• No results found

Fear and Disgust in Consumer Behavior: An Evolutionary Perspective

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Fear and Disgust in Consumer Behavior: An Evolutionary Perspective"

Copied!
62
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Fear and Disgust in Consumer Behavior: An Evolutionary

Perspective

Research Master thesis, Marketing profile

University of Groningen, Faculty of Economics and Business

(2)

Abstract

On the basis of an evolutionary approach, the present research examined the hypotheses that

fear-motivated avoidance and disgust-fear-motivated avoidance can automatically activate the

disease-avoidance system and, in turn, affect consumer behavior. In Experiment 1 I shown that exposure to

disfigured faces (fear-related stimuli) determines a more positive attitude toward healthy foods,

whereas in Experiment 2 I show that exposure to dirty images (disgust-related stimuli) determines a

less positive attitude toward second hand products. Although none of these relationships are

mediated by disease avoidance activation, these results foster new theoretical development in

consumer behavior from an evolutionary perspective that can be used as a basis for further research.

(3)

Tables of Contents

1. Introduction. ... 4

2. Theoretical Framework: Disease avoidance system ... 6

2.1 Disease Avoidance and Fear... 9

2.2 Disease avoidance and disgust ... 12

3. Method ... 15

3.1 Study 1 ... 15

3.3 Study 2 ... 26

3.4 Results and Discussion ... 30

4. General Discussion ... 37

4.1 Managerial Implications ... 40

4.2 Limitations and directions for future research ... 41

References ... 45

(4)

1. Introduction.

Although the choices made by modern consumers seem to have everything to do with

contemporary culture, and little to do with human nature, more and more psychologists are

informing their research by drawing on an evolutionary perspective, which suggests modern

behavior is the result of the survival instincts developed over time by our ancestors (Schaller et al.

2006).

In particular, this emerging perspective suggests that motivational systems have been shaped by

natural selection to produce behaviors that increase reproductive fitness. To reproduce successfully,

namely to produce viable offspring and raise them to reproductive age, human beings need to

achieve many goals, including affiliation, self-protection, status, mate acquisition, mate retention,

and child rearing. Some of these goals may appear similar, but they are qualitatively distinct:

Successful attainment of each goal requires different, and sometimes opposing, cognitive and

behavioral responses. The objective of this research is to study the effects of one of these goals on

consumer behavior. In particular, I analyze how the activation of the disease avoidance system can

influence consumer attitudes, spending intentions and choices in terms of healthy foods and second

hand products.

The aforementioned deep-seated ancestral motives in fact, continue to influence much modern

behavior, although not always in obvious or conscious ways, giving rise to counterintuitive

implications especially from a consumer research perspective. For example, Griskevicius and

Kenrick (in press) presented a framework that offers a host of empirical implications for connecting

basic evolutionary psychological and behavioral processes – from attention to memory to cognition

to choice – in ways that have direct implications for theory and research in consumer behavior. In

addition, Griskevicius et al. (2009) demonstrated that an evolutionary approach can explain how

arousal-inducing contexts, such as frightening or romantic television programs, influence the

(5)

goals in men increased their willingness to spend on conspicuous luxuries but not on basic

necessities, whereas in women inducing mating goals boosted public – but not private – helping.

The mate acquisition motive was also studied in another contribution of Griskevicius et al. (2006),

where the authors explored the effects of mating motivation on creativity, discovering that romantic

motives enhanced creativity. In another contribution Griskevicius et al. (2007) examined how

self-protection and mate attraction influenced conformity. They found that a self-protective goal

increases conformity for both men and women. In contrast, the effects of a romantic goal depended

on sex, causing women to conform more to others’ preferences while engendering nonconformity in

men.

These contributions are just some examples of an emerging perspective that highlights a

different lens through which to look at motivation, distinguishing between proximate and ultimate

(ancestral) motives. While there could be innumerable proximate motives for behavior, there is a

much smaller set of ultimate evolutionary functions that this behavior might serve (Kenrick et al.

2010). Consumer research literature, like most social scientists, has typically been concerned with

proximate reasons for behavior (for instance, what people want to feel or seek to avoid feeling),

whereas an evolutionary viewpoint emphasizes that there is a deeper level of explanation rooted in

the adaptive function of behavior (Griskevicius & Kenrick, in press). Therefore, according to the

aforementioned contributions, marketing scholars are focusing their investigation on the

understanding this deeper level of motivations determining as a result, the presence of an emerging

stream of research that aims to study the implications of activating ancestral motives on modern

consumer behavior. Given the novelty of this topic further research is required to analyze in greater

depth the implications of each of the seven fundamental motives on consumer behavior. As a result,

the focus of this thesis is on the effects of the activation of disease avoidance on consumer attitudes,

evaluations and choices.

In general, each fundamental motives, as well as disease avoidance, can be activated by

(6)

(Kenrick et al. 2010). My research project aims to focus especially on threats related to disease

avoidance activation. In particular, I want to study the role of two possible threats that can result

from the activation of the disease avoidance system: threats arising from the fear of disease and

threats arising from the disgust of contagion.

Consequently my research questions are:

1. What is the effect of fear resulting from the activation of the disease avoidance system on food attitudes, food purchase intentions and food choices of customers?

2. What is the effect of disgust resulting from the activation of the disease avoidance system on

second hand product attitudes, second hand product price evaluations and second hand product choices of customers?

2. Theoretical Framework: Disease avoidance system

Biologists estimate that infectious diseases have been an important selection pressure on the

human species (Gangestad & Buss, 1993). Despite the medical and technological discoveries that

have controlled and limited many diseases over time, virus and bacteria contagion still represent a

daily threat for humans. The result of this ever-present pathogen threat has been the evolution of a

biological immune system to fight off infection, and also the evolution of a psychological “behavioral immune system” that helps humans to avoid infection through their behaviors (Schaller & Park, 2011). In this perspective, considering the ramifications of interacting with an individual

who is potentially diseased, disease avoidance activation helped people over time to readily identify

diseased individuals and actively avoid them (Park, Faulkner & Schaller, 2003). In keeping with

most evolved mechanisms, these processes have been described as occurring quickly, with very

little conscious, rational thought or deliberation (Townsend & Hamilton, 2012). Consequently,

according to Griskevicius & Kenrick (in press), it is possible to define the disease avoidance system

(7)

infections in order to preserve their health. As a result, this avoidance tendency against all sources

of pathogens and infections, is generally associated with negative emotions, such as fear, anxiety,

disgust, etc. The theoretical and research contributions of the past two decades have established

emotions as a legitimate area of scientific inquiry in the field of marketing. Extensive efforts have

been devoted to investigating the role of emotions in marketing, borrowing theories of emotions

from psychology (Huang, 2001). Most of the existing theoretical contributions distinguish between

negative and positive emotions (see for example, Izard,1977; Mehrabian & Russell, 1974; Plutchik,

