• No results found

An examination of the moderating role of core self-evaluation in the relationship between psychological contract breach and job satisfaction and performance.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "An examination of the moderating role of core self-evaluation in the relationship between psychological contract breach and job satisfaction and performance. "

Copied!
35
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

CAN EMPLOYEES WITH HIGH CORE SELF-EVALUATION BETTER DEAL WITH PSYCHOLOGICAL CONTRACT BREACH?

An examination of the moderating role of core self-evaluation in the relationship between psychological contract breach and job satisfaction and performance.

Master’s thesis

MSc Human Resource Management

University of Groningen, Faculty of Economics and Business

June 2015

KAREN VAN DER MEULEN Student number: 1884077

Kleine Beer 64 9742 RJ Groningen tel.: +31 (0)644093000

e-mail: k.m.van.der.meulen@student.rug.nl

Supervisor: prof. dr. O. Janssen

(2)

2 ABSTRACT

This research examines the possible mediating role of intrinsic work motivation on the relationship between psychological contract breach (PCB) and job satisfaction and job performance. Core self- evaluation is examined as a possible moderating variable on the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance. Direct effects of PCB on intrinsic work motivation, and intrinsic work motivation on job satisfaction and job performance are hypothesized. Moreover, this research investigates whether intrinsic work motivation mediates the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and performance. Finally, the hypothesis that core self-evaluation moderates the negative indirect relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and performance through intrinsic work motivation was examined. Results were obtained through an online survey, filled out by employees and their supervisors. Results showed evidence of a negative relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation and a positive relationship between intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction, a mediating role of intrinsic work motivation in the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction, and no moderating role of CSE in the indirect relationships between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance. This study contributes to the theoretical and practical knowledge of PCB, intrinsic work motivation and job outcomes. Furthermore, results of this study could help managers get a better understanding of the process of PCB and its consequences for their employees within an organization.

Key words: psychological contract breach, intrinsic work motivation, affective events theory, job

satisfaction, job performance, core self-evaluation.

(3)

3

INTRODUCTION

Within an employment relationship many situations, including negative ones, can occur. When an employee perceives that his or her employer does not fulfill his obligations or promises, this is called a psychological contract breach (Rousseau, 1989). An employee’s perception of a psychological contract breach (PCB) can have many negative consequences for core job outcomes. Earlier literature has shown that perceiving a PCB is negatively related to job satisfaction (Gakovic & Tetrick, 2003) and job performance (Turnley & Feldman, 1999; Lester, Turnley, Bloodgood, & Bolino, 2002).

Another consequence of perceiving a PCB may be that an employee’s intrinsic work motivation might decrease.

The central issue of this study, used as the independent variable, is the phenomenon of

psychological contract breach. A psychological contract is explained as an employee’s perception of

what they owe to their employers and what their employers owe to them (Robinson, 1996). The breach

of a psychological contract is a subjective experience, and as mentioned before, is defined as one’s

perception that the employer or organization has failed to fulfill the promised obligations of the

psychological contract (Rousseau, 1989). In recent decades, many research has been conducted on the

phenomenon of PCB. So far, interesting relationships have become evident between PCB and different

(job) outcomes. As explained, PCB has a negative influence on job satisfaction (Gakovic & Tetrick,

2003) and job performance (Turnley & Feldman, 1999; Lester et al., 2002). However, it is yet unclear

which mechanism(s) may explain these negative relationships between PCB and job satisfaction and

performance. It might be that this relationship is mediated through a construct such as intrinsic work

motivation. There is evidence that employees may lose trust in their organization when PCB occurs

(Bal, De Lange, Jansen, & Van der Velde, 2008), and it is assumed that PCB will elicit negative

affective reactions within employees, such as feelings of anger and betrayal (Robinson & Morrison,

2000). This evidence is in line with Weiss’ and Cropanzano’s affective events theory (1996), which

explains that a negative event within a work environment will cause negative emotional reactions,

leading to a decrease of intrinsic work motivation over time (Morrison & Robinson, 1997; Zhao,

Wayne, Glibkowski, & Bravo, 2007). Moreover, Beal and Ghandour (2011) studied affective

dynamics at work, looking at intrinsic work motivation as a positive affective event. They found that

(4)

4

intrinsic work motivation was positively related to daily positive affect and negatively related to daily negative affect. These findings seem to relate to the current research model, as PCB is related to negative affect, and thus might suggest that PCB negatively influences intrinsic work motivation.

Furthermore, PCB has been found to cause potentially valuable employees to reduce their contributions to their organizations (Robinson, Kraatz, & Rousseau, 1994). Concerning the construct of intrinsic work motivation, research has shown that it is positively related to job satisfaction and performance (Grant, 2008; Agho, Mueller, & Price, 1993). Thus, the higher levels of intrinsic work motivation employees have, the more satisfied they are with their job and the higher their job performance becomes, and vice versa. These findings show that the second half of the potential mediating effect is already evident. Based on the affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), in this study PCB is seen as a negative event experienced by employees at work. As mentioned, earlier research suggests that intrinsic work motivation might be a missing link in explaining the negative relationship between PCB and job outcomes (Morrison & Robinson, 1997; Zhao et al., 2007; Grant, 2008; Agho et al., 1993; Beal & Ghandour, 2011). Since little is yet known about the explanatory mechanisms of the influence of PCB on job outcomes, this study is conducted to gain more insight in the potential mediating effect of intrinsic work motivation in the negative relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance. It is suggested that employees who perceive a PCB, will have lower levels of intrinsic work motivation, which in turn will negatively influence their job satisfaction and performance.

By now it is evident that PCB has a negative influence on job satisfaction and job performance. However, probably not all employees will have the same motivational reaction to PCB.

In addition to this negative relationship, there could be a potential boundary condition in the form of employees’ core self-evaluations. Core self-evaluations are defined as the “fundamental premises that individuals hold about themselves and their function in the world” (Judge, Erez, & Bono, 1998).

Individuals with high levels of core self-evaluation (CSE) are more likely to think positively about

their worth, competence and capabilities (Zhang, Kwan, Zhang, & Wu, 2012). The study by Zhang et

al. (2012) showed that CSE had a moderating effect on the relationship between abusive supervision

and intrinsic motivation. Since a negative event such as PCB might be similar in comparison to

(5)

5

abusive supervision, it is assumed that CSE has a potential moderating effect on the relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation, which in turn will influence job satisfaction and performance. It is suggested that employees who have higher levels of CSE are less affected by PCB and therefore their intrinsic work motivation might not decrease as much as it would for employees with low levels of CSE. In turn, the assumption is that employees with high levels of CSE will be less affected by PCB in terms of their job satisfaction and job performance than employees with low levels of CSE.

