• No results found

THE INFLUENCE OF INFORMAL HIERARCHY ON CREATIVITY MODERATED BY ORGANIZATIONAL STRUCTURE

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "THE INFLUENCE OF INFORMAL HIERARCHY ON CREATIVITY MODERATED BY ORGANIZATIONAL STRUCTURE "

Copied!
21
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

CAN I PROPOSE THIS IDEA?:

THE INFLUENCE OF INFORMAL HIERARCHY ON CREATIVITY MODERATED BY ORGANIZATIONAL STRUCTURE

Master Thesis

Master Human Resource Management

University of Groningen, Faculty of Economics and Business

July 15, 2016

Tim Leegte

Studentnumber: S2197502 Stadhouderslaan 47a

9717AJ, Groningen tel.: +31 636207504

e-mail: t.h.s.leegte@student.rug.nl

Supervisor

MSc. J. Oedzes

Second Assessor

Prof. Dr. O. Janssen

(2)

ABSTRACT

In working to understand the predictors of creativity in groups, this research focuses on informal hierarchy. Previous literature has found a negative relation between formal hierarchy and creativity. This research adds to the literature by looking at the negative effect of informal hierarchy on creativity. This research also looked at the moderating role of organizational structure on this relationship. To investigate this, data has been collected by means of

questionnaires, among a sample of 56 groups from Dutch organizations across a wide range of

industries. The results show that informal hierarchy has no significant negative effect on team

creativity. However, organizational structure does moderate this relationship in such a way

that creativity is highest under an organic structure with a low informal hierarchy.

(3)

INTRODUCTION

Numerous scholars have argued that improving creative performance is a step towards competitive advantage for organizations (Amabile, 1988; Devanna & Tichy, 1990; Kanter, 1983; Shalley, 1995). The initiation and implementation of new ideas enhances an

organization's ability to respond to opportunities and, thereby, to adapt, grow, and compete (Van de Ven, 1986). Amabile et al. (1996) argue that creativity is a starting point for innovation. (Oldham & Cummings, 1996). Therefore, creativity can be highly important within an organization.

A vast amount of research on the antecedents of group creativity show that organizational characteristics like organizational hierarchy predict group creativity. For instance, numerous academics have shown that creative tasks are often performed better by groups with less hierarchy (Anderson & Brown, 2010; Berdahl & Anderson, 2005; Bloom, 1999; Siegel & Hambrick, 2005). In the same vein, Caniëls & Rietzschel (2015) demonstrate that constraints have a negative impact on creativity. These examples illustrate how formal rules, and procedures have the ability to influence the way people cooperate and interact, and thus influence their ability to be creative.

However, Theoretical work by McEvily, Soda & Tortoriello (2014) argues that formal characteristics of teams are not the only factors that influence the manner of cooperation. The behavior and interactions of people are also influenced by the informal relations that exist between them. For instance, in teams that exhibit a great difference in the level of influence of individual members, less information will be shared amongst the team members, which would diminish creativity (Bunderson & Reagans, 2011). Many organizations have turned to team- based work systems (Mohrman, Cohen & Mohrman, 1995) in which informal relations are more frequent (Diefenbach & Sillince, 2011). Therefore, this research will focus on the effects of informal hierarchy on a team based level. Specifically, I argue that a high informal hierarchy has a negative effect on creativity.

Informal hierarchy, i.e. a hierarchy based on influence that emerges from social

interactions between group members (Magee & Galinsky, 2008), can have negative effects on

group creativity, in a similar way as formal hierarchical differences can. Informal hierarchy

differs from formal hierarchy in that informal hierarchy is a person dependent social relation

which emerges from social interaction. In a formal hierarchy members are clearly defined and

(4)

demarcated from each other. The rank order of all members is formalized (Diefenbach &

Sillince, 2011).

Not only hierarchy of the group determines the level of creativity of employees within a group. This creativity can also be limited by the freedom that the overall structure of an organization can give an employee (Anderson & Brown, 2010). Certain organizations are structured mechanistically, meaning that the organization has strict rules and procedures and tries to regulate the behavior of all employees. On the other hand, organizational structures can be organic, which means that the organization is less strict in its rules and procedures and gives the employee more freedom (Burns & Stalker, 1961).

