• No results found

Therelationbetweenfamilymealsandhealthofinfantsandtoddlers:Areview Appetite

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "Therelationbetweenfamilymealsandhealthofinfantsandtoddlers:Areview Appetite"

Copied!
13
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Contents lists available atScienceDirect

Appetite

journal homepage:www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

The relation between family meals and health of infants and toddlers: A review

Chantal L. Verhage

a

, Marleen Gillebaart

b

, Shelley M.C. van der Veek

c

, Carel M.J.L. Vereijken

a,∗

aDanone Nutricia Research, Uppsalalaan 12, 3584 CT Utrecht, The Netherlands

bUtrecht University, Social, Health, and Organizational Psychology, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands

cLeiden University, Institute of Education and Child Studies, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands

A R T I C L E I N F O

Keywords:

Family meal Infant Toddler Eating behavior Health

A B S T R A C T

Family meals are associated with multiple health benefits in children and adolescents including evidence that eating together as a family may play a role in reducing childhood obesity. The current review aims to investigate whether the beneficial health effects of the family meal also apply to infants and toddlers.

PubMed, Web of Science, Scopus and PsycInfo were searched and 14 empirical studies were identified. The findings were discussed according to frequency of having a family meal and parental perception, associations between the family meal and health aspects (e.g., eating behaviors and diet quality) and causal influences of these associations.

Descriptive data showed that mothers offer food at a structured mealtime, but that eating together as a family was not always upheld. The frequency of family meals was positively associated with more nutrient-dense food intake and a more balanced diet. Different advantages (e.g., social importance, practical considerations) and obstacles (e.g., planning, possible mess) of the family meal were mentioned by parents. Further, having struc- tured mealtimes and family meals was associated with more food enjoyment and less fussy and emotional eating.

Finally, no causal studies were identified.

The limited number of studies suggests that the pattern of positive associations between family meal and child health which has been shown in older children may also exist in infants and toddlers.

More specific research is needed to examine the causality of the associations between the family meal and health of the infant and toddler. The associations between the family meal and less fussiness and emotional eating, more food enjoyment and better nutrient intake suggest that the family meal is a valuable moment to promote healthy eating in toddlers and infants.

1. Introduction

According to the World Health Organization the estimated number of children under the age offive with overweight reached 42 million in 2015, which is six percent of all children worldwide (World Health Statistics, 2016). Over the past decade, accumulating studies have shown that frequently eating together as a family may play a role in reducing childhood obesity (e.g., Cook & Dunifon, 2012; Fulkerson, Larson, Horning, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2014; Gable, Chang, & Krull, 2007). For example, a meta-analysis demonstrated that children - be- tween approximately three and 17 years - who shared a meal as a family three or more times a week, were at decreased odds for being over- weight, and had healthier dietary and eating patterns (i.e., less dis- ordered eating) (Hammons & Fiese, 2011). This suggests that family

meals may play an important role in promoting healthy eating behavior in children. The current review aims to investigate whether family meals are associated with health benefits in infants and toddlers.

The family meal can be seen as a family ritual that is accompanied by different routines.Spagnola and Fiese (2007)suggested that“family routines and meaningful rituals provide both a predictable structure that guides behavior and an emotional climate that supports early de- velopment” (p.284). A significant number of articles varying in ap- proach (e.g., longitudinal study, qualitative study, and meta-analysis) demonstrated that family meals are associated with numerous physical and mental health benefits in preschool and school-aged children (e.g., Fiese & Schwartz, 2008;Gable et al., 2007;Hammons & Fiese, 2011;

Ochs & Shohet, 2006;Quick, Fiese, Anderson, Koester, & Marlin, 2011).

Martin-Biggers et al. (2014) reviewed 81 studies (of which a small

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.04.010

Received 21 July 2017; Received in revised form 26 January 2018; Accepted 10 April 2018

Corresponding author.

E-mail addresses:chantal.verhage@external.nutricia.com(C.L. Verhage),m.gillebaart@uu.nl(M. Gillebaart),SVeek@FSW.leidenuniv.nl(S.M.C. van der Veek), carel.vereijken@danone.com(C.M.J.L. Vereijken).