This study contributes to the theoretical and empirical knowledge of psychological contract

breach, intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction and job performance. As mentioned earlier, PCB

is a construct that has been investigated frequently. However, mostly single relationships were tested

between PCB and variables such as job satisfaction and performance. What has not been investigated

is the potential mediating effect of intrinsic work motivation between PCB and job satisfaction and

performance. Furthermore, no study has yet been conducted on the extent to which employees differ in

their reactions to PCB influenced by their level of core self-evaluation. It is likely that employees with

different levels of CSE will have different outcomes on job satisfaction and job performance as a

result of PCB. It is important to investigate this relationship, since this knowledge could help

managers gain a better understanding of the process of PCB and its consequences for different types of

employees within their organization. The current study examines the relationship between a perceived

psychological contract breach and job satisfaction and job performance, with intrinsic work motivation

as a mediator and core self-evaluation as a moderator. Since it is known that PCB can have a negative

effect on employees’ job satisfaction and job performance, results of this research can make

contributions to the understanding of this problem and create insights into possible solutions to this

problem.

(6)

6 Psychological

contract breach

THEORY AND HYPOTHESES

In this section, the theoretical background for the current study will be discussed. Figure 1 provides a visual representation of the conceptual model.

FIGURE 1. Conceptual model

Psychological contract breach in relation to job satisfaction and job performance

The psychological contract was first mentioned in academic literature in 1962 by Levinson, Price, Munden, Mandl, and Solley. With their research and later studies of Schein (1965) and Rousseau (1989,1995) the psychological contract theory was developed as a framework for understanding the relationship between employers and employees within organizations. In previous research, different definitions of the psychological contract are applied. This study employs the psychological contract by the definition of Robinson (1996), stating that the psychological contract is an employee’s perception of what they owe to their employers and what their employers owe to them. This psychological contract theory states that if an employee perceives that his or her employer did not fulfill his or her obligations, the employee is likely to react with feelings of anger, frustration, and betrayal (Robinson

& Morrison, 2000). The breach of a psychological contract is thus a subjective experience. It is an

“employee’s belief that a breach has occurred that affects his or her behavior and attitudes, regardless of whether that belief is valid or whether an actual breach took place” (Robinson, 1996).

Employees within an organization find it important that they like their job and that they have a good relationship with their employer (Rode, 2004). Job satisfaction is often described as a function of

Job satisfaction Intrinsic work

motivation Core self-

evaluation

Job performance

(7)

7

the perceived relationship between what an employee wants from his job and what the employee perceives it as offering (Locke, 1969), and is defined as the degree to which individuals like their job (Price & Mueller, 1986). Looking at the definition of job satisfaction by Locke (1969), it appears logical that a difference between what an employee wants from his job and what the employee receives, will lead to feelings of dissatisfaction (Zhao et al., 2007). One main study that investigated the eventual relationship between PCB and job satisfaction was the study of Gakovic and Tetrick (2003). They found that PCB, thus the failure of the organization to fulfill obligations, was associated with emotional exhaustion and job dissatisfaction for the employees. Zhao et al. (2007) developed a clear overview on this subject through their meta-analysis of a large number of studies on PCB, in which they described that PCB is negatively related to job satisfaction. The same result also became evident by the study of Suazo (2009). Thus when an employee experiences PCB, his or her job satisfaction will likely decrease.

The second dependent variable in this model is job performance. This variable has also been researched in combination with PCB (Zhao, Wayne, Glibkowski, & Bravo, 2007). However, in most studies the variable job performance was a small part of a larger research with multiple measured outcomes (Turnley, Bolino, Lester, & Bloodgood, 2003; Turnley & Feldman, 1999; Lester et al., 2002). Research by Robinson (1996) suggested that PCB is negatively related to the extent to which employees report performing the duties prescribed as part of their job. Turnley and Feldman (1999) found evidence that PCB was related to the extent to which employees neglected their in-role duties.

Following this, it appears logical that PCB has a negative influence on employee’s job performance.

Research has shown that job performance indeed typically decreases after experiencing PCB (Turnley

& Feldman, 1999; Lester et al., 2002; Suazo, 2009). However, this statement is not always fully confirmed. In the study of Turnley et al. (2003) the data provided limited support for the hypothesis that employees reduce their work effort after perceiving a PCB. Their results suggest that psychological contract fulfillment with regard to the employment relationship is more strongly related to employee job performance than psychological contract fulfillment is with regard to pay.

Nevertheless, looking at the other studies that have been mentioned, it appears to be clear there is a

relationship between perceived psychological contract breach and job performance. In the available

(8)

8

research on this relationship, the data on job performance has mostly been collected through reports by employees within an organization as well as through reports of their supervisors (Turnley et al., 2002;

Lester et al., 2002). Therefore, this study will also collect the reports of job performance by employees’ direct supervisors.

Though the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance might seem obvious, there seems to be one overall explanation missing for the lower job satisfaction and performance when PCB is perceived. The theory that might explain this relationship is the earlier mentioned affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). According to this theory, the experience of a positive or negative work event, such as PCB, can elicit affective reactions (such as anger) that contribute to the formation of work attitudes and behaviors (Mignonac & Herrbach, 2004;

Rupp & Spencer, 2006). Work attitudes are employees’ evaluation of the employer and the work in general (Zhao et al., 2007). Thus, looking at work attitudes, the affective events theory (AET) might explain why PCB is related to lower job satisfaction. Considering work behaviors, AET might explain the negative relationship between PCB and job performance. Furthermore, it might be that these relationships are mediated by a construct such as intrinsic work motivation, which has not yet been investigated. It is known that PCB has a big influence on emotions, as explained by AET (Weiss &

Cropanzano, 1996), which are more short term aspects. Besides influencing emotions, it might also influence intrinsic work motivation, which can be seen as a longer term aspect. This study is thus more interested in the effects of PCB on longer term variables. The construct of intrinsic work motivation is explained below.