In this study, I will focus on the combined effect of informal hierarchy and

organizational structure on team creativity. I expect informal hierarchy to have a negative effect on creativity and I expect this relation to be moderated by organizational structure in such a way that the level of creativity of group members within a team is high only when the organizational structure is organic and the informal hierarchy is low. I will investigate this proposition in a field study among Dutch organizational work teams. In doing so, I aim to make two theoretical contributions.

Former research has been done on whether formal group hierarchy influences performance and creativity within groups (Anderson & Brown, 2010; Blinder & Morgan, 2007; Haslam et al., 1998, Mccurdy & Eber, 1953; Mccurdy & Lambert, 1952). However, too little attention has been paid to informal hierarchy, even though such hierarchies become more and more important in today’s organizations where people work together on a day-to- day basis on relatively complex tasks (Mohrman, Cohen & Mohrman, 1995). The present study fills a gap in the literature by looking at informal hierarchy and investigating whether informal hierarchies have the same negative effect on creativity. Furthermore, I aim to contribute to the literature by researching whether the organizational structure moderates this relation.

This research will also add to practice. Results will show that if an organization’s aim

is to increase the level of creativity among their employees, they should organize their

company in such a way that informal hierarchy is low and that the organizational structure is

an organic one. Organizations can implement an organic structure by limiting the amount of

rules and procedures and by making the hierarchical structure within the whole company flat

(Burns & Stalker, 1961). An organization can implement a low informal hierarchy by creating

(5)

teams where every actor has the same social status. Organizations could also maintain that equality by instructing teams that the teams should remain egalitarian.

THEORY

Informal team hierarchy can be defined as “person-dependent social relationships of dominance and subordination which emerge from social interaction and become persistent over time through repeated social processes ” (Diefenbach & Sillince, 2011, p 1517). Whereas formal hierarchy is an official system where the official roles and positions of all team

members are clearly defined and demarcated from each other (Mousnier, 1973), informal hierarchy is less well-defined and is a form of vertical social relations that is formed by unofficial mechanisms, such as interactions between group members. In this paper, informal hierarchy will be measured by the level of centralization of influence in a team. According to Bunderson et al. (2015), centralization is an approach that views hierarchy as the

concentration of power, status, or privilege in one member or in a small subset of the full membership of a social group. Centralization is maximized when one actor scores at the maximum and all other actors score at the minimum on some dimension, and is minimized when all actors have the same score. (Bunderson et al., 2015)

Creativity is defined as “the creation of a valuable, useful new product, service, idea, procedure, or process by individuals working together in a complex social system”

(Woodman, 1993, p. 293). It has been heralded as one of the keys for enduring advantage (Amabile, Barsade, Mueller & Staw, 2005). A study by Amabile et al. (1996) assumes that the social environment can influence creative behavior. However, little empirical work has systematically examined the possibility that characteristics of the social environment

contribute significantly to creative performance at work (Amabile, 1988; Shalley, 1991; Staw, 1990) (Oldham & Cummings, 1996).

Previous studies have reported that a larger difference between actors on influence

diminishes creativity in a number of ways. Paulus (2000) argues that sharing ideas within a

team increases the chances of creating novel ideas; this requires the group members to attend

and listen to one another. However, these activities are interpersonally risky, since actors

become vulnerable to criticism, judgment, sanction and disapproval (Detert & Edmondson,

2011; Edmondson, 1999). Groups with a larger difference amongst group members share

(6)

fewer ideas within the group and listen less to each other. For instance, Bunderson and Reagans (2011) report that differences in power and status can create an environment in which lower-ranking actors do not feel safe enough to engage in openly sharing information and perspectives, to consider different ideas, and to experiment with unproven approaches (Bunderson & Reagans, 2011). Brooks (1994) found that members of R&D teams did not feel free enough to engage in group reflection when centralization was high. In addition, Keltner et al. (2003) concluded that a power disadvantage prompts an “inhibition” response pattern.

This basic proposition has been supported by a growing body of evidence: individuals in position of lower power experience more negative emotions (Langer & Keltner, 2008), act in more situationally constrained ways (Galinsky et al., 2008), are less likely to take risks during social interactions (Magee et al. 2007), and are less likely to take initiative (Galinsky et al.