1980). Whereas positive emotions generally involve a sense of pleasure resulting from a positive

event, negative emotions can result from problems with ongoing plans and failures to achieve

desired goals. They express an attempt or intention to exclude, keeping away or destroying what is

perceived as a threat (see also Stein, Liwag, & Wade, 1996). Consistent with these statements, this

thesis will focus on two particular negative emotions that can arise from disease avoidance

activation: fear and disgust. Since both disgust and fear have a dominant behavioral tendency of

avoidance (Izard, 1993), there is a considerable overlap between these two emotions. However they

present several differences. First, fear-motivated avoidance protects the person from perceived risk

in a general sense (i.e., any physical danger that might compromise his or her own health, Woody &

Teachman, 2000), whereas disgust-motivated avoidance appears to have the more specific function

of avoiding contaminations (Olatunji, Cisler, McKay, & Phillips, 2010). In addition, while fear

heightens activity in preparation for fight or flight (Phillips, Senior, Fahy, & David, 1998), disgust

involves a suspension of activity. With respect to the disease-avoidance system, the preparation for

fight associated with fear implies worry and alarm generated by a possible threat of infection,

whereas the suspension of activity associated with disgust implies the rejection (or refusal) of a

potential source of contagion. These arguments in turn imply a temporal distinction between the

two emotions, and in fact, fear precedes disgust. In particular, while fear arises as a form of caution

before being exposed to or in contact with a possible infectious source, disgust arises after being

(8)

Finally, with regard to the disease avoidance system, fear and disgust are distinguishable in

terms of certainty. Physical signals indicating an illness tend to be perceived as a threat that

prepares and motivates a person to escape from it generating fear (Izard, 1977; Townsend &

Hamilton, 2012). Consistent with its function as a reaction to threat, fear has been described as a

state of maximal uncertainty – given the precarious nature of threatening situations, the uncertainty in fear stems from the frightened person’s insecurity over what he or she should do and whether he or she will be able to escape or avoid the fear-eliciting object (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985).

Conversely, disgust has been conceptualized as “a revulsion” at the prospect of incorporation of an

offensive substance (Rozin & Fallon, 1987). This definition suggests that feelings of disgust are

related to physical contact between the disgusting object and the human body. In stark contrast to

fear, disgust has been associated with a strong sense of certainty, because a disgusted person is

certain what the problem is, knows how to deal with it, and is confident in his or her ability to do so

(Smith & Ellsworth, 1985).

Accordingly, fear of disease should be elicited by stimuli indicating threat of infection of an

uncertain nature, whereas disgust should be elicited by stimuli that clearly indicate a threat of contamination.

As a result, exposure to any physical signal indicating an abnormality (e.g., disfigured faces or

a person affected by disabilities or obesity) should affect fear-motivated avoidance, and not

disgust-motivated avoidance. This is because although people affected by disabilities are not able to infect

others, they are perceived as a possible threat of disease transmission (Faulkner et al. 2004).

However, the nature of this threat is not supported by specific reasons and therefore it is uncertain.

Conversely, the exposure to images related to dirtiness (e.g., dirty toilets or insects) should affect

disgust-motivated avoidance, and not fear-motivated avoidance. This because these images

unequivocally indicates a risk of contagion (Ryan, Oaten, Stevenson, & Case, 2012), and are

(9)

Consequently, in this research fear-avoidance will be elicited through stimuli showing physical

abnormalities (i.e., faces disfigured by a wine stain birthmark), whereas disgust-avoidance will be

elicited through stimuli showing dirtiness (i.e., dirty toilets, dirty foods, or vomit). Depending on

the triggered emotions (fear vs. disgust), the activation of disease avoidance system can lead to

interesting implications especially from a consumer research perspective. These implications will be

illustrate in the following two sections.

2.1 Disease Avoidance and Fear

Infections and disease transmission are perceived as a threat that scares people, generating fear.

The fear of contamination is the most common theme observed in obsessive-compulsive disorder

(Rasmussen & Tsuang, 1986). Recent theoretical developments have highlighted the complex,

powerful, and persistent nature of this fear (Rachman, 2004). Consequently, fear of disease creates a

series of avoiding behaviors, such as compulsive safety behaviors (i.e., excessive washing or

cleaning rituals) geared toward disinfection of the self and the environment (Rachman & Shafran,

1998), or becoming more socially avoidant, more introverted, or less tolerant for foreigners

(Mortensen, Becker, Ackerman, Neuberg, & Kenrick, 2010). Many other phenomena in the realm

of social cognition are influenced by fear of disease and by individual differences in chronic

concerns about disease transmission (Schaller & Duncan, 2007). For example, Park, Faulkner, &

Schaller (2003) demonstrated that people who feel particularly vulnerable to disease have fewer

friends with physical disabilities.

The disease avoidance system is mainly activated by cues suggesting the presence of

pathogens, skin lesions, or abnormalities (Schaller, Miller, Gervais, Yager & Chen, 2010). These

images can lead to individuals’ unconscious activation of specific avoidance behavior strategies that

(10)

For example, after seeing photographs that make germs and infections salient, people view

themselves as less extroverted and less open to new experiences (Mortensen et al. 2010). More

generally, disease avoidance activation predicts ethnocentric attitudes, antipathy toward individuals

who are obese or physically disabled, and preferences for facial characteristics associated with good

health (Faulkner et al. 2004). From these contributions it is possible to note that disease avoidance

activation generates a series of avoidance behaviors strategies. Accordingly, another possible

avoidance behavior strategy resulting from disease avoidance activation could be having a more

positive attitude toward healthy (vs. unhealthy) foods, and therefore avoiding unhealthy foods.

Healthy food is defined as any food believed to be “good for the health”, especially if high in fiber, natural vitamins, etc. Healthy foods are believed to reduce cholesterol, atherosclerosis and

risk of stroke; to help control glucose; to halt progression of osteoporosis; and to reduce the risk of

infections and cancer (source The Oxford English Dictionary).

Unhealthy food is defined as any food that is not regarded as being conducive to maintaining health. It includes fats, in particular of animal origin; “fast” foods – (low in fiber and vitamins); “junk food” (e.g., potato and corn chips, pretzels, crackers – high in salt and tropical oils); and “white sauces” (northern Italian cuisine, high in fat) (source The Oxford English Dictionary).