Intrinsic work motivation as a mediating process

Employees’ perception of a PCB has a negative effect on job satisfaction (Gakovic & Tetrick, 2003)

as well as on job performance of employees (Turnley & Feldman, 1999; Lester et al., 2002). However,

this evidence is not assumed to explain everything about this relationship. Since many relationships

with PCB have been investigated, there is ample knowledge on different constructs that have a linkage

to PCB. Thus, to gain more insight into the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and

performance, it might be useful to examine whether there is a construct that can function as an

(9)

9

explaining link between these three variables. Looking at earlier research on the affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), intrinsic motivation has also been part of the research on AET.

Beal and Ghandour (2011) have studied affective dynamics at work, in which they looked at intrinsic task motivation as a positive affective event. Results showed that intrinsic work motivation was positively related to daily positive affect and negatively related to daily negative affect. These findings seem to relate to the current research model, as PCB is related to negative affect, and PCB has a negative effect on job satisfaction and performance. Therefore, this study investigates the relationships between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance from an intrinsic motivational perspective.

Intrinsic motivation describes the reasons why people truly want to engage in goal-relevant behavior, in contrast to feeling that they have to (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Intrinsic work motivation in turn is defined as “the degree to which a person wants to work well in his or her job, in order to achieve intrinsic satisfaction” (Warr, Cook, & Wall, 1979: 135). In other words, it is the feeling that comes from within when a person truly likes doing his or her job, to achieve intrinsic satisfaction. The construct of intrinsic work motivation has rarely been investigated as an outcome variable in relation to PCB. However, other research suggests PCB and intrinsic work motivation do relate to each other.

When employees experience PCB, they may lose trust in their employer or organization (Bal et al., 2008). In addition, the experience of PCB is generally assumed to elicit negative reactions in employees, such as feelings of anger and betrayal (Robinson & Morrison, 2000). This evidence is in line with the already mentioned affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), which explains that a negative event within a work environment will cause negative emotional reactions. Due to these negative emotional reactions, employees achieve less intrinsic satisfaction through their jobs, leading to a decrease of intrinsic work motivation over time (Morrison & Robinson, 1997; Zhao et al., 2007).

In turn, intrinsic work motivation may influence the outcome variables job satisfaction and job

performance. No studies that solely tested the possible relationship between intrinsic work motivation

and job performance have been found. Nevertheless, this research suggests that these relationships

exist and that it is important to gain more information on these possible relationships. For example,

Herzberg (1966) and McGregor (1960) proposed that work can be inherently interesting and

enjoyable. According to Gagné and Deci (2005), intrinsic motivation refers to the desire to make an

(10)

10

effort based on interest in and enjoyment of the work itself. This research assumes that when an employee has high levels of intrinsic work motivation, he or she will put more effort in the job because he or she truly likes to do the job, which will then probably lead to a better job performance.

Also, since Warr et al. (1979) stated that intrinsic work motivation is the degree to which a person wants to work well in his or her job in order to achieve intrinsic satisfaction, it appears logical that when an employee has a high level of intrinsic work motivation, he or she will likely be more satisfied with his or her job.

Unfortunately, there is no hard evidence so far on the relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation. De Lange, Bal, Van der Heijden, De Jong and Schaufeli (2011) were the first to examine the relationship between PCB and work motivation over time, with age as a moderator. As opposed to what the researchers expected, they could not support their hypothesis that experienced PCB has a negative effect on intrinsic work motivation over time. However, looking at the earlier research mentioned, this research suggests that PCB and intrinsic work motivation are related. De Lange et al. (2011) examined work motivation across time, but this study examines intrinsic work motivation over a short time period. It is suggested that for this short timeframe PCB will have a negative influence on intrinsic work motivation, as explained earlier. Furthermore, some research was conducted on the moderating role of intrinsic motivation on the relationship between prosocial motivation and performance productivity (Grant, 2008). Support was found for this hypothesis, which is why it is assumed that the variables intrinsic work motivation and job performance are related.

Finally, there is ample literature on the relationship between intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction. This relationship is mostly examined in large studies with multiple measured outcomes, for example in the study of Agho, Mueller and Price (1993). They found significant positive effects on job satisfaction by distributive justice, positive affectivity and intrinsic work motivation. Thus if work motivation is low, job satisfaction will be low as well.

As explained before, a thorough examination of the literature has not resulted in finding

previous research on this exact combination of variables. This study investigates the relationship

between PCB and job performance and satisfaction from an intrinsic motivational perspective, since it

is assumed that intrinsic work motivation is the missing mediating link between these constructs. AET

(11)

11

is assumed to explain why the current variables might be related. Since PCB can be seen as a negative event, it is supposed to lead to a lower intrinsic work motivation. Intrinsic motivation can also be seen as positive affect, according to Beal and Ghandour (2011), which could explain why it has a positive influence on job satisfaction and performance. It is therefore suggested that when employees experience PCB, their intrinsic work motivation will decrease, which will in turn decrease their job satisfaction and performance.

Thus, the hypotheses are

Hypothesis 1: Psychological contract breach will be negatively related to intrinsic work motivation

Hypothesis 2a: Intrinsic work motivation will be positively related to job satisfaction Hypothesis 2b: Intrinsic work motivation will be positively related to job performance Hypothesis 3a: The relationship between experienced psychological contract breach and job satisfaction will be mediated by reduced intrinsic work motivation

Hypothesis 3b: The relationship between experienced psychological contract breach and job performance will be mediated by reduced intrinsic work motivation

Core self-evaluation

Up to this point the relationship between PCB, intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction and job

performance has been thoroughly explained. However, the main question in this study is whether

individual employees differ in their motivational reactions to PCB, which in turn would then

differently influence their levels of job satisfaction and job performance. With the knowledge that

PCB has a negative influence on job satisfaction (Gakovic & Tetrick, 2003; Zhao et al., 2007; Suazo,

2009), as well as on job performance (Turnley & Feldman, 1999; Lester et al., 2002; Suazo, 2009), the

question might rise whether these outcomes will be the same for all employees. The affective events

theory of Weiss and Cropanzano (1996) predicts that personality has a substantial impact on which

affective states are experienced and on how people typically feel at home and at work (Wegge, Van

Dick, Fisher, West & Dawson, 2006). From research on emotions it is evident that discrete emotions

such as anger or anxiety are triggered by specific events, and the consequences of these specific

(12)

12

emotions can differ substantially (Payne & Cooper, 2001). AET does not specify which work environments or work events might be associated with positive or negative reactions, but many clues are available in earlier research (Wegge et al., 2006), some of which will be explained below. Looking at the mentioned AET-studies, AET could explain why some people react differently to certain events such as PCB. It is therefore proposed there might be an individual construct that accounts for a difference in employees’ outcomes as a reaction to PCB.

