• No results found

University of Groningen Religiosity and Reproductive Decisions in Europe Bein, Christoph

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "University of Groningen Religiosity and Reproductive Decisions in Europe Bein, Christoph"

Copied!
23
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Religiosity and Reproductive Decisions in Europe Bein, Christoph

DOI:

10.33612/diss.151942579

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date: 2021

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Bein, C. (2021). Religiosity and Reproductive Decisions in Europe. University of Groningen. https://doi.org/10.33612/diss.151942579

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

Chapter 4

Do costs and benefits of children matter for religious

people? - Perceived consequences of parenthood and

fertility intentions in Poland

Christoph Bein, Monika Mynarska, Anne H. Gauthier

A previous version of this chapter has been published as:

Bein C, Mynarska M & Gauthier AH (2020) Do Costs and Benefits of Children Matter for Religious People? - Perceived Consequences of Parenthood and Fertility Intentions in Poland.

(3)

Abstract

The aim of this paper is to examine the positive relationship between religiosity and fertility from the perspective of perceived consequences of parenthood. Previous studies on Germany found that highly religious people ascribe higher benefits and lower costs to having children. Furthermore, the impact of costs and benefits on fertility is less pronounced among the highly religious. This study tests these mechanisms for fertility intentions and in the context of Poland, a country with a low fertility rate and high religiosity in comparison to other European countries. A sample of 4892 men and women of childbearing age from the second wave of the Polish version of the Generations and Gender Survey is used. First, it is analysed to what extent perceived costs and benefits mediate the impact of religiosity on fertility intentions. Second, it is examined whether religiosity moderates the impact of perceived costs and benefits on fertility intentions. The results show that part of the positive effect of religiosity on fertility intentions can be explained by more religious people seeing higher benefits of having children. Furthermore, but only in the case of women, religiosity moderates the impact of perceived costs on fertility intentions, suggesting that the effect of perceived costs decreases with increasing religiosity.

(4)

95

Introduction

Despite the ongoing trend of secularization and a decline of the relevance of religion in public life, religiosity continues to be one of the key determinants of fertility. More religious people have been found to be more likely to have children in comparison to the less religious in many different county settings (Adserà, 2006b; Hubert, 2015; Peri-Rotem, 2016; Okun, 2017; Dilmaghani, 2019). Explanations for the positive association of religiosity and fertility are manifold. It has been argued that religions encourage their followers to have children by providing social support and networks for families (Krause et al., 2001; Okun, 2017), by promoting traditional gender roles and family values (Sherkat, 2000; Goldscheider, 2006; Klingorová and Havlicek, 2015), or by direct teachings such as the Biblical command “be fruitful and multiply”.

Another mechanism of how religiosity may facilitate childbearing relates to how religious people perceive, and how strongly they react to, various costs and benefits of having children. Religions encourage their followers to have children by accentuating and communicating the benefits of parenthood. The support that religious faith gives in different life situations may also lead to lower perceived costs of having children. Furthermore, it may lead to the belief that a deep faith can shield religious people from high costs of having children (Philipov, 2011). It follows that even those religious people who perceive high costs of having children may still want to have them. Previous research using German data confirmed a positive relationship between religiosity and higher perceived benefits of having children (Brose, 2006), and showed that this is one of the reasons why the more religious are more likely to start a family (Arránz Becker and Lois, 2017). What is however not clear from this literature is whether these results hold for other countries, too. The exact mechanism of how the perceived costs and benefits of children intertwine with religiosity is also yet to be established.

The goal of this study was to contribute to the literature by examining the interrelations between religiosity and perceived costs and benefits of having children in the context of Poland, and in regards to fertility intentions. Poland, as a majority-Catholic country with a low fertility rate constitutes an interesting case study due to its marked contrast to the German context of earlier studies. Moreover, and in contrast to previous studies, this one focuses on fertility intentions. If the whole fertility decision making process is considered (Miller, 1994; Miller, 2011b), fertility intentions are the link between the perceived costs and benefits on one side and behaviour on

(5)

96

the other. By focusing on intentions, this study was able to go deeper into the role of religiosity in the fertility decision making process.

