• No results found

Aanbevelingen voor vervolgonderzoek

SLI gemiddeld

5.1 Aanbevelingen voor vervolgonderzoek

Een aspect uit het onderhavige onderzoek dat verder onderzocht dient te worden is de mate waarin jongens en meisjes met SLI of PDD-NOS verschillen in hun ToM-vaardigheden en wat de relatie is met taal. Uit het onderzoek van Blijd-Hogewys (2009) blijkt al dat meisjes voorlopen in hun ToM-ontwikkeling en ook het onderzoek van Cheung (2006) wijst uit dat de spiegelneuronen van vrouwen meer uitgesproken reacties vertonen dan de spiegelneuronen van mannen.

Ook het relateren van spiegelneuronen aan ToM en de invloed van taal hierop zou veel duidelijkheid verschaffen over de ontwikkeling van sociaal-emotioneel gedrag.

Zoals eerder aangegeven zou het onderzoek opnieuw uitgevoerd kunnen worden waarbij de twee onderzoeksgroepen gematcht worden op taalniveau. In dit kader kan de relatie tussen ToM en taal verder uitgediept worden. De resultaten zouden bovendien een uitspraak doen over de validiteit van de ToM-test van Steerneman et al. (2003). Ook is het mogelijk om andere testen zoals de Nijmeegse Pragmatiektest in het onderzoek te betrekken om ook andere pragmatische vaardigheden naast de resultaten te kunnen leggen. Het vergroten van de onderzoeksgroepen zou bovendien de betrouwbaarheid van de resultaten ten goede komen.

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 47

Samenvatting

Kinderen met SLI komen in Nederland meestal terecht op scholen voor kinderen met ernstige spraak en/of taalproblemen. Kinderen met PDD-NOS die vooral problemen ondervinden in hun taalontwikkeling en communicatieve vaardigheden worden ook vaak op deze scholen geplaatst waardoor beide groepen kinderen bij elkaar in de klas komen. Hoewel de twee stoornissen verschillende oorzaken en vaak ook uiteenlopende taalontwikkelingspatronen hebben, is er overlap tussen de communicatieproblemen die deze twee groepen kinderen ondervinden. Uit onderzoek is gebleken dat zowel kinderen met SLI (Holmes, 2002; Tucker, 2004) als kinderen met PDD-NOS (Sicotte & Stemberger, 1999; Blijd-Hoogewys, 2008) een vertraagde ontwikkeling van hun Theory of

Mind (ToM) hebben. ToM is het vermogen om een theorie te vormen van de gedachten en emoties

van een ander (Astington, 1998; Astington & Jenkins, 1999). De ToM-ontwikkeling is op te delen in verschillende bouwstenen (Steerneman et al., 2003). Een verstoorde ToM veroorzaakt problemen in de sociale interactie.

Het onderhavige onderzoek heeft tot doel om de patronen van beide groepen kinderen uiteen te zetten wat betreft de relatie tussen hun taalvaardigheden en hun ToM-vaardigheden. Er is een groep kinderen met SLI (n=11) en een groep kinderen met PDD-NOS (n=8) in de leeftijd van 7;04 jaar tot 10;00 jaar onderzocht. Hiervoor zijn diverse taaltesten afgenomen waarmee hun receptieve en productieve semantische en morfosyntactische vaardigheden in kaart zijn gebracht. Deze vaardigheden zijn vervolgens gerelateerd aan de bouwstenen in de ToM-ontwikkeling, die zijn getest middels de ToM-test van Steerneman et al. (2003). Ten eerste is bepaald in hoeverre de twee onderzoeksgroepen van elkaar verschillen wat betreft de taalvaardigheden en de ToM-vaardigheden. Het blijkt dat de groep kinderen met SLI op bijna alle taaltesten en op alle ToM-vaardigheden lager scoort dan de groep kinderen met PDD-NOS. Ten tweede is voor beide onderzoeksgroepen de relatie beschreven tussen hun taalvaardigheden en hun ToM-vaardigheden. Tot slot zijn de groepen met elkaar vergeleken wat betreft de relaties tussen hun taalvaardigheden en hun ToM-vaardigheden. Het blijkt dat er geen overeenkomstige correlaties zijn. De groepen laten dus een verschillend patroon zien. Waar kinderen met SLI mogelijk wel begrijpen wat er in een ander om gaat, maar problemen krijgen wanneer hen verbaal wordt gevraagd hier uiting aan te geven, zit bij kinderen met PDD-NOS waarschijnlijk al een probleem in het zich daadwerkelijk verplaatsen in een ander.