The construct of core self-evaluations is such an individual construct. Core self-evaluations (CSE) are defined as the “fundamental premises that individuals hold about themselves and their function in the world” (Judge, Erez, & Bono, 1998). According to Judge, Locke and Durham (1997) the four traits self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, emotional stability, and locus of control are identified as CSE traits. The construct of CSE has been a popular subject for research in the last decades, as is shown by Chang, Ferris, Johnson, Rosen and Tan’s review (2012). Many direct effects of CSE have been researched in combination with different variables, such as job satisfaction and motivation. CSE has a strong, positive relationship with both job and life satisfaction (Chang et al., 2012). Furthermore, there is a positive relationship between CSE and intrinsic motivation. This suggests that individuals with high CSE are more likely to be autonomously motivated (Gagné &

Deci, 2005). The study of Zhang, Kwan, Zhang and Wu (2012) demonstrated that CSE had a

moderating effect on the relationship between abusive supervision and intrinsic motivation. Since PCB

is also a negative event similarly to abusive supervision, it is assumed that CSE might have a

moderating effect on the relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation. People with high

CSE are motivated to actively and effectively set and attain high goals and are likely to show high

levels of job motivation (Erez & Judge, 2001). Additionally, CSE encourages employees to perceive

their jobs from a positive perspective, thus leading them to focus on the desirable characteristics of

their jobs (Harris, Harvey, & Kacmar, 2009). Thus, employees with high CSE are less likely to lose

their enjoyment with and focus on their jobs following a negative event (Zhang et al., 2012). For the

current study, this could mean that high-CSE employees will still try to achieve intrinsic satisfaction

through the enjoyment of their job after experiencing PCB, thus not lowering their intrinsic work

motivation. Employees with low CSE will show a low degree of self-esteem and confidence and less

(13)

13

effective coping behavior when facing hardship (Kammeyer-Mueller, Judge, & Scott, 2009). Thus, after experiencing PCB low-CSE employees might find it hard to still achieve intrinsic satisfaction by doing their job, and have a lowered level of intrinsic work motivation. This study therefore suggests that employees with high levels of CSE, who thus have higher self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, emotional stability and locus of control, will be affected less by the experience of PCB, and thus their intrinsic work motivation will not decrease as much as for the employees with low CSE.

To this research’ best understanding, CSE has not yet been tested as a moderator in the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and performance. However, as explained above, it is assumed that there is a difference in intrinsic work motivation as a reaction to PCB between individuals with a high versus a low CSE, based on AET. Thus, the hypothesis is that

Hypothesis 4: Core self-evaluation will moderate the negative relationship between experienced psychological contract breach and intrinsic work motivation, such that this relationship will be weaker for employees with a high rather than a low core self-evaluation.

Mediated moderation model

The overall model tested in this study is a mediated moderation model (Muller, Judd & Yzerbyt, 2005;

Morgan-Lopez & MacKinnon, 2006). For this study, the relationship between PCB and decreased job satisfaction and job performance is suggested to be explained by decreased intrinsic work motivation.

Additionally, intrinsic motivation will be higher in reaction to PCB for employees with a high level of CSE. Thus, the study predicts that the score on CSE affects the influence that experienced PCB has on intrinsic work motivation. To conclude, this signifies that the indirect effect of PCB on job satisfaction and performance through intrinsic work motivation, is moderated by the employee’s level of CSE.

Therefore, the overall hypotheses of this study model are

Hypothesis 5a: Core self-evaluation moderates the negative indirect relationship between psychological contract breach and job satisfaction through intrinsic work motivation, so that this indirect relationship will be weaker for employees with a high rather

than a low core self-evaluation.

Hypothesis 5b: Core self-evaluation moderates the negative indirect relationship between

(14)

14

psychological contract breach and job performance through intrinsic work motivation, so that this indirect relationship will be weaker for employees with a high rather

than a low core self-evaluation.

METHOD Participants

Participants were 84 employees (27 males) from different organizations. The individuals have been acquired through the social network of the researcher. During the data collection, nine responses were deleted from the study, because they were not completed and no names were filled out, which were necessary to process the results. The 84 respondents who completed the study are Dutch employees with age ranging from 16 to 60. The mean age of the participants was 35.67 (SD= 11.05; fourteen respondents did not report their age). Furthermore, most participants finished an intermediate vocational education (27.4%), or higher professional education (57.1 %). The researcher has tried to acquire a sample which forms a reflection of the Dutch employee population. The companies or teams within which the employees are working are very diverse, ranging from a call center to an intermediate vocational education school.

Procedures

All English items were translated to Dutch, since the population for this study consists of Dutch employees. Two online surveys have been created in the online survey program Qualtrics. The two surveys were designed to acquire multisource data. The dependent variable job performance was measured by the ratings of the employees themselves, as well as the ratings of their direct supervisors.

For this reason, data was collected by approaching work teams to participate in this study, which made

the data processing much more feasible. The main survey was filled out by 84 employees, the other

survey on job performance was filled out by their 15 team leaders. Each measured variable was

represented by several matching items that have been tested in earlier research. Below all measures are

explained.

(15)

15 Measures

In this section the used measures of the surveys are explained in chronological order. First an informed consent was shown. After the respondents agreed to participate, the actual study started. All parts of the survey were measured on a Likert-scale, unless indicated otherwise.

Psychological contract breach was measured by five items from Robinson and Morrison (2000). An example was: “I have not received everything promised to me in exchange for my contributions”. PCB was measured on a five-point scale (1= not at all, to 5= to a very great extent; α = .87).

Job performance was measured by the seven-item in-role performance scale by Williams and Anderson (1991) on a five-point scale (1= strongly disagree, to 5= strongly agree; α = .92). An example was: “I adequately complete all of my assigned duties”. This scale was also used in the research on PCB by Lester et al. (2002).

Job satisfaction was measured with five items on a seven-point scale from Bono & Judge (2003) (1= strongly disagree, to 7= strongly agree). An example being: “I feel fairly satisfied with my present job” (α = .81).