Available online 11 April 2018

0195-6663/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

T

(2)

number were longitudinal studies and the majorities were cross-sec- tional studies) on these associations in young children and adolescents (approximately from two to 18 years). More frequent family meals were associated with higher intake of healthy dietary components and nu- trients (e.g., fruit and vegetables, iron, calcium) and lower energy in- take (e.g., soft drinks). Furthermore, Martin-Biggers and colleagues indicated that children who had more frequent family meals had a healthier BMI, which may be related to factors such as parental control during mealtime and a structured mealtime setting. The importance of the family meal may extend to other areas than nutrition and adiposity as well: in adolescence, sharing at leastfive family meals a week was associated with less substance abuse and higher scores on family sup- port and self-esteem. Finally, some of the included studies also revealed positive associations between family meals and language and academic development, which may be due to family talk and debate during mealtime (Martin-Biggers et al., 2014).

Family meals may be an opportunity for parents to model healthy eating behavior.Palfreyman, Haycraft, and Meyer (2015)investigated the influence of modeling appropriate behavior during mealtimes. Re- sults demonstrate that maternal modeling, both verbal (e.g., expressing likes/dislikes) and behavioral (e.g., eating certain itemsfirst), during mealtimes with children between zero and six years was related to less food responsiveness (e.g., eating too much or eating most of the time), less emotional over-eating and more food enjoyment. The American Psychological Association (2006)andAmerican College of Pediatricians (2014)both acknowledge the importance of family meals for children's nutritional health and consider it as a strategy to prevent childhood obesity. They suggest that the family meal is a good moment for adults to model appropriate behavior, as well as to intervene in inappropriate behavior, and concluded therefore that parents should be encouraged to plan family meals.

The review byMartin-Biggers et al. (2014)and the meta-analysis by Hammons and Fiese (2011)show that the majority of studies on the family meal has been conducted with children from the age of two years until adolescent age, resulting in a lack of knowledge on how family meals affect health and healthy eating behavior in infants and toddlers.

Feeding can be seen as a developmental task which children have to successfully master in thefirst years of life. They have to progress from a restricted and guarded food environment in which milk is the sole source of nutrients to a varied and free food environment (Vereijken, Weenen, & Hetherington, 2011). It has also been shown that there are sensitive periods for food learning in thefirst years of life (Harris &

Mason, 2017). The importance of this period in life for the development of healthy food preferences and eating habits is also supported by studies showing that early food patterns track to later eating patterns (Moore, Tapper, & Murphy, 2007; Nicklaus & Remy, 2013). Several studies have shown that individual preferences for food categories in thefirst three years of life predict individual preferences in four-eight year olds (Skinner, Carruth, Bounds, & Ziegler, 2002), as well as in adolescents (Nicklaus, Boggio, Chabanet, & Issanchou, 2004). Further- more, healthy nutrition at a young age and good parental feeding practices have consequences for health and well-being later in life (e.g., healthy weight, better cognition) (Birch & Fisher, 1998; Pem, 2015;

Walker et al., 2007). Thus, thefirst years of life seem especially im- portant for the establishment of healthy food preferences and eating habits.

Therefore, the purpose of the present review is tofill the knowledge gap by investigating whether characteristics of the family meal are associated with outcomes in terms of health benefits in infants and toddlers (e.g., better nutrition quality and healthy eating behavior), and to gain more insight in the factors that are related to these potential outcomes.

Different aspects should be considered when defining the family meal. For example, the number of family members who are attending (e.g., are both parents present during the meal?), the setting in which the meal takes place (e.g., is the meal at the table or in front of

television?), and also the types of meals that are served (e.g., healthy or unhealthy meals?) should be considered.Meiselman (2009)summar- ized different criteria which are often used in the literature when de- fining meals, namely: time of day, energy content, social interaction, food combinations, and a combination of all these criteria. Thus, the definition of a family meal depends on which criteria are prioritized, for example if social interaction is a preferred criterion of the meal, eating alone will not be defined as a meal. For the current review a definition of the family meal was used, inspired by the review ofMartin-Biggers et al. (2014)andMeiselman (2009). Defining the family meal is com- plex for this age range as the child will pass through different stages regarding feeding (e.g., from breastfeeding to solid foods). Due to this, the definition has to be broad as eating together with an infant (e.g., focus on parent-child interaction and new food exposure) can be dif- ferent from eating together with a toddler (e.g., focus lies more on toddler's autonomy and in-depth mealtime interactions). The current review therefore uses the following definition: ‘The family meal can be seen as a social moment of the day during which food is eaten together with at least one family member.’