The study used the second wave of the Polish version of the Generations and Gender Survey. Of the countries that participated in the survey, Poland was the only one to include a series of items tapping into perceived benefits and costs of having children, which allowed for a thorough analysis of their relationship with religiosity and fertility. The study aimed to examine the extent to which the costs and benefits of having children act as mediators for the effect of religiosity on fertility intentions and to assess whether religiosity acts as a moderator on the relationship between perceived costs and benefits and fertility intentions.

Theoretical background

Religiosity and perceived costs/benefits of having children

Perceived costs and benefits of children are a key component of numerous theoretical models of fertility decisions. These models assume that high perceived costs and low perceived benefits have a negative effect on the decision to have a child. This assumption is part of many economic models (e.g. Becker, 1960; Schultz, 1973) as well as models formulated in psychology or sociology (Hoffman and Hoffman, 1973; Miller, 1994; Nauck, 2014). In the economic theories, direct monetary costs and indirect costs related to the allocation of time to housework and foregone wages are most frequently considered (Becker, 1965; Schultz, 1973; Becker, 1981). Costs cover also other possible negative aspects of having children, such as stress, worries over children, discomforts of pregnancy or limited time for leisure (Miller, 1995; Guedes et al., 2015). Similarly, benefits of children encompass numerous motivations or reasons for which people have children. They include emotional aspects such as affiliation, a sense of accomplishment, novelty (children bring new experiences), but also instrumental aspects such as economic utility or fulfilling norms regarding morality and religion (Hoffman and Hoffman, 1973; Miller, 1995; Guedes et al., 2015).

The role of costs and benefits of children for fertility choices has already been tested in a wide range of studies (e.g. Bulatao, 1981; Shah and Nathanson, 2004). These have shown that, for the first child, emotional and psychological benefits are the most relevant, while for higher parities, economic and instrumental values grow in importance (Bulatao, 1981). They also

(6)

97 indicated that emotional values of children became central in modern societies, while the importance of instrumental values diminished (Nauck, 2007; Mayer and Trommsdorff, 2010; Nauck, 2014). In terms of costs, it has been highlighted that due to rising employment of women, opportunity costs of having children have risen considerably (Moss, 1988) and represent an important part of the overall costs of childbearing (Duggan, 2003).

Despite the recognized importance of perceived costs and benefits of children in fertility decisions, one missing link is the possible role of religiosity. Theoretically, Goldscheider (1971) and McQuillan (2004) do identify the perceived costs and benefits as one possible link between religiosity and fertility. This link seemed apparent in traditional societies, where instrumental values of children related to fulfilment of norms regarding morality and religion also had a large influence on the decision to have children (Westoff and Jones, 1979; van Poppel, 1985; Lesthaeghe and Meekers, 1987). The Second Demographic Transition theory suggests that those norms have however become less relevant over time in Western countries, as secularization spread and welfare systems expanded (Lesthaeghe, 2010). Yet, a large number of studies have found that religiosity still matters in individual childbearing decisions (Philipov and Berghammer, 2007; Buber-Ennser and Skirbekk, 2016; Peri-Rotem, 2016; Bein et al., 2017; Dilmaghani, 2019), and some findings even suggested that its importance has grown over time (Adserà, 2006a; Adserà, 2006b). In light of the continuing relevance of individual religiosity for childbearing decisions despite the general context of a decline in the influence of religions, the question emerges how religiosity relates to, and might influence, other perceived costs and benefits of having children. The current study considered two possible mechanisms: the mediating mechanism and the moderating mechanism.

The mediating mechanism

Considering the role of religiosity for fertility choices, it is postulated that more-religious people might perceive more benefits of having children overall. This becomes apparent by looking at the ways in which religions encourage their followers to have children. One aspect is the influence of direct religious teachings or commands (McQuillan, 2004). The Catholic Church, which is the most influential religious institution in Poland, where the current study is located, has a particularly explicit pronatalist stance in its teachings. In the Social Doctrine of the Catholic Church, children are seen as a gift from God to parents, family and the whole society

(7)

98

(Pontifical Council for Justice and Peace, 2004). The prime goal of marriage, one of the sacraments of the church, is seen in procreation. The Catholic Catechism states that large families are to be seen as a sign of God’s blessing (Catechism 2366-2379). Beyond pronatalist stances, many religions also put an emphasis on family values and traditional gender roles (Lehrer, 2004; McQuillan, 2004). These examples show that many religious doctrines emphasize the positive sides and benefits of having children. Brose (2006) showed in a study using German data, that religious people indeed attach more positive values to children than less religious people and suggested that they might therefore be more likely to have them. Moreover, among the more religious, a stronger association between the number of children and happiness has been reported (Cranney, 2017).