Intrinsic work motivation was measured with a twelve-item scale developed by Van Yperen (2003), which is also used in the study of De Lange et al. (2011) on PCB and work motivation. An example was: “I work for the pleasure I feel while learning new things in my job”. These items consist of a seven-point scale (1= strongly disagree, to 7= strongly agree; α = .90).

Core self-evaluation was measured with twelve items by the five point-scale of Judge et al.

(2003), which is also used in the research of Zhang et al (2012) on CSE and intrinsic motivation (1=

strongly disagree, to 5= strongly agree; α = .81). An example being: “I am capable of coping with most of my problems”.

Demographical items were presented at the end of both surveys. Employees and supervisors

were asked the same following questions; “What is your age?”, “What is your gender?”, and “What is

your highest level of education?” Educational level was measured on a scale from 1 (lower secondary

school) to 8 (master’s degree). To finish the survey, participants were obligated to fill in their names,

(16)

16

since without this answer the results would be useless. The employee’s survey results were connected to the team leader’s survey results to measure the dependent variable job performance.

All survey items can be found in the Appendix of this study.

Data analysis

First, the reliability of the items of the scales of the four conceptual variables was tested by calculating the Cronbach’s Alpha. If α was above .70, the measurement scales were marked as reliable and mean scores for the variables were computed. Then, means, standard deviations and zero-order correlations for all tested variables were computed. Only control variables that significantly correlated with the dependent variable (i.e. job satisfaction and/or job performance) were incorporated in further analyses, as recommended by Becker (2005).

To test hypotheses 1 and 2a and 2b, regarding the relationships between psychological contract breach and intrinsic work motivation, and intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction and job performance, a linear regression analysis was conducted. To test hypothesis 3a and 3b, a mediation analysis was conducted by using model 4 of A.F. Hayes’ interactive PROCESS tool for SPSS (2013).

Model 1 of the same PROCESS tool was used to test the moderation effect of core self-evaluation on the relation between psychological contract breach and intrinsic work motivation (hypothesis 4), and model 7 was used to test hypothesis 5a and 5b which predicted a moderated mediation. Prior to analyses, all predictor variables were standardized.

RESULTS

Descriptive statistics

Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations are presented in Table 1. Regarding the

demographic variables, age was slightly positively correlated with job satisfaction (r = .24, p < .05)

and intrinsic work motivation (r = .24, p < .05). Educational level was slightly negatively correlated

with intrinsic work motivation (r = -.23, p < .05). Thus, only the control variable age was correlated

(17)

17

with one of the dependent variables, job satisfaction. It could be that the older an employee, the more satisfied he or she will be with the job (Thielgen, Krumm, Rauschenbach, & Hertel, 2015). Therefore, age was incorporated in further analyses.

Hypotheses testing

Hypotheses 1, 2a, and 2b were tested by regression analysis. Results are shown in table 2. Hypothesis 1 predicted that psychological contract breach is negatively related to intrinsic work motivation.

Results of the regression analysis support this hypothesis (B= -.23, p < 0.05). Hypothesis 2a predicted that intrinsic work motivation is positively related to job satisfaction. Test results confirm a significant relationship between intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction (B= .41, p < 0.001). Hypothesis 2b predicted that intrinsic work motivation is positively related to job performance. This relationship cannot be confirmed by the results (B= .09, p > 0.05).

Hypotheses 3a, 3b, 4, 5a and 5b were tested respectively by the use of model 4, 1, and 7 of

A.F. Hayes’ interactive PROCESS tool (available from www.afhayes.com). Results are presented in

table 2 and 3. The mediation test confirmed the indirect relationship between psychological contract

breach and job satisfaction through intrinsic work motivation as predicted by hypothesis 3a (indirect

effect = -.10; 95% CI -.23 to -.02). Hypothesis 3b predicted the mediation effect of intrinsic work

motivation between psychological contract breach and job performance. Results cannot confirm this

relationship (indirect effect = -.02; 95% CI -.09 to .01). All hypotheses that were tested were

controlled for the control variable age. In none of the hypotheses a significant relationship was found

between the control variable and dependent outcomes, as is shown in tables 2 and 3.

(18)

TABLE 1

Means, standard deviations, and correlations between study variables

Note: Gender (n=84) was coded 1=male, 2=female; highest completed education (n=84) was coded 1=preparatory secondary vocational education, 2=senior general secondary education, 3=pre-university education, 4=gymnasium, 5=intermediate vocational education, 6=higher vocational education, 7=university bachelor’s degree, 8=university master’s degree; psychological contract breach (n=83), job performance (n=80) and core self-evaluation (n=84) were measured on a 5-point scale from 1=totally disagree to 5=totally agree. Job satisfaction (n=84) and intrinsic work motivation (n=84) were measured on a 7-point scale from 1=totally disagree to 7=totally agree.

N = 84. *p < .05. **p < .01.

Variables Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Age 35.67 11.05

2. Gender 1.68 .47 -.14

3. Education 5.63 1.12 .03 -.18

4. Psychological contract breach

1.80 .72 -.08 -.13 .01

5. Job performance

4.55 .64 -.13 .06 .02 -.08

6. Job satisfaction 5.95 .86 .24* .05 -.17 -.48** .26*

7. Intrinsic work motivation

5.64 .77 .24* .11 -.23* -.31** .05 .47**

8. Core self- evaluation

3.99 .46 .19 .17 .06 -.11 .10 .26** .22*

(19)

TABLE 2

Results of regression analyses for testing hypotheses 1, 2a and 2b and results of mediation analysis for testing hypothesis 3a and 3b

Predictor

Mediator variable Intrinsic work motivation Hypothesis 1

Constant

Psychological contract breach Age

B 5.05

-.23 .02

SE .31 .09 .01

t 16.36 -2.62 1.92

P .00 .01 .06 Predictor

Dependent variable Job satisfaction Hypothesis 2a B

Constant 5.64 Intrinsic work motivation .41

Age .01 SE

.32 .09 .01

t 17.77

4.59 1.10

P .00 .00 .28 Predictor

Dependent variable Job performance Hypothesis 2b

Constant

Intrinsic work motivation Age

B 4.89 .09 -.01

SE .27 .08 .01

t 18.12 1.24 -1.28

P .00 .22 .21

Predictor

Indirect relationship between psychological contract breach and job satisfaction through intrinsic work motivation Hypothesis 3a

Constant

Psychological contract breach Intrinsic work motivation Age

B 5.64 -.28 .33 .01

Indirect effect

-.10

SE .30 .09 .09 .01

SE

.05

t 18.90 -3.25 3.65 1.13

95 confidence interval*

-.23, -.02

P .00 .00 .00 .26

Note: *based on 1,000 bootstrap samples

(20)