The current review intends to provide an overview of all relevant research regarding the family meal in infants and toddlers, in order to map the associations between family meals and health benefits for this age group. The review is guided by three aims. Thefirst aim is to gather descriptive data and/or data on parental perceptions of the family meal with children between the ages of zero and three years. This includes for example how regularly parents are actually eating together with their young children, but also what obstacles or advantages parents perceive regarding the family meal as perceived benefits or barriers are important predictors of (health) behavior (Champion & Skinner, 2008, pp. 45–65). The second aim is to review the associations between fa- mily meals and health benefits in children aged zero to three years. In line withMartin-Biggers et al. (2014), it is expected that the family meal is associated with healthier food intake, better nutrition and fa- milial relationships and less eating disorders in children aged zero to three years. Finally, the third aim is to review evidence for causality of these associations.

2. Methods 2.1. Literature search

The electronic databases PsycInfo, Scopus, PubMed and Web of Science were searched. No restrictions were made regarding publica- tion year or language. Key search categories were family (i.e., family OR parental OR parents OR shared), meal (i.e., meal* OR dinner* OR breakfast* OR lunch* OR food OR eating OR supper OR table), and age (i.e., toddler* OR infancy OR baby OR “young children” OR “little children” OR “little child” OR infant* OR “early childhood” OR new- born* OR neonate* OR“small child*"). The search resulted in a total of 12,209 articles. The titles of the articles were screened by thefirst author based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria (listed inTable 1), which yielded 306 articles. The abstracts of these articles were screened by thefirst author for relevance and 85 articles were selected for full reading. Based on the full text and reference list searches (one extra article was included), 14 articles were selected by thefirst author. Fi- nally, a quality assessment was conducted (more details in the‘quality assessment’ section below) and all 14 articles were included. The article selection process is outlined inFig. 1.

2.2. Quality assessment

To determine the quality of the selected articles the quality assess- ment tool ofMoore (2012)was used. The tool can be used for quali- tative and quantitative studies as it focuses on both methods. The quality assessment tool consists of 11 items (e.g.,‘How defensible is the design?’, ‘Is the context clearly described?’, ‘Are the findings

(3)

convincing?’), which were rated with a score of 2 (‘yes’), 1 (‘partly’) or 0 (‘no’). The total quality score ranges from 0 to 22, with higher scores suggesting a higher quality. All articles were rated by thefirst author, while the other three authors each rated a different subset of papers that were randomly assigned. Any disagreement on total scores be- tween the reviewers was resolved through consensus. All 14 articles were included.

3. Results

3.1. Synthesis of results

Due to heterogeneity of studies in terms of study design and out- come measures the results are summarized in a narrative form.

3.2. Description of different aspects investigated

The main aspects which were reported and which corresponded to the aims of the review are: 1) the frequency of family meals; how this differs per feeding approach chosen; parental perception, reported stressors and influences on having family meals; 2) associations

between the family meal and nutritional quality and eating behavior;

associations between parent-child mealtime interaction and nutritional intake; and 3) approaches to influencing parental attitude regarding the family meal, increasing communication during mealtime and the fre- quency of eating together.Table 2presents an overview of the relevant aspects of the reviewed studies.

4. Summary of mainfindings

4.1. Frequency of having a family meal and parental perception about sharing meals

Six quantitative studies (Cameron et al., 2013;Chan et al., 2011;

Fitzpatrick et al., 2007;Le Heuzey & Turberg-Romain, 2015;Le Heuzey et al., 2007; Morison et al., 2016) and three qualitative studies (Contreras & Horodynski, 2010;Horodynski et al., 2009;Spence et al., 2016) were found. Studies were conducted in France, New-Zealand, Australia and the United States (US) and had a mean quality score of 14 (range of scores 6–18).

