• No results found

The Father-infant relationship beyond caregiving sensitivity

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The Father-infant relationship beyond caregiving sensitivity"

Copied!
7
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Tilburg University

The Father-infant relationship beyond caregiving sensitivity

van Bakel, H. J. A.; Hall, R. A. S.

Published in:

Attachment & Human Development

DOI:

10.1080/14616734.2019.1589058 Publication date:

2020

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Link to publication in Tilburg University Research Portal

Citation for published version (APA):

van Bakel, H. J. A., & Hall, R. A. S. (2020). The Father-infant relationship beyond caregiving sensitivity. Attachment & Human Development, 22(1), 27-31. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2019.1589058

General rights

Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

• You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal Take down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

(2)

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at

https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rahd20

Attachment & Human Development

ISSN: 1461-6734 (Print) 1469-2988 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rahd20

The Father-infant relationship beyond caregiving

sensitivity

Hedwig J. A. van Bakel & Ruby A. S. Hall

To cite this article: Hedwig J. A. van Bakel & Ruby A. S. Hall (2019): The Father-infant relationship beyond caregiving sensitivity, Attachment & Human Development, DOI: 10.1080/14616734.2019.1589058

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2019.1589058

© 2019 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group.

Published online: 28 Mar 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 686

View related articles

(3)

The Father-infant relationship beyond caregiving sensitivity

Hedwig J. A. van Bakel and Ruby A. S. Hall

Social and Behavioral Sciences, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands

ABSTRACT

Parental sensitive behavior is seen as one of the main determinants of attachment security. However, studies also suggest that other behaviors may be stronger predictors of the parent-child relationship (Lucassen et al.,2011). The lack of a significant association between sensitive behavior and the quality of the attachment relationship between father and child in Olsavsky et.al’s study indeed seems to reflect the notion that other interactive behaviors may be specific in father-child relationships (i.e., destabilizing behavior followed by sensitivity). Since the strength of the association between the quality of parental interactive behavior (e.g., sensitive, responsive behavior) and child attachment security is moderate (Fearon & Belsky,2016), and weaker for fathers (Lucassen et al.,2011) there is a need to examine other factors beyond caregiving sensitivity in attachment research. For example, the important role of reparation of mismatch-ing to matchmismatch-ing states and fathers’ own attachment history as related to frightening behavior and mentalization are factors that need to be taken into account. It is essential to apply a broader view of father’s parenting behaviors in different contexts (distress, non-distress, or risky contexts) and a broader view of child attachment behaviors in relationship with the father.

KEYWORDS

paternal sensitivity; fathers; child behavior

It is generally acknowledged that parental sensitive behavior is one of the main determi-nants of attachment security, but previous studies also showed sensitivity to be a weak predictor of father-infant attachment security. Studies suggest that other behaviors may be stronger predictors of the parent-child relationship (Lucassen et al., 2011). However, the concept of sensitivity has received persistent interest in research on parent-infant attach-ment, whereas the role of other interactive behaviors has received less attention. Therefore, thefindings of the paper of Olsavsky and colleagues (this issue) are important in this respect and emphasize the impact of paternal stimulating behavior on the development of secure attachment relationships. The lack of a significant association between sensitive behavior and the quality of the attachment relationship between father and child indeed seems to reflect the notion that other interactive behaviors that may be specific to the father should be studied (i.e., destabilizing behavior followed by sensitivity). Olsavsky et al. describe their findings in light of theoretical models that define the father-infant relationship as similar to the mother-infant relationship in some ways, but also with unique features. They examined the quality of the father-child attachment by using the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP),

CONTACTHedwig J. A. van Bakel H.J.A.vanBakel@uvt.nl

ATTACHMENT & HUMAN DEVELOPMENT https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2019.1589058

© 2019 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group.

(4)

showing significant relations between fathers’ play behaviors (stimulation in combination with low-to-moderate levels of intrusiveness) and the child’s future attachment quality.

From a theoretical viewpoint, the SSP would not capture the full nature of the father-child attachment relationship. When viewing the father-infant relationship as a“secure-exploration concept” (and not only as a secure-base or safe-haven concept; Grossmann et al.,2002), the focus of father-child attachment should be not only on separation and stress responses, but also account for a range of behaviors, from seeking close bodily proximity to confident exploration, depending on the situation and the child’s state of anxiety (Grossmann, Grossmann, & Zimmermann, 1999). Moreover, the concept of the “father-child activation relationship” by Paquette (2004) and similar descriptions by Hazen, McFarland, Jacobvitz, and Boyd-Soisson (2010) suggest that fathers may possess a distinctive ability to excite and destabilize their infant while simultaneously providing safety, warmth, and security. Crucial, however, is that fathers show sensitivity after their destabilizing behavior, helping their infants to regulate their intense emotions.