Researchers posit that unhealthy and healthy foods consumption, and its perceptual and sensory

drivers, has an evolutionary basis. For example, Wansink & Huckabee (2005) note that "fatty foods

helped our ancestors weather food shortages,... [and] sugar and the sweetness associated with it

helped them distinguish edible berries from poisonous ones". Furthermore, previous literature

shows that energy-dense (i.e., unhealthy) foods, while worse for long-term health, are better than

non-energy-dense foods in providing short-term energy stores, which human beings have been

hardwired to favor (Ostan et al. 2010). Consequently, from an evolutionary perspective unhealthy

foods are preferred to healthy foods.

However, the activation of fear of disease could reverse this expected effect. “Fear salience" is

(11)

event (Gore et al. 1998). A credible fear cue leads to increased threat appraisal perceptions (Witte &

Allen, 2000). This has important implications in the food domain, in that the short-term health

related costs of eating unhealthily are not always obvious (Schlosser, 2001). Thus, the use of a

credible fear cue should increase threat appraisal perceptions surrounding unhealthy eating. When

threat appraisal perceptions increase, people are more likely to adopt adaptive health protection

behaviors (Witte & Allen, 2000), and consequently the use of a fear cue should lead to positive and

adaptive healthy behavior. Thus, threat appraisal perceptions (through fear) should cause consumers

to muster more willpower to resist the natural impulsive urge to overconsume (underconsume)

unhealthy (healthy) food.

In this perspective, the disease avoidance system can be activated using the concept of

disfigurement. In fact, several authors have claimed that avoidance of people with disabilities, facial

lesions and disfigurement, among others characteristics, arises because such individuals trigger an innate disease avoidance system, which is primed to detect “disease-like” signs irrespective of their veracity (Oaten, Stevenson & Case, 2011). According to these studies the discomfort felt upon

viewing facial abnormalities may arise from a primal fear of contacting infections. Fear of

contagion arises due to the non conformity to the "normal facial standard" (without deformities)

generated by the specific abnormality (e.g., a disfigurement). This in turn generates a sense of

uncertainty about the health of the person with the disfigured face, which results in fear (and not in disgust)of contagion. As a result, subjects perceive disfigurement as a disease threat, implementing

an avoidance defense system against it. Consequently, I propose that the activation of the disease

avoidance system – and thus the fear of illness – through facial disfigurement exposure could

promote a more positive attitude, better purchase intentions, and choice of a more salutary food

option.

Therefore:

H1a: Fear of illness leads to more positive healthy food attitudes, and this relationship is mediated

(12)

H1b: Fear of illness leads to more positive healthy food purchase intentions, and this relationship is

mediated by the activation of disease avoidance.

H1c: Fear of illness leads to healthy food choice, and this relationship is mediated by the activation

of disease avoidance.

2.2 Disease avoidance and disgust

As previously mentioned, humans have a repertoire of behaviors that function to minimize

contact with pathogens. Some of these behaviors, and arguably the most important for humans in

the context of disease avoidance, are those resulting from the emotion of disgust (Curtis, Aunger,

& Rabie, 2004).

Disgust is typically experienced as a feeling of revulsion, sometimes accompanied by nausea,

along with a strong desire to withdraw from the eliciting stimulus (Rozin, Haidt & McCauley,

2000).

The proposed primary biological function of disgust is to protect the organism against disease

and contamination (Marzillier & Davey, 2004). However, some theorists have extended this basic “disease avoidance” view of the disgust response to suggest that the disgust emotion has evolved more sophisticated adaptive functions that protect not just the physical body but also the “psychological” body from contamination and harm by providing reminders of human mortality related to the threat of contamination by disgusting substances (Haidt, McCauley, & Rozin, 1994).

Viewed from this “psychological” perspective, disgust also serves to regulate behavior in social and

interpersonal contexts, affecting consumer social attitudes.

Disgust may produce specific autonomic responses, such as reduced blood pressure, heart rate

deceleration, and decreased skin conductance (Stark, Walter, Schienle, & Vaitl, 2005). A central

feature of certain cues that evoke disgust is that contact with them can result in a neutral object

(13)

Morales and Fitzsimons (2007) demonstrated the strong influence of disgust in a consumer

context. Using evidences from several studies, the authors developed a theory of product contagion,

in which disgusting products are believed to transfer offensive properties by physical contact with

other products they touch, thus influencing evaluations. The law of contagion argues that objects or

people can affect each other merely by touching; consequently, contagion happens when a source

(either a person or an object) passes some or even all of its properties to the target through touch.

Since the source is believed to transfer a contagious entity or “essence” to the target on contact, the

properties remain part of the target even after they are no longer touching (Morales & Fitzsimons,

2007). The law of contagion was first tested by Rozin, Millman, & Nemeroff (1986) in the realm of

disgust to show that these contagion beliefs are not limited to primitive cultures, but operate in

Western cultures as well. They found, for instance, that a drink touched briefly by a sterilized

cockroach became undesirable, as did a laundered shirt previously worn by a disliked person.

Because contact between objects is often how true microbial contamination occurs, a biological

view suggests that contagion beliefs have adaptive value and could have originated as a form of

protection against true physical contamination (Morales & Fitzsimons, 2007). This is in line with

Rozin and Fallon’s (1987) theory, that disgust is also elicited by physical contact with unpleasant or

unknown people. Consequently, according to the product contagion theory, another possible

response to disgust of contamination could be the avoidance of products touched by others. In fact,

Argo, Dahl & Morales (2006) demonstrated that consumers evaluate products previously touched

by other shoppers less favorably. This is because products touched by others can be perceived as

infected, carriers of germs. In addition, this perception of “infection” can in turn have interesting implications regarding the consumers’ attitude toward second hand products. In this scenario such products can be perceived as less desirable than brand new products because they are previously

used, and therefore have been touched (and "infected") by others. In a O’Reilly, Rocher, Hughes,

Goran, & Hand (1984) study, 76% of respondents indicated that they would not buy used

(14)

desirability of the second hand products was to avoid contamination. Because the original owners of

the used products remained anonymous to consumers, these feelings could not be based on

associations but instead suggest a belief that the clothing was contaminated simply because it had

been handled and owned by someone else previously.

From this perspective, the disease avoidance system can be activated using the concept of

dirtiness. Curtis & Biran (2001) defined the most common elicitors of disgust among several

different cultures. They found that substances of animal origin, poor hygiene, and bodily secretions

are perceived as disgusting, activating the disease avoidance motive. Also material that is seen to be “contaminated” or dirty, such as a stained toilet, clothes, sheets, or teeth, or rubbish on a beach, are able to generate disgust (Curtis & Biran, 2001). Disgust arises due to the deviation from the “normal standard of hygiene” generated by the dirty images which unequivocally creates a sense of revulsion, which results in turn, in disgust (and not in fear). As a result, subjects perceive stimuli

which are contaminated by others, or dirty stimuli more generally as a disease threat, implementing

an avoidance defense system against them.