20 TABLE 2 (continued)

Predictor

Indirect relationship between psychological contract breach and job performance through intrinsic work motivation Hypothesis 3b

Constant

Psychological contract breach Intrinsic work motivation Age

B 4.88 -.06 .07 -.01

Indirect effect

-.02

SE .27 .08 .08 .01

SE

.02

t 18.04 -.77 .95 -1.26

95% confidence interval*

-.09, .01

P .00 .45 .35 .21

Note: *based on 1,000 bootstrap samples

Hypothesis 4 predicted that core self-evaluation would moderate the negative relationship between

psychological contract breach and intrinsic work motivation, such that this relationship would be

weaker for employees with high rather than low core self-evaluation levels. Results show no

interaction effect between psychological contract breach and core self-evaluation in predicting

intrinsic work motivation (B = -.09, p > .05). As a result, hypothesis 5a cannot be confirmed by the

moderated mediation analysis. The indirect relationship between PCB and job satisfaction through

intrinsic work motivation (hypothesis 5a) did not significantly differ for low values (M-1 SD; indirect

effect = -.048; 95% CI = -.18 to .07) and high values (M+1 SD; indirect effect = -.125; 95% CI = -.33

to -.02) of core self-evaluation. Also, hypothesis 5b cannot be confirmed by the moderated mediation

analysis. The indirect relationship between PCB and job performance through intrinsic work

motivation did not significantly differ for low values (M-1 SD; indirect effect = -.011; 95% CI = -.09

to .01) and high values (M-1 SD; indirect effect = -.029; 95% CI = -.15 to .01).

(21)

TABLE 3

Results of mediated moderation analyses for testing hypotheses 4, 5a and 5b Predictor Mediator variable: Intrinsic work motivation

B SE t p

Hypothesis 4

Constant 5.10 .31 16.31 .00

Core self-evaluation .14 .10 1.48 .14

Psychological contract breach

-.21 .10 -2.33 .02

Core self-evaluation  psychological contract breach

-.09 .10 -.95 .35

Age .01 .01 1.70 .10

Predictor Dependent variable: Job satisfaction Predictor Dependent variable: Job performance

B SE t P B SE t P

Hypothesis 5a Hypothesis 5b

Constant 5.64 .30 18.90 .00 Constant 4.88 .27 18.04 .00

Psychological contract breach

-.28 .10 -3.25 .00 Psychological

contract breach

-.06 .08 -.77 .45

Intrinsic work motivation .33 .10 3.65 .00 Intrinsic work motivation .07 .08 .95 .35

Age .01 .01 1.13 .26 Age -.01 .01 -1.26 .21

Indirect relationship between psychological contract breach and job satisfaction through intrinsic work motivation at low, middle, and high values

of core self-evaluation

Indirect relationship between psychological contract breach and job performance through intrinsic work motivation at low, middle, and high values

of core self-evaluation Conditional

indirect effect

SE 95% confidence interval*

Conditional indirect effect

SE 95% confidence interval*

Hypothesis 5a

Core self-evaluation

Hypothesis 5b Core self-evaluation

Low (M – 1SD) -.048 .06 -.18, .07 Low (M – 1SD) -.011 .02 -.09, .01

Middle -.087 .05 -.22, -.02 Middle -.020 .02 -.09, .01

High (M + 1SD) -.125 .08 -.33, -.02 High (M + 1SD) -.029 .03 -.15, .01

Note: *based on 1,000 bootstrap samples

(22)

DISCUSSION

The main goal of this study was to gain more insight in the relationship between psychological contract breach (PCB) and job satisfaction and performance. Hypotheses suggested that this relationship could be explained by reduced intrinsic work motivation. Furthermore, the individual construct core self-evaluation was used as a possible moderator within the research model. It was suggested that high core self-evaluation (CSE) employees that experienced PCB would exhibit less influence of this PCB on their job satisfaction and job performance than employees with low levels of CSE. Results of statistical analyses confirmed the negative relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation, thus in this study employees showed lower intrinsic work motivation after experiencing (more) PCB. Furthermore, the positive relationship between intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction was confirmed, which means that highly intrinsically motivated employees were more satisfied with their jobs than less intrinsically motivated employees. The relationship between intrinsic work motivation and job performance could not be confirmed, thus this study cannot state that higher intrinsic work motivation will lead to better job performance.

In addition to the before mentioned relationships, evidence was found for the mediating role of intrinsic work motivation in the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction, as suggested by hypothesis 3a. This evidence implicates that job satisfaction decreases as a result of experienced PCB, through reduced intrinsic work motivation. However, no evidence could be found for the mediating role of intrinsic work motivation in the relationship between PCB and job performance (hypothesis 3b). Hypothesis 4 stated that the negative relationship between PCB and intrinsic motivation would be weaker for employees with high rather than low CSE levels. Results could not confirm this moderation effect of core self-evaluation. Hypotheses 5a and 5b concerned the overall mediated moderation model with both dependent variables job satisfaction and job performance. These hypotheses were also rejected.

Theoretical implications, potential limitations and suggestions for future research and practical

implications will now be discussed.

(23)

23 Theoretical implications

Several theoretical implications can be derived from the results discussed above. First, the relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation was investigated. Test results confirmed that outcomes of earlier research that studied the relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation over time (De Lange et al., 2011), also applied to the relationship between PCB and ‘short-term’ intrinsic work motivation. This evidence is also in line with the earlier explained affective events theory (Weiss &

Cropanzano, 1996) which stated that a negative event can have negative consequences for one’s affective reactions. The evidence which showed PCB has a strong negative influence on employee’s intrinsic work motivation can be of great importance for employers and their employees. Employers should bear in mind that PCB can have major consequences for their employee’s intrinsic work motivation.

Based on previous findings by Agho, Mueller and Price (1993) and the AET by Weiss &

Cropanzano (1996), this study suggested that intrinsic work motivation is positively related to job satisfaction. The results add new empirical evidence to the findings of Agho et al. (1993) by reinstating this relationship. Also based on AET and on research on performance productivity by Grant (2008) the current study suggested that intrinsic work motivation is positively related to job performance. This relationship could not be confirmed.