Bij beide groepen blijkt dat diverse taalvaardigheden een rol spelen in de ontwikkeling van ToM. Zowel bij kinderen met SLI als bij kinderen met PDD-NOS zijn zowel receptieve als productieve semantische en morfosyntactische vaardigheden gerelateerd aan de prestaties op de verschillende ToM-vaardigheden. Semantiek lijkt de belangrijkste rol te spelen. De gevonden relaties zijn meestal te verklaren uit de overeenkomst tussen de vraagstellingen waarmee de verschillende bouwstenen getest worden en de desbetreffende taalvaardigheden. Het feit dat de gebruikte ToM-test (Steerneman et al., 2003) een talige test is waarbij gebruik gemaakt is van complexe morfosyntactische constructies, wordt hierbij als kanttekening geplaatst. In aansluiting op Cheung et al. (2004) blijkt uit het onderzoek dat taal in zijn totaliteit een belangrijke bijdrage levert aan Theory

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 49

Summary

In the Netherlands, not only children with SLI attend special schools for children with severe speech and language problems. Also children with PDD-NOS attend these schools, but only on condition that they show problems in social interaction or in their language development. This results in a situation where these groups of children are being placed in the same classes. Between these groups there is some overlap in the problems they experience with communication, although both of the disorders have different aetiologies and vary in their patterns of language development. Both children with SLI (Holmes, 2002; Tucker, 2004) and children with PDD-NOS (Sicotte & Stemberger, 1999; Blijd-Hoogewys, 2008) are found to have a delayed development of Theory of Mind (ToM). ToM is the cognitive ability to attribute mental states, like thoughts and emotions, to oneself and others (Astington, 1998; Astington & Jenkins, 1999). The development of ToM can be divided into various stages (Steerneman et al., 2003). A disabled ToM causes problems with social interaction.

This study examines the patterns of both groups, concerning the relation between their language skills and their ToM-skills. Eleven children with SLI and eight children with PDD-NOS were included in the study. Their ages vary from 7;04 years to 10;00 years. Through different sets of tests their receptive and productive semantic and morphosyntactic skills were tested. These skills were related to the different ToM-skills, which were examined with the ToM-test (Steerneman et al., 2003). The first goal of the study is to asses to what extend the two groups differ in their language skills and their ToM-skills. Results showed lower scores for the children with SLI on almost all of the language tests. They also showed lower scores on the ToM-skills than children with PDD-NOS. A second goal of the study is to describe for both groups to what amount their language skills are related to their ToM-skills. Finally, a comparison is made between both groups concerning these relations. Results show no overlap in the correlations, which means that the two groups show different patterns. Previous research shows that children with SLI are able to attribute a mental state to others, but have problems with expressing these emotions and thoughts verbally. In contrast, children with PDD-NOS experience difficulties earlier in the ToM-process, particularly with understanding the mental states of others.

For both groups, several language skills seem to play a role in the development of ToM. Receptive as well as productive semantic and morphosyntactic skills are found to be related to their performances on various ToM-skills for both groups. The results of this study show an important effect of semantics on this relation.The presented relations could be explained by the structure of the questions in the ToM-test (Steerneman et al., 2003). A similarity is identified in the syntactic structure of these questions and the tested language skills. The ToM-test contains several complex morphosyntactic constructions, which is an important comment on the results. According to Cheung et al. (2004), language as a whole plays an important role in Theory of Mind. It facilitates the ability to attribute emotions, thoughts and ideas to others in order to express them. The results of this study support these findings.

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 51

Referenties

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th

edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Allen, D.A. & Rapin, I. (1992) Autistic children are also dysphasic. In H. Neruse & E.M. Ornitz (Eds.)

Neurobiology of infantile autism. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Antonietti, A., Liverta-Sempio, O., Marchetti, A., & Astington, J. W. (2006). Mental language and understanding of epistemic and emotional mental states: Contextual aspects. In A. Antonietti, O. Liverta-Sempio, & A. Marchetti (Eds.), Theory of mind and language in developmental contexts (p. 1 – 30). Dordecht: Springer.