Consequently, I propose that the activation of the disease avoidance system – and thus the

disgust of contagion – through exposure to dirty stimuli could promote a less positive preference in

terms of attitude, price evaluation and choice for brand new products.

Therefore:

H2a: Disgust leads to less positive attitudes toward second hand products, and this relationship is

mediated by the activation of disease avoidance.

H2b: Disgust leads to less positive price evaluation of second hand products, and this relationship is

mediated by the activation of disease avoidance.

H2c: Disgust leads to packaged product choice, and this relationship is mediated by the activation

(15)

3. Method

In order to test my hypotheses, I conducted two different lab experiments. The first experiment

tested how fear resulting from the activation of the disease avoidance system influences customers

attitudes, purchase intentions and choice of healthy (vs. unhealthy) foods, whereas the second

experiment tested how disgust resulting from the activation of the disease avoidance system

influences customers attitudes, price evaluation and choice of brand new (vs. second hand)

products. The two experiments were conceptually identical in design, but they differed in (1) the

stimuli used for eliciting fear vs. disgust, (2) the stimuli that were rated and chosen as dependent

variables (DV) (i.e., foods vs. products), and (3) the use of some specific control variables.

Specifically, in Study 1, fear was elicited following the Ryan et al. (2012) procedure, using faces

disfigured by a wine stain. Then participants rated a (pre-tested) pool of healthy vs. unhealthy foods

in terms of attitude and purchase intentions. In Study 2, disgust was elicited through International

Affective Picture System (IAPS) images related to dirtiness, and then participants rated a pool of

brand new vs. second-hand products in terms of attitude and price evaluation.

3.1 Study 1

Study 1 aimed to test three hypotheses. The first hypothesis was that exposure to disfigured

faces could promote a more positive attitude toward healthy (vs. unhealthy) foods, and this

relationship is mediated by the activation of the disease avoidance system. The second hypothesis

was that exposure to disfigured faces could promote a more positive purchase intention toward

healthy (vs. unhealthy) foods, and this relationship is mediated by the activation of the disease

avoidance system. Finally, the third hypothesis was that the exposure to disfigured faces could

promote a more healthy food choice, and this relationship is mediated by the activation of the

disease avoidance system.

Participants. The original sample was composed of 50 participants however six of them (five

(16)

not understand the study’s instructions. Consequently, the statistical analyses for Study 1 are conducted on a sample of 44 undergraduate students (17 males and 27 females), who participated in exchange for 4 Euro’s. The average age of the sample is 22.25 years (SD = 2.45), with an average weight of 68,86 kg (SD = 21,15) and an average height of 172,66 cm (SD = 11,10). In addition, only

10 of the 44 participants were following a diet. Finally, the results of the debriefing procedure

revealed that none of the 44 participants included into the analyses recognized the real purpose of

the experiment.

The study used a 2 (exposure: disfigured faces vs. control faces) x 2 (product: healthy vs.

unhealthy foods) design, with the first factor manipulated between-subjects and the second factor

within-subjects. As a result, the presence of the disfigured faces was the independent variable (IV)

that is a dummy variable equal to 1 if participants were exposed to the images, and equal to zero

otherwise. Consumer attitude, purchase intentions and consumer choice toward healthy (vs.

unhealthy) foods were the DVs.

Procedure. On arrival at the lab participants were informed that they would participate in a

series of unrelated studies. The experimenter placed each participants in a separate room with a

computer that provided all further instructions. Participants in the experimental condition were

primed with the disfigured faces, whereas participants in the control condition were primed with the

same faces but without disfigurement (the exact procedure will be explained further in the next

section). After target (vs. control) faces exposure, the disease avoidance system was measured by

asking participants to complete a task related to illnesses following the Greenberg, Pyszczynski,

Solomon, & Simon (1994) procedure.

Then, participants answered the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) questions (α

= .91; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) in order to verify that participants in the experimental

condition were effectively scared by disfigured faces. After that a (fictitious) second study started

where participants were asked their attitudes toward a pool of healthy (10) and unhealthy (10) foods

(17)

= .70). This scale is composed of 15 items designed to assess either general beliefs about personal

susceptibility to infectious diseases or discomfort in situations in which the possibility of disease

transmission was salient (Duncan, Schaller & Park, 2009). After that, participants were asked about

their purchase intentions of healthy (vs. unhealthy) foods through a budget allocation task that will

be explained later.

Finally, in order to ascertain that nobody identified the real purpose of the study, participants

answered the “funneled debriefing” questions (Bargh & Chartrand, 2000) and then some

demographic questions (including gender, age and questions related to weight, stature and whether

participants were following a diet).

Subsequently, food choice was measured using the following procedure: participants were

informed that the study was completed and that in few minutes a research assistant would proceed

to pay them. Meanwhile they were offered, as a sort of additional reward, a choice between

chocolates (M&M’s) or grapes, contained in two separate transparent dispensers. The expectations

were that participants in the experimental condition would be more likely to choose grapes than

participants in the control condition. After the food choice the experimenter debriefed, paid and

thanked participants for their attendance.

Independent Variable: Exposure to disfigured faces. On the basis of Oaten et al.’s (2011)

paper, the IV was operationalized choosing six neutral faces (three of females and three of males)

each of which was modified adding a wine stain birthmark (each face is shown in the Appendix

section).

Participants in the experimental condition were exposed to the modified faces (from here on:

target faces), whereas participants in the control condition were exposed to the neutral faces (from

here on: control faces). Target (vs. control) faces were randomly shown using the following

procedure: participants were exposed to six screenshots, each showing six images, in which two of

them were identical. Each of the six screenshots contained one different target (vs. control) face

(18)

select the pair of identical (unrelated) images as quickly as possible. This task allowed participants

to have six exposures to the target vs. control faces. In order to avoid demand effect, the position of

the target (vs. control) faces and the position of the two identical unrelated images were changed for

each screenshot. Figure 1 shows one of the six screenshots used in the experimental condition.

Figure 1: One of the six screenshots used in the experimental condition

Dependent Variables.

Food Attitude was measured through three 7-point scales (where 1 = “Not at all/Very negative”

and 7 = “Very much/Very positive”). Participants were asked three questions about their attitude toward a pool of 20 (10 healthy and 10 unhealthy) pre-tested alternatives foods (“how much do you like …” “what is your attitude toward …”, “how desirable is …” ). In order to have two overall attitudinal scores, I first performed a reliability analysis for the three attitude items used for both

healthy (αHealt_Food = .89) and unhealthy food (αUnhealt_Food = .88) and then I constructed two overall

average attitudinal scores for healthy and unhealthy foods.