Sensitivity after destabilization of child behavior

The model of communication systems described by Tronick (2017) in his recent bu ffer-transducer model and previous mismatch-repair theory (Tronick & Cohn,1989) stresses the important role of reparation of mismatching to matching states. Adequately balan-cing between mismatch and match powerfully affects the development of infants’ sense of self and adds significantly to the emotional quality of relationships. If the resulting experience of dysregulation is brief and quickly repaired, it may be growth-promoting because infants make non-symbolic meanings that they and their caregivers are com-petent to repair ruptured interactions. When fathers use mild levels of intrusiveness during play and challenging tasks, this destabilizes the child for a moment (mismatch in emotional states). When fathers are able to sensitively turn destabilizing behavior into a state of matched affect this behavior may have positive effects. Repairing a mismatch can create “positive stress,” which may promote infants’ regulatory skills and socio-emotional competence (Tronick & Beeghly,2011). This may also apply to Olsavsky et al.’s results, where fathers who are stimulating and display low to moderate levels of intrusiveness during play are more likely to have infants who develop secure attach-ments with their fathers. These fathers are able to repair a mismatch and turn it into a matching state. Stimulating the infant’s play in a more intrusive way does not promote security in the father-infant relationship, because of assumed ongoing destabilizing mismatches.

In Olsavsky’s et al.’s study, the SSP was used to measure the quality of child attach-ment. Although the SSP has been acknowledged as the gold standard to measure parent-child attachment, in studies with fathers the procedure “the Risky Situation (RS)” that was developed by Paquette and Bigras (2010), may be proposed. The RS is a specific procedure to assess the quality of the father-child relationship and is based on the activation relationship theory (Paquette, 2004). Central to this procedure is the father-child distance, in which the father is neither too close nor too far from the child, which allows the father to protect the child in case of danger while providing the child with the necessary room to practice abilities independently. This different view

(5)

on the attachment relationship between fathers and children may be an attractive focus for future research.

Fathers’ own attachment history as related to frightening behavior and mentalization

Interestingly, Olsavsky et al.’s study showed that only fathers showing stimulation in combination with (at most) moderate levels of intrusiveness promoted future attachment security. The question is whether fathers’ own attachment history is involved in the relationship between stimulation, intrusiveness, and child attachment. A meta-analytic study conducted by Verhage et al. (2016) showed that parents’ own histories of attachment relationships led to individual differences in the quality of their child’s attachment relation-ship through the quality of dyadic interactions, consistent with theory (Bowlby,1969/1982). Recently, Ahnert et al. (2017) provided direct evidence that fathers who had experienced severe childhood adversities (e.g., removed from their homes, raised by foster parents or in orphanages) demonstrated significantly lower play quality in interaction with their children than fathers without childhood adversities. In other words, fathers who have experienced much adversity and stress in their own childhoods may have greater difficulty maintaining a level of mild intrusiveness during play with their child.

Olsavsky et al. also referred to the study of Hazen et al. (2010), which showed that fathers with secure attachment relationships with their infants were more likely to exhibit sensitivity after displaying a frightening behavior. In contrast, fathers with insecure attachments with their infants were less likely to exhibit sensitive behaviors after displaying a frightening behavior. Surprisingly, the authors found that many fathers, secure as well as insecure, engaged in frightening behaviors with their infants. However, secure-autonomous fathers were more likely than insecure fathers to show sensitive and frightening behavior concur-rently (Hazen et al.,2010). This suggests that fathers’ own attachment history may play an important role in balancing sensitive, stimulating, and mildly intrusive behavior during father-infant interactions.

Moreover, parents’ own attachment history relates to their capacity to mentalize about the relationship with the infant and to parent-child attachment (Madigan, Hawkins, Plamondon, Moran, & Benoit,2015). Even during pregnancy, fathers– like mothers – start to mentalize about the baby. They have fantasies, ideas, and expectations about their (unborn) infants and develop attachment representations regarding them. The quality of these prenatal attach-ment representations is relatively stable (Vreeswijk, Maas, Janneke, Rijk, & van Bakel,2014), with high concordance levels pre- and postnatally. Paternal representations of the infant are important predictors of future interactive behaviors. Fathers with balanced (“secure”) repre-sentations of their infants in the first months of life display a higher quality of interactive behavior towards their infants, which is in turn correlated with infant developmental out-comes (Hall et al., 2014). Being able to mentalize and take the child’s perspective, as a balanced father is capable of, is needed to attune the level of destabilization/intrusiveness and sensitivity. In Olsavsky et al.’s study, when fathers took their 9-month-old infant’s point of view into account (i.e., by exhibiting average to low intrusiveness), while stimulating their infant more frequently and intensely (physically or using an object), their infants were more likely to demonstrate a secure attachment to their father. Fathers’ mentalizing capacities may be a key factor in their ability to balance stimulation with mild intrusiveness.