Astington, J. W. (2000). Language and metalanguage in children’s understanding of mind. In J. W. Astington (Ed.), Minds in the making: Essays in honor of David R. Olson (p. 267 – 284). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Astington, J.W. & Jenkins, J.M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and

Theory of Mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35 (5), 1311-1320.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Baron-Cohen, S. Leslie, A.M. & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a Theory of Mind? Cognition, 21, 37-46.

Baron-Cohen, S., & Goodhart, F. (1994). The “seeing leads to knowing” deficit in autism: the Pratt and

Bryant probe. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 397-402.

Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1989). Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires. Child Development, 60, 946-964.

Bishop, D.V.M. (2000). Pragmatic language impairment: A correlate of SLI, a distinct subgroup, or a part of the autistic continuum? In D.V.M. Bishop and L.B. Leonard (Eds.). Speech and Language

Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcome (p. 99-113). New York,

NY: Psychology Press.

Bishop, D.V.M. (2003). Autism and specific language impairment. Novartis Foundation Symposium 251, 213-234.

Bishop, D.V.M. (2004). Specific language impairment: Diagnostic dilemmas. In L.T. Verhoeven and H. Van Balkom (Eds.), Classification of Developmental Language Disorders: Theoretical Issues and

Clinical Implications, (p. 309-326). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bishop, D.V.M. & Adams, C. (1992). Comprehension problems in children with specific language

impairment: literal and inferential meaning. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 35, 119-129.

Bishop, D.V.M., Adams, C.V., & Norbury, C.F. (2006). Distinct genetic influences on grammar and

phonological short-term memory deficits: evidence from 6-year-old twins. Genes, Brain and Behavior,

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 52

Blakemore, S.J. (2008). Development of the social brain during adolescence. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61 (1), 40 – 49.

Blijd-Hoogewys, E.M.A. (2008). The development of Theory of Mind and the Theory of Mind

Storybooks. Lessons from a non-linear approach of developmental data in typically developing children and children with PDD-NOS. Groningen: Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

Bloom, P., & German, T.P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of

mind. Cognition, 77, 25-31.

Bon, van, W.H.J. (1982). Handleiding Taaltests voor Kinderen TvK, Lisse: Swets & Zeittinger B.V. Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a ‘theory of mind’? In M.E. Lamb & L.R. Sherrod (Eds.), Infant social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Buitelaar, J.K. & van der Gaag, R.J. (1998). Diagnostic rules for children with pdd-nos and multiple

complex developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 911-920.

Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great

apes. Child Development, 70, 381–395.

Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. Website CBS. Verkregen op 8 november 2010 van

http://statline.cbs.nl/StatWeb/publication/?VW=T&DM=SLNL&PA=37746SOL&D1=0&D2=0-45&D3=a&D4=7,l&HD=090218-1409&HDR=T,G3,G2&STB=G1

Chakrabarti, S. & Fombonne, E. (2001). Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285 (24), 3093-3099.

Charman T. & Shmueli-Goetz Y. (1998). The relationship between theory of mind, language, and

narrative discourse: An experimental study. Cahiers de Psychologie Cogntive/Current Psychology of

Cognition, 17 (2), 245-271.

Cheung, H. (2006). False belief and language comprehension in Cantonese-speaking children. Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin

Cheung, H., Hsuan-Chih, C., Creed, N., Ng, L., Wang, S. P. & Mo, L. (2004). Relative roles of general

and complementation language in theory-of-mind development: Evidence from Cantonese and English. Child Development, 75, 1155 – 1170.

Conti-Ramsden, G., Crutchley, G. & Botting, N. (1997). The extent to which psychometric tests

differentiate subgroups of children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40,

765-777.

Cutting, A.L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family

background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70 (4), 853-865.

Dapretto, M., Davies, M.S., Pfeifer, J.H., Scott, A.A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S.Y. & Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 53

Dawson, J., Stout, C. & Eyer, J. (2003) Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test (SPELT-3). De Bruin, E.I., Verheij, F. & Ferdinand, R.F. (2006). WISC-R subtest but no overall VIQ-PIQ difference in

Dutch children with PDD-NOS. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 263-271.

De Jong, J. (1999). Specific Language Impairment in Dutch: Inflectional Morphology and Argument

Structure. Groningen: Rijksuniversiteit Groningen

De Villiers, J. G. (1995). Steps in the mastery of sentence complements. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.