Le Heuzey et al. (2007) showed in their French national survey which focused on feeding behavior and food consumption - among mothers (N = 713) of children younger than three years - that eating together regularly increases when the child gets older. At eight to 12 months 26% of the mothers reported that they ate regularly together with the child, which increased to 64% at 13 to 18 months. Moreover, 91% of the mothers reported eating regularly together with the child at the age of three years. Further, they showed that more than half of the mothers reported that their child ate the same as them at 13–18 months, which increased to 98% at the age of three. In a similar French national surveyfive years laterLe Heuzey and Turberg-Romain (2015) showed that 24% of the mothers (N = 1188) reported eating regularly together at eight to 11 months, which increased to 51% at 12–17 months. Further, 92% reported eating regularly together at the age of three. Finally, almost half of the mothers reported that their child ate the same as the parents at 12–17 months, which increased to 93% at the age of three.

In their studies conducted in New-Zealand,Morison et al. (2016) andCameron et al. (2013)showed (respectively, N = 26 and N = 199) that mothers of children aged between six to 12 months who were following baby-led weaning (i.e., infants are not spoon fed, but feed themselves pieces of food) ate significantly more family meals together (p's < 0.04) compared to mothers who fed their child with a spoon.

This shows that the frequency of having family meals differs depending on the complementary feeding approach chosen by mothers.

Contreras and Horodynski (2010) showed that 40% of African- American mothers (N = 20) of children aged 15–36 months living in the US were assisted by an older sibling or spouse during mealtimes and that 65% indicated that somebody else fed their children sometimes.

Furthermore, 40% of the mother reported that their child regularly ate meals in another room than the dining room and half of mothers re- ported having the television on during mealtimes. Another descriptive Australian study of the family meal is the study fromChan et al. (2011).

They examined mothers (N = 740) of children aged 12–36 months about the use of certain feeding practices that were related to the family meal table (e.g., let the child eat with the rest of the family and let the child sit down during mealtime). Results showed that more than half of the mothers reported offering food at a structured mealtime and less than half reported eating the main meal with the rest of the family all of the time. Approximately one third of the mothers reported that their child ate the same food as the rest of the family all of the time and more than two third reported that their child sits down during a meal all of the time. In addition, 14% reported watching TV during mealtime often.

Another study showed that also ethnicity may play a role when it comes to having family meals. The study ofFitzpatrick et al. (2007)- conducted in the US - showed (N = 1336) that the number of nights the Table 1

Inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Inclusion Criteria Exclusion Criteria

Empirical data Review studies

Peer reviewed Case studies

Sample size under 10 Studies on eating together as a family

(sharing meals)

Studies about activities during family mealtime (e.g., TV watching)

Studies on parental perceptions about family mealtimes Family meal associated with health aspects (e.g., weight, dietary intake)

Children under the age of three or with a mean age of three

Parental disorders (e.g., depression or eating disorders)

Children with disorders (e.g., autistic) or medical conditions (e.g., cysticfibrosis) Focus on effects of general feeding styles that did not consider a family meal (e.g., emotional feeding)

Studies with the focus on children above three years

Animal samples Breastfeeding studies

Fig. 1. Flow-Chart of the systematic article selection.

(4)

Table2 Summaryofthendingsoftheincludedarticles. Authorsandstudyobjective(s)ParticipantsandsampleDesignandmethodsRelevantaspectsofthefamily mealinvestigatedQuestionsaskedand/ordata collectedrelatedtothefamilymealFindingsrelevanttothefamilymealQuality Ratings (scoresout of22) Frequencyandparentalperceptionsoffamilymeals LeHeuzey,Turberg-Romain,and Lelievre(2007) Examinesthefeedingcontextof theyoungchildandmothers' experienceintheirnourishingrole.

N=713,mothersofchildrenaged betweenzeroandthreeyearslivingin France.

Quantitative Cross-sectional,national survey.

Frequencyof: 1)eatingtogetherwiththe parents;

1)Howoftenchildeatswith parents(never,occasionally(once ortwiceperweekandlessoften)or regularly(dailyoralmostdailyand threeorfourtimesperweek)).

1)Ateight-12months,26%ofthe mothersreportedeatingregularly togetherwiththechild;at13-18 monthsofage64%andat31-36 months91%.

9 2)eatingthesameasthe parents.2)Howoftenchildeatsthesame foodasparents(never, occasionally(onceortwiceper weekandlessoften)orregularly (dailyoralmostdailyandthreeor fourtimesperweek)).

2)At13-18months53%ofthe mothersreportedthattheirchild regularlyeatsthesameasthem,this increasedto98%at31-36months. LeHeuzeyandTurberg-Romain (2015) Examinesthefeedingcontextof theyoungchildandmothers' experienceintheirnourishingrole.