Food Purchase Intention was operationalized through an allocation budget task. Participants

in both conditions were informed that they had a fixed budget (i.e., 10 €) that had to be allocated

(19)

two healthy and two unhealthy (randomly selected from the pre-tested pool of healthy/unhealthy

foods). In particular, using a scenario, participants were asked for each of the five food combinations to allocate their budget (10 €) among the four foods, choosing the combination and the quantity that they liked most. Each food shown in the screenshots was assigned the same price;

in particular, the foods presented in three screenshots were priced at 1€, whereas the foods

presented in the remaining two screenshots were priced at 2 €. This allowed me to control for the

price sensitivity of participants.

The expectation was that participants in the experimental treatment preferred to allocate the

majority of their budget to healthy (vs. unhealthy) foods in comparison to the control group. Figure

2 shows one of the five screenshots used in the study.

Figure 2: One of the five budget allocation task used in Study 1

Food Choice was operationalized as following: before leaving the lab, participants were

allowed to choose (as a sort of additional reward) between chocolates (M&M’s) or grapes (black

and white) placed in two transparent containers. In particular, participants were informed that they

caould have a free snack as additional compensation for their attendance at the experiment. This

(20)

experimental treatment preferred grapes (proxy for healthy food) to chocolate (proxy for unhealthy

food) in comparison to the control group.

Mediator. Disease Avoidance Activation was operationalized following the Greenberg et al.

procedures (1994). Participants completed a set of 25 incomplete words by filling in one or more

missing letters. Participants could complete a part of these words as either neutral or illness-related

words, and the remaining words served as filler items (e.g., “Ill …” can be completed as “illness” or “illumed” or “…alad ...” can be completed as “malady” or “salads” etc.).

The Appendix section includes a complete list of all variables used in Experiment 1 plus the

results of the pre-test performed to choose the healthy vs. unhealthy foods pool.

3.2 Results and Discussion

As previously mentioned, the statistical analyses for Study 1 were conducted on a sample of 44

subjects that ensured the presence of at least 20 subjects for the experimental and control condition.

The results of the debriefing procedure revealed that none of the participants recognized that

they were exposed to disfigured faces, and therefore none recognized the real purpose of the

experiment. This enabled us to include all the 44 participants in the analyses.

Manipulation Check

In order to assess whether exposure to disfigured faces activated fear of disease, a series of

manipulation check-items using 7 point Likert statements (1 = Fully disagree, 7 = Fully agree)

(21)

(Faulkner, Schaller, Park, & Duncan, 2004). The analyses for “scary” mood showed that on average

the experimental and control conditions were statistically equivalent (MExp = 2.30 SD = 1.46, MContr

= 2.00 SD = 1.27; F(1,42) = .54, ns). This means that participants were not scared by disfigured

faces. Consequently, the manipulation was not successful. This represents a first weakness of the

study, because the experimental stimuli were not able to activate fear.

Conversely, the manipulation was successful with regard to “ashamed” and “guilty” moods at

5% and 10% respectively. In fact, participants in the experimental condition were more ashamed

(Mexp = 3.13, SD = 1.91; Mcontr = 2.10, SD = 1.45; F(1,42) = 4.04, p = .05) and guilty (Mexp = 2.17,

SD = 1.23; Mcontr = 1.52, SD = 1.12; (F(1,42) = 3.33, p = .07). This means that even though

participants were not scared, they were at least disturbed by the exposure to disfigured faces.

Apart from “guilty” and “ashamed” moods, analyses revealed that no other PANAS items

statistically affected the cognitive accessibility of disease-related words, food attitude, food

spending intention, or food choice.

PDV scale.

Analysis of variance on PDV items (F(1,44) = .72, ns) indicated that participants’ perceived

vulnerability to diseases was not affected by exposure to disfigured faces. Hence, personal

perceived vulnerability to diseases cannot account for the differences in cognitive accessibility of

disease-related words, food attitude, food spending intention, or food choice.

Disease Avoidance Activation.

An ANOVA on the number of completed disease-related words among the experimental (vs.

control) condition showed that on average the experimental and control conditions were statistically

equivalent (Mexp = 6.61, SD = 2.62, Mcontr = 7.14, SD = 2.41; F(1,42) = .49, ns). Consequently, there

was no difference between participants that were exposed to disfigured faces and participants that

(22)

Food Attitude.

In order to test the presence of a main effect between target faces exposure and healthy (vs.

unhealthy) food attitude, I conducted a 2 (exposure: target vs. control faces) x 2 (attitude: Healthy

vs. Unhealthy food) ANOVA, with the first factor manipulated between subjects and the second

factor within subjects.

In line with the predictions, the test of within-subjects effects gives a marginally significant

interaction effect between healthy food attitude and experimental (vs. control) condition (F(1,42) =

2.38, p =.06), indicating that participants exposed to disfigured faces evaluated healthy foods more

positively (MHealthyExp = 5.77, SD = .62) than participants in the control condition (MHealthyCont =

5.36, SD = .75). This implies that the exposure to faces disfigured by a wine stain birthmark was

able to influences food attitudes, but only for healthy food evaluation. In fact, the results show that

exposure to target faces did not significantly influenced unhealthy food attitude (MUnhealthyExp = 4.41,

SD = .71; MUnhealthyCont = 4.66, SD = .87; F(1,42) = 1.06, ns). Figure 3 shows the estimated marginal

means for both healthy and unhealthy food attitudes across the experimental and control condition.

From the graph it is possible to note that the difference of the mean levels between healthy and

unhealthy food attitudes is higher in the experimental condition than in the control condition. This

implies that, as expected, the exposure to disfigured faces generates a sense of discomfort that in

turn increases positive attitudes toward actions that reduce illness threat over time, promoting

therefore a long-term survival instinct, through a more positive attitude toward healthy (vs.

(23)

Figure 3: Estimated marginal means for both healthy and unhealthy food attitude across experimental and control conditions.

Consequently, although disfigured faces were not able to scare participants and to activate the

disease avoidance system, they were able to generate a sense of discomfort (embarrassment and

guilt) that generated a more positive attitude toward healthy foods.

These results allow me to state that exposure to disfigured faces leads to higher healthy food

attitudes.

Food Purchase Intentions.

With respect to the original sample of 44 participants, only 34 subjects completed the budget

allocation task (15 participants for the experimental condition and 19 for the control condition),

whereas the remaining 10 participants skipped the task not indicating any spending intentions. In

order to test the presence of a main effect between target images exposure and healthy (vs.

unhealthy) food spending intentions, I first aggregated all the spending intentions given for healthy

foods and all the spending intentions given for unhealthy foods, obtaining two overall spending

intention scores: one for healthy foods and another one for unhealthy foods.