Religiosity may also impact childbearing decisions by reducing the perceived costs of having children. For instance, a part of the perceived costs of childbearing that are mentioned in the literature consists of difficulties in combining family and career. The opportunity costs that stem from this conflict are particularly high for women and play an important role in childbearing decisions (McDonald, 2000; Thévenon and Gauthier, 2011; Goldscheider et al., 2015; Baizan et al., 2016). As many religions promote traditional gender roles and view childrearing and housework as areas of women’s specialization (Sherkat, 2000; Goldscheider, 2006; Seguino, 2011; Klingorová and Havlicek, 2015), these costs may be perceived as less relevant by the religious people. Moreover, some religious institutions help reducing the costs of childbearing by providing activities and childcare for families (Blume, 2014). Another reason is related to differences in the interest in material gains. Previous research found indications that more religious people might be less materialistic than the less religious (Jensen and Jensen, 1993; Veer and Shankar, 2011; Rakrachakarn et al., 2015). It might therefore be possible that the religious perceive lower costs of children because they think less in terms of economic benefits and costs in their decisions. In other words, there can be a mediating effect at work through which religiosity leads to higher fertility by influencing perceived costs and benefits of having children. This mediating effect has already been found for perceived benefits in a German study by Arránz Becker and Lois (2017) on the likelihood of having a first child. Therefore, the first hypothesis is:

H1: The effect of religiosity on fertility intentions can be explained by more-religious people

(8)

99 The moderating mechanism

It is also possible that religiosity controls, or moderates, how much costs and benefits actually influence the decision to have a child, which is another mechanism considered in the current study. Highly religious people are frequently in contact with other highly religious people due to their involvement in Church activities, gaining valuable sources of social support in coping with problems and stressful situations that may arise in larger families (Krause et al., 2001). Religious faith can also give further spiritual support in stressful situations (Maton, 1989), and more religious people might rely on their faith to be shielded from high costs of children (Philipov, 2011). Consequently, the second hypothesis refers to a moderating role of religiosity:

H2: The negative effect of perceived costs of having children on fertility intentions is less

pronounced for more religious people

On the one hand, there is no reason to expect that emotional values have different effects on childbearing intentions among religious and non-religious individuals. On the other hand, as benefits of parenthood include instrumental ones as well as benefits related to traditional values, it could be expected that they have a stronger effect for religious individuals. In contrast with this assumption, the already mentioned study of Arránz Becker and Lois (2017) found that the effect of both perceived costs and benefits on fertility is weaker among the highly religious. That study argued that religious individuals are generally less inclined to consider childbearing in terms of costs and benefits. Yet, only emotional benefits were included in that study.

Consequently, no explicit hypothesis on how religiosity moderates the impact of perceived benefits of childbearing on fertility intentions was formulated. Nonetheless, to gain a full picture of all possible channels of influence, this effect was tested as well.

The Polish context

Poland is religiously homogenous and can be characterized as highly religious as compared to most other European countries. A recent Pew survey on religion in Europe found that a vast majority of the population in Poland (87%) is affiliated with the Catholic Church. Around 61% of the population indicated that they attend religious services at least monthly (the highest value of all countries surveyed in Europe), compared to 24% in Germany, 11% in the Czech Republic or 17% in Russia. Only 7% indicated that they do not identify with a faith, which is lower than

(9)

100

in neighbouring countries like Russia (15%), the Czech Republic (72%) or Germany (24%) (Pew Research Center, 2018). Among younger adults, who are on average less religious than the general population, Poland still stands out as comparably religious. According to findings from the European Social Survey (2014-16), young people in Poland are the most likely to identify with a faith and attend religious services and pray at least weekly among all European countries surveyed (Bullivant, 2018). The comparably high religiosity of the population is also reflected in the influence of the Catholic Church on society and government (Zuba, 2010; Szwed and Zielińska, 2017). Part of that influence is rooted in the fact that during the communist period, the Church was one of the leading opposition forces and played an important role in overcoming the previous regime and introducing democracy. Consequently, after the collapse of communism, the Church gained a strong position in society (Szwed and Zielińska, 2017). That position for example enabled the Church to successfully lobby for stricter abortion laws, turning them into one of the most restrictive rules within Europe (Szelewa, 2016).