2003). Furthermore, past research suggests that the perspectives and insights of higher- ranking members are given disproportionate weight, whereas the contributions of lower- ranking members are overlooked (Bunderson, 2003; Pitcher & Smith, 2001; Westphal &

Zajac, 1995) and high-power subjects were less likely to adopt the perspective of others (Galinsky et al., 2006; Lammers et al., 2008). A more centralized hierarchy causes group members with low power to interact less and causes group members with high power to listen less, which decreases the chance of creating novel ideas (Paulus, 2000). Furthermore,

Anderson & Brown (2010) argue that when groups work on tasks that benefit from a broader range of opinions and perspectives (e.g. creative tasks), flatter structures should be more advantageous. A flat hierarchical structure is less controlling and thus better for creativity than a steep one. In addition, West (2002) said that participation in teams could, under

appropriate conditions, lead to high levels of creativity. King and Anderson (1990) stated that the probability of creative outcomes may be highest when leadership is democratic and collaborative. Structures of communication could constrain the ability to listen to everyone’s ideas and it could constrain participation within teams. According to the “intrinsic

motivation” perspective (Amabile, 1988), the context in which an individual performs a task influences his or her intrinsic motivation, which in turn affects creative achievement.

Supervision that is experienced as controlling undermines intrinsic motivation and shifts an

employee's focus of attention away from work activities and toward external concerns (Deci

et al., 1989; Deci & Ryan, 1987). Thus, hierarchy will have a negative effect on creativity by

affecting these processes.

(7)

Although the previous research links formal hierarchy to creativity. In this study, I argue that the same effect will be seen for informal hierarchy. I argue that creativity is most likely when informal hierarchy centralization is low. This is the case when all group members have approximately equal positions in terms of influence.

The first hypothesis will therefore be:

Hypothesis 1: Informal hierarchy has a negative effect on the creativity within a team This hypothesized relation will not be the same amongst various types of

organizations. The effect of hierarchy will depend on the type of organization (Burns &

Stalker 1961; Katz & Kahn, 1966; Scott 1998). Burns and Stalker (1961) suggested that there are two kinds of organizational systems, the mechanistic and the organic. A mechanistic management system is a contractual, hierarchical form of organization. Here work is distributed among specialist roles within a clearly defined hierarchy. A mechanistic

organization is viewed as more suitable for static environments. The organic form is less rule based, and can be seen more as loosely coupled networks of workers. Mechanistic

organizations have lower complexity, higher formalization and centralization, lower internal and external communication, and higher vertical differentiation than organic organizations.

Burns and Stalker indicated that the organic structure was more suited for change and thus more conducive for creative outcomes (Aiken, 1971).

Evidence suggests that mechanistic organizational designs constrain the ability of the system to produce creative outcomes. In a meta-analysis of 23 empirical studies, 21 articles and two books, Damanpour (1991) looked at the characteristics of an organic organization, e.g. high complexity, high internal and external communication and low vertical

differentiation, and found that all characteristics of an organic organization, except vertical differentiation, correlated with creative outcomes. Similarly, creativity is enhanced by adaptive, flexible organizational structures such as network designs and parallel or collateral structures (Damanpour, 1991; King, 1990; Rubinstein & Woodman, 1984; Zaltman, Duncan,

& Holbek, 1973). In addition, King and Anderson (1990) also listed group structure as an

antecedent of creativity. They argued that the probability of creative outcomes is higher in an

organic structure than in a mechanistic one, since an organic structure does not constrain the

group members.

(8)

This paper argues that both informal hierarchy and organizational structure are important for creativity. If the organization has a mechanistic design, it will matter less whether informal hierarchy is low or not. In both cases, creativity will remain low, since the characteristics of a mechanistic organization, e.g. low internal and external communication, high formalization and high vertical differentiation, constrain the ability of group members to be creative. This paper expects that the combination of an organic organizational design and a low informal hierarchy among employees creates the best environment for creativity.

This research argues that organizational design will moderate the previously discussed relation between informal hierarchy and creativity in such a way that under an organic

structure and a low informal hierarchy, creativity will be high, and, under a mechanistic structure, regardless of the level of informal hierarchy, creativity will be low. Therefore, the second hypothesis will be:

Hypothesis 2: Organizational structure moderates the negative relation between informal hierarchy and creativity, such that creativity within a team is highest under an organic structure with a low informal hierarchy

METHOD

Data and Sample

To test the hypotheses, data has been collected among a sample of 56 groups from

Dutch organizations across a wide range of industries. Groups had consist of at least 4

members, interact often, share resources and information and combine efforts to meet

common goals (Kozlowski & Bell, 2003). A questionnaire was send to 314 employees of

which 56 were leaders. A total of 205 employees have completed the survey. To be able to

receive the questionnaire, the leader of the team had to fill in a team description form with the

email addresses of every individual team member. Every member of the team then got a

unique link to their questionnaire. The measures and questions were in both English and