Next, based on AET the hypotheses 3a and 3b suggested that intrinsic work motivation

mediates the negative relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance. Results

were consistent with this prediction for the dependent variable job satisfaction, but not for job

performance. The finding of the mediation effect of intrinsic work motivation in the relationship

between PCB and job satisfaction provide new theoretical insight. To current knowledge, this exact

mediation effect had not been examined before, but is thus confirmed in the present study. This

outcome makes it possible to better understand why employees who experienced PCB will have lower

levels of job satisfaction. Namely, through reduced intrinsic work motivation. Though the results are

significant, they indicate that reduced intrinsic work motivation does not explain the whole

relationship between PCB and job satisfaction, implying that there are probably more factors

accountable for this relationship. An example of another factor could be overall employee well-being

(24)

24 (Schaufeli, Taris & Van Rhenen, 2008).

Lastly, the theoretical implications derived from analyses on moderation effects of core self- evaluation will be discussed, concerning hypotheses 4, 5a and 5b. CSE had not been investigated as a moderator in the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance before, but in this research was suggested to moderate this relationship based on earlier research on CSE and intrinsic motivation by Zhang, Kwan, Zhang and Wu (2012) and Chang et al. (2012), and research on AET by Wegge, Van Dick, Fisher, West, and Dawson (2006). It was hypothesized that the negative relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation will be weaker for employees with high CSE rather than low CSE (hypothesis 4), and that those employees subsequently will show less lower job satisfaction and job performance (hypotheses 5a and 5b). Results showed no significant differences in these relationships, either for low or high levels of CSE. This implies that CSE does not serve as a boundary condition in the direct relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation, nor in the indirect relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance. For the investigated sample of this study, it is therefore excluded that CSE has any effect on the degree to which employees that experienced PCB show lower job satisfaction and job performance. This implies that there should be other individual difference constructs which determine to what degree employees that experienced PCB will have lower job satisfaction and job performance.

To conclude, the major theoretical implications from this research are 1) confirmation of previous findings on the negative relationship between PCB and intrinsic work motivation and on the positive relationship between intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction; 2) a mediating role of intrinsic work motivation in the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction, and 3) no moderating role of CSE in the indirect relationships between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance.

However, these results should be interpreted with caution, since there are several limitations to the current study, that will now be discussed.

Limitations and suggestions for future research

The current study is subject to several weaknesses. The main limitation might be the sample. The

current sample is not perfectly representative of the Dutch active labor force. For example, 53% of the

(25)

25 Dutch employees are men, while in this simple only 32% were male. Furthermore, the relatively small sample size increases the chance of drawing false conclusions. In case of a small sample chances are that outliers cause biased results, resulting in falsely rejecting or accepting hypotheses. To ensure more reliable results on this subject in future research, the sample size should be increased (Cohen, 1992).

Another limitation of this study might be that it concerned a correlational study. With a correlational study, it is hard to make statements about the underlying causalities. There might be other factors or variables that were not included in this study, that cause certain outcomes to occur.

Furthermore, this study concerned a cross-sectional study, which means that all variables were measured at the same moment. However, the different variables that were measured may influence each other over time. For future research it might therefore be better to first measure PCB, then a few months later to measure intrinsic work motivation, and still a few months later to measure job satisfaction and job performance. This way, effects can be determined over time, which would make the results of a study more valid.

A third limitation of this study might be the degree of psychological contract breach that the respondents reported to have experienced. Looking at the data specifically, only seven of the 83 respondents reported a mean score of 3 or higher (PCB ranging from 1=not at all agree to 5=totally agree). This implies that the respondents were all quite satisfied with how their supervisors treated them during their recruitment phase and after, concerning the promises the supervisors made to them.

This way it was hard to test the model with PCB, since hardly any PCB was experienced. However, it might also be that, though it was thoroughly emphasized in the survey that results would be treated anonymously and supervisors would not have insight in their answers, respondents answered somewhat in a socially desirable way.

A fourth limitation is that the examined variables in this study might be influenced by other

factors. First, intrinsic work motivation might be influenced by other events or by the atmosphere at

work. The affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996) explained that work events of all sorts

influence employees’ work attitudes, which also implies positive events. In this study, respondents

have not been asked about their colleagues or the overall atmosphere at work, which might have

influenced the variable intrinsic work motivation, and in turn job satisfaction and job performance, as

(26)

26 these are all work attitudes (Zhao et al., 2007). Further, job satisfaction as well as job performance might be influenced by another kind of motivation, namely extrinsic work motivation. This motivation is defined as the motivation to do ones job based on the incentives one gets in return (Frey, 1997).

When extrinsic work motivation is high, job satisfaction and job performance might also increase.

Also, employee well-being may be a factor that is influenced by PCB and in turn may influence job satisfaction and job performance (Schaufeli, 2008). Future research should take into account extrinsic work motivation and/or employee well-being in this model to investigate this relationship.

Lastly, in this study the variable core self-evaluation was examined. CSE was not found to moderate the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance. However, earlier it was explained personality has a substantial impact on which affective states are experienced and on how people typically feel at home and at work (Wegge et al., 2006). Since CSE is an individual personality construct, it should not be excluded that it moderates the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance in any way. As mentioned before, the current sample did hardly report any PCB in this survey. It might be that more significant results will appear when more respondents have experienced PCB in their work.

In conclusion, there are several factors that may affect the suggested model of this study.

However, for this study it was not possible to examine all these factors. For future research the current conceptual model could be refined. The roles of factors such as extrinsic work motivation and employee well-being in the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance could be addressed in order to achieve more insight in what organizations can do to increase employees’ job satisfaction and job performance. CSE should not be excluded as a moderator in examining this relationship, since CSE might possibly be confirmed as a moderator when employees do report they have experienced PCB. Moreover, to collect more statistically powerful results in the future, the sample size should be increased.

Implications for practice

Despite the research limitations, the results of this study do have some practical implications. From the

collected data it was confirmed that PCB is negatively related to intrinsic work motivation, and that

(27)

27 intrinsic work motivation is positively related to job satisfaction. Intrinsic work motivation was found to mediate the relationship between PCB and job satisfaction. This new information gives HR practitioners a better understanding of the strong negative consequences PCB can have on employees.

When an employee perceives PCB, his or her intrinsic work motivation will probably decrease, and in turn their job satisfaction will decrease. Depending on how important employers and supervisors think their employees’ motivation and job satisfaction is, they can act on this in practice and can try to prevent PCB from happening.