At the same time, the fertility rate in Poland is very low. The total fertility rate declined from around 2.1 children per woman in 1990 to the lowest-low level of 1.2 in 2003, which was related most of all to the economic shock that the country experienced after the end of communism. Changes in the labour market, high unemployment and less stable employment, as well as the reduced state support for families increased the vulnerability of families and resulted in profound changes in family formation (Kotowska et al., 2008). In the following years and especially after the accession to the EU in 2004, Poland has been on a sustained growth path and even posted economic growth during the crisis in 2009 (Duszczyk, 2014). At the time of data collection for the current study (2014 and 2015), Poland continued its strong growth and had a declining unemployment rate of around 8% (Eurostat, 2019). Nevertheless, the fertility rate recovered only slightly to 1.3 children per woman (Eurostat, 2019). This has been partly explained by the weak governmental support for families (Matysiak and Weziak-Bialowolska, 2016) and the difficulties for women to combine career and family. These economic and institutional changes have been argued to be one of the reasons why the comparably high religiosity of Poland has not been able to shield Poland from fertility decline (Matysiak and Vignoli, 2013). Neither has the high religiosity prevented the widespread use of contraceptives (Mynarska, 2016). Only for the most religious people, religiosity matters in their decision on contraceptive use (Mishtal and Dannefer, 2010). Nevertheless, the more religious in Poland express more positive attitudes towards marriage and are more inclined to choose marriage over

(10)

101 cohabitation (Baranowska-Rataj et al., 2014). The religious also express larger desired family sizes (Mishtal, 2009).

Overall, there is a pronounced contrast between Poland and Germany, the focus of the previous studies of Arránz Becker and Lois (2017), and Brose (2006). Even though Germany is also characterised by low fertility, its institutional and cultural setting differs. Compared to Poland, gender attitudes in Germany are less traditional (Matysiak and Weziak-Bialowolska, 2016) and childlessness is more tolerated (Merz and Liefbroer, 2012). Germany is also characterized by a more mixed and more secularized religious landscape (Pew Research Center, 2018). Therefore, testing those mechanisms in the Polish context may add to a further understanding of how religiosity shapes childbearing decisions in different contexts.

Methods

Dataset

The dataset used in this study was the second wave of the Polish version of the Generations and Gender Survey (Kotowska et al., 2019). The Generations and Gender Survey (GGS) is a survey with nationally representative samples having a target population of all 18-79-year-old non-institutionalized residents. So far, it has been conducted in 19 countries. Its questionnaire encompasses a wide range of items on causes and consequences of family change. In the second wave of the Polish version of the survey, a wide range of statements regarding perceived costs and benefits of children were introduced and asked to be evaluated in terms of their relevance for having children by each respondent.

In the first wave of the Polish GGS, which was conducted in 2010 and 2011, 19,987 people were interviewed. The initial sample of the second wave, conducted in 2014 and 2015, consisted of 12,294 respondents, bringing the attrition to 38.4%. The sample in the second wave was expanded by a number of respondents aged 18-22, who were not interviewed in Wave 1, bringing the total number to 13,480 respondents at Wave 2. Despite this sizeable attrition, patterns of fertility intentions were the same when comparing respondents present at both waves and respondents who dropped out of the sample after Wave 1 (Brzozowska and Mynarska, 2019). This indicates that any attrition biases that might have occurred due to the exclusive use of the second wave of the survey can be considered negligible in studying fertility intentions.

(11)

102

For this study, the sample was restricted to those of childbearing age (N = 5459), i.e. men aged 18–49 and women aged 18–44. Furthermore, cases in which the female respondent or the partner of the male respondent was pregnant, as well as cases in which the respondents declared themselves or their partner infecund were removed, further reducing the sample to 5176 respondents. Cases in which information was missing on fertility intentions (47 cases), religiosity (226 cases), the importance of perceived costs and benefits of having children (9 cases) and on the control variables (2 cases) were dropped as well. The final analytical sample consisted of 4892 respondents.