Dutch. Teams that were more proficient in English received the English survey and teams that

were more proficient in Dutch received the Dutch survey. The survey was not conducted

anonymously. For the researchers, it was retrievable how high the specific teams scored on

certain dimensions. Finally, we had 160 employees and 45 leaders who have filled in the

questionnaire. This resulted in 38 teams that could be used for statistical analysis. For the

employee sample, 41.3 % were males, the average age was 36.19 (SD = 12.4) years, and the

(9)

average organizational tenure and the average team tenure were 9.37 (SD = 10.7) and 4.11 (SD = 5.2) years, respectively. Most of the teams said they were situated in education

(15.6%), healthcare (13.3%) or business or professional services (11.1%), although the largest amount of teams said they could be placed in a category not listed by us, such as sports and administrative services (37.8%). Of the people who filled in the questionnaire, 42% was male and 58% was female, the mean age was 36.19 years, and the size of the group varied from 4 to 12 members (M = 5.47, SD = 2.16).

The leaders of the teams had to fill in a different survey in which they were asked about the general features of the team and the organization. All the employees had to fill in a questionnaire that was more from their point of view.

Measures

Informal hierarchy. For this study, the informal hierarchy was measured by asking every team member to rank every other team member on three questions regarding influence, e.g. influences me, asks me for advice, and I give this person advice, on a three-point scale (1=never, 2=sometimes, 3= often). This was converted to a team centralization measure by using Freeman’s centralization index (Freeman, 1978):

Centralization = Σ (I

max

– I

i

) / (n-1)

Where I

max

is the maximum individual influence score within a group, I

i

is the influence score of an individual member i, and n is the total number of group members. This quantifies the distance between the highest scoring actor and all the other actors. It calculates the sum in terms of differences in centrality between the most central actor and all the other actors and divides this quantity by the theoretically largest such sum of differences in any network of the same size.

Organizational structure. Organizational structure was measured through seven

questions of the organization structure scale (Covin & Slevin, 1988). An example item

includes “either my organization has a focus on a uniform management style throughout

organization”, or “it is allowed that the management style of executives may vary from very

formal to very informal”. These questions were then scored on a seven-point scale (1 highly

favoring the former choice which would be mechanistic and 7 highly favoring the latter

choice indicating an organic design). Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .86.

(10)

Creativity. Creativity within the teams was measured by asking the leaders to grade the separate members on their individual creativity on a seven-point scale (1=not creative, 7=highly creative). Based on the individual scores a mean was composed for every team. This mean was used as the score for group creativity. A Cronbach’s alpha was not calculated for the individual creativity scores, since it is possible that a highly creative team has a member that is less creative and vice versa.

Control variables. On the demographic page of the questionnaire, the participants provided information about their highest level of education (1, ‘primary school,’ 2, ‘high school,’ 3, ‘technical secondary school,’ 4, ‘university of applied sciences’, 5,’ university’ 6,

‘doctor’s degree’) and their income. These socio-demographics have been entered as control variables in the statistical analyses reported below because they correlated with creativity and team centralization. It might be possible that these variables correlate with creativity, since creative jobs are often done by highly educated people (Shaheen, 2010) and highly educated people often earn a higher income.

Analysis

For the analysis of the questionnaire responses, SPSS was used. A dataset was created with the answers on the questionnaires. The internal reliability was measured with the

“Cronbach’s α”. Then, the descriptive statistics for each variable was computed. Furthermore, the Pearson’s correlation coefficient was calculated to determine whether our variables were correlated to each other. . Based on the rankings made by the team members the steepness, centralization and linearity of every team has been calculated. To test the hypothesis, the process macro from Hayes has been used (Hayes, 2012).

RESULTS Descriptive Statistics and Correlations

Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and zero-order Pearson correlations

among all variables in this study. The only variable that is correlating with other variable is

education level. All the other variables did not significantly correlate with each other.

