• No results found

A DISCRETE CHOICE AND ELICITED CHOICE PROBABILITY COMPARISON THROUGH TIME: AN APPLICATION TO E-BIKES.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "A DISCRETE CHOICE AND ELICITED CHOICE PROBABILITY COMPARISON THROUGH TIME: AN APPLICATION TO E-BIKES."

Copied!
13
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

A DISCRETE CHOICE AND ELICITED CHOICE

PROBABILITY COMPARISON THROUGH TIME:

AN APPLICATION TO E-BIKES.

by

(2)

INTRODUCTION

Getting insights into the influence of uncertainty on conjoint valuations:

• Uncertainty in choice

• Two conjoint analysis methods

• Uncertainty over time

• Two time periods

Applied to E-bikes

(3)

LITERATURE REVIEW

• Choice-Based Conjoint: subjects chose one single alternative as preferred choice (Louviere and Woodworth, 1983)

• Assumption: Subjects face no uncertainty over the preferred product choice.

• Elicited Choice Probability Conjoint: subjects set probability to each alternative in choice set (Manski, 1999).

• Permits subjects to express uncertainty.

Hypothesis choice model

• a: The estimated mean utilities differ significantly between CBC and ECPC.

(4)

LITERATURE REVIEW

• Durable good trade-off (Dubé et al., 2014)

• Inordinately optimistic about future (Kahneman and Lovallo 1993; Zauberman and Lynch 2005).

• Small influence of durable good trade-off due to time period, but immediate effects have less uncertainty.

Hypothesis choice over time

• a: There is a significant difference in the means of the utilities of the model with immediate vs. future effects.

(5)

LITERATURE REVIEW

• Willingness to pay (WTP) quantifies the maximum price customers are willing to pay.

• ECPC models are more accurate (Manski, 2004).

• No inordinately optimistic about future effect (Kahneman and Lovallo, 1993; Zauberman and Lynch, 2005).

Hypothesis willingness to pay

H5a: The estimated WTP differs between CBC and ECPC models.

(6)

METHODOLOGY

• 2x2 within-subjects experimental design

• Decision fatigue (Baumeister, 2003)

• Holdout tasks for reliability (Orme, 2015)

• Number of levels effect (Currim et al 1981; Wittink et al 1989)

• Levels determined via websites (Fietsenonline.com; saturn.de)

Attributes Number of levels Levels

Price 3 €1750, €2000, €2250

Range 3 80km, 100km, 120km

Battery charging time 3 2 hours, 4 hours, 6 hours

(7)

METHODOLOGY

Data description

• N = 115 completed surveys.

• Average age of 32.92 years.

• 77.4 % is working and 19.2% is student.

• Sample representative for young professionals.

Distribution of probabilities

• Lower probabilities more information.

• Subjects are aware and willing to answer with probabilities (Manski 2004, Manski et al. 2010).

(8)

RESULTS

• Individual estimation conducted via Hierarchical Bayes with 10,000 iterations and 10,000 draws.

• Non-parametric Welch’s two-sided T-test.

Choice model

• Enough evidence of a significant difference between CBC and ECPC in both time periods.

Choice over time

(9)

RESULTS

Willingness to pay

• WTP differ across CBC and ECPC.

• Highest WTP for first incremental improvement in performance.

• Models with effect in one month receive higher WTP.

Demographics effects

• Variables possession of E-bike, interest in E-bike and willingness to buy E-bike have greatest influence on cluster formation

1) Working enthusiasts

(10)

DISCUSSION

• ECPC analysis outperforms the CBC analysis in terms of efficiency.

• No significant differences across time dimensions; immediate effect vs. effects with a future narrative.

• Contradicting results in the predictive accuracy.

• Willingness to pay show that subjects are willing to pay extensively more for first higher attribute level.

(11)

CONCLUSION AND RECOMMENDATION

• Expression of uncertainty in each choice task allows for more coherence of the customer’s preference.

• Extra efficiency does not translate in higher ECPC hit rates and MAE scores.

• Contradicting evidence of time dimension effects.

Management recommendation

(12)

LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH

Limitations

• Sample size and sample bias.

• One hold-out task per conjoint model.

• Rounding error in ECPC probabilities.

• Paucity of an incentive alignment.

Future research

• Understanding of the time dimension in less complex settings.

(13)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

This is also initially the case for my sample, but by taking an organizational learning approach, it is found that over time firms that engage in open

The institutional environment of Spain, considered as a country with a high regulative, normative and cognitive distance in comparing with the Netherlands, is with

We draw a customer time slot choice model based on Model 3 (see Table 4) to study the effect of using the green label or the $2 price incentive versus no incentives in

était ceintrée pour donner la hauteur de Ia chapelle. Cette disposition ne peut se concevoir que si le clayonnage a été préfabriqué.. Logiquement, en tenant

Bekijken we nu eens een aantal van deze nederzettingen in close-up Welden (fig. van de Schelde verwijderd. van het alluviaal gebied, een zone die in de vroege

Although South African courts have not decided a case involving the vicarious liability of the church for a wrongful act of a priest, these developments will almost

v Bourdouane EAT case 110/95 (Sept 10, 1996) In this case the employee worked for an employer who organised birthday parties for children The employee was harassed by the father

Non-invasive imaging including conventional echocardiography, speckle tracking echocardiography (STE) and cardiac magnetic resonance imaging (CMR), detects clinical as well