Variables

In the models, the dependent variable “fertility intentions” was constructed based on the question: “Do you intend to have a/another child during the next three years?”. Respondents were given the following four response categories: “definitely not”, “probably not”, “probably yes” and “definitely yes”. For the purpose of this study, the variable was dichotomized by grouping the responses “definitely not” and “probably not” into “no” and the responses “probably yes” and “definitely yes” into “yes”.

The main explanatory variables were religiosity and perceived costs and benefits of children. There are many ways in which religiosity could be conceptualized and measured, such as the frequency of attending religious services or the importance of religion in one’s life. In this paper, the latter was used, as this more subjective way of measuring religiosity aligns with the subjectivity of perceived costs and benefits. It was asked in the survey on a 5-point scale ranging from “completely unimportant” to “very important”.

The Polish GGS included 19 items on perceived costs and benefits of having children. Respondents were presented with each of these items and then asked to evaluate their importance for them at the moment. Table 4.1 shows all of the benefits and costs of having a child included in the survey. Answers were given on a 5-point scale. They were recoded so that the highest value corresponds to “very important” and the lowest to “completely unimportant”. On the basis of these items, two scales were then computed: one encompassing the perceived benefits and another one the perceived costs. They were calculated as the average scores of importance across benefits and costs for each individual. However, for the perceived benefits scale, the item related to religiosity (“Parenthood means fulfilling religious feeling about family

(12)

103

life”) was removed, because of its collinearity with religiosity itself. Cronbach’s alpha for the

scale of perceived costs amounted to 0.88 for men and 0.88 for women. For the scale of perceived benefits, the corresponding values were 0.86 for men and 0.83 for women.

Table 4.1: List of items used to measure perceived costs and benefits of having children

Benefits of children Costs of children

• Children are necessary for the future of Polish economy (people to work)

• Children will support their elderly parents • Children will inherit parents’ properties

• Children prevent parents from feeling lonely in older age

• Children take over parents’ personal characteristics and values

• We experience a special love and closeness through parenthood

• We want to watch children grow and develop • Parenthood makes a union stronger and more

committed

• A parent feels more complete as a woman / a man • Parenthood means fulfilling religious feeling about

family life (dropped)

• Raising children makes it more difficult for parents to engage in paid employment and to have a professional career

• Partners have less time for each other when children are born

• Raising children limits parents’ leisure time • Raising children is difficult financially

• Raising children brings many worries and concerns • For women, it is difficult to combine work and

childbearing

• Raising children brings too much responsibility • Pregnancy and delivery are strenuous for women • A fear that a child will be born ill

Individual religiosity in part also depends on personal characteristics, such as education (Aleksynska and Chiswick, 2015). Within the models, this was taken into account by adding additional variables to control for age, age squared (to take the non-linear relationship between age and fertility intentions into account), partnership status (married or cohabiting vs. single or not living with a partner) and education level (recoded into: low – up to basic vocational training, medium – up to post-secondary education, and high – academic education).

Analysis

The analytical strategy of this study consisted of two parts, in which the two different mechanisms of how religiosity may matter for shaping fertility intentions were considered. In the first part, the goal was to verify whether, and to what extent, religiosity affects fertility intentions through perceived costs or benefits of parenthood, i.e. whether and to what extent its effect is mediated by them. Therefore, the total effect of religiosity on fertility intentions needed to be disentangled into the direct and indirect effect. The direct effect describes the effect of religiosity on intentions independently of the perceived costs and benefits. The indirect effect,

(13)

104

on the other hand, describes that part of the total effect that emerges because more and less religious respondents perceive costs and benefits to a different degree and therefore differ in their fertility intentions.

In the analysis, the part of the indirect effect that goes through perceived costs and the other part that goes through perceived benefits were distinguished. To capture both, the khb-package developed by Kohler et al. (2011) for Stata was used. In the case of linear models, the decomposition of the total effect into the direct and the indirect effect would be straightforward, as the coefficients of the model with and without the mediator could be compared and the difference between them captures the indirect (mediated) effect. Since the models in this study were logistic models, confounding as well as the rescaling of the model, that can take place when additional variables are introduced, needed to be considered. The khb-package does not only take these issues into account but also provides the necessary tools to decompose the effects of multiple mediator variables.