De Villiers, P.A., Burns, F. & Pearson, B. (2003). The role of language in theory of mind development of language-impaired children: Complementing theories. In B. Beachley, A. Brown & F. Conlin (Eds.). Proceedings of the 27th annual Boston University conference on language development, 1, Cascadilla Press, Somerville, MA.

De Villiers, J.G., & Pyers, J.E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the

relationship between complex syntax and false-belief understanding. Cognitive Development, 17,

1037 – 1060.

De Villiers, J.G. & De Villiers, P.A. (2000). Linguistic determinism and false belief. In P. Mitchell & K. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and the Mind. Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press.

Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor

events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180.

Dunn, J. & Brophy, M. (2005). Communication, relationships, and individual differences in children's understanding of mind. In Astington, J.W. & Baird, J.A. (Eds.), Why Language Matters for Theory of

Mind, Oxford: Oxford University Press

Dunn, L.M. & Dunn, L.M. (2005). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III-NL, Nederlandse versie door

Liesbeth Schlichting. Harcourt Assessment B.V., Amsterdam

Farrant, B.M., Fletcher, J. & Maybery, M.T. (2006). Specific Language Impairment, Theory of Mind,

and Visual Perspective Taking: Evidence for Simulation Theory and the Developmental Role of Language. Child Development, 77 (6), 1842–1853.

Farrar, M.J., Johnson, B., Tompkins, V., Easters, M., Zilisi-Medus, A. & Benigno, J.B. (2009). Language

and theory of mind in preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of

Communication Disorders, 42 (6), 428-441.

Filipek, P.A., Accardo, P.J., Baranek, G.T., Cook Jr., E.H., Dawson, G., Gordon, B., ..., Volkmar, F.R. (1999). The screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29 (6), 439-484.

Flavell, J., Miller, P. & Miller, S. (1993). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Flobbe,L., Verbrugge, R., Hendriks, P. & Krämer, I. (2007). Children's application of Theory of Mind in

reasoning and language. Manuscript, University of Groningen & Radboud University Nijmegen.

Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 493–501.

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 54

Gathercole, S.E., & A.D. Baddeley. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered

children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336-360.

Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55.

Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. (1988). Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child

Development, 59, 26-37.

Gordon, R. (1986). Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language, 1, 158–171.

Hale, C.M. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: a training study. Developmental Science 6 (3), 346-359.

Happé, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory-of-mind tasks performance of

subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843-855.

Harris, P. (1988). Children’s understanding of real and apparent emotions. In Astington, J., Harris, P. & Olson, D. (Eds.) Developing theories of mind. Cambridge: University Press.

Harris, P. (1996). Desires, beliefs, and language. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of

theories of mind (pp. 200–220). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Heal, J. (1986). Replication and functionalism. In J. Butterfield (Ed.), Language, mind, and logic (p. 135–150). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Holmes, A.M. (2002). Theory of mind and behavior disorders in children with specific language

impairment. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 62, 5376.

Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 16, 233 – 253.

Jenkins, J.M. & Astington, J.W. (1996). Cognitive factors and family structure associated with Theory

of Mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology 32 (1), 70-78.

Kort, W., Schittekatte, M., en Compaan, E. (2008) CELF-4-NL: Clinical Evaluation of Language

Fundamentals-vierde-editie. Pearson Assessment and Information B.V., Amsterdam

Leonard, L.B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Leslie, A. (1987). Pretence and reperesentation: the origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychological Review, 94, 412-426.

Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children’s understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315 – 324.

Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 55

Lucariello, J., Le Donne, M., Durand, T. & Yarnell, L. (2006) Social and intrapersonal theories of mind. “I Interact Therefore I Am”. In A. Antonietti, O. Liverta-Sempio, A. Marchetti. Theory of Mind and

Language in Developmental Contexts (p. 149-171). New York: Springer.

Mayes, L., Volkmar, F., Hooks, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Differentiating pervasive developmental

disorder not otherwise specified from autism and language disorders. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 23, 79-90.

Miller, C.A. (2004). False belief and sentence complement performance in children with specific

language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39 (2), 191–

213.

Miller, C.A. (2006). Developmental relationships between language and Theory of Mind. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15, 142–154.

Milligan, K., Astington, J.W.& Dack, L.A. (2007). Language and Theory of Mind: Meta-analysis of the

relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78 (2), 622 –

646.