(24)

After that a 2 (exposure: target vs. control faces) x 2 (spending intentions: healthy vs. unhealthy

food) ANOVA was performed, with the first factor manipulated between subjects and the second

factor within subjects.

Contrary to expectations, the interaction effect between the experimental (vs. control) condition

and healthy (vs. unhealthy) food spending intentions is not statistically significant (MHealthy_Food_EXP

= 26.20, SD = 4.66; MHealthy_Food_CONTR = 23.58, SD = 6.07; MUnhealthy_Food_EXP = 14.53, SD = 4.88;

MUnhealthy_Food_CONTR = 16.26, SD = 5.94; F(1,32) = 1.33, ns).

On the basis of these results it is possible to state that the exposure to disfigured faces does not

lead to greater healthy food purchase intentions.

Food Choice.

In order to test the presence of a main effect between target images exposure (IV) and food

choice (chocolates, proxy for unhealthy foods vs. grapes, proxy for healthy foods, DV), I performed

a Chi-Square Test. However, there is not statistically difference between the distribution of

chocolates vs. grapes in the experimental vs. control condition (chocolates_exp = 6; chocolates_contr =

10; grapes_exp = 17; grapes_contr = 11, χ2 = .21, ns). Therefore, it is not possible to conclude that the

exposure to the target images creates a greater tendency to choose healthy (vs. unhealthy) foods.

Mediation Analysis.

A mediation analysis (cf. Baron & Kenny, 1986; Zhao, Lynch & Chen, 2010) tested the

hypothesis that disease avoidance activation mediates the relation between IV (target faces

exposure) and the three different analyzed DVs (attitude, purchase intentions and choice of healthy

vs. unhealthy foods). However, since experimental condition did not significantly influence food

spending intentions, food choice (DVs) or disease avoidance activation, the mediation analysis will

(25)

Mediation Analysis – healthy food attitude (DV). The first regression analysis, with healthy

food attitude as the dependent variable and target faces exposure (dummy coded) as the predictor,

yielded a significant relation (β = -.30, p = .05). A second regression analysis, with the mediator

(disease avoidance activation) as the dependent variable and target faces exposure as the predictor,

showed that target faces exposure did not influence disease avoidance activation significantly (β =

.11, ns). Since there is not significant relationship between target faces exposure (predictor) and

disease avoidance activation (mediator) there is no reason to continue to test for the mediation

effect of the disease avoidance system on the relationship between target face exposure (IV) and

healthy food attitude (DV).

These findings do not support the hypothesis that disease avoidance can be induced by priming

with disfigured faces, and therefore mediate the relationship between target face exposure and

healthy food attitude.

Mediation Analysis – unhealthy food attitude (DV). Since there is not a significant

relationship between target faces exposure (predictor) and disease avoidance activation (mediator)

there is no reason to test for the mediation effect of the disease avoidance system on the relationship

between target faces exposure (IV) and unhealthy food attitude (DV). Consequently, disease

avoidance does not mediate the relationship between target faces exposure and unhealthy food

attitude.

On the basis of these results it is possible to conclude that H1 must be rejected, with the

exception of H1a that can be partially accepted. Consequently, faces disfigured by a wine stain

birthmark do not activate fear of illness and do not lead to healthy food preferences in terms of

purchase intentions and choice, but lead to healthy food preference in terms of attitude. However,

(26)

3.3 Study 2

Study 2 aimed to test three hypotheses. The first hypothesis was that the exposure to IAPS

target images could promote a less positive attitude toward second-hand (vs. brand new) products,

and this relationship is mediated by the activation of the disease avoidance system. The second

hypothesis was that the exposure to IAPS target images could promote a less positive price

evaluation of second hand (vs. brand new) products, and this relationship is mediated by the

activation of the disease avoidance system. Finally, the third hypothesis was that the exposure to

IAPS target images could promote a higher likelihood of choosing packaged products, and this

relationship is mediated by the activation of the disease avoidance system.

Participants. The original sample was composed of 55 participants, however one of them was

removed from the analyses because his answers revealed that he did not understand the study’s instructions. Consequently, the statistical analyses for Study 2 are conducted on a sample of 54

undergraduate students (26 female, 28 male; Mage = 22.15 years, SD = 3.06), who participated in

exchange for 4 Euro’s. The study used a 2 (exposure: dirty vs. control image) x 2 (product: brand new vs. second hand products) design, with the first factor manipulated between-subjects and the

second factor within-subjects. As a result, the presence of the dirty images was the IV that is a

dummy variable equal to 1 if participants were exposed to the images, and equal to zero otherwise.

Consumer attitude, price evaluation and choice of packaged (vs. unpackaged) products were the

DVs.

Procedure. On arrival at the lab participants were informed that they would participate in a

series of unrelated studies. The experimenter placed each participants in a separate room with a

computer that provided all further instructions. Using the same procedure illustrated for Study 1,

participants in the experimental condition were primed with the dirty images, whereas participants

(27)

exposure, the disease avoidance system was measured by asking participants to complete a task

related to illnesses following the Greenberg et al. (1994) procedure. Then, in order to assess their

mood and emotions, participants answered PANAS questions (α = .89; Watson et al. 1988). After

that, in order to verify that participants in the experimental condition were effectively disgusted by

IAPS target images, they answered the Brief Mood Introspection (BMIS) scale (α = .82; Mayer &

Gaschke, 1988) with the addition of two specific emotions related to nausea (i.e., disgust and

cloyed), and to four emotions (two positives and two negatives) provided by Izard (1977)

classification (that were not present in the PANAS and BMIS scales).

Next, a (fictitious) second study started where participants were asked about their attitudes

toward a pool of 10 brand new and 10 second hand products shown randomly. In order to control

for their perceived vulnerability to disease, participants completed the PDV scale (α = . 89; Duncan

et al. 2009). After that, they were asked to assign a reasonable price to each brand new and second

hand product previously shown in the attitudinal evaluation task. Then, price sensitivity of

participants (α = .69) was measured using the Lichtenstein, Netemeyer, & Burton (1990) scale.

After that, participants completed eight items of the disgust scale (α = .79) provided by Haidt,

McCauley & Rozin, 1994. Finally, in order to ascertain that nobody identified the real purpose of

the study, participants answered the “funneled debriefing” questions (Bargh & Chartrand, 2000) and

then demographic questions (gender and age). Next, product choice was measured using the

following procedure: participants were informed that the experiment was completed and that in few

minutes a research assistant would proceed to pay them. Meanwhile, they were offered free candies

as a sort of additional reward. However, participants could choose between packaged candies

(proxy for uncontaminated and therefore for a brand new product) or the same candies but

unpackaged (proxy for contaminated/touched by others and therefore for second hand product)

positioned in two separate dishes. The expectation was that participants in the experimental

(28)

condition. After the candy choice the experimenter debriefed, paid and thanked participants for their

attendance.

