• No results found

Naming and remembering atypically colored objects: Support for the processing time account for a secondary distinctiveness effect

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Naming and remembering atypically colored objects: Support for the processing time account for a secondary distinctiveness effect"

Copied!
6
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Tilburg University

Naming and remembering atypically colored objects

Westerbeek, H.G.W.; van Amelsvoort, M.A.A.; Maes, A.A.; Swerts, M.G.J.

Published in:

CogSci 2014

Publication date: 2014

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Link to publication in Tilburg University Research Portal

Citation for published version (APA):

Westerbeek, H. G. W., van Amelsvoort, M. A. A., Maes, A. A., & Swerts, M. G. J. (2014). Naming and

remembering atypically colored objects: Support for the processing time account for a secondary distinctiveness effect. In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane, & B. Scassellati (Eds.), CogSci 2014: Cognitive Science Meets Artificial Intelligence: Human and Artificial Agents in Interactive Contexts

http://cognitivesciencesociety.org/conference2014/index.html

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

(2)

Naming and remembering atypically colored objects:


Support for the processing time account for a secondary distinctiveness effect

!

Hans Westerbeek (h.g.w.westerbeek@tilburguniversity.edu)
 Marije van Amelsvoort (m.a.a.vanamelsvoort@tilburguniversity.edu)


Alfons Maes (maes@tilburguniversity.edu)
 Marc Swerts (m.g.j.swerts@tilburguniversity.edu)

Tilburg center for Cognition and Communication (TiCC), Tilburg University, The Netherlands

!!

!

Abstract

The secondary distinctiveness effect is the effect that stimuli that are unusual or different from stored knowledge are re-membered better than common stimuli. We investigate the processing time explanation for this effect, i.e., that distinctive stimuli receive more attention and thus more processing time during encoding, by combining methodology from object recognition with memory tasks. Participants in our experi-ment name common and distinctive items (typically and atyp-ically colored objects), and then memory is tested. Our results replicate the secondary distinctiveness effect, as recognition scores are higher for atypically colored objects than for typi-cal ones. Crucially, analyses of response times in the naming task show that atypically colored objects are processed signif-icantly slower than typical ones. We take these findings as providing support for the processing time hypothesis for the secondary distinctiveness effect.

Keywords: Memory, secondary distinctiveness, color,

processing time, object recognition

Introduction

Items that are unusual or distinctive are remembered better than common items (e.g., Hunt & Worthen, 2006). Over the years, this distinctiveness effect has been replicated many times, and remains a field of investigation in current exper-imental psychology (e.g., Michelon, Snyder, Buckner, McAvoy, & Zacks, 2003; McDaniel & Bugg, 2008). The distinctiveness effect is often divided into two types (Schmidt, 1991). Primary distinctiveness is the effect that items that are different from the other items presented in the same (experimental) setting are remembered better (e.g., because they belong to a different semantic category; Schmidt, 1985). For example, a dog in a list of fruits is re-called better than an orange in that same list.

Secondary distinctiveness is the effect that items that are felt as being unusual as compared to general knowledge are more memorable than common items. For example, a pic-ture of something that is unusual in reality (like a green lion) is more memorable than a picture of something that is nor-mal. Because such secondary distinctive items are regarded as 'strange', the secondary distinctiveness effect is some-times called a 'bizarreness' effect (e.g., McDaniel & Bugg, 2008).

This effect has been replicated using a wide variety of research designs and stimulus materials, in order to explore the conditions under which it occurs. Research designs for example vary in how memory is tested (e.g., Graesser, Woll, Kowalski, & Smith, 1980), whether stimuli are learned

in-tentionally or implicitly (e.g., Nicolas & Marchal, 1998), and in the time span between learning and testing (e.g., O'Brien & Wolford, 1982; McDaniel & Einstein, 1986). With regard to stimulus materials, a notable distinction can be made between studies that present participants with sen-tences describing situations that are secondary distinctive (e.g., "The goldfish was eating out of the bowl on the sofa"; McDaniel and Einstein, 1986), and studies that use pictures of objects that are different from stored knowledge (e.g., a dog with a watering can as a head, or a candle with wicks on its sides; Michelon et al., 2003; Gounden & Nicolas, 2012).

While significant advances have been made in under-standing the boundary conditions of the secondary distinc-tiveness effect, scholars have reached little consensus on the various explanations for the effect. The (not mutually exclu-sive) accounts can be roughly distinguished into those that propose that secondary distinctive stimuli are encoded dif-ferently than common ones (e.g., Kline & Groninger, 1991), and accounts stating that secondary distinctive stimuli con-tain more (distinctive) cues that can be helpful in retrieval (e.g., McDaniel & Einstein, 1986).

One intuitive encoding-based explanation for the sec-ondary distinctiveness effect is the processing time

hypothe-sis (e.g., Kline & Groninger, 1991; Gounden & Nicolas,

2012). According to this account, secondary distinctive items attract more attention than common ones during learn-ing, and as a consequence more time is spent on the distinc-tive items, leading to superior memory for these stimuli.

(3)

The aforementioned studies manipulated presentation

time to investigate a potential modulating role of processing time on the secondary distinctiveness effect. However,

pre-sentation time is not necessarily the same as processing time. In the current research, it is reasoned that manipula-tions of presentation time make it difficult to ascribe modu-lations of a secondary distinctiveness effect to differences in processing time. This is not only because presentation time and processing time are not necessarily the same concepts, but also because one cannot know how quickly common and distinctive items are processed. Also, processing time is likely to vastly differ between different kinds of stimuli. Presentation times in experiments can be too short to obtain the 'necessary' encoding effect for secondary distinctive items. They can also be too long, such that distinctive items that are potentially harder to process get sufficient process-ing time anyway, nullifyprocess-ing a potential modulation of the memory effect.

A solution is to consider secondary distinctive items that are known to require more processing time than common items. Studies in the field of object recognition provide evi-dence that pictures of secondary distinctive objects require more time to be processed. In object recognition, it is well established that pictures of objects that have an atypical color (e.g., red banana) are less quickly processed (i.e., rec-ognized and named) than pictures of typically colored ob-jects (e.g., Naor-Raz, Tarr, & Kesten, 2003; Tanaka, Way-ward, & Williams, 2001; Therriault, Yaxley, & Zwaan, 2009). Objects that have an atypical color are secondary distinctive: they are unusual compared to stored knowledge, which contains information about the default color of an object (Naor-Raz et al., 2003). So, object recognition studies show that processing atypically colored objects takes more time, but we do not know whether this influences memory. The current experiment

We want to investigate the processing time hypothesis as an explanation for the secondary distinctiveness effect, tak-ing an interdisciplinary approach by combintak-ing methodolo-gy from object recognition with procedures from memory research. We administer a naming task with pictures of typi-cally and atypitypi-cally colored objects as encoding task, so we can measure processing time (i.e., naming latency) for common and secondary distinctive items. Consecutively, memory is tested in old/new recognition tests. In that way, we can investigate whether a difference in processing time is associated with better memory for these items.

Experiment

In this experiment, we asked participants to name typically and atypically colored everyday objects. As the participants were not instructed about the successive memory tests, our paradigm entails incidental learning. Directly after naming, the memory task − an old/new recognition task − was ad-ministered to test whether incidental learning was success-ful. Secondary distinctiveness effects are often found when there is a sufficient delay between encoding and testing (e.g., McDaniel & Einstein, 1986; Michelon et al., 2003), and therefore the memory task was re-administered two weeks later.

Method

Participants Forty undergraduate students (all speakers of Dutch, eight men and thirty-two women, median age 22 years) participated for course credit. They were not instruct-ed about the fact that their memory would be testinstruct-ed. None of the participants were color blind, which was assessed in a test after the experiment.

!

Materials Seventy-six everyday objects were selected on the basis of stimuli used in object recognition studies (e.g., Therriault et al., 2009). These were all color-diagnostic ob-jects (i.e., obob-jects that have one or a few typical colors asso-ciated with them). For each object a high quality photo was selected and edited, such that the object was seen on a plain white background. For the atypically colored versions, fur-ther photo editing was done to change the objects' color. Atypical colors were determined by rotating colors across the various objects, such that the number of objects in each color (red, blue, yellow, orange, green, brown, pink) was the same in both typicality conditions. Figure 1 presents some examples of objects in typical and atypical colors, as we used them in the experiment.

The seventy-six objects were equally distributed over two lists. In each list of thirty-eight objects, half of the objects was typically colored, and the other half was atypical. We ensured that an object never appeared in more than one col-or within each list. Of both lists, as second version was as-sembled in which color typicality was reversed: objects that were typically colored in one version were atypical in the other and vice versa. This resulted in two versions of two lists of objects.

The lists were matched for color frequency, whether the objects are easily named (nameability), whether the typical-ly colored pictures matched mental prototypes (prototypical-ity), how frequent the object's name is in the language (Dutch), the length of the name in syllables, and the lu8mi-nosity (i.e., brightness) of the pictures. We also made sure

(4)

that luminosity was not different for typical and atypical objects within each list. Nameability and prototypicality of the typically colored objects were determined by pretests. Name frequencies were assessed using an on-line corpus (Keuleers, Brysbaert, & New, 2010). Luminosity was mea-sured using MATLAB.

!

Procedure The experiment was performed in a dimly lit sound proof cabin, to minimize distraction. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the stimulus lists. They were instructed that they would get to see a number of pic-tures on a computer screen, and that they had to name the depicted objects as fast as possible. The instructions did not mention that memory would be tested after the naming task. The objects appeared in a random order, one by one. The presentation time for each object was exactly 3000 ms, pre-ceded by a fixation cross (800 ms) and followed by a blank screen (1000 ms). The first three items were filler objects, after which the thirty-eight stimulus objects were presented in a random order.

Immediately after the naming task, the participants had to perform a second task. They were instructed that the photos from the first task would be shown once again, but that new objects would be mixed in. Participants had to say out loud (and as quickly as possible) whether each object was part of the naming task ("yes") or not ("no"). The new objects were the objects from the list that the participant did not name. The old and new objects were presented in a random order.

The participants were asked to return to the lab about two weeks later, but they were not instructed about the purpose of this second meeting. All participants returned to the lab and performed the old/new recognition task again. Due to practical constraints, the delay between the tasks ranged from 11 to 18 days across participants (the median delay was 15 days, most participants returned after 14, 15 or 16 days). After this task, color blindness was assessed.

Responses were recorded with a head-mounted micro-phone. Stimulus randomization, timing, and voice recording were administered using E-Prime (Schneider, Eschman, & Zuccolotto, 2002). Reaction times were measured by ana-lyzing the audio recordings in Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2012).

!

Research design and statistical analysis For the naming task, we compared response times for typically and atypical-ly colored objects in a within-participants design. For the recognition task, we compared hits, false alarms and recog-nition scores in a similar within-participants design. Re-sponse times and recognition data were analyzed using re-peated measures ANOVAs, both on participants means (F1) as

on item means (F2).

Results

Naming task Despite the pretests, five of the seventy-six objects (blackberry, celery, pickle, red cabbage, sprout) yielded disproportionally high numbers of incorrect re-sponses or non-rere-sponses, and were excluded from all analyses (especially the atypically colored versions of these objects turned out to be problematic). Response times for incorrect responses were also discarded. An outlier analysis on response times for correctly named objects, in which we removed response times that were faster than 500 ms or longer than 2500 ms, resulted in discarding of 0.3 percent of the data.

Analysis of the processing time in the naming task, shown in Figure 2, revealed a main effect of color typicality:

F1(1,39)=92.29, p<.001, ηp²=0.703; F2(1,70)=65.97, p<.001,

ηp²=0.485. Typically colored objects were named

signifi-cantly faster (M=1119 ms, SD=119 ms) than atypically col-ored ones (M=1282 ms, SD=167 ms).

!

Recognition tasks As is common practice in analyzing re-sponses for old/new tasks, we corrected for response bias by calculating a recognition score (e.g., McDaniel & Einstein, 1986). This recognition score corrects the percentage of hits (i.e., the participant saying that an object was seen when it actually was) for the percentage of false alarms (i.e., the participant saying that an object was seen while it actually was not), and is calculated as (Phit−Pfalse alarm)/(1−Pfalse alarm).

Results of the immediate recognition task showed no ef-fects of color typicality on hits, false alarms, and on recog-nition scores; all p's > .07. Performance was near perfect as hit rates and recognition scores were both well above 95 percent.

Figure 2: Mean processing times (in milliseconds) in the naming task, for atypically and typically colored objects.

Error bars represent standard deviations.

M ea n proc es si ng t im e (m s) 0 500 1000 1500 Atypically

colored Typically colored

Table 1: Results of the delayed recognition task,
 in percentages, collapsed over participants.


Standard deviations are in parentheses. Typically


(5)

Results of the delayed recognition task are shown in Table 1. Analyses of hit rates revealed a main effect of color typi-cality, such that there were significantly more hits for atypi-cally colored objects: F1(1,39)=35.85, p<.001, ηp²=0.479; F2(1,70)=27.89, p<.001 ηp²=0.285. A weaker, marginally

significant effect was found for false alarms: F1(1,39)=4.27,

p=.046, ηp²=0.099; F2(1,70)=3.46, p=.07. Importantly,

recognition scores were higher for atypically colored objects than for typically colored ones: F1(1,39)=27.17, p<.001, ηp²=0.411; F2(1,70)=20.24, p<.001, ηp²=0.224 . 1

Those items that were recognized best in the delayed memory task, often required more time to be recognized in the naming task: processing times in the naming task were significantly correlated with recognition scores in the de-layed memory task (Pearson r=.34, n=142, p<.001). Re-garded per condition, processing times and recognition scores were significantly correlated for typically colored objects (r=.27, n=71, p=.025), and marginally significant for atypically colored ones (r=.23, n=71, p=.053). In both con-ditions, items that were recognized best in the delayed memory task were associated with longer processing times in the naming task.

Discussion

We report an experiment in which participants first named typically and atypically colored objects, followed by tests of memory for these objects. Atypically colored objects are secondary distinctive: they are different from stored repre-sentations of everyday normal objects. We combine an ob-ject naming task with an old/new recognition memory task, in order to investigate the processing time hypothesis of the secondary distinctiveness effect. In the naming task, we found that when the color of an object is atypical (e.g., red banana), the object is recognized less quickly than when its color is typical (e.g., red strawberry), replicating results found in object recognition studies (e.g., Therriault et al., 2009). Atypically colored objects were remembered better than typically colored ones as shown in a recognition task that was administered two weeks after the naming task.

We thus found that items that received longer processing in encoding lead to better recognition during the delayed memory test. These results are taken to support a processing time explanation for the secondary distinctiveness effect.

The underlying mechanisms facilitating the secondary distinctiveness effect are subject to debate. The processing time hypothesis explains the effect in terms of mechanisms that occur during encoding of items: distinctive items are processed longer than common ones, and therefore are more memorable (e.g., Kline & Groninger, 1991; Gounden & Nicolas, 2012). Alternative accounts focus on different mechanisms for common and distinctive items at retrieval (e.g., Wadill & McDaniel, 1998). Our experiment con-tributes to this debate by showing that secondary distinctive

items for which a memory effect is obtained (i.e., better recognition in a delayed memory test) indeed receive more processing time during encoding.

Although we have focused on an encoding-based account of the secondary distinctiveness effect, and our results lend support to this account, we do not rule out the importance of retrieval processes. We take the present results to indicate that differential processing at encoding may account for at least a portion of the secondary distinctiveness effect, but this does not preclude effects of differences in retrieval. In fact, the correlation we find between processing time in naming and recognition score in memory is significant, but not very strong, leaving variation to be explained by re-trieval-based interpretations of the superior memory for secondary distinctive items over common items. This can be researched for example by measuring retrieval times. Our research design however did not allow us to do that, as re-sponse times in the old/new recognition task not only reflect retrieval, but also the perceptual process of recognizing the objects on the screen. And, as we have seen, typically and atypically colored objects significantly differ on that mea-sure.

Our findings give rise to further questions. One question concerns the nature of the distinctiveness effect that can be obtained with atypically colored stimuli. Changing the color of stimuli is arguably a very subtle manipulation of sec-ondary distinctiveness. More extreme manipulations may however boost retrieval based effects. This is suggested by a cue-based explanation of the effect, which states that dis-tinctive items provide more cues that can be used during retrieval, and that therefore the secondary distinctiveness effect occurs (e.g., Wadill & McDaniel, 1998). When, for example, stimuli are distinctive because they consist of two objects 'fused' into one (e.g., Michelon et al., 2003), or be-cause they possess multiplied protruding attributes (e.g., Gounden & Nicolas, 2012; Nicolas & Marchal, 1998), such items also have more cues to be used during retrieval. Our stimuli however were minimally different: the only differ-ence between common and distinctive items was their color. Accordingly, secondary distinctive items did not contain a higher number of cues or attributes that distinguished them from common items, but only attributes with a different 'value'. This makes it less likely that these cues may lead to differential effects during retrieval. Further research may therefore address the hypothesis that different encoding of distinctive and normal stimuli only accounts for secondary distinctiveness effects when stimuli that are minimally dif-ferent from common stimuli are used. Only in such a case, during retrieval no higher number of cues is available for distinctive stimuli.

Other directions for future research concern the experi-mental design of our study. The recognition memory task was administered twice for each participant: directly after

Initial analyses concerning whether the number of days between the initial and delayed memory test affected recognition scores showed a

1

(6)

the naming task and two weeks later. We opted for this de-sign so that we could determine from the immediate memo-ry test whether using a naming task as an incidental learning paradigm was successful. However, it is yet unclear what the role of the immediate memory task is in the results we found in the delayed task. Educational psychologists point out that initial testing significantly improves outcomes in a successive test (i.e., test-enhanced learning; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006). Although we have no reasons to assume that such a testing effects may interact with a secondary distinctiveness effect, in future studies we will not adminis-ter an immediate test.

Additionally, in future work the naming task may be re-placed by other tasks which do not involve retrieving the verbal label for the objects, but measure how quickly visual-ly presented objects are recognized in another way. For ex-ample, a verification task can be used (e.g., Therriault et al., 2009, experiment 1b). By doing so, processing time of visu-ally presented objects is measured more directly than with a naming task, as potential effects caused by retrieving the verbal label from memory or producing a response can be avoided. For example, naming latencies for atypically col-ored objects may be longer because participants suppress mentioning the object's color. This is however unlikely, as our instructions stressed responding as quickly as possible (encouraging brief responses), and as participants had no trouble suppressing mentioning color (and hardly ever did so).

Acknowledgements

We thank Karin van Nispen, Eva van den Bemd, and four anonymous reviewers for commenting on an earlier version of this paper, and Ruud Mattheij for his assistance in using

MATLAB.

References

Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2012). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer, version 5.3.06.

Gounden, Y. & Nicolas, S. (2012). The impact of processing time on the bizarreness and orthographic distinctiveness effects. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 287−294.

Graesser, A. C., Woll, S. B., Kowalski, D. J., & Smith, D. A. (1980). Memory for typical and atypical actions in scripted activities. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Human Learning and Memory, 6(5), 503−515.

Hunt, R. R. & Worthen, J. B. (2006). Distinctiveness and

Memory. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2010). SUBTLEX -NL: A new measure for Dutch word frequency based on film subtitles. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 643−650. Kline, S. & Groninger, L. D. (1991). The imagery

bizarreness effect as a function of sentence complexity and presentation time. Bulletin of the Psychonomic

Society, 29(1), 25−27.

McDaniel, M. & Bugg, J. (2008). Instability in memory phenomena: A common puzzle and a unifying explanation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 237−255.

McDaniel, M. A. & Einstein, G. O. (1986). Bizarre imagery as an effective memory aid: The importance of distinctiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12(1), 54−65.

Michelon, P., Snyder, A. Z., Buckner, R. L., McAvoy, M., & Zacks, J. M. (2003). Neural correlates of incongruous visual information: An event-related fMRI study.

NeuroImage, 19(4), 1612−1626.

Naor-Raz, G., Tarr, M. J., & Kersten, D. (2003). Is color an intrinsic property of object representation? Perception,

32(6), 667−680.

Nicolas, S. & Marchal, A. (1998). Implicit memory, explicit memory and the picture bizarreness effect. Acta

Psychologica, 99(1), 43−58.

O'Brien, E. J. & Wolford, C. R. (1982). Effect of delay in testing on retention of plausible versus bizarre mental images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, and Cognition, 8(2), 148−152.

Roediger, H. L. & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249−255.

Schmidt, S. R. (1985). Encoding and retrieval processes in memory for conceptually distinctive events. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(3), 565−578.

Schmidt, S. R. (1991). Can we have a distinctive theory of memory? Memory and Cognition, 19(6), 523−542. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002).

E-prime 2.0 user's guide. Psychology Software Tools, Inc.,

Pittsburg.

Tanaka, J., Weiskopf, D., & Williams, P. (2001). The role of color in high-level vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,

5(5), 211−215.

Therriault, D., Yaxley, R., & Zwaan, R. (2009). The role of color diagnosticity in object recognition and representation. Cognitive Processing, 10(4), 335−342. Waddill, P. J. & McDaniel, M. A. (1998). Distinctiveness

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

significantly from the neutral control condition. Both CV and CVC primes yielded significant priming effects for CV and CVC targets when compared to the neutral control

&amp; Ingstad, B., 2012, ‘Accessing community health services: Challenges faced by poor people with disabilities in a rural community in South Africa’, African Journal

Moreover, most studies on patterns of anticipatory coarticulation in children make use of a shadowing paradigm to elicit speech , while little attention seems to

The current study aimed to find differences between patients with temporal lobe epilepsy and healthy participants in narrative discourse production tasks, and also.. investigate

In this thesis, we have presented various approaches for texture analysis with the goal of classifying between normal controls and MCI/early-stage AD: 1) texture analysis at the

21 Con- sistent with this observation, in the phase I/II trial of brigatinib, the median PFS of patients with advanced ALK-positive NSCLC who previously received crizotinib (most

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright

Furthermore, overexpression of a cell wall surface anchor family protein (LLKF_0311) led to the decreased attachment of cells to milk proteins from 98% (±0.7) to about 31% (±47%) in