(11)

**=Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).**

*=Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).*

Tests of Hypotheses Hypothesis 1

Team hierarchy has a negative effect on creativity within a team

In order to analyze whether or not a lower team hierarchy leads to a higher creativity, a regression analysis has been performed. The results of this regression, R2 = 0.050, B= 0.86, t(38)= 1.383, p= .175, show that there is no significant relationship between informal

hierarchy and creativity. Therefore, a lower informal hierarchy does not lead to a significantly higher creativity.

Hypothesis 2

Organizational structure moderates the negative relation between team hierarchy and creativity within a team

In order to analyze hypothesis 2 and to find out whether or not organizational structure moderates the relation between team hierarchy and creativity, Hayes’ process macro has been performed. The results are summarized in table 2.

Table 1 M SD 1 2 3 4 5

1. Team hierarchy 0.49 1.99

2. Organizational Structure 4.24 1.18 -.077

3. Creativity 4.98 0.75 .225 .266

Control variables

4. Education Level 3.88 0.72 .409** .335* .446**

5. Income 4.70 2.09 .155 -.215 .208 -.125

(12)

Table 2 Moderation Effects of organizational structure on informal hierarchy in Predicting Creativity (N=38)

Step Variable r B SE b Beta

1 Informal

hierarchy

.225 .086 .062 .225

2 Organizational structure

.266 .168* .099 .266

3 Informal

Hierarchy x Organizational Structure

-.256** .108

(Constant) 3.89*** .78

*p<0.1, **p<0.05, ***p<0.01

Results indicate that greater organizational structure, i.e. a more organic organization, is associated with a higher level of creativity (b = .168, SEb =.099, β = .266, p < .01). The interaction between informal hierarchy and organizational structure is also significant (b = - .256, SEb = .108, p < .005), suggesting that the effect of informal hierarchy on creativity depended on the level of organizational structure.

Simple slopes for the association between distance and satisfaction were tested for low (-1 SD below the mean), moderate (mean), and high (+1 SD above the mean) levels of

organizational structure. Each of the simple slope tests did not reveal a significant negative association between informal hierarchy and creativity, but for low (b=0.39 t(32)= 1.68 p=0.10) and high (b=-0.21 t(32)=-1.66 p=0.11) levels of organizational structure the

significance is higher than for a moderate (b=0.09 t(32)= 0.64 p=0.53) level of organizational

structure. Figure 1 plots the simple slopes for the interaction.

(13)

Figure 1

Therefore, Hypothesis 2 can be accepted, organizational structure does moderate the negative relation between team hierarchy and creativity within a team.

DISCUSSION

Drawing from psychology research I predicted that informal hierarchy would relate negatively to creativity. Furthermore, I predicted that organizational structure would moderate the

relation between informal hierarchy and creativity. In a study of 56 work groups I found support for the second hypothesis that organizational structure moderates the relation between informal hierarchy and creativity, but no support for the first hypothesis concerning the negative effect of informal hierarchy on creativity.

Implications for Research

This study suggests that there is no significant negative relation between informal

hierarchy and creativity. However, this study has shown that organizational structure has been

found to moderate this relation. This means that in organic organizations the negative relation

between informal hierarchy and creativity is higher, than in mechanistic organizations.

(14)

The current findings add to a growing body of literature on creativity. Multiple precedents of creativity have already been found in scientific literature; however, not a lot of research had been done on informal hierarchy.

Further research might explore the mediating effect of risk-taking and experimentation and knowledge transfer to creativity. Bunderson & Reagans (2011) found that these variables mediated the effect of power and status differences on learning outcomes, but these mediators also seem highly important in creating a creative environment. Further research might also usefully explore the other measures for hierarchy. This research focused on centralization, but more research can be done for instance on steepness. The current research looked at creativity within the work groups on an individual level. Initially I wanted to research the influence of informal hierarchy on creativity on a group level. However, these results were not significant in this research. The small sample size could lead to this relation not being significant. Thus, further research could still be done on the influence of informal hierarchy on creativity on a group level. Further research could also add some extra moderators. For instance it is possible that a team with a highly creative leader would benefit from a centralized hierarchy.

Therefore, further research could be done on the moderating effect of leader creativity on the relation between informal hierarchy and creativity.

Implications for Practice

This research has several practical applications. When organizations want to enhance group creativity, it would be wise to implement an organic structure. This can be done by altering the characteristics of the organization. Organizations can be organic by having a high level of complexity, a high internal and external communication and low vertical

differentiation, and a low level of formalization. Therefore, employees should be allowed to communicate with everybody in the organizations and not be bogged down by organizational lines of communication. They should also be allowed to communicate and work with people outside the organization and with people that are higher in the organizational hierarchy. Lastly employees should be free in how they solve their work and they do not have to write down the procedures or follow written rules and procedures (Damanpour, 1991).