N=1188,mothersofchildrenaged betweenzeroandthreeyearslivingin France.

Quantitative Cross-sectional,national survey.

Frequencyof: 1)eatingtogetherwiththe parents;

1)Howoftenchildeatswith parents(never,occasionally(once ortwiceperweekandlessoften)or regularly(dailyoralmostdailyand threeorfourtimesperweek)).

1)Ateight11months24%ofthe mothersreportedeatingregularly togetherwiththechild;at12-17 monthsofage51%andat30-35 months92%.

6 2)eatingthesameasthe parents.2)Howoftenchildeatsthesame foodasparents(never, occasionally(onceortwiceper weekandlessoften)orregularly (dailyoralmostdailyandthreeor fourtimesperweek)).

2)At12-17months48%ofthe mothersreportedthattheirchild regularlyeatsthesameasthem,this increasedto93%at30-35months. Morisonetal.(2016) Describesthefamilymealsoffered toinfantsofmothersfollowing baby-ledweaningoratraditional spoonfeedingapproach.

N=26,mothersofinfantsbetweensix andeightmonthslivinginNew-Zealand.Quantitative Cross-sectional,dietary assessment.

Frequencyof: 1)eatingamealwithatleast oneadult;

1)Numberofinfantseatingtheir mealwiththefamily(breakfast, lunch,dinner;answerscalesnot available).

1)Childrenofmothersfollowing baby-ledweaning(BLW:infantsare notspoonfed,butfeedthemselves piecesoffood)atesignicantlymore familylunches(p=.001)and eveningmeals(p=.02)together thanmotherswhofollowed traditionalspoonfeeding(TSF; motherfeedsthechildwitha spoon).

18 2)eatingthesameingredients asthefamilymeal;2)Numberofinfantswith ingredientsthesameasthefamily meal(breakfast,lunch,dinner; 1=exactlythesameand 4=mostlydierent).

2)Childrenofmothersfollowing BLWatesignicantlymoreofthe sameingredientsduringlunch (p=.001)andeveningmeal (p=<.001)astherestofthe family,comparedtotheTSFgroup. 3)havingthesamemeal preparationastherestofthe familyaccordingtothe feedingmethodchosen.

3)Numberofinfantswithmeal preparationthesameasthefamily meal(breakfast,lunch,dinner; 1=exactlythesameand 4=mostlydierent).

3)Signicantlymorechildrenofthe motherswhofollowedBLWhadthe samemealpreparationastherestof thefamilyforlunch(p<.001)and eveningmeals(p<.001)compared toTSF. N=199,mothersofinfantssixto12 monthslivinginNew-Zealand.Quantitative Cross-sectional,onlinesurvey.Frequencyofeatingwiththe infantatthesametime(could bedierentfood)astherestof thefamily.

Whetherbabysharesamealwith thefamily,eveniffoodisdierent (none,some,most,all).

ChildrenofmothersfollowingBLW sharedsignicantlymoremealsasa familycomparedtochildrenof motherswhofollowedthespoon- feedingapproach(p=.04).

15 (continuedonnextpage)

(5)

Table2(continued) Authorsandstudyobjective(s)ParticipantsandsampleDesignandmethodsRelevantaspectsofthefamily mealinvestigatedQuestionsaskedand/ordata collectedrelatedtothefamilymealFindingsrelevanttothefamilymealQuality Ratings (scoresout of22) Cameron,Taylor,andHeath(2013) Examinesfeedingpracticesand health-relatedbehaviorsin familiesfollowingbaby-led weaningortraditionalspoon feedingmethodstointroduce complementaryfoods. Chan,Magarey,andDaniels(2011) Exploresfeedingpractices.N=740,(N=374validreturned questionnaires)mothersofchildrenaged 12–36monthslivinginAustralia.

Quantitative Cross-sectional,self- administratedquestionnaire.

Frequencyof: 1)havingastructuredmeal;1)Oerfoodwhen:‘Itisat structuredmeal/snacktime(allof thetime,veryoften,often, sometimes,andhardlyever).

1)Intotal,59%ofthemothers reportedofferingfoodata structuredmealtimeallofthetime.

18 2)eatingtogetherwiththe family;2)‘Mychildeatsmainmealwith therestofthefamily(allofthe time,veryoften,often,sometimes, andhardlyever).