Against expectations, there was no significant evidence found concerning the other dependent variable, job performance. Thus with the current sample, it appears that PCB does not have an influence on employee’s job performance. If employers do engage in PCB, they may not have to worry that their employees will perform less in their jobs. However, earlier research did make clear that there is a negative relationship between PCB and job performance (Zhao et al., 2007). Therefore, it might be that the current research model could find a significant relationship with other populations or employees that experienced more PCB.

Next, also against expectations of the current model, the indirect relationship between PCB and job satisfaction and job performance was not determined by the employee’s core self-evaluation level. In practice, this means that employees will have lower job satisfaction in reaction to experiencing PCB regardless of their level of core self-evaluation. Employers should therefore make no difference in treating employees differently regarding PCB, based on their core self-evaluation. All employees should be monitored to try to prevent them from experiencing PCB, since it is clear that PCB can have large negative consequences on job outcomes. Employees should be asked regularly how they feel about their job and their employer, to keep their job satisfaction and job performance at the highest possible level.

In conclusion, it is recommended that attention is paid to all employees in order to prevent

them from having lower levels of intrinsic work motivation and job satisfaction as a response to

experiencing PCB. In order to detect employees who have experienced PCB, job satisfaction should be

regularly measured. Employees who have experienced PCB, should be asked what bothered them in

the relationship with their employer. The employer can then try to change his or her actions in the

(28)

28 working atmosphere. Subsequently, the employees’ job satisfaction can be monitored to evaluate whether the behavioral change of the employer made a difference for the employment relationship between employee and employer.

(29)

29 REFERENCES

Agho, A. O., Mueller, C. W., & Price, J. L. 1993. Determinants of employee job-satisfaction: An empirical test of a causal model. Human Relations, 46(8): 1007-1027.

Bal, P. M., De Lange, A. H., Jansen, P. G. W., & Van der Velde, M. E. G. 2008. Psychological contract breach and job attitudes: A meta-analysis of age as a moderator. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72(1): 143-158.

Beal, D. J., & Ghandour, L. 2011. Stability, change, and the stability of change in daily workplace affect. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32(4): 526-546.

Becker, T. E. 2005. Potential problems in the statistical control of variables in organizational research:

An analysis with recommendations. Organizational research methods, 8: 274-289.

Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. 2003. Self-concordance at work: Toward understanding the motivational effects of transformational leaders. Academy of Management Journal, 46(5): 554-571.

Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. Beroepsbevolking; geslacht en leeftijd http://statline.cbs.nl/Statweb/publication/?DM=SLNL&PA=82579ned&D1=a&D2=0&D3=a&

D4=0%2c7&D5=0%2c6%2c15%2c(l-2)-l&VW=T. Posted on 24 September 2014.

Chang, C. H., Ferris, D. L., Johnson, R. E., Rosen, C. C., & Tan, J. A. 2012. Core self-evaluations: A review and evaluation of the literature. Journal of Management, 38(1): 81-128.

Cohen, J. 1992, Statistical power analysis. Current directions in psychological science, 1(3):98-101.

De Lange, A. H., Bal, P. M., Van der Heijden, B. I. J. M., De Jong, N., & Schaufeli, W. B. 2011.

When I’m 64: Psychological contract breach, work motivation and the moderating roles of future time perspective and regulatory focus. Work & Stress, 25(4): 338-354.

Erez, A., & Judge, T. A. 2001. Relationship of core self-evaluations to goal setting, motivation, and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86: 1270-1279.

Faugier, J., & Sargeant, M. 1997. Sampling hard to reach populations. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 26: 790-797.

Frey, B. S. 1997. On the relationship between intrinsic and extrinsic work motivation. International

journal of industrial organization, 15(4): 427-439.

(30)

30 Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. 2005. Self-determination theory and work motivation. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 26: 331-362.

Gakovic, A., & Tetrick, L. E. 2003. Psychological contract breach as a source of strain for employees.

Journal of Business and Psychology, 18(2): 235-246.

Grant, A. M. 2008. Does intrinsic motivation fuel the prosocial fire? Motivational synergy in predicting persistence, performance, and productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(1):

45-58.

Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. 1980. Work redesign. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Harris, K. J., Harvey, P., & Kacmar, K. M. 2009. Do social stressors impact everyone equally? An examination of the moderating impact of core self-evaluations. Journal of Business and Psychology, 24: 153-164.

Hayes, A. 2013. An introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford.

Herzberg, F. 1966. Work and the nature of man. Cleveland, OH: World Publishing.

Judge, T. A., Erez, A., & Bono, J. E. 1998. The power of being positive: The relation between positive self-concept and job performance. Human Performance, 11(2/3): 167-187.

Judge, T.A., Erez, A., Bono, J.E., & Thoresen, C.J. 2003. The Core Self-Evaluations Scale:

Development of a measure. Personnel Psychology, 56: 303-331.

Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., & Durham, C. C. 1997. The dispositional causes of job satisfaction: A core evaluations approach. Research in Organizational Behavior, 19: 151-188.

Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D., Judge, T. A., & Scott, B. A. 2009. The role of core self-evaluations in the coping process. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94: 177-195.

Lester, S. W., Turnley, W. H., & Bloodgood, J. M. 2002. Not seeing eye to eye: Differences in supervisor and subordinate perceptions of and attributions for psychological contract breach.

Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23(1): 39-56.

Levinson, H,, Price, C., Munden, K., Mandl, H., & Solley, C. 1962. Men, management, and mental

health. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Model 2 represents the relationship between the dependent variable of absenteeism and the independent variables of well-being and job satisfaction taking into account

Looking at the team level and considering different levels of extraversion, the size of the work unit might play a role for the development of LMX quality8. As leaders have

This research studied the influence of power on people’s gossip behaviors, especially negative gossip, as well as the mediating effect of task satisfaction and moderating effect of

Therefore, by means of this explanation, we expect that job satisfaction can explain why extraverted employees in general have better employee job performance than those

The aim of this study is to determine whether or not different types of employment contracts have an effect on the relationship between employee intrinsic

Last, previous research of Walker, Churchill and Ford (1977) found that intrinsic motivation is positively related to effort and effort is positively related to job performance,

The most interest is into the moderating effect of trust in the supervisor on this relationship between subjectivity in performance evaluation and pay

CONTACT was not significant, and therefore shows that both trust and frequency of contact have no influence on the relationship between the use of subjectivity in