When an organization is organic, a lower informal hierarchy will further improve the level of creativity within teams. Therefore, organizations that are organic should decrease the informal hierarchies within teams. This can be done by instructing team leaders to have an egalitarian structure within a team and to stress that every group member is equally important and that all suggestions are welcome and valid. Group leaders should adopt a socialized use of power so the most influential members see power as a means for advancing collective

interests and concerns (Bunderson & Reagans, 2011).

Limitations

This study has a number of limitations. First, research has been done solely in Dutch

companies. The research has been done by students studying in the Netherlands with no

access to companies in different companies. Thus, the findings of this research can be biased

towards a Dutch culture. Secondly, the sample size of this study is small. Questionnaires have

(15)

been send to 314 employees in 56 groups; however, only 205 employees have filled out the questionnaire, leaving a sample of 38 groups on which statistical analysis could be done. This will lead to a situation in which it is difficult to find significant relationships from the data, as statistical tests normally require a larger sample size to ensure a representative distribution of the population. Thirdly, this research makes use of cross sectional data. This research

observes actors at the same moment in time, without regard to differences in time. This provides us with a snapshot of the sample; however, it does not show us the same sample at different times. Lastly, this research suffers from common source bias. This research uses the same common survey source to measure both the independent variable and the moderator, but ignores the potential bias this may produce. Both the independent variable and the moderator are determined by the survey send to the leader of the group. According to Favero and Bullock (2015), using a common (or related) source to produce perceptual measures of behavioral or organizational variables can be problematic.

CONCLUSION

This research has empirically confirmed the moderating role of organizational

structure on the relation between informal hierarchy and creativity within groups. In doing so, this study contributes to the understanding of the consequences of organizational structure. In an organic organization a higher centralized informal hierarchy will diminish creativity. When one person dominates in how influential that person is in the group, all the individual

members will be less creative. However the opposite is true in a mechanistic organization. I

suggested that an informal hierarchy would have a negative effect on creativity within a

group, regardless of the organizational structure. However, as this is not the case, just the

level of centralization of influence in a group seems not to matter in influencing the creativity

within that group. I hope these findings will stimulate further research on informal hierarchy,

and creativity and help organizations.

(16)

REFERENCES

Aiken, M., & Hage, J. 1966. Organizational alienation. American Sociological Review, 31:

497-509.

Aiken, M., & Hage, J. 1971. The organic organization and innovation. Sociology, 5 (1): 63- 82.

Amabile, T. M. 1988. A model of creativity and innovation in organizations. Research in organizational behavior, 10: 123-167.

Amabile, T. M. 1996. Creativity in Context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press

Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., Staw, B. M. 2005. Affect and Creativity at Work. Administrative science quarterly, 50 (3): 367-403.

Amabile, T. M., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J., & Herron, M. 1996. Assessing the work environment for creativity. Academy of management journal, 39 (5): 1154-1184.

Anderson, C., & Brown, C. E. 2010. The functions and dysfunctions of hierarchy. Research in organizational behavior, 30: 55-89.

Bain, P., Mann, L., & Pirola-Merlo, A. 2001. The innovation imperative: the relationships between team climate,innovation, and performance in research and development teams. Small Group Research, 32(1): 55–73.

Bavelas, A. 1950. Communication patterns in task oriented groups. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 57: 271–282

Berdahl, J. L., & Anderson, C. 2005. Men, women, and leadership centralization in groups over time. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 9: 45–57.

Blinder, A. S., & Morgan, J. 2007. Leadership in groups: A monetary policy experiment.

NBER Working Paper No. 13391, September 2007

Bloom, M. 1999. The performance effects of pay dispersion on individuals and organizations.

The Academy of Management Journal, 42: 25–40.

Bunderson, J. S. 2003. Team member functional background and involvement in management teams: Direct effects and the moderating role of power centralization. Academy

Management Journal, 46(4): 458–474.

(17)

Bunderson, J. S., & Reagans, R. E. 2011. Power, Status, and Learning in Organizations.

Organization Science, 22(5):1182-1194.

Burningham, C., & West, M. A. 1995. Individual, climate, and group interaction processes as predictors of workteam innovation. Small Group Research, 26(1), 106–117.