2)Intotal,43%reportedeatingthe mainmealwiththerestofthefamily allofthetimeand27%veryoften. 3)eatingthesameastherest ofthefamily;3)‘Mychildeatsthesamefoodas therestoffamily(allofthetime, veryoften,often,sometimes, hardlyever).

3)Intotal,34%reportedthatthe childeatsthesamefoodastherest ofthefamilyallofthetime. 4)sittingdownduringmeal;4)‘Mychildsitsdownwhenhaving meals(allofthetime,veryoften, often,sometimes,andhardlyever).

4)Intotal,71%reportedthatthe childsitsdownwhenhavingameal allofthetime. 5)watchingTVduring mealtimes.5)Mychildwatchestelevision whenhavingmeals(allofthetime, veryoften,often,sometimes, hardlyever).

5)Intotal,39%watchedTVduring mealtimesatleastsometimesand 14%watchesTVduringmealtime often. Horodynski,Brophy-Herb,Henry, Smith,andWeatherspoon (2009) Studiesmaternalexpectationsand experienceswithtoddlerfeeding, mealtimeenvironmentand mealtimeinteractions.

N=27,African-Americanmothersof childrenaged12–36monthslivinginthe UnitedStates.

Qualitative Cross-sectional,focusgroup discussions.

1)Perceptionsofmealtime environment;1)‘Whataremealtimeslikeinyour home?1)Reportedmealtimechallengesby motherswere:potentialmesswhen toddlerisself-feeding,toddler's mealtimeattitude(e.g.,tootalkative orsloweater).Athirdofthemothers reportedpickyeatingbehaviorasa reasonforthemtoleavethetable. Halfofthemothersperceivedthe mealtimeasavaluablemomentto socialize.

17 2)activitiesduringmealtime.2)‘Whatisthesettingandmanner inwhichyoufeedyourtoddler?2)Halfofthemothershadthe televisiononandcompletedother tasksduringmealtime.Mealstook sometimesplaceinotherroomsthan thediningroom/kitchen. ContrerasandHorodynski(2010) Collectsinsightsintohowfood choicesandmealtimebehaviors areaffectedbyhousehold stressors.

N=20,African-Americanmothersof childrenaged15–36monthslivinginthe UnitedStates.

Qualitative Cross-sectional,focusgroup discussions.

1)Identifyingmealtime stressorswhileeatingwiththe child,includingelementsof culturalcontext,parentaland childcharacteristics,stressors;

1)Perceptionsofchild's temperament,identicationand appraisaloffeedingchallenges; andmaternalpsychologicaland physicalstates.

1)Mealtimestressorsreportedby themothers:a)child'sbehavior (70%)(e.g.,picky/greedy),b) throwingfood/mess(allmothers) andc)mothers'ownpsychological state(athirdofthemothers)(e.g., tired).

12 2)copingbehaviors;2)Answerscalesnotavailable.2)Mothersreportedusingdierent copingmechanisms,suchasexible familyroles,familialsupport(40%) andindulgentfeeding(60%). (continuedonnextpage)

(6)

Table2(continued) Authorsandstudyobjective(s)ParticipantsandsampleDesignandmethodsRelevantaspectsofthefamily mealinvestigatedQuestionsaskedand/ordata collectedrelatedtothefamilymealFindingsrelevanttothefamilymealQuality Ratings (scoresout of22) 3)TVonduringmealtimes.3)Answerscalesnotavailable.3)Intotal,55%hadtheTVon duringmealtimes. Spence,Hesketh,Crawford,and Campbell(2016) Exploresmother'sperceptionsof inuencesontheirfeeding practicesamongstasamplethat participatedinanintervention focusingonpromotinghealthy childfeedingpractices.

N=26,mothersofchildrenagedtwo yearslivinginAustralia.Qualitative Cross-sectional,telephone interviews(semi-structured).

Inuencesoneatingwiththe child.Partofthesevenquestions included:“Doyouhaveanyrules aboutfoodormealtimes?”,“Are thereanytimeswhenmealsare morechallengingandhowdoyou respond?”,“Canyouthinkofany reasonswhyyoudothosethings?

