responses to written words. ​PloS one​, ​7​(5), e36042.

Bradley, S.D., Angelini, J.R., Lee, S. (2007) Psychophysiological and Memory Effects of Negative Political ADS: Aversive, Arousing, and Well Remembered. Journal of Advertising, 36 (4), 115-127. 

Boukes, M., and Vliegenhart, R. (2017) News consumption and its unpleasant side        effect. Journal of Media Psychology, 29, 137-137. 

Boyle, M. P., Schmierbach, M., Armstrong, C. L., McLeod, D. M., Shah, D. V., & Pan, Z. (2004). Information seeking and emotional reactions to the September 11 terrorist attacks. ​Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly​, ​81​(1), 155-167.

Brinson, M.E., and Stohl, M. (2012) Media Framing of Terrorism: Implications for        Public Opinion, Civil Liberties, and Counterterrorism Policies. Journal of        international and intercultural communication, 5(4), 270-290. 

Cooper, H.A., 2001. Terrorism: the problem of definition revisited. American        Behavioral Scientist, 44 (6), 881–893. 

Dinesen, P.T., and Jæger, M.M. (2013) The Effect of Terror on Institutional Trust: New Evidence from the 3/11 Madrid Terrorist Attack. Political Psychology, 34(6), 917-926.

Entman, R. M. (2004). ​Projections of power: Framing news, public opinion, and US foreign policy​. University of Chicago Press.

Woods, J. (2007). What we talk about when we talk about terrorism: Elite press coverage of terrorism risk from 1997 to 2005. ​Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics​, ​12​(3), 3-20.

Gadarian, S.K., & Albertson, B. (2014). Anxiety, immigration, and the search for information. Political Psychology, 35 (2), 133-164.

Coleman, R. (2010). Framing the pictures in our heads. ​Doing news framing analysis:

Empirical and theoretical perspectives​, 233-261.

Gadarian, S. K. (2010). The politics of threat: How terrorism news shapes foreign policy attitudes. ​The Journal of Politics​, ​72​(2), 469-483.

(39)

Nacos, B. L., Bloch-Elkon, Y., & Shapiro, R. Y. (2011). ​Selling fear:

Counterterrorism, the media, and public opinion​. University of Chicago Press.

Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2013). Conditional process modeling: Using structural equation modeling to examine contingent causal processes.

Chyi, H. I. and McCombs, M. (2004) Media Salience and the Process of Framing: coverage of the Columbine School Shooting. J&MC Quarterly, 81(1), 22-35.

De Vreese, C. H. (2005). News framing: Theory and typology. ​Information design

journal & document design​, ​13​(1).

Hall, C. M. (2002) Travel Safety, Terrorism and the Media: The significance of the        issue-attention cycle. Current Issues in Tourism, 5(5), 458-466. 

Huddy, L., Feldman, S., Taber, C., & Lahav, G. (2005). Threat, anxiety, and support        of antiterrorism policies. American Journal of Political Science, 49, 593-608 

Lerner, J.S:, Gonzales, R.M., Small, D.A., and Fischhoff, B. (2003) Effects of Fear and Anger on Perceived Risks of Terrorism: A National Field Experiment. Psychological Science, 14(2), 144-150.

McQuail, D. (1987). ​Mass communication theory: An introduction​. Sage Publications, Inc.

Nellis and Savage (2012) Does watching the News affect Fear of Terrorism? The        Importance of Media Exposure on Terrorism Fear. Crime and Delinquency, 58(5),        748-7768. 

Papacharissi, Z., & de Fatima Oliveira, M. (2008). News frames terrorism: A comparative analysis of frames employed in terrorism coverage in US and UK newspapers. ​The international journal of press/politics​, ​13​(1), 52-74.

Schacht A, Sommer W (2009) Time course and task dependence of emotion effects in        word processing. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 9, 28–43. 

Scheufele, D. A., & Tewksbury, D. (2006). Framing, agenda setting, and priming: The evolution of three media effects models. ​Journal of communication​, ​57​(1), 9-20.

Schindler, J., Krämer, B., and Müller, P. (2017) Looking left or looking right? Effects of newspaper layout style on the perception of political news. European Journal of Communication, 32(4), 348-366. 

Slone, M. (2000) Response to Media Coverage of Terrorism. Journal of Conflict        Resolution, 44(4), 508-522.  

(40)

Soroka, S., & McAdams, S. (2015). News, politics, and negativity. Political Communication, 32 (1), 1-22. 