Independent Variable: Disgust. The IV was operationalized choosing 12 pictures from the

IAPS database, six that elicited dirtiness and six other unrelated images (each image is shown in the

Appendix). Participants in the experimental condition were randomly exposed to the dirty images

(from here on: target images), whereas participants in the control condition were randomly exposed

to the unrelated images (from here on: control images) using the same procedure previously

described for Study 1. Figure 4 shows one of the six screenshots used in the experimental

condition.

Figure 4: One of the six screenshots used in the experimental condition

Dependent Variables.

Product Attitude was measured through three 7-point scale questions (where 1 = “Not at

(29)

…”). In particular, for brand new products, 10 different products (four cooking utensils, two unisex accessories, two bedroom accessories, a bathrobe, and a towel set) were chosen and presented to

participants, whereas for second hand products the same 10 brand new products images were

presented but using an eBay framework (see Appendix for the complete list of the used images). In

order to have two overall attitudinal scores, I first performed a reliability analysis for the three attitude items used for brand new (αBrand_New = .89) and second hand products (αSecond_Hand = .89) and

then I constructed two overall average attitudinal scores for both brand new and second hand

products.

Using a scenario, participants were informed that they had to evaluate a series of products

furnished by a (fictitious) e-commerce web site that sells both brand new and second hand products,

and that they could recognize the second-hand products by the eBay framework presence.

Product Price Evaluation was operationalized by asking participants to give a reasonable price

to the same brand new (vs. second hand) products shown for the attitude measurement questions. In

particular, participants were informed that the (fictitious) e-commerce web site was interested in

knowing a possible reasonable price for their products, so participants were asked “What is in your

opinion a reasonable price (in Euros) for this product?” All the price evaluations given for brand

new products and all the price evaluations given for second hand products were then aggregated,

obtaining two overall price evaluation scores: one for brand new products, and one for second hand

products.

Again, participants were informed that the products with the eBay framework were second

hand products.

Product Choice was operationalized as following: before leaving the lab, participants were

offered a choice (as a sort of additional reward) between packaged or unpackaged candies placed in

two separate trays. In particular, participants were informed that they could have some free candies

(30)

participants in the experimental treatment would have a higher preference for packaged candies

(proxy for brand new product) than for unpackaged candies (proxy for contaminated/touched by

others, or second hand product) in comparison to participants in the control group.

Mediator: Disease Avoidance Activation was operationalized using the same procedure as

Experiment1. The Appendix section provides a complete list of all variables used in the Experiment

2 plus the brand new vs. second hand product stimuli.

3.4 Results and Discussion

As previously mentioned, the statistical analyses for Study 2 were conducted on a sample of 54

subjects, that ensured the presence of at least 20 subjects for the experimental and control

conditions.

The results of the debriefing procedure revealed that none of the participants recognized that

they were exposed to IAPS images, and therefore none recognized the real purpose of the

experiment. This enable us to include all 54 participants in the analyses.

Manipulation Check.

In order to assess whether target IAPS images exposure activated disgust of contamination, a

series of manipulation check-items using 7 point Likert statements (1 = Fully disagree, 7 = Fully

agree) were administered. More specifically, I used the two BMIS items (modified for the study purpose) “Disgusted” and “Cloyed” and the disgust scale mean. Then, I performed the analysis of variance with respect to the experimental (vs. control) condition.

The results revealed that on average there is not a statistical difference between the control and

experimental conditions with regard to the BMIS items of disgust (Mexp = 2.46, SD = 1.77; Mcontr =

1.96, SD = 1.37; F(1,52) = 1.34, ns) and cloyed (Mexp = 3.39, SD = 1.20, Mcontr = 3.42, SD = 1.03;

(31)

experimental conditions with regard to the disgust scale mean (Mexp = 4.37, SD = 1.20, Mcontr =

4.14, SD = .97; F(1,52) = .55, ns). This means that participants were not disgusted by target IAPS

images. Consequently, the manipulation was not successful, implying that participants in the

experimental condition were not significantly disgusted and disturbed by IAPS target images

exposure.

In addition, analyses revealed that none of the other BMIS items statistically affected the

cognitive accessibility of disease-related words, products attitudes, products price evaluations and

product choice.

PDV scale.

Analysis of variance on PDV items (F(1,52) = .35, ns) indicated that participants’ perceived

vulnerability to diseases was not affected by target images exposure. Hence, personal perceived

vulnerability to diseases cannot account for the differences in cognitive accessibility of

disease-related words, products attitudes, products price evaluations and product choice.

Price sensitivity scale.

In order to demonstrate that participants’ product evaluation (in terms of price) was not affected

by their price sensitivity, the analysis of variance on price sensitivity scale items with respect to the

price evaluation of brand new and second hand products was conducted. The results demonstrated

that both brand new product evaluations (F(1,52) = 1.06, ns) and second hand products evaluations

(F(1,52) = .94, ns) were not affected by respondents’ price sensitivity.

Mood.

Analysis of variance on the positive items of the PANAS (F(1,52) = .01, ns) and the negative

items of the PANAS (F(1,52) = .14, ns) indicated that participants’ mood states were not affected

(32)

accessibility of disease-related words, products attitudes, products price evaluations and product

choice.

Disease Avoidance Activation.

An ANOVA on the number of completed disease-related words in the experimental (vs.

control) condition showed that on average the experimental and control conditions were statistically

equivalent (Mexp = 6.96, SD = 2.01, Mcontr = 6.35, SD = 3.12; F(1,52) = .759, ns). Consequently,

there is no difference between participants who were exposed to target IAPS images and

participants who were exposed to control IAPS images in the disease avoidance activation.

Product Attitude.

In order to test the presence of a main effect between target IAPS images exposure and brand

new (vs. second hand) products attitude I conducted a 2 (IAPS images: target vs. control) x 2

(products attitude: brand new vs. second hand) ANOVA, with the first factor manipulated between

subjects and the second factors within subjects.

In line with the predictions, the test of within-subjects effects gives a significant interaction

effect of second hand product attitude at 5% of significance (F(1,52) = 7.11, p =.01), indicating that

participants exposed to IAPS target images evaluated second hand products less positively

(MSec_Hand_Exp = 3.06, SD = .75) than participants in the control condition (MSec_Hand_Cont = 3.47, SD =

.73). This implies that the exposure to IAPS images related to dirtiness is able to influences

products attitudes, but only for second hand product evaluation. In fact, the results show that IAPS

target images exposure did not significantly influence brand new product attitude (MBrand_New_Exp =

4.91, SD = .60; MBrand_New_Cont = 4.64, SD = .80; F(1,52) = 2.14, ns). Figure 5 shows the estimated

marginal means for both healthy and unhealthy food attitude across the experimental and control

conditions. From the graph it is possible to note that the difference of the mean levels between

brand new and second hand products attitude is higher in the experimental condition than in the

(33)

attitude toward products previously owned or touched by others is lower than to when they are

exposed to control images. The dirtiness elicited by target images is presumably transferred to

second hand products, which are perceived as contaminated and therefore less desirable.