Severalinuencesonhavingfamily mealswerementionedbymothers: (1)theexposuretofeedingmessages abouteatingtogether(N=8),(2) practicalconsiderationslikeeasiness regardingcookingandtime scheduling(N=12),(3)thesocial importanceofthemeal(N=13) andpartnersupportregarding structuredtimeandhealthyeating (N=9),(5)one'sownupbringing (N=19),and(6)recommendations ofhealthcareprofessionals(N=2).

18 Prompts:“Isthathowyouwere broughtup,orwhatyourfriendsor familydo,orwhatyou‘vefound works?

Reasonsfornothavingfamilymeals werescarcityoftime,feelingsof tiredness(N=10)andparental workinghours. Associationsbetweenthefamilymealandeatingbehavioranddietquality Fitzpatrick,Edmunds,andDennison (2007) Investigatesassociationsofeating dinnerasafamilyandhavingthe televisiononduringdinneron childfeedingbehaviors(fruit, vegetablesandmilkconsumption).

N=1336,parents/guardiansofchildren agedbetweenoneand4.9years(mean ageof2.8)livingintheUnitedStates.

Quantitative Cross-sectional,survey.Thefrequencyof: 1)eatingdinnertogetherasa family;

1)Numberofdaysduringthe previousweekthatthefamilyate dinnertogether(never,sometimes (0–3timesaweek),most(4–6 timesperweek)anddaily).

Aftercontrollingforparentalrace andeducation: 1)theoddsofservingfruits(odds ratio=1.14)andvegetables(odds ratio=1.15)signicantlyincreased witheachnightthefamilyatedinner together(bothp's<.001).No signicanteffectsonmilkservings werefound.

11 2)havingtheTVonduring mealtimes.2)Numberofdaysduringthe previousweekthattheTVwason duringdinner(never,sometimes (0–3timesaweek),most(4–6 timesperweek)anddaily).

2)theoddsofservingfruits(odds ratio=0.95)andvegetables(odds ratio=0.94)decreasedwitheach nighttheTVwasonduringdinner (respectively,p<.05and p<.01). Swansonetal.(2011) Investigatessocio-cognitive predictorsofmaternalfeeding behaviorandtheirrelationship withyoungchildren'sdietary quality.

N=300,mothersofchildrenagedtwo yearslivinginScotland.Quantitative Cross-sectional,survey.Actualfrequencyof: 1)havingapropersitting- downmeal(i.e.,eatingasa familyandsittingarounda table)perweek;

1)‘Thinkingaboutthelastweek, howoftendid(child)haveaproper sitdownmeal?(scorerange0–7).

1)Theactualfrequency(i.e.,direct measure)ofhavingfamilymealswas signicantlyhigherformothersof childrenwithabetterqualitydiet (p<.006).

17 2)cookingfromscratch.2)‘Howoftendidyoucookfrom scratch?(scorerange0–7).2)Theactualfrequencyofcooking fromscratchwashighlysignicantly correlatedwithhavingafamilymeal (r=.74,p<.0001). Jansen,Mallan,Nicholson,and Daniels(2014) Constructsandevaluatesa questionnaireforassessing parentalfeedingpracticesthat supporthealthyeatingbehavior.

N=462,mothersofchildrenaged21-27 monthslivinginAustralia.Quantitative Cross-sectional,TheFeeding PracticesandStructure Questionnaire(FPSQ).

1)“FamilyMealSetting constructincludeseating mealswiththefamilyand eatingthesamefoodasthe rest.

1)“Mychildeatsmainmealswith therestofthefamily”,“Mychild eatsthesamemealsastherestof thefamily”,“Icookseparatemeals formychild(never,rarely, sometimes,often,always).

21 (continuedonnextpage)

(7)

Table2(continued) Authorsandstudyobjective(s)ParticipantsandsampleDesignandmethodsRelevantaspectsofthefamily mealinvestigatedQuestionsaskedand/ordata collectedrelatedtothefamilymealFindingsrelevanttothefamilymealQuality Ratings (scoresout of22) 1)Eatingmoreofteninafamily mealsettingwassignicantly negativelycorrelatedwithfussiness (r=.40,p<.001),emotional undereating(r=.15,p<.01), emotionalovereating(r=.-13, p<.01)andpositivelycorrelated withenjoymentoffood(r=.29, p<.001). 2)“StructuredMealtimeSetting constructincludessittingat thetable.