Soroka, S., Young, L., and Balmas. (2015) Bad News or Mad News? Sentiment        Scoring of Negativity, Fear and Anger in News Content. AAPSS, 659(1), 108-121.

 

Shoshanim, A. & Slone, M. (2008) The Drama of Media Coverage of Terrorism:        Emotional and Attitudinal Impact on the Audience, Studies in Conflict & Terrorism,        31:7, 627-640  

Van Dijk, T. A. (1995). Power and the news media. ​Political communication and

action​, ​6​(1), 9-36.

Vasilopoulos, P., Marcus, G.E., adn Valentino, N.A. (2016) ​Fear, anger, and voting for the far right: Evidence from the November 13, 2015 Paris terror attacks. Political Psychology, 0(0), 1-24. 

Vuilleumier P, Richardson MP, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ (2004) Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nat Neurosci 7, 1271–1278

 

Gabriel Weimann, “The Theater of Terror: The Effects of Press Coverage.” ​Journal of Communication, ​33 (Winter 1983), pp. 38–45.  

Woods, J. (2011) Framing terror: an experimental framing effects study of the        perceived threat of terrorism. Critical Studies of Terrorism, 4(2), 199-217. 

Woods, J., Eyck, T. A. T., Kaplowitz, S. A., & Shlapentokh, V. (2008). Terrorism risk perceptions and proximity to primary terrorist targets: how close is too close?. ​Human

Ecology Review​, 63-70.

Joffe, H. (2008) The Power of Visual Material: Persuasion, Emotion and        Identification. International Council for Philosophy and Human Studies. 55(84),        84-93.  

Norris, P., Ken, M., and Just, M. (2003) Framing Terrorism: The News Media, the        Government, and the Public. Routledge. 1-315.  

Bromley, M. (1998). The “tabloiding” of Britain:“quality” newspapers in the 1990s.

Sex, Lies and Democracy: The Press and the Public​, 25-38.

Coleman, R. (2010). Framing the pictures in our heads. ​Doing news framing analysis:

Empirical and theoretical perspectives​, 233-261.

Rodriguez, L., & Dimitrova, D. V. (2011). The levels of visual framing. ​Journal of Visual Literacy​, ​30​(1), 48-6.

(41)

Cho, J. (2008). Political ads and citizen communication. ​Communication Research​,

35​(4), 423-451.

Coulthard, M. (2014). ​An introduction to discourse analysis​. Routledge.

Jackson, J. (2015, December 16). ​BBC rated most accurate and reliable TV news,

says Ofcom poll. The Guardian. Retrieved from

https://www.theguardian.com/media/2015/dec/16/bbc-rated-most-accurate-and-reliabl e-tv-news-says-ofcom-poll

Coulter, K. S., & Coulter, R. A. (2005). Size does matter: The effects of magnitude representation congruency on price perceptions and purchase likelihood. ​Journal of Consumer Psychology​, ​15​(1), 64-76.

(42)

Appendix 1

Survey Stimulius             41

(43)

Manipulation 1 - Small Font - Terrorist Attack 

 

   

(44)

Manipulation 2 - Small Font - Attack

 

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Om hierdie doel te bereik, word die denkontwikkelingsvlak van 'n groep graad eenkinders wat kleuterskole besoek het, vergelyk met 'n groep graad eenkinders wat

The binding to FcRn receptor is not or only moder- ately influenced by the glycan occupancy, as similar affinity at pH 6 was measured using deglycosylated IgG1 compared to

Deze hield een andere visie op de hulpverlening aan (intraveneuze) drugsgebruikers aan dan de gemeente, en hanteerde in tegenstelling tot het opkomende ‘harm reduction’- b

Simulations: Monte Carlo simulations were performed to compare a simple staircase method, PSI method and a random staircase method. A stochastic psychophysical model was applied

Hypothesis 5A predicts that evaluations of the line extension is higher for personal brands of artists in the electronic music industry that score high on symbolic

Deze twee casussen laten zien dat het plaatsen van implantaten aan de hand van een virtuele planning bijdraagt aan de voorspelbaarheid van de behandeling van

Study 2, Mean future status ratings under high and low competition in the organization as a function of temporal social comparison (Ego’s performance development (PD) better over time

H7: Indien er in een flaptekst van een zelfhulpboek bij burn-out een uitgebreide metafoor is gebruikt voor het beschrijven van burn-out(-herstel) die onderdeel is van de argumentatie