Burns, T., & Stalker, G. M. 1961. The management of innovation. London: Tavistock.

Caniëls, M. C. J., & Rietzschel, E. F. 2015. Organizing Creativity: Creativity and Innovation under Constraints. Creativity and innovation management. 24 (2): 184-196.

Clegg, S. R., Courpasson, D., & Phillips, N. 2006. Power and organizations. London: Sage.

Courpasson, D., & Dany, F. 2003. Indifference of obedience? Business firms as democratic hybrids. Organization Studies, 24: 1231-1260

Covin, J. G., & Slevin, D. P. 1988. The influence of organization structure on the utility of an entrepreneurial top management style. Journal of management studies, 25 (3): 217- 234.

Damanpour, F. 1991. Organizational innovation: A meta-analysis of effects of determinants and moderators. Academy of management Journal, 34(3): 555-590.

Devanna, M. A., & Tichy, N. 1990. Creating the competitive organization of the 21st century:

The boundaryless corporation. Human Resource Management, 29: 445-471.

Detert, J. R., & Edmondson, A. C. 2011. Implicit voice theories: Taken-for-granted rules of self-censorship at work. Academy of Management Journal, 54(3): 461–488.

Diefenbach, T., & Sillince, J. A. A. 2011. Formal and Informal Hierarchy in Different Types of Organization. Organization Studies 32(11): 1515-1537.

Edmondson, A. 1999. Psychological Safety and Learning Behavior in Work Teams.

Administrative Science Quarterly, 44 (2): 350-383

Favero, N., & Bullock, J. B. 2015. How (Not) to Solve the Problem: An Evaluation of

Scholarly Responses to Common Source Bias. Journal of Public Administration

Research & Theory, 25: 285-308.

(18)

Fay, D., Shipton, H., West, M. A., & Patterson, M. 2015. Teamwork and Organizational Innovation: The Moderating Role of the HRM Context. Creativity and innovation management, 24 (2): 261-277.

Freeman, L. C. 1978. Centrality in Social Networks Conceptual Clarification. Social Networks, 1: 215-239.

Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee , J. C.. 2003. From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3): 453–466.

Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. 2006. Power and perspectives not taken. Psychological science. 17(12): 1068–1074.

Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Gruenfeld, D. H., Whitson, J. A., & Liljenquist, K. A. 2008.

Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6) 1450–1466.

Hayes, A. F. 2012. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper]. Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/ public/process2012.pdf

Haslam, S. A., McGarty, C., Brown, P. M., Eggins, R. A., Morrison, B. E., & Reynolds, K. J.

1998. Inspecting the emperor’s clothes: Evidence that randomly-selected leaders can enhance group performance. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research and Practice, 2:

168–184.

Hill, G. 1982. Group versus individual performance: Are N+1 heads better than one?

Psychological bulletin, 91, 517-539.

Hoff, E. V., & Öberg, N. K. 2015. The role of the physical work environment for creative employees: a case study of digital artists, The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 26 (14): 1889-1906.

Kanter, R. M. 1988. When a thousand flowers bloom: Structural, collective, and social conditions for innovation in organizations. Research in organizational behavior, vol.

10: 169-211.

Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. 1966. The social psychology of organizations. New York: Wiley.

(19)

Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. 2003. Power, approach, and inhibition.

Psychological Review 110(2): 265–284.

Khandwalla, P. N. 1977. The design of organizations. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

King, N., & Anderson, N. 1990. Innovation in working groups. Innovation and creativity at work: 81-100.

Kozlowski, S.W.J., & Bell, B.S. 2003. Work groups and teams in organizations. In W.C.

Borman, D.R. Ilgen, & R.J. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology: Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 12: 333-375. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons.

Lammers, J., Grodijn, E. H., & Otten, S. 2008. Looking through the eyes of the powerful.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5): 1229–1238.

Lang, J. C., & Lee, C. H. 2010. Workplace humor and organizational creativity. The international journal of human resource management, 21: 46-60.

Langner, C. A., & Keltner, D. 2008. Social power and emotional experience: Actor and partner effects within dyadic interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3): 848–856.

Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. 2008. Social hierarchy: The self-reinforcing nature of power and status. Academy of Management Annals, 2: 351–398.

McCurdy, H. G., & Eber, H. W. 1953. Democratic versus authoritarian: A further investigation of group problem-solving. Journal of Personality, 22: 258–269.