In the second step, possible moderation effects of religiosity on the effects of perceived benefits and costs of having children on intentions were examined. The primary goal of this step was to determine whether the effect strength of perceived costs on fertility intentions declines with increasing religiosity. In order to present a complete picture, interaction effects between perceived benefits and religiosity were included as well, even though there were no specific hypotheses regarding this moderation in this study. For these analyses, four logistic regression models on fertility intentions were run. All models included religiosity as an explanatory variable. Models 1 and 2 included perceived benefits and Models 3 and 4 included perceived costs. The moderation effect was assessed by the interaction effect between religiosity and perceived benefits or costs in Model 2 and 4, respectively.

For both the mediation and moderation analysis, all models were constructed separately for men and women.

Results

Descriptive results

In Table 4.2, descriptive statistics are shown separately for women and men for all variables used in the models. Women were on average ascribing a higher importance to religion in their

(14)

105 daily lives than men did. Overall, both men and women scored higher on the index on perceived benefits of having children than on the index of perceived costs. Furthermore, there were only slight differences between men and women in their average index scores on benefits and costs and virtually no difference in their intentions. Around 22% of women and 21% of men intended to have a(nother) child over the next three years. In terms of the number of children respondents already had, there were some notable differences between men and women. More than a half of the men in the sample reported being childless, compared to around 40% of women. In turn, almost 40% of women had 2 or more children, compared to around 32% of men. Differences appeared in the level of education and partnership status as well: women were better educated and were more likely to live with a partner and less likely to be single than men.

Table 4.2: Descriptive statistics of the analytical sample used in the models (means and standard deviations for continuous variables, percentage distribution for categorical variables)

Men Women

M SD M SD

Religiosity Index (1-5)1 3.54 1.06 3.81 0.94

Benefits of children Index (1-5)2 3.89 0.60 3.97 0.60

Costs of children Index (1-5)2 3.18 0.74 3.15 0.78

Age 31.82 9.93 30.90 8.38 N % N % Fertility intentions No 1837 79.1 2012 78.3 Yes 486 20.9 557 21.7 Number of children 0 1233 53.1 1041 40.5 1 350 15.1 509 19.8 2 or more 740 31.9 1019 39.7 Partnership status Partnered 1152 49.6 1490 58.0 No partner 1171 50.4 1079 42.0

Education level Low 1039 44.7 756 29.4

Medium 819 35.3 1009 39.3

High 465 20.0 804 31.3

Total N 2323 2569

1 Higher index values indicate higher religiosity

2 Higher index values indicate higher benefits or costs of having children

Perceived benefits and costs as a mediator for the effect of religiosity on fertility intentions

The results of all mediation analyses were summarized in Table 4.3 and 4.4. In the upper part of Table 4.3, the results of the mediation analysis were summarized and the total effect of religiosity was split up into the direct effect and the indirect effect (the part that was mediated by perceived costs and benefits). The lower part of that table shows the share of the total effect

(15)

106

that was mediated by each mediator separately and by both combined. Table 4.4 shows the associations between religiosity and the two mediator variables of perceived costs and benefits.

Table 4.3: Separation of the total effect of religiosity on short-term fertility intentions (over the next three years) into the direct effect and the effect mediated through perceived benefits/costs (mediation analysis)

Men Women

Determinants of fertility intentions Logit p Logit p Benefits of children Index (1-5) 0.589 <0.001 0.620 <0.001 Costs of children Index (1-5) -0.214 0.008 -0.275 <0.001 Religiosity Total effect 0.215 <0.001 0.267 <0.001 Direct effect 0.140 0.016 0.173 0.008 Indirect effect 0.076 <0.001 0.094 <0.001

N 2323 2569

% %

Proportion mediated through p. benefits 31.2 29.9 Proportion mediated through p. costs 3.9 5.4

Proportion mediated in total 35.1 35.3

All models controlled for: age, age squared, parity (number of children), partnership status, education level

Table 4.4: Linear regression of the relationship between religiosity and the two mediator variables (perceived costs and perceived benefits)

Men Women

Determinants of perceived benefits Beta p Beta p Religiosity Index (1-5) 0.201 <0.001 0.203 <0.001 Determinants of perceived costs Beta p Beta p Religiosity Index (1-5) -0.056 0.008 -0.064 0.001

All models controlled for: age, age squared, parity (number of children), partnership status, education level