(6)

Final remarks

Overall, there is a need to examine other factors beyond caregiving sensitivity in attach-ment research. The strength of the association between the quality of parental interactive behavior (e.g., sensitive, responsive behavior) and child attachment security is moderate (Fearon & Belsky, 2016), and weaker for fathers (Lucassen et al., 2011). Therefore, it is essential to apply a broader view of fathers’ parenting behaviors in different contexts (distress, non-distress, or risky contexts) and a broader view of child attachment behaviors in relationship with the father. Olsavsky et al.’s study yields additional evidence that paternal behavior contributes to the relationship that the infant develops with the father. It points to the fact that specific interactive behavior patterns in fathers help the young child to develop a good relationship with the caregiver from which she may develop further capacities and skills. Thefinding that specific paternal behaviors (stimulating behaviors) and not sensitive behavior were related to attachment security gives direction to further research on the specific role fathers have in their infants’ first years of life and beyond.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

Ahnert, L., Teufl, L., Ruiz, N., Piskernik, B., Supper, B., Remiorz, S., … Nowacki, K. (2017). Father-child play during the preschool years and child internalizing behaviors: Between robustness and vulnerability. Journal of Infant Mental Health, 38, 1–13.

Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Fearon, P., & Belsky, J. (2016). Precursors of attachment security. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed., pp. 291–313). New York: Guilford Press.

Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H., Sheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2002). The uniqueness of the child-father attachment relationship: Fathers’ sensitive and challenging play as a pivotal variable in a 16-year longitudinal study. Social Development, 11, 307–331.

Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., & Zimmermann, P. (1999). A wider view of attachment and exploration: Stability and change during the years of immaturity. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 760–786). New York: Guilford Press.

Hall, R. A. S., De Waard, I. E. M., Tooten, A., Hoffenkamp, H. N., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., & Van Bakel, H. J. A. (2014). From the father’s point of view: How father’s representations of the infant impact on father– Infant interaction and infant development. Early Human Development, 90, 877–883.

Hazen, N. L., McFarland, L., Jacobvitz, D., & Boyd-Soisson, E. (2010). Fathers’ frightening behaviors and sensitivity with infants: Relations with fathers’ attachment representations, father-infant attachment, and children’s later outcomes. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 51–69. Lucassen, N., Tharner, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Volling, B. L.,

Verhulst, F. C.,… Tiemeier, H. (2011). The association between paternal sensitivity and infant– Father attachment security: A meta-analysis of three decades of research. Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 986–992.

Madigan, S., Hawkins, E., Plamondon, A., Moran, G., & Benoit, D. (2015). Maternal representations and infant attachment: An examination of the prototype hypothesis. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36, 459–468.

(7)

Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the father-child relationship: Mechanisms and developmental outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193–219. .

Paquette, D., & Bigras, M. (2010). The risky situation: A procedure for assessing the father-child activation relationship. Early Child Development and Care, 180, 33–50.

Tronick, E., & Beeghly, M. (2011). Infants’ meaning-making and the development of mental health problems. American Psychologist, 66, 107–119.

Tronick, E. Z. (2017). The caregiver–Infant dyad as a buffer or transducer of resource enhancing or depleting factors that shape psychobiological development. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy, 38, 561–572.

Tronick, E. Z., & Cohn, J. F. (1989). Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: Age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Development, 60, 85–92.

Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S., Fearon, P., Oosterman, M., Cassibba, R., … van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: A synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 337–366. Vreeswijk, C. M., Maas, J. M., Janneke, A., Rijk, C. H., & van Bakel, H. A. (2014). Fathers’ experiences during pregnancy: Paternal prenatal attachment and representations of the fetus. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 15, 129–137.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

• Provides insights into the effect of customer satisfaction, measured through online product reviews, on repurchase behavior!. • Adresses the question whether the reasons for

This issue of Infectious Disease Reports addresses the challenge of antimicrobial resistance from different perspectives and provides examples of different solutions that

This chapter described the running-in of rolling-sliding contacts on macroscopic and microscopic level. 1) On macro-scale, the geometrical change of the contacting

Reiman quotes philosopher Richard Wasserstrom who in 1978 already observed that all information collected about him could produce a ‘picture of how I had been living

In this paper, we propose a Markov Decision Problem (MDP) to prescribe an optimal query assignment strategy that achieves a trade-off between two QoS requirements: query response

An additional finding was that levels of parenting stress have strong associations with child psychopathology, and that different associations for mothers and fathers came to

This suggests again that, in case of two-vehicle crashes, the second vehicle being a light truck increases the equivalent fatality rate for the first vehicle and, in case of

This argument states that when a nation perceives a high security threat, it would be more inclined to pursue nuclear weapons, while it would give up its nuclear program if