Moore, C., Bryant, D. & Furrow, D. (1989). Mental Terms and the Development of Certainty. Child Development, 60 (1), 167-171.

Muris, P., Steerneman, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Horselenberg, R., Van den Hogen, T. & Van Dongen, L. (1999). The TOM Test: A new instrument for assessing Theory of Mind in normal children

and children with Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder,

29 (1).

Nishitani, N., Avikainen, S., & Hari, R. (2004). Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation

sequences in Asperger’s syndrome. Annals of Neurology, 55, 558–562.

Orgassa, A. (2009). Specific Language Impairment in a Bilingual Context: The Acquisition of Dutch

Inflection by Turkish-Dutch Learners. Utrecht: LOT dissertation series 220.

Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M. & Leekam, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the Autistic child’s theory of

mind: Knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60, 689 – 700.

Perner, J., Sprung, M., Zauner, P. & Haider, H. (2003). Want That is Understood Well before Say That, Think That, and False Belief: A Test of de Villiers’s Linguistic Determinism on German-Speaking

Children. Child Development, 74 (1), 179–188.

Peterson, C. C. (2004). Theory-of-mind development in oral deaf children with cochlear implants or

conventional hearing aids. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1096 – 1106.

Piaget, J. (1945/1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton. (Origineel werk gepubliceerd in 1945).

Piaget, J. (1954). Construction of Reality in the Child. Basic Books.

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 56

Plaut, D.C. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1993) Representational development and theory-of-mind

computations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16 (1) 70-71.

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have 'theory-of-mind'? Behaviour and Brain Sciences, 4, 515-526.

Ramachandran, V. (2006). Het bewustzijn. Een korte rondleiding. Nijmegen: Pearson Education Benelux

Ramachandran, V.S. & Oberman, L.M. (2006) Broken mirrors. Scientific American, 295 (5), 62-69. Rapin, I. & Dunn, M. (1997). Language disorders in children with autism. Seminars in pediatric neurology, 4 (2), 86-92.

Ruffman, T. (2000). Nonverbal theory of mind: Is it important, is it implicit, is it simulation, is it relevant to autism? In J.W. Astington (Ed.), Minds in the making: Essays in honor of David R. Olson (p. 456–479). New York: Blackwell.

Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Rowlandson, K., Rumsey, C., & Garnham, A. (2003). How language relates to

belief, desire, and emotion understanding. Cognitive Development, 18, 139 – 158.

REC2 Noord Holland-Flevoland. Indicatiecriteria. Verkregen op 12 september van

http://www.rec2holland-flevoland.nl/RecZeon_C01/default.asp?comid=7&modid=273&itemid=0&time=942

Sicotte, C. & Stemberger, R.M.T. (1999). Do children with PDDNOS have a theory of mind? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29 (3), 225-233.

Siegal, M. & Beattie, K. (1991). Where to look first for children’s knowledge of false beliefs. Cognition, 38, 1-12

Slade, L., & Ruffman, T. (2005). How language does (and does not) relate to theory-of-mind: A

longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief. British Journal of

Developmental Psychology 23, 117 – 141.

Smith, P.K. (1996). Language and the evolution of mind-reading. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.),

Theories of theories of mind (p. 344-354). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Smith, M., Apperly, I., & White, V. (2003). False-belief reasoning and the acquisition of relative clause

sentences. Child Development, 74, 1709 – 1719.

Steerneman, P. (2001). Sociaal onhandig: aangeboren of (niet) geleerd. Mobiel, 6

Steerneman, P. & Huskens, B (1996). Can individuals with autism be trained to develop a theory of

mind? The development of a social cognition training. Workshop at the 5th Autism European Conference Barcelona.

Steerneman, P., Meesters, C. & Muris, P. (2003). ToM-test. Antwerpen-Apeldoorn: Garant.

Steerneman, P., Muris, P. & Meesters, C. (1999). ‘Theory of Mind’: Analyse van een construct. Diagnostiekwijzer, 2, 71-80.

T h e o r y o f M i n d e n t a a l b i j S L I e n P D D - N O S - W i e b r i g K r a k a u 57

Steerneman, P. & Pelzer, H. (1994). Sociale cognities en sociale competentie bij kinderen en

jeugdigen. Leuven/Apeldoorn: Garant.

Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). What language reveals about the understanding of minds in children with autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: