• No results found

“Who gets moved by novel sustainability information?”

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "“Who gets moved by novel sustainability information?”"

Copied!
47
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

 

Master  thesis  

“Who  gets  moved  by  novel  sustainability  

information?”

                             

University  of  Groningen    

Faculty  of  Economics  and  Business     M.Sc.  Marketing  Management     December  2016     Sebas  Baneke   S2787830  

 

Moesstraat  51A   9717  JV  Groningen     (+31)  63  061  3553  s.p.w.baneke@student.rug.nl  /  sebas.baneke@live.nl       First  Supervisor    

Dr.  Jan  Willem  Bolderdijk    

(2)

Preface    

A   couple   of   years   ago,   my   academic   journey   in   Groningen   began;   and   after   overcoming   many   obstacles,  the  end  is  now  in  sight.  This  thesis  is  the  final  piece  of  work  from  my  time  at  the  university.   Since   I   developed   an   interest   in   the   psychology   of   marketing   during   my   master   programme   and   became   convinced   that   this   aspect   is   the   key   to   success   in   persuasion,   I   chose   this   subject   as   the   central   theme   of   my   examination.   What   goes   on   in   the   mind   of   a   consumer?   It   is   a   question   that   triggers  me.  In  this  thesis,  I  have  tried  to  find  an  answer  to  it  with  regard  to  the  sustainability  market.   It  is  an  attempt  to  determine  what  the  label  of  sustainability  does  to  consumers  and  their  behaviour.      

Before  I  begin  the  thesis,  I  would  like  to  extend  my  warmest  gratitude  to  those  who  have  made  a   contribution  to  the  quality  of  this  thesis.  First  of  all,  I  owe  credit  to  Dr.  J.W.  Bolderdijk.  Without  his   suggestions   that   helped   me   to   obtain   a   clear   scope,   this   thesis   would   still   be   a   mess.   Dr.   J.W.   Bolderdijk   not   only   assisted   me   regarding   the   content,   but   he   also   pushed   me   to   write   on   point,   ensuring  that  every  sentence  is  of  quality.  I  look  back  on  our  meetings  with  pleasure:  our  talks  were   informal,  pleasant  and  constructive,  with  a  lot  of  interesting  discussions.  In  addition,  I  would  like  to   thank  my  fellow  students  who  researched  the  same  topic.  Especially  Lukas  helped  me  a  great  deal.   He  was  always  ready  to  answer  my  many  questions,  which  regularly  began  with  the  words:  ‘Do  you   think  that  it  is  possible  to..?’.    

 

Finally,  I  want  to  thank  my  friends  and  family,  who  were  there  for  me  in  the  times  I  needed  them.   They  have  given  me  the  necessary  mental  and  academic  support  not  only  during  the  creation  of  this   thesis,  but  also  during  my  entire  study,  and  I  could  always  depend  on  them  when  needed.    

 

Groningen,  December  2016    

(3)

Abstract

   

Sustainability  is  a  buzzword  nowadays,  including  in  marketing.  But  although  consumers  claim  to  be   willing   to   contribute   to   a   better   environment,   they   do   not   (always)   buy   sustainable   products.   By   confronting  consumers  with  novel  information  about  sustainability,  this  thesis  aims  to  shed  new  light   on  this  discrepancy.    

Previous  studies  have  shown  that  people  react  to  novelty  in  a  positive  way,  and  that  it  increases  their   interest  and   purchase   intention   regarding   a   product.   The  present   research   applies  this   idea   to   the   sustainability   market,   and   adds   the   level   of   cognitive   capacity   as   moderator.   It   hypothesises   that   cognitive   capacity   explains   individual   differences   in   the   sensitivity   to   novel   information   regarding   sustainability.    

The   results   of   the   between-­‐subjects   experiment   design   show   that   all   consumers   have   a   greater   interest  and  a  higher  purchase  intention  when  they  are  confronted  with  novel  information  regarding   sustainability.  With  regard  to  the  intention  to  buy  a  particular  sustainable  product,  this  novelty  effect   increases  with  a  higher  level  of  cognitive  capacity.    

Keywords:  

Novelty,  Cognitive  capacity,  Sustainability,  Purchase  intention,  Interest  

 

(4)

Table  of  Content  

 

Preface                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                            2  

 

Introduction                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                          

Fout!  Bladwijzer  niet  gedefinieerd.  

 

Theoretical  Background                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                7  

 

Information  regarding  sustainability                              7     Novelty  of  information                                  7     Cognitive  capacity                                  8     Hypotheses                                10     Conceptual  Model                              11

 

 

Research  Methodology  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

         12  

  Participants  and  data  collection                          12     Design                                  12     Procedure                                                  13     Stimuli  and  cover  story                                          13     Measurements                                14  

 

Results  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

         17  

  Randomization  check                              17     Manipulation  check                              17     Main  effect:  interest                              17     Moderation  effect:  interest                            18     Main  effect:  purchase  intention                          18     Moderation  effect:  purchase  intention                          18  

 

Discussion  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

         20    

Main  effect                                20   Moderation  effect                              21   Conclusion                                22

 

 

Limitations    

 

 

 

 

 

 

                             

 

       23  

 

Managerial  Implications  

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

         24  

 

(5)

Introduction  

When  Leonardo  DiCaprio’s  climate  change  documentary  ‘Before  The  Flood’  was  released  in  October   of   last   year,   it   reached   more   than   30   million   viewers   within   two   weeks.   People   from   all   over   the   world  watched  (a  portion  of)  the  film  across  linear,  digital,  and  social  platforms,  making  it  the  best-­‐ viewed  documentary  since  2000  (The  guardian,  2016).  

As  a  topic,  the  endangered  environment  is  hot.  People  are  aware  of  the  fact  that  the  world  is  not   going  to  sustain  itself  infinitely  and  that  –  if  nothing  changes  –  its  natural  resources  will  be  depleted   in   the   near   future   (McDonagh   &   Prothero,   2014).   These   people   want   to   contribute   to   the   environment   and   are   increasingly   interested   in   sustainable   products   (Sammer,   Katharina   &   Wüstenhagen,  2006).    

Research  shows  that  many  consumers  seek  information  about  sustainability  regarding  products  and   are  likely  to  react  favourably  to  brands  that  are  environmental  friendly  (Chen  &  Chang,  2013;  Auger,   Devinney,   Louviere   &   Burke,   2008).   Therefore,   companies   nowadays   are   increasingly   including   sustainable   products   in   their   portfolio,   and   using   sustainability   as   a   marketing   tool   to   persuade   consumers  to  buy  their  goods.  

Despite   this   expansion   of   companies   and   marketers,   however,   consumers   still   do   not   behave   in   a   sustainable  way  (Kim,  Oh,  Yoon    &  Shin,  2016;  Fowler  &  Close,  2013).  It  could  be  that  the  information   about   sustainable   products   does   not   meet   consumers’   needs,   as   they   do   not   seem   to   always   pay   (sufficient)   attention   to   that   information   (Peattie,   &   Crane,   2005;   Prothero   et.   al,   2011;   Fowler   &   Close,   2013).   This   mismatch   would   explain   the   contradiction   between   the   marginal   success   of   sustainable   products   and   consumers’   claims   of   being   willing   to   change   the   environment   for   the   better  and  buy  those  goods.  

A   possible   explanation   of   the   mismatch   in   the   provided   information   and   attention   could   be   that   sustainable  products  are  promoted  with  slogans  and  additional  information  that  are  largely  similar  to   one   another.   Instead   of   convincing   consumers   into   buying   their   products   by   distinguishing   themselves  as  being  ‘green’,  companies  have  created  an  overload  of  sustainable  claims  and  product   information  (Nyilasy,  Gangadharbatla  &  Paladino,  2014;  Delmas  &  Burbano,  2011).    

(6)

eventually  results  in  a  lack  of  interest  (Fisher,  1993;  Singh,  Kristensen  &  Villaseñor,  2009;  Fennis  &   Stroebe,  2016).      

A   possible   key   to   success   for   marketers   in   the   sustainability   market   could   be   the   use   of   novelty.   Scholars  have  found  that  when  a  message  contains  novel  information,  people  react  differently  than   when   they   are   confronted   with   information   that   they   already   know.   Novelty   provokes   attention,   curiosity,  and  interest,  factors  that  can  lead  to  persuasion  (Daffner  et.  al,  2000;  Venkatesan,  1973;   Cacioppo   &   Petty,   1979).   Regarding   the   sustainability   market,   novelty   can   be   used   to   entice   consumers  to  buy  sustainable  products.  

If  and  how  the  consumer  responds  to  new  information  depends,  among  other  things,  on  his  or  her   cognitive  capacity  (Paas,  Renkl    &  Sweller,  2003;  Daffner  et.  al,  2000;  Spassova,  2010).  Not  everyone   reacts  to  novelty  in  a  positive  way;  this  is  a  delicate  matter.  Namely,  when  exceeding  the  amount  of   novelty   that   one   can   handle,   one   may   become   overloaded,   which   in   turn   can   lead   to   erroneous   decision-­‐making  (Van  Hiel  &  Kruglanski,  2013).  

Research  shows  that,  in  general,  the  higher  one’s  cognitive  capacity  is,  the  more  sensitive  one  is  to   novel   information   (Miller,   1956;   Hirschman,   1980;   Song   &   Schwarz,   2009).   The   current   study   examines  whether  this  is  also  the  case  within  the  context  of  sustainability.  Does  the  level  of  cognitive   capacity   play   a   role   in   people’s   response   when   they   are   provided   with   new   information   about   a   product?  

By  analysing  consumers  individually  based  on  their  cognitive  capacity,  this  study  aims  to  discover  a   link  between  novel  information  and  persuasion  within  the  sustainability  market.  It  is  an  attempt  to   add  knowledge  to  the  field  of  marketing  in  this  market  in  particular,  by  searching  for  a  better  way  to   persuade  consumers  to  buy  sustainable  products.    

Including   cognitive   capacity   as   a   factor,   this   study   will   draw   conclusions   about   the   way   in   which   consumers  react  to  novel  marketing  messages  (Sweller,  Van  Merrienboer  &  Paas,  1998;  Campbell  &   Kirmani,  2000).  The  level  of  persuasion  is  established  by  measuring  the  consumers’  interest  as  well  as   their  intention  to  purchase  a  particular  sustainable  product.  

This  leads  to  the  following  research  question:    

(7)

Theoretical  background

 

Information  regarding  sustainability

 

The  central  concept  in  this  study  is  sustainability,  which  has  different  meanings  in  different  fields.  In   this   study,   sustainability   is   defined   as   the   balance   between   consumers’   satisfaction   and   their   necessity  to  preserve  the  environment  (Corral-­‐Verdugo  &  Frías-­‐Armenta,  2015;  Cooper,  2005;  OECD,   2002).  This  definition  is  commonly  known  among  consumers  (OECD,  2002).  

 

In  the  1960s,  sustainability  became  a  popular  subject  in  research  papers  (Connelly,  Ketchen  &  Slater,   2011;  Homer,  2009).  Scholars  started  to  increasingly  examine  the  extent  to  which  the  environment  is   endangered,  and  tried  to  make  society  aware  of  the  fact  that  the  natural  climate  change  is  nowhere   near   as   strong   and   fast   as   the   human-­‐caused   factors   of   global   warming   (NASA,   2016; McMichael,   Woodruff  &  Hales,  2006).  

 

As   a   result,   sustainability   became   a   trending   concept   among   consumers.   Step   by   step,   awareness   arose   of   the   damaging   footprint   that   mankind   leaves   behind,   and   nowadays   people   are   well   informed   about   the   inconvenient   truth   of   global   warming   and   the   status   of   the   world   (Ehrenfeld,   2008).  

Therefore,   it   was   hypothesised   in   general   that   when   companies   started   advertising   with   sustainability,  it  would  attract  consumers’  attention  (Vermeir  &  Verbeke,  2006;  Belz,  2006;  Peattie  &   Crane,   2005).   Customers   feel   that   it   is   their   moral   duty   to   contribute   to   a   better   world,   and   even   state   that   they   are   willing   to   pay   more   for   sustainable   products   (Sammer   &   Wüstenhagen,   2006).   From  that  moment,  marketers  decided  to  capitalise  on  this  need.    

Oddly  enough,  this  advertising  strategy  was  not  found  to  be  a  qualified  success  (Vermeir  &  Verbeke,   2006;  Fowler  &  Close,  2013).  The  big  breakthrough  of  sustainability  as  a  marketing  instrument  has   not  yet  occurred.  Consumers  still  fail  to  behave  in  a  sustainable  way  (Kim,  Oh,  Yoon    &  Shin,  2016;   Fowler  &  Close,  2013),  and  seem  to  find  it  difficult  to  fulfil  their  self-­‐imposed  duty  to  contribute  to  a   better  environment  (McDonagh  &  Prothero,  2014;  Fowler  &  Close,  2013).    

 

Novelty  of  information    

(8)

concerned   with   the   environment   and   are   willing   to   make   (financial)   sacrifices   to   change   it   for   the   better,  yet  they  do  not  buy  sustainable  products.  

A   possible   way   for   marketers   to   improve   the   sale   of   such   products   is   with   the   use   of   novel   information  (Sheinin,  Varki    &  Ashley,  2011;  Smith,  Chen,  and  Yang,  2008).  Novelty  is  the  degree  of   difference  between  a  consumer’s  present  perception  and  his  or  her  past  experience  (Pearson,  1970;   Fennis     &   Stroebe,   2016).   It   concerns   adding   knowledge   to   what   a   consumer   already   knows   (Dictionary,  2016).    

Although  not  specifically  examined  within  the  sustainability  market,  novelty  has  been  proven  to  be  a   successful  strategy  in  advertising  (Sheinin,  Varki    &  Ashley,  2011;  Smith,  Chen,  and  Yang,  2008).  By   formulating   and   displaying   not   yet   known   information   on   a   product,   marketers   are   able   to   break   through  the  clutter  of  advertisements  with  which  consumers  are  confronted  today.    

Novelty   draws   attention.   As   the   stimulus   in   the   form   of   information   about   the   product   is   not   encoded   in   the   mind   of   the   consumer   at   an   earlier   moment,   the   ‘newness’   of   the   information   disconfirms   his   or   her   expectations   (Åberg   &   Nilsson,   2001).   This   results   in   a   response   of   surprise   (Fennis  &  Stroebe,  2016).    

This  salience  effect  could  in  turn  lead  to  a  positive  attitude  towards  the  product  (Smith  et.  al,  2007).   Novel   information   triggers   the   consumer’s   interest   in   the   text.   Provided   that   the   information   is   useful,  this  interest  can  lead  to  the  intention  to  buy  a  particular  product  (Smith,  Chen  &  Yang  2008;   Smith  et  al.  2007).  This  study  proposes  that  this  phenomenon  also  applies  within  the  context  of  the   sustainability  market.    

Cognitive  capacity  

(9)

response   after   reading   persuasive   information   (Roets,   Van   Hiel   &   Kruglanski,   2013;   Paas,   Renkl   &   Sweller,  2003;  Sweller  1988).  Marketers  should  therefore  be  aware  of  consumers’  cognitive  capacity.  

Cognitive  capacity  differs  from  one  person  to  another.  Therefore,  so  does  one’s  response  to  novel   information.  Marketing  and  providing  information  about  products  is  thus  highly  delicate.  In  order  to   draw   consumers’   attention   and   convince   them   to   buy   a   certain   product,   marketers   should   ensure   that   the   (amount   of)   novelty   matches   their   target’s   cognitive   capacity   (Anderson   and   Rubin   1986;   Sweller,  1994).  

Processing  novelty  requires  more  mental  effort  than  processing  information  that  is  already  known   (Paas,  Renkl    &  Sweller,  2004;  Daffner  et.  al,  2000;  Spassova,  2010).  According  to  research  by  George   Miller   (1956),   the   average   consumer   who   is   confronted   with   novel   information   can   only   process   seven  novel  items  a  time.  However,  more  recent  theories  suggest  that  it  is  likelier  that  consumers   can  obtain  and  hold  only  around  three  new  items  (Mayer,  2002; Paas,  Renkl  &  Sweller,  2003).    

When   this   maximum   amount   of   novelty   is   exceeded,   consumers   may   become   overloaded   (Ariely,   2000;  Mayer  &  Moreno,  2003).  In  the  case  of  overload,  a  mismatch  occurs  between  the  required  and   the   available   cognitive   resources   to   fully   scrutinise   and   understand   the   information   provided.   This   imbalance  could  lead  to  erroneous  decision-­‐making  (Van  Hiel  &  Kruglanski,  2013),  as  the  consumer’s   cognitive   capacity   will   become   dysfunctional,   uncertain,   and   unpredictable   (Malhotra,   1984;   Ludewig,  Geyer,  Ramseier  &  Vollenweider,  2005).  

The   precise   amount   of   novel   information   that   one   can   handle   –   without   becoming   overloaded   –   depends   on   one’s   cognitive   capacity.   Whereas   those   with   a   lower   cognitive   capacity   can   easily   become  overwhelmed  by  novelty,  those  with  a  high  cognitive  capacity  can  process  many  novel  items   (Lynch  &  Srull,  1982; Paas,  Renkl    &  Sweller,  2004).    

Because   the   latter   can   retrieve   information   from   long-­‐term   memory   more   rapidly   than   less   cognitively  capable  consumers  can,  they  do  not  need  to  pay  much  attention  to  information  that  is   already  known.  When  a  text  only  comprises  known  information,  then,  this  can  lead  to  the  message   being  perceived  as  tedious,  which  could  eventually  result  in  a  lack  of  interest  (Fisher,  1993;  Singh,   Kristensen  &  Villaseñor,  2009;  Fennis  &  Stroebe,  2016;  Daffner  et.  al,  2000).  

(10)

cognitive   capacity   to   think   about   the   text   of   the   advertisement,   making   them   more   curious,   motivated,   and,   above   all,   interested   (Johnston   et   al.   1990;   Daffner   et.   al,   2000;   Spassova,   2010).   Therefore,  it  could  be  that  novelty  also  leads  to  a  higher  purchase  intention  among  highly  cognitive   consumers.  Interest  is  namely  a  condition  for  persuasion,  although  it  is  no  guaranty  for  success  (Petty   &  Cacioppo,  1986;  Peattie,  &  Crane,  2005;  Prothero  et.  al,  2011;  Fowler  &  Close,  2013).  Therefore,   marketers  must  realise  that  the  novelty  effect  is  not  unlimited:  the  risk  of  overload  has  to  be  taken   into  account.  

In  summary,  consumers  with  a  high  cognitive  capacity  comprehend  (novel)  information  more  easily   than  people  with  a  low  cognitive  capacity  do.  Additionally,  cognitively  capable  consumers  are  more   stimulated   by   novel   information.   This   raises   the   question   of   whether   this   also   applies   to   the   sustainability  market.  It  could  be  that,  when  consumers  are  confronted  with  novel  information  about   a  sustainable  product,  cognitive  capacity  plays  a  role  in  raising  their  interest  and  purchase  intention   with  regard  to  sustainable  products.    

 

Hypotheses

 

From  the  theory  described  in  the  previous  paragraphs,  four  hypotheses  are  derived.  First,  as  novelty   in  general  leads  to  more  purchasing  behaviour,  the  following  hypothesis  is  formulated  to  examine   whether  this  idea  also  applies  within  the  context  of  sustainability:    

H1a:  Novel  sustainability  information  will  have  a  positive  effect  on  consumers’  interest.    

H1b:  Novel  sustainability  information  will  have  a  positive  effect  on  consumers’  purchase  intention.  

In   addition,   previous   research   shows   that   cognitive   capacity   plays   a   role   in   processing   novel   information.   To   examine   whether   varieties   in   cognitive   capacity   lead   to   different   reactions   to   novelty,  the  following  hypothesis  is  formulated:  

H2a:   Novel   sustainability   information   will   have   a   stronger   impact   on   the   interest   of   consumers   with  a  higher  cognitive  capacity.  

(11)

Conceptual  model  

Based  on  the  literature  review,  it  is  possible  to  create  a  meaningful  conceptual  framework  (Figure  1)   to  visually  reflect  the  present  research.  From  this  framework,  it  is  possible  to  extract  the  effect  of   cognitive  capacity,  novelty,  and  the  moderation  of  those  two  factors  on  consumers’  interest,  as  well   as  the  effect  of  cognitive  capacity,  novelty,  and  the  moderation  of  those  two  factors  on  consumers’  

purchase  intention  –  all  within  the  sustainability  market.  

     

(12)

Research  methodology    

Participants  and  data  collection  

To  achieve  the  aim  of  this  study,  an  online  survey  was  conducted  (see  appendix  A  for  a  copy  of  the   survey).  The  questionnaire  was  accessible  for  two  weeks  and  participation  was  voluntary:  individuals   could   stop   at   any   given   moment.   The   survey   was   distributed   through   online   channels   such   as   Facebook,   LinkedIn,   and   Twitter,   and   carried   out   at   different   schools.   These   two   channels   of   distribution  were  chosen  to  reach  a  large  amount  of  participants  in  a  short  amount  of  time,  and  to   ensure   that   people   with   different   backgrounds   filled   out   the   survey.   It   was   necessary   to   include   a   group  of  participants  with  a  wide  variety  of  cognitive  capacities,  as  cognitive  capacity  is  an  important   variable  in  this  study.  

In  total,  245  participants  started  the  survey.  Unfortunately,  however,  some  irregularities  were  found.   Some   individuals   missed   the   attention   check,   did   not   find   the   survey   clear   overall,   failed   the   manipulation  check,  or  did  not  finish  the  survey.  Furthermore,  some  participants  filled  out  the  survey   in  less  than  a  minute  or  did  not  look  at  the  warning  text  for  longer  than  3  seconds,  which  is  simply   too   short   to   fully   read   and   comprehend   the   text.   After   excluding   these   cases,   the   final   sample   consisted   of   192   participants   whose   answers   were   suited   for   analysis.   Of   these   participants,   most   were  between  21  and  30  years  old  (74%);  and  had  finished  a  master’s  degree  (43.2%).  Moreover,  90   were  female  (46.9%)  and  102  were  male  (53.1%).  All  of  them  had  the  Dutch  nationality  (see  appendix   B  for  the  socio-­‐demographics).    

Design    

The  data  collection  in  this  study  was  done  with  the  use  of  an  experiment  with  a  between-­‐subjects   design.  The  independent  variable  consisted  of  two  levels:  novel  versus  already  known  information.   The   moderator,   cognitive   capacity,   ranged   on   a   scale   from   1   (low   cognitive   capacity)   to   12   (high   cognitive  capacity).  

The  online  survey  tested  these  variables  in  multiple  ways,  by  changing  the  direction  of  the  question   or  asking  for  the  same  information  in  a  reversed  manner.  This  strategy  revealed  whether  participants   had  read  the  questions  thoroughly.  All  questions  in  the  survey  were  formulated  in  Dutch,  as  it  was   expected   that   the   survey   would   reach   mainly   inhabitants   of   the   Netherlands,   and   reading   information  that  is  written  in  one’s  native  language  generally  requires  the  least  amount  of  cognitive   capacity.  

 

(13)

Procedure

The  online  survey  started  by  asking  the  participants  for  their  informed  consent.  After  that,  they  were   shown  several  words  and  instructed  to  remember  them.  After  10  seconds,  the  words  disappeared   and  the  participants  were  asked  to  write  down  as  many  of  them  as  they  could  remember.  The  next   page   included   a   measure   that   checked   their   perspectives   on   sustainability   and   their   purchase   behaviour  with  regard  to  sustainable  products.

Thereafter,   the   participants   were   confronted   with   the   cover   story   and   the   stimulus.   They   were   randomly  assigned  a  version  of  the  stimulus:  one  with  novel  information  on  sustainability  or  one  with   already  known  information  on  the  topic.  After  the  participants  had  read  the  text,  several  pages  with   questions   followed   that   measured   the   participants’   perception   of   the   stimulus   and   the   potential   influence   on   their   purchase   intention.   Then,   in   the   midst   of   the   questions   about   their   purchase   intention,  a  check  was  done  to  ensure  that  the  participant  was  paying  attention.  On  the  final  pages,   the  participants  had  to  answer  some  questions  about  their  socio-­‐demographics  and  about  whether   they  had  found  the  total  survey  clear.    

Stimuli  and  cover  story  

(14)

information,  and  whether  it  stimulated  the  participants  to  change  their  future  consumption  and  thus   their  purchase  intention.    

After  constructing  the  stimuli  and  the  survey,  the  questionnaire  was  pre-­‐tested  (n=15)  among  fellow   students.   This   pre-­‐test   was   conducted   to   determine   whether   the   questionnaire,   and   the   manipulation   check   in   particular,   was   correctly   perceived.   It   was   important   that   the   participants   received  the  novel  information  as  in  fact  being  novel,  and  the  already  known  information  as  being   information   that   they   already   knew.   Figure   2   presents   both   stimuli,   with   the   novel   and   already   known  information  underlined  for  clarity.  In  the  real  stimuli,  this  information  was  not  underlined.    

           

 

 

 

Measurements  

 

Independent  variable:  In  this  study,  the  independent  variable  –  information  regarding  sustainability  –   has  two  levels:  novel  versus  already  known,  making  it  a  dichotomous  variable.  It  was  manipulated  by   randomly  presenting  the  participant  with  either  the  known  or  the  novel  information.  

 

Moderator:  The  moderator  in  this  study  is  cognitive  capacity,  which  was  established  with  the  aid  of  a   recall   test   that   has   been   used   in   several   previous   studies   (Miller,   1956;   Mayer,   2002;   Goldstein,   2007).   In   this   study,   recall   was   measured   using   the   amount   of   words   that   a   participant   could   remember  (Lynch  &  Srull,  1982;  Thiede,  1996).  

 

During  the  survey,  participants  were  exposed  to  12  words  for  10  seconds,  which  they  were  asked  to   remember  (Yorkston  &  Menon,  2004;  Goldstein,  2007).  Miller’s  (1956)  cognitive  load  theory  states   that   the   maximum   number   of   words   that   individuals   can   remember   after   looking   at   them   for   10  

Figure  2.    –  Already  known  warning  text  (left)   and  novel  information  (right)    

(15)

seconds  is  around  seven.  Based  on  this  theory,  it  can  be  assumed  that  the  task  that  participants  were   asked  to  complete  was  impossible  to  do  (Tulving  &  Pearlstone,  1966;  see  the  survey  in  appendix  A  for   the  entire  glossary).  After  the  10  seconds  had  expired,  the  words  disappeared  and  participants  were   asked  to  recall  as  many  words  as  they  could  (Mayer,  2002;  Paas,  Renkl  &  Sweller,  2003).  The  more   correct  words  the  participant  memorised,  the  lower  the  cognitive  load  was,  meaning  that  they  had  a   higher   cognitive   capacity,   and   vice   versa:   the   fewer   correct   words   they   recalled,   the   higher   the   cognitive  load  was,  implying  a  lower  cognitive  capacity  (Campbell  &  Kirmani,  2000).  

 

The  answers  of  the  recall  test  were  analysed  manually,  by  counting  the  words  that  the  participant   had  remembered.  The  number  of  correct  answers  ranged  logically  on  a  progressive  scale  from  1  to   12,   as   the   test   consisted   of   12   words.   As   stated   in   the   previous   paragraph,   the   more   words   the   participant   remembered,   the   higher   his   or   her   cognitive   capacity   was.   Participants   in   the   study   showed  a  moderate  cognitive  capacity  (M  =  5.41,  SD  =  2.19).  In  appendix  C  a  table  is  shown  with  the   total  amount  of  words  recalled.  

 

Dependent  variables:  One  of  the  dependent  variables  is  interest.  Participants’  interest  was  measured   using  duration.  In  the  manipulation  of  the  stimuli  (see  figure  2),  one  can  see  that  both  texts  have  the   same  length.  Therefore,  it  can  be  assumed  that  both  would  take  the  same  amount  of  time  to  read.   Assuming  that  looking  at  a  text  longer  means  that  one  reads  the  information  more  thoroughly,  thinks   about   it,   and   has   a   greater   interest   in   the   information  (Lohse,   1997,   James   &   Kover,   1992),   participants’  interest  was  measured  by  recording  the  duration  of  time  for  which  they  stayed  on  the   page  of  the  information.    

 

The  other  dependent  variable  is  purchase  intention.  Participants’  purchase  intention  was  measured   with  the  use  of  a  question  on  a  7-­‐point  bipolar  scale.  Participants  were  asked  to  indicate  which  of  the   following  products  they  were  most  likely  to  buy:  a  large  amount  of  unsustainable  shrimps  or  a  small   amount  of  sustainable  shrimps.    

 

In   addition,   the   participants   were   confronted   with   three   statements   on   a   7-­‐point   Likert   scale   (1   =   strongly  disagree  and  7  =  strongly  agree):  ‘I  will  buy  less  but  sustainable  shrimps  in  the  future’;  ‘I  will   buy  more  but  unsustainable  shrimp  in  the  future’;  and  ‘I  will  buy  sustainable  shrimps  next  time’.  The   second  question,  which  focuses  on  unsustainable  shrimp,  was  reverse-­‐coded  for  the  analysis.    

 

(16)

statements   showed   a   strong,   significant   correlation   (see   appendix   D).   This   was   confirmed   by   the   reliability  test,  indicating  that  the  items  could  be  combined  into  a  single  variable  that  measures  the   construct  ‘purchase  intention’  (Cronbach’s  α  =  0.928,  M  =  4.408,  SD  =  1.504).  The  scale  mean  shows   that   participants   score   slightly   above   average   on   the   7-­‐point   Likert   scale,   indicating   they   tend   to   purchase  sustainable  shrimps;  regardless  from  the  stimulus  they  saw.  

 

Manipulation   check:   A   manipulation   check   was   added   to   the   online   survey   to   assess   whether   all   participants  received  the  novel  or  the  already  known  information  in  the  manner  that  was  intended.     This  check  was  done  by  confronting  the  participants  with  the  following  four  statements  on  a  7-­‐point   Likert  scale  (1  =  strongly  disagree  and  7  =  strongly  agree):  ‘I  experienced  the  text  as  novel’;  ‘The  text   pushed   me   to   think’;   ‘The   information   was   already   known’;   ‘The   information   draws   my   attention’.   After   reverse   coding   the   opposite   question   (regarding   already   known   information)   into   the   same   direction,   a   Spearman’s   Rho   was   performed.   This   revealed   a   strong,   significant   correlation   (see   appendix  E).  A  reliability  test  indicated  that  the  four  items  could  be  combined  into  a  single  variable   measuring   novelty   (Cronbach’s   α   =   0.888,   M   =   4.320,   SD   =   1.468).   The   scale   mean   indicates   that   participants   perceived   the   stimuli   on   average   slightly   more   novel   than   already   known.   Again,   regardless  from  the  stimulus  they  saw.  

 

Randomization  check:  With  the  randomization  check  this  study  tries  to  found  out  if  the  participants   with   different   perspectives   on   sustainability   and   purchase   behaviours   with   regard   to   sustainable   products  are  randomly  allocated  to  the  stimuli.  The  participants  had  to  indicate  to  which  extent  they   agreed  to  the  following  statement  on  a  7-­‐point  Likert  scale  (1  =  strongly  disagree  and  7  =  strongly   agree):  ‘I  consider  myself  environmentally  conscious’;  ‘  I  never  read  information  about  sustainability’;   ‘I   regularly   buy   sustainable   products’:   ‘I   often   read   sustainable   information’.   The   second   question,   which  is  about  never  reading  information  about  sustainability,  is  reverse  coded.  The  results  of  the   Spearman’s  Rho  showed  a  moderate  correlation  (see  appendix  F).  The  reliability  analysis  showed  the   same  result,  indicating  the  items  can  be  combined  in  a  single  variable  measuring  the  preference  and   behaviour  of  participants  towards  sustainability  (Cronbach’s  α  =  0.818,  M  =  4.130,  SD  =  1.239).  The   scale  mean  reveals  that  participants  are  neither  sustainable  nor  unsustainable.      

 

(17)

Results  

 

Randomization  check  

The  output  of  the  independent  sample  T-­‐test  reveals  the  stimulus  conditions  do  not  differ  in  terms  of   participants  preference  and  behaviour  towards  sustainability  t(190)  =  1.603,  p  =  .524.  Therefore,  the   randomization  check  can  be  deemed  as  successful.  

 

Manipulation  check  

An   independent   sample   T-­‐test   was   conducted   for   the   manipulation   check.   The   results   show   that   when  participants  were  asked  to  rate  the  novelty  of  the  information  presented,  participants  in  the   novel  condition  rated  the  information  as  significantly  more  novel  (N  =  92,  M  =  4.8777,  SD  =  1.252)   than  the  participants  in  the  already  known  condition  did  (N  =  100,  M  =  3.807,  SD  =  1.45),  t(190)  =     -­‐5.420,  p  =  .000.  This  difference  is  visually  illustrated  in  the  appendix  (see  appendix  G).  Although  the   difference  is  smaller  than  expected,  it  is  possible  to  proceed  with  the  analysis.  

 

Main  effect:  interest    

Some  outliers  needed  to  be  excluded  from  the  dataset.  As  participants  looked  at  the  stimuli  for  an   average  of  23  seconds  (M  =  22.869  SD  =  16.798),  staring  at  the  information  for  more  than  60  seconds   seemed   unreasonably   long.   Therefore,   two   participants   who   looked   at   the   already   known   information  and  five  participants  who  were  confronted  with  the  novel  text  were  excluded.    

Comparing  the  residual  plot  of  the  one  with  seven  outliers  and  the  one  without  them,  one  can  see   the  data  are  better  fitted  to  the  regression  line  in  the  latter  (see  appendix  H  and  I).  This  raised  the   coefficient   of   determination   from   4.9%   (R2  =.049)   to   10.0%   (R2  =   0.100).   By   conducting   an  

independent  sample  T-­‐test  on  the  data  without  those  outliers,  it  could  be  seen  that  participants  in   fact  look  longer  with  novel  information  (N  =  87,  M  =  23.439,  SD  =  8.632)  than  with  already  known   information  (N  =  98,  M  =  17.483,  SD  =  9.229).    

(18)

Moderation  effect  (interest)  

With  regard  to  the  output  of  the  multiple  regression  (see  table  1),  it  is  possible  to  determine  that  the   model  is  overall  significant  (F(3,181)=  7.164,  p  =.000).  However,  the  interaction  effect  was  found  not   to  be  significant  (b  =  -­‐.628,  p  =  .309),  suggesting  that  the  level  of  cognitive  capacity  has  no  significant   influence  on  the  relation  between  the  way  in  which  information  is  presented  and  the  interest  of  a   participant.  The  variance  explained  in  the  model  is  10.6%  (R2=.106).  

     

Constant   17.51**  

Novelty  of  information     5.94**  

Cognitive  capacity  mean  centered   (words  correct)  

0.354  

Novel   information   x   cognitive   capacity  (interaction  effect)  

-­‐0.628   F   7,16   P   .000   R2   .106   Adjusted  R2   .091  

 

 

 

Main  effect    

Participants  who  were  confronted  with  novel  information  (N  =  92,  M  =  4.910,  SD  =  1.426)  stated  that   they  would  almost  certainly  buy  the  sustainable  shrimps,  whereas  the  individuals  who  saw  the  text   containing  already  known  text  (N  =  100,  M  =  3.945,  SD  =  1.430)  were  fairly  neutral  regarding  their   purchase  intention  regarding  these  sustainable  animals.  

A  simple  regression  showed  that  the  type  of  information  (b  =  .965,  p  =  .000)  indeed  has  a  significant   effect  on  a  participant’s  purchase  intention  (F(1,190)  =  21.899,  p  =  .000).  The  variance  explained  in   this  model  is  10.3%  (R2  =  .103).      

Moderation  effect  

In   the   output   of   the   multiple   regression   (see   table   2),   one   can   see   the   interaction   effect  between   cognitive   capacity   and   novel   information   on   the   purchase   intention   of   participants   (b   =   .313,   p   =   .001).   The   results   suggest   that   the   level   of   cognitive   capacity   has   a   significant   influence   on   the  

(19)

relation  between  the  way  in  which  information  was  presented  and  a  participant’s  purchase  intention   (F(3,188)   =   11.481,   p   =   .000).   This   implies   that   when   a   participant   was   confronted   with   novel   information,  a  high  level  of  cognitive  capacity  had  a  positive  effect  on  his  or  her  purchase  intention.   The  variance  explained  in  the  model  is  15.5%  (R2  =  .155).      

 

Constant   3,939**  

Novelty  of  information     .956**  

Cognitive  capacity  mean  centered   (words  correct)  

-­‐  .095  

Novel  information  x  cognitive   capacity  (interaction  effect)  

.313**   F   11,481   P   .000   R2   .155   Adjusted  R2   .141      

 

Table  2.  –Regression  analysis  results   Dependent:  Purchase  intention  

(20)

Discussion    

In   the   context   of   marketing,   research   shows   that   novelty   moves   (Sheinin,   Varki     &   Ashley,   2011;   Smith,   Chen,   and   Yang,   2008).   By   formulating   and   displaying   not   yet   known   information   on   a   product,  marketers  are  able  to  persuade  consumers  to  buy  that  product.  

 

The   present   study   examined   whether   the   novelty   effect   is   also   applicable   within   the   sustainability   market.  If  so,  the  use  of  novel  information  could  be  a  strategy  to  improve  the  success  of  sustainable   products.   Although   consumers   claim   that   they   are   concerned   with   the   environment   and   want   to   change  it  for  the  better,  the  breakthrough  of  sustainable  goods  has  not  yet  happened  (Kim,  Oh,  Yoon     &  Shin,  2016;  Fowler  &  Close,  2013).  People  do  not  always  seem  to  pay  (sufficient)  attention  to  the   information  on  sustainable  products  (Peattie,  &  Crane,  2005;  Prothero  et.  al,  2011;  Fowler  &  Close,   2013).  

Because  scholars  have  concluded  that  cognitive  capacity  plays  a  role  in  the  way  in  which  consumers   respond  to  (novel)  information  (Anderson  and  Rubin,  1986;  Sweller,  1994),  this  study  hypothesised   that  the  novelty  effect  is  influenced  by  one’s  level  of  cognitive  capacity.  The  higher  that  level  is,  the   stronger  the  impact  of  novel  sustainability  information  is  on  persuasion.    

Using  a  between-­‐subjects  experiment  design,  the  impact  of  already  known  versus  novel  sustainability   information   was   measured   and   connected   to   cognitive   capacity   among   245   participants.   In   the   context   of   this   study,   impact   was   analysed   by   measuring   the   participants’   interest   and   purchase   intention.    

Main  effect  

Concerning   the   main   effects,   this   study   concludes   that   the   sustainability   market   is   similar   to   the   market   in   general:   novelty   has   a   ‘positive’   effect   on   consumers.   As   expected,   the   results   of   this   experiment   revealed   that   novel   sustainability   information   had   a   greater   impact   on   the   purchase   intention  than  already  known  information  did.  This  result  is  similar  regarding  the  influence  of  novel   information  on  interest  and  purchase  intention.    

(21)

existing   theories   on   novelty   in   the   field   of   marketing   in   general,   which   state   that   novelty   triggers   consumers’  interest  (Smith,  Chen  &  Yang  2008;  Smith  &  Yang  2004).  

The  results  regarding  the  changes  in  the  purchase  intention  of  consumers  who  are  confronted  with   novel  information  are  also  in  line  with  previous  research.  Existing  literature  has  shown  that  when  the   novel  information  in  a  text  like  advertisement  is  useful,  it  can  lead  to  the  intention  to  buy  a  particular   product  (Smith,  Chen  &  Yang  2008;  Smith  &  Yang  2004;  Smith  et  al.  2007).  The  present  study  reveals   that   this   idea   also   applies  to   the   sustainability   market.   Participants   who   saw   the  text   with   already   known  information  were  less  eager  to  buy  the  sustainable  product  than  those  who  were  confronted   with  the  novel  text.  