Figure 5: Estimated marginal means for both healthy and unhealthy food attitude across the experimental and control conditions.

Consequently, although the IAPS target images were not able to disgust participants and to

activate the disease avoidance system, they were able to generate a less positive attitude toward

second hand products. This evidence suggests that the exposure to images related to dirtiness

generates a more negative attitude toward second hand products.

Product Price Evaluation.

In order to test the presence of a main effect between target IAPS images exposure and brand

new (vs. second hand) products evaluation in terms of price, I conducted a 2 (IAPS images: target

vs. control) x 2 (price evaluation: brand new vs. second hand products) ANOVA, with the first

factor manipulated between subjects and the second factor within subjects. Contrary to

(34)

expectations, the interaction effect between the experimental (vs. control) condition and second

hand products price evaluation is not statistically significant (MSecond_Hand_EXP = 6.03, SD = 3.26;

MSecond_Hand_CONTR = 7.95, SD = 3.62; MBrand_New_EXP = 17.86, SD = 8.70; MBrand_New_CONTR = 18.11,

SD = 6.50; F(1,52) = 1.079, ns).

On the basis of these results it is possible to state that the exposure to target IAPS images does

not lead to lower second hand product price evaluation.

Product Choice.

In order to test the presence of a main effect between target IAPS images exposure (IV) and

product choice (packaged candies, proxy for brand new product, vs. unpackaged candies, proxy for

second hand product; DV), I performed a Chi- Square Test. Only 48 participants accepted a candy

as an additional reward, while the remaining six refused it. The results revealed that there is no

statistically significant difference between the distribution of packaged vs. unpackaged candies in

the experimental vs. control condition (packaged_exp = 12; packaged_contr = 16; unpackaged_exp= 12;

unpackaged_contr = 8, χ

2

= 1.37, ns). Therefore, it is again not possible to conclude that the exposure

to the target IAPS images creates a greater tendency to choose packaged vs. unpackaged products.

Mediation Analysis.

A mediation analysis (cf. Baron & Kenny, 1986; Zhao et al. 2010) tested the hypothesis that

disease avoidance activation mediates the relation between IV (IAPS target images exposure) and

the three different analyzed DVs (attitude, price evaluation and choice of brand new vs. second

hand products). However, since experimental condition did not significantly influence product price

evaluation, product choice (DVs) and disease avoidance activation, the mediation analysis will be

presented only for second hand and brand new products attitude DVs.

Mediation Analysis – second hand products attitude (DV). The first regression analysis, with

(35)

coded) as the predictor, yielded a significant relation at 5% of significance (β = 1.92, p = .04). A

second regression analysis, with the mediator (disease avoidance activation) as the dependent

variable and target IAPS images exposure as the predictor, showed that target images exposure did

not influence disease avoidance activation significantly (β = -.12, ns). Since there is not a

significant relationship between target images exposure (predictor) and disease avoidance activation

(mediator) there is no reason to continue to test for the mediation effect of disease avoidance system

on the relationship between target images exposure (IV) and second hand products attitude (DV).

These findings do not support the hypothesis that disease avoidance can be induced by priming

with IAPS images, and therefore mediate the relationship between IAPS target images exposure and

second hand products attitude.

Mediation Analysis – brand new products attitude (DV). Since there is not a significant

relationship between target images exposure (predictor) and disease avoidance activation (mediator)

there is no reason to test for the mediation effect of the disease avoidance system on the relationship

between target images exposure (IV) and brand new products attitude (DV). Consequently, disease

avoidance does not mediate the relationship between target faces exposure and unhealthy food

attitude.

These findings do not support the hypothesis that disease avoidance can be induced by priming

IAPS target images, and therefore mediate the relationship between IAPS target images exposure

and brand new products attitude.

In sum, the results of the two experimental studies show that the exposure to disfigured faces

generates a more positive attitude toward healthy foods, and that exposure to IAPS images related

to dirtiness generates a less positive attitude toward second hand products. A more detailed

(36)

Table 1 Results Summary

HYPOTHESIS DESCRIPTION GRAPHICAL DESCRIPTION RESULT

H1a

H1a: Fear of illness leads to more

positive healthy food attitudes, and this relationship is mediated by the activation of disease avoidance.

Interaction Effect significant; Mediation ns

H1b

H1b: Fear of illness leads to more

positive healthy food purchase intentions, and this relationship is mediated by the activation of disease

avoidance.

Rejected

H1c

H1c: Fear of illness leads to healthy food

choice, and this relationship is mediated by the activation of disease avoidance.

Rejected

H2a

H2a: Disgust leads to less positive

attitudes toward second hand products, and this relationship is mediated by the

activation of disease avoidance.

Interaction Effect significant; Mediation ns H2b .

H2b: Disgust leads to less positive price

evaluation of second hand products,and this relationship is mediated by the

activation of disease avoidance.

Rejected

H2c

H2c: Disgust leads to packaged product

choice, and this relationship is mediated by the activation of disease avoidance.

Rejected

Fear illness Healthy

Food Att +

Dis.Avoidance

Fear illness Healthy

Food Choic +

Dis.Avoidance

+

Fear illness Healthy Food

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

based on clinical Symptoms has been used because occasional patients with factor VIII or factor IX level < l % exhibit little or no spontaneous bleeding and appear to be

In order to explam the different venous thrombotic nsks in first-degree relatives of probands of selected thrombophilic families and first-degree relatives of unselected patients

These results of the Northwick Park Heart Study indicate that in the one third of the population with the highest factor VIII levels, the risk of the development of coronary

This natural and direct approach enables the children to develop their own way to play with the digital images as something “natural” and as part of their everyday world.

Hence, most studies tend to use information on industrial Input-Output (IO) relationships at the country level and identify effects that cannot be attributed in

As the state-of-the-art review showed, there is a lack of utilities that help to analyse energy demands in order to obtain state related energy demands for machine tools while being

‹$JURWHFKQRORJ\DQG)RRG6FLHQFHV*URXS/LGYDQ:DJHQLQJHQ85 

Studying implementation fidelity of OHL-interventions, their moderators, including barriers and facilitators affecting implementation, and long-term outcomes, are