McCurdy, H. G., & Lambert, W. E. 1952. The efficiency of small human groups in the solution of problems requiring genuine cooperation. Journal of Personality, 20: 478–

494

McEvily, B., Soda, G., & Tortoriello, M. 2014. More formally: Rediscovering the missing link between formal organization and informal social interaction. The academy of management annals, 8: 299-345.

Mohrman, S., Cohen, S., & Mohrman, A. (1995). Designing team based organizations: New

forum for knowledgework. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

(20)

Nonaka, I. 1991. The knowledge-creating company. Harvard Business Review, 85 (7/8): 162–

171.

Oldham, G. R., & Cummings, A. 1996. Employee creativity: Personal and contextual factors at work. Academy of Management journal, 39 (3):607-634.

Paulus, P. B. 2000. Groups, teams and creativity: the creative potential of idea generating groups. Applied psychology, 49: 237-262.

Paulus, P. B., & Yang, H. 2000. Idea generation in groups: a basis for creativity in organizations. Organizational behavior & human decision processes, 82: 76-87 Pirola-Merlo, A, & Mann, L. 2004. The relationship between individual lcreativity and team

creativity: aggregating across people and time. Journal of organization behavior, 25:

235-257.

Pitcher, P., & Smith, A. D. 2001. Top management team heterogeneity: Personality, power, and proxies. Organizational Science, 12(1): 1–18.

Rubinstein, D., & Woodman, R. W. 1984. Spiderman and the burma raiders: collateral organization theory in practice. The journal of applied behavioral science, 20: 1-21.

Sanner, B., & Bunderson, J. S. 2015. When feeling safe isn’t enough: Contextualizing models of safety and learning in teams. Organizational Psychology Review, 5 (3): 224-243.

Scott, W.R. 1998. Organizations: rational, natural and open systems(4

th

edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Shaheen, R. 2010. Creativity and Education. Creative education, 1 (3): 166-169.

Shalley, C.E. 1995. Effects of coaction, expected evaluation, and goal setting on creativity and productivity. Academy of Management Journal, 38: 483-503.

Siegel, P. A., & Hambrick, D. C. 2005. Pay disparities within top management groups:

Evidence of harmful effects on performance of high-technology firms. Organization Science, 16: 259–274.

Simonton, D. K. 1997. Creative productivity: a predictive and explanatory model of career

trajectories and landmarks. Psychological review, 104 (1): 66-89.

(21)

Staw, B. M. 1990. An evolutionary approach to creativity and innovation.. Innovation and creativity at work: 287-308.

Van de Ven, A. H. 1986. Central problems in the management of innovation. Management Science, 32: 590-607.

West, M. A. 2002. Sparkling fountains or stagnant ponds: An integrative model of creativity and innovation implementation in work groups. Applied psychology, 51(3): 355-387.

Westphal, J. D., & Zajac, E. J. 1995. Who shall govern? CEO/board power, demographic similarity, and new director selection. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40(1): 60–83.

Woodman, R. W., Sawyer, J. E., & Griffin. R. W. 1993. Toward a theory of organizational creativity. Academy of Management Review, 18: 293-321.

Zaltman, G., Duncan, R., & Holbeck, J. 1973. Innovations and organizations: New York: Wiley.

Appendix A

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Hypothesis 4: A creative star´s network centrality moderates the indirect effect of their individual creativity on team creativity via creative collaboration, such that

Concluding, this study seeks to advance the knowledge of how ill-defined problems are constructed (1) by proving that a situational regulatory focus state affects the

Thus, although the goal of many empowering leaders is to create egalitarian team in which all team members can engage in influence behaviors, I argue that, due to

The current research will contribute to this work by showing that influence hierarchy steepness (i.e. the strength of the influence hierarchy) is an important factor for

Number of good ideas (original and feasible). Number of good ideas, which are feasible and original were used to measure creative performance. Hypothesis 2 predicted

Also, motivation is one requirement for creativity (Damasio, 2001). As said before movement toward a goal is linked to persons with a high BAS, persons with a high BAS probably

Keep in mind that aggressive and self-defeating humour are the independent variables, that job satisfaction, psychological empowerment, and social support are the

Pneumoperitoneum in the newborn has long been accepted as evidence of perforation of an abdominal viscus and an indication for immediate surgical intervention.'·3 In 1966 Mestel et