Moderation  effect  

Novel  sustainability  information  has  a  positive  effect  on  consumers’  interest  and  purchase  intention.   By  considering  the  level  of  cognitive  capacity,  this  study  aimed  to  go  even  further  and  reveal  whether   it  plays  a  role  in  consumers’  sensitivity  to  novelty.  As  novel  information  takes  more  brainpower  to   fully   comprehend,   previous   research   has   found   that   it   stimulates   people   with   a   high   cognitive   capacity   more   than   already   known   information   does   (Johnston   et   al.   1990;   Daffner   et.   al,   2000;   Spassova,  2010).  Consumers  with  a  low  cognitive  capacity,  on  the  other  hand,  become  more  easily   overwhelmed  by  novel  information  (Lynch  &  Srull,  1982;

 

Paas,  Renkl    &  Sweller,  2004).  Therefore,   this  study  hypothesised  that  the  higher  the  cognitive  capacity  is,  the  stronger  the  novelty  effect  will   be.

 

This  was  not  found  to  be  unambiguously  true.  In  contrast  to  what  was  expected,  highly  cognitively   capable   participants   who   were   confronted   with   the   novel   information   did   not   spend   significantly   more   time   scrutinising   the   text   than   individuals   with   a   lower   cognitive   capacity   did.   This   finding   suggests  that  the  level  of  cognitive  capacity  in  this  research  cannot  be  connected  to  the  interest  of   consumers  who  read  the  text  containing  novel  sustainability  information.  Contrary  to  what  previous   research  on  cognitive  capacity  has  suggested,  within  the  sustainability  market  the  present  study  did   not   find   that   highly   cognitively   capable   consumers   were   stimulated   by   novelty   to   a   greater   extent   than   individuals   with   a   lower   cognitive   capacity,   making   them   more   curious,   motivated,   and   interested.    

(22)

confronted  with  the  novel  version  of  the  text,  the  purchase  intention  of  those  who  were  more  highly   cognitively  capable  was  found  to  be  stronger  than  that  of  those  who  were  less  cognitively  capable.   Thus,   the   results   support   the   hypothesis   that   the   higher   the   level   of   cognitive   capacity   is,   the   stronger  the  novelty  effect  will  be.  

Conclusion  

The   results   provide   empirical   support   for   the   statement   that   novel   information   has   an   impact   on   consumers   in   the   sustainability   market.   When   confronted   with   a   text   that   contains   novel   sustainability   information,   consumers   respond   in   a   more   positive   manner   than   when   they   see   a   marketing   text   that   provides   information   that   they   already   know.   Regarding   persuasion,   novelty   simply   has   a   greater   impact.   Not   only   do   consumers   spend   more   time   reading   novel   sustainable   product   information,   which   implies   a   higher   interest,   but   their   intention   to   buy   that   particular   sustainable  product  is  also  greater  with  novelty  than  with  already  known  information.  These  results   suggest  that  consumers  do  not  always  pay  (sufficient)  attention  to  the  (already  known)  information   about  sustainability  in  the  texts  like  advertisements  that  marketers  use  nowadays  (Peattie,  &  Crane,   2005;  Prothero  et.  al,  2011;  Fowler  &  Close,  2013).    

This   study,   however,   went   deeper   than   the   impact   of   novelty   in   the   sustainability   market:   it   also   examined  whether  the  cognitive  capacity  of  an  individual  has  an  impact  on  his  or  her  response  to   novel   sustainability   information.   The   main   research   question   was:   ‘Does   cognitive   capacity   explain   individual  differences  in  the  sensitivity  to  novel  information?’    

The  results  show  that  this  question  cannot  be  answered  conclusively.  Cognitive  capacity  does  have   an   impact   on   the   effect   that   novel   information   can   have   on   consumers,   but   only   regarding   their   intention  to  purchase  a  sustainable  product.  When  confronted  with  novel  sustainability  information,   consumers   who   are   cognitively   capable   to   a   greater   extent   seem   to   be   more   eager   to   buy   the   particular  product  than  individuals  who  have  a  lower  cognitive  capacity  do.  Regarding  interest,  the   results  provide  no  significant  evidence  that  the  level  of  cognitive  capacity  plays  a  role  in  the  effect  of   novel   sustainability   information.   One   cannot   conclude   that   the   interest   of   a   highly   cognitively   capable   consumer   is   triggered   by   novel   sustainability   information   more   than   an   individual   whose   cognitive  capacity  is  lower.  Nor  can  any  conclusion  be  drawn  about  the  statement  of  scholars  that   interest  is  a  condition  but  no  guarantee  of  consumers’  purchase  intention  (Petty  &  Cacioppo,  1986;   Peattie,  &  Crane,  2005;  Prothero  et.  al,  2011;  Fowler  &  Close,  2013).  

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Depending on the dataset, it might be useful to – next to a dataset deposit - publish a data paper that provides information on the data the research setup, the methods used and

The results suggest that companies with higher scored assurance on their sustainability disclosure are more likely to have lower environmental performance.. This effect is

Does independent, third-party assurance on sustainability disclosures increase the perceived firm value by investors, and is this increase affected by the level and provider

buitenmuur van de in 1892 afgebroken zuidbeuk wel op de oude plaats stond, maar naar een foto omzeggens gans met baksteen hernieuwd was, en er dus geen gegevens

The researcher is of the opinion that this form of training can be beneficial when informing managers on the company's sexual harassment policies and procedures (Refer

The research population for this study are Dutch students that decided to move out of their student home and return to their parental home during the Covid-19 crisis in The

b) Draw the corresponding flowchart. Return the number of eggs found as a result. Tip: The return is the very last statement that can be called. Mimi must thus first go back to

This reveals the identity information Web 2.0 users want to have of other participants and are willing to provide about themselves, the importance of role information of