2)“Iallowmychildtowander aroundduringameal”,“Iinsistmy childeatsmealsatthetable”, “Howoftenareyourmabout whereyourchildshouldeat?”,“My childsitsdownwhenhaving meals(never,rarely,sometimes, often,always).

2)Amorestructuredmealtime settingwassignicantlynegatively associatedwithsatiety responsiveness&slownessofeating (r=.16,p<.001),fussiness (r=.14,p<.01),emotional undereating(r=.153,p<.01), emotionalovereating(r=.136, p<.01)andwaspositively correlatedwithfoodenjoyment (r=.25,p<.001). 3)“StructuredMealTiming constructincludeshavinga structuredtimeofhavingmeals.

3)“Iletmychilddecidewhen(s)he wouldliketohavehermeal”,“I decidewhenitistimeformychildto haveasnack”,“Idecidethetimes whenmychildeatshis/hermeals (never,rarely,sometimes,often, always). 3)Astructuredmealtimetimingwas signicantlypositivelycorrelated withemotionalunder-eating (r=.09,p<.05). Weatherspoon,Venkatesh, Horodynsky,Stommel,and Brophy-Herb(2013) Comparesfoodpatternsand mealtimebehaviorsand determinespredictivefactorsof nutrient-denseandenergy-dense foodintakebetweentwodierent ethnicgroups.

N=399,African-American(AA)mothers andnon-Hispanicwhite(NHW)mothers ofchildrenaged12–36months(ofwhich 37childrenwereaged36-48months) livingintheUnitedStates.

Quantitative Cross-sectional,TheGladys BlockFoodFrequency Questionnaire(FFQ);TheToddler andParentMealtimeBehavior Questionnaire(TPMBQ);oneor twomealtimeobservation(s).

1)Howoftenmother accompaniesthechildandhas completeattentionforthe childduringmealtimes.

1)‘Isitdownwithmychildat meals”,“Ilistentomychildat meals”,“Italktomychildat meals”,“MychildandIeat together”,“MychildandIface eachotherwheneating”,I encouragemychildtotalkduring meals”,“Mychildgetsmyfull attentionduringmealtimes”,“I encouragemychildtotalkright afterhe/shenisheseating (never,rarely,sometimes,often, andalways).

1)Mother'spresenceandher emphasisonsocialinteraction duringmealtimeswiththetoddler signicantlydecreasedthe frequencyofenergy-densefood consumption(B=.132,p=.001) andincreasedthefrequencyof consumingnutrient-densefood (B=.097,p=.046;CI95%)inboth groups.

20 2)HowoftentheTVwason duringmealtime.2)“TheTVisonduring mealtimes”,“TheTVisonduring mealtimesinthesameroomweeat ourmealsin,”“IwatchTVduring mealtimes”,“MychildwatchesTV duringmeals(never,rarely, sometimes,often,andalways).

2)ThemoreTVwatchingwas emphasizedbyAAmothers,theless frequenttheirtoddlersconsumed nutrient-densefoods(B=.063, p<.027).Nosignicanteffects werefoundforNHWtoddlers. Causalinuencesoffamilymealsonchild'shealth (continuedonnextpage)

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Chapter 1 Page 24 The field of study will benefit from this research because this type of study has not been undertaken at a colliery before and there is limited literature on

This, by using varying water reducing chemical admixtures or optimising the particle packing of the fine aggregate of the mix, while maintaining constant levels of workability,

Deze herkomsten uit Duitsland zijn een waardevolle aanvulling op het uitgangsmateri- aal van es in Nederland en kunnen daarmee worden opgenomen in de Aanbevelende Rassenlijst

De gunstige neven- effecten van inkrimping van de veestapel en extra export van pluimveemest op de stik- stof- en ammoniakproblematiek kunnen verder worden versterkt door gebruik

Alternative fits are performed with the efficiency correction based on the newly defined Dalitz plot variable, and the resultant changes of the Dalitz plot parameters with respect

His concept of enlightened catastrophism would call on all members of society to accept that nuclear power and nuclear armed conflicts as well as climate change

The view of reality is a salvific theology of God’s engagement with humanity through the kingdom of God, covenant, culture, church and ecumenism.. Accordingly, PCI’s theological

Erratum a` « Pertinence des indicateurs de risques psychosociaux a` l’hoˆpital pour la pre´vention du burnout »M. [Arch Mal Prof Environ