Table 4.3 shows a significant positive total effect of religiosity on men’s fertility intentions. Around 65% of that total effect was not mediated by perceived benefits and costs of having children. The remaining 35.1% of the total effect could be explained by the mediation effect of the perceived costs and benefits. Most of this mediation was carried by perceived benefits of having children: more religious men perceived higher benefits to having children, which can be seen in Table 4.4. These perceived benefits themselves were associated with higher fertility intentions, as shown in Table 4.3. This pathway explained 31.2% of the total effect of religiosity on intentions and constituted a large part of the mediation effect. The other mediation pathway went through perceived costs and explained the remaining 3.9% of the total effect and therefore only a small part of the mediation. Even though the effect was much weaker, more religious men showed higher fertility intentions also because they perceived less costs of having children

(16)

107 (Table 4.4) and higher perceived costs themselves were associated with lower fertility intentions (Table 4.3).

As to women, their results resembled those of men. Overall, women’s fertility intentions were also higher among the more religious. Again, this total positive effect of religiosity was split up into the direct effect, which accounted for 65% of the total effect, and the indirect (mediated) effect, which was responsible for the remaining 35%. There were some slight differences in the distribution of the mediation effect in comparison to men: around 30% of the effect of religiosity on fertility intentions was mediated by the fact that more religious women saw higher benefits in having children, which then were associated with higher fertility intentions. Compared to men, the share of the total effect mediated by the perceived costs, again associated with lower fertility intentions, was higher for women and stood at 5.4%.

All in all, the results showed that a part, but not all, of the positive effect of religiosity on fertility intentions could be explained by more religious people perceiving children as more beneficial. Notably, the proportion that was mediated was almost identical between men and women. Furthermore, only a very small part of that overall effect could be explained by religious people perceiving lower costs of having children. Among women, perceived costs explained a slightly larger part of the total effect than among men.

The moderating impact of religiosity

Tables 4.5 and 4.6 show the result of the models on the moderating impact of religiosity on the effect of perceived benefits and costs on fertility intentions for men and women, respectively. These tables are accompanied by Figure 4.1, showing the predicted probabilities of fertility intentions for all calculated interaction effects in the models.

The results of the first model mirrored those found in the mediation analysis and demonstrated that men who perceived higher benefits of having children and more religious men were significantly more likely to intend to have a(nother) child over the next three years than men who perceived lower benefits and less religious men, respectively. In Model 2, an interaction term between religiosity and perceived benefits was introduced. This interaction term was not significant however, and Figure 4.1 shows that fertility intentions increased with higher perceived benefits irrespectively of the level of religiosity.

(17)

108

Table 4.5: Results of the logistic moderation analysis on the effects on short-term fertility intentions (over the next three years) for men

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

Logit p Logit p Logit p Logit p

Religiosity Index (1-5) 0.150 0.009 0.452 0.22 0.205 <0.001 0.018 0.938 Benefits of children Index (1-5) 0.577 <0.001 0.846 0.014

Costs of children Index (1-5) -0.187 0.019 -0.402 0.144

Rel. × Benefits -0.078 0.406

Rel. × Costs 0.060 0.414

Age 0.776 <0.001 0.776 <0.001 0.768 <0.001 0.767 <0.001 Age squared -0.013 <0.001 -0.013 <0.001 -0.013 <0.001 -0.012 <0.001

Number of children 0 (ref.)

1 -0.881 <0.001 -0.885 <0.001 -0.818 <0.001 -0.818 <0.001 2 or more -3.076 <0.001 -3.075 <0.001 -2.948 <0.001 -2.949 <0.001

Partnership status No partner (ref.)

Partnered 1.325 <0.001 1.331 <0.001 1.363 <0.001 1.363 <0.001

Education level Low (ref.)

Medium 0.076 0.591 0.073 0.604 0.101 0.469 0.101 0.472 High 0.628 <0.001 0.633 <0.001 0.603 <0.001 0.605 <0.001 Constant -15.09 <0.001 -16.12 <0.001 -12.33 <0.001 -11.64 <0.001

N 2323 2323 2323 2323

Pseudo R² 0.243 0.243 0.232 0.233

Model 3 assessed the impact of religiosity and perceived costs of having children on fertility intentions. Similarly to the first model, there was a significant positive effect of religiosity. In line with expectations, higher perceived costs of children significantly reduced fertility intentions. The last model again incorporated an interaction term, this time between religiosity and perceived costs. While that interaction effect was positive, which would be in line with the theoretical expectations, it was not significant.

(18)

109

Figure 4.1: Predicted probabilities of intending to have a(nother) child over the next three years according to religiosity and perceived benefits or costs of having children (graphical representation of all interaction effects)

Per ce iv ed co n seq u en ce s o f p ar en th o o d an d f er tili ty in ten tio n s in Po lan d

(19)

110

Table 4.6: Results of the logistic moderation analysis on the effects on short-term fertility intentions (over the next three years) for women

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

Logit p Logit p Logit p Logit p

Religiosity Index (1-5) 0.190 0.003 0.080 0.839 0.245 <0.001 -0.324 0.191 Benefits of children Index (1-5) 0.625 <0.001 0.520 0.174

Costs of children Index (1-5) -0.278 <0.001 -0.980 0.001

Rel. × Benefits 0.028 0.774

Rel. × Costs 0.182 0.018

Age 0.767 <0.001 0.767 <0.001 0.778 <0.001 0.780 <0.001 Age squared -0.014 <0.001 -0.014 <0.001 -0.014 <0.001 -0.014 <0.001 Number of children 0 (ref.)

1 -1.185 <0.001 -1.183 <0.001 -1.077 <0.001 -1.081 <0.001 2 or more -3.534 <0.001 -3.534 <0.001 -3.372 <0.001 -3.383 <0.001 Partnership status No partner (ref.)

Partnered 1.832 <0.001 1.831 <0.001 1.839 <0.001 1.841 <0.001 Education level Low (ref.)

Medium 0.257 0.114 0.258 0.113 0.250 0.123 0.255 0.116 High 0.825 <0.001 0.825 <0.001 0.785 <0.001 0.801 <0.001 Constant -14.49 <0.001 -14.07 <0.001 -11.42 <0.001 -9.27 <0.001

N 2569 2569 2569 2569

Pseudo R² 0.296 0.296 0.287 0.290

In the case of women, the results in the first two models resembled those of men. Again, there was a significant positive effect of religiosity and perceived benefits of having children on fertility intentions. The interaction term was not significant, and Figure 4.1 shows only marginal differences in the relationship between perceived benefits and fertility intentions by the level of religiosity, akin to the situation of men. Model 3 repeated the findings for men as well – a significant negative effect of perceived costs of having children on the likelihood of intending to have a(nother) child over the next three years. In the fourth and last model, the interaction effect between religiosity and perceived costs was introduced. For women, this interaction term was positive and significant. It indicated that the negative effect of perceived costs on fertility intentions was weaker for more religious women than for less religious women. Figure 4.1 shows that for highly religious women, fertility intentions stayed at a relatively high level independently of the amount of costs perceived. For less-religious women on the other hand, fertility intentions dropped considerably as the perceived costs increased.

Summarizing the findings on the moderation analysis, it follows that only in the case of women, higher religiosity lead to a lower impact of perceived costs on fertility intentions. For men, no significant effect was observed. Furthermore, there were no moderation effects of religiosity on the impact of perceived benefits of having children neither for men nor women, indicating that

(20)

111 higher perceived benefits increased the likelihood of intending to have a(nother) child irrespectively of religiosity.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate mechanisms behind the positive influence of religiosity on fertility intentions that has been observed in many countries (Philipov and Berghammer, 2007; Bein et al., 2017). Following theoretical considerations and previous research on this topic (Brose, 2006; Arránz Becker and Lois, 2017), two possible mechanisms linking religiosity, perceived costs and benefits of having children, and fertility intentions were examined. First, it was hypothesized that religions influence how people perceive costs and benefits of having children: religious individuals might perceive higher benefits and lower costs of parenthood and consequently, they might be more likely to intend a(nother) child compared to their less religious counterparts. In other words, it was hypothesized that the effect of religiosity on childbearing intentions is mediated by the perceived costs and benefits of children (H1).

As for the second possible mechanism, it was postulated that religiosity moderates how perceived costs translate into fertility intentions. It was hypothesized that the negative effect of perceived costs on fertility intentions may be weaker among the more religious, meaning that for the more religious, perceptions of cost play only a small role in the formation of their fertility intentions (H2). To get a full picture, the moderation effect of religiosity was tested also in relation to how perceptions of benefits impact fertility intentions.

The results indicated that around one third of the effect of religiosity on fertility intentions was mediated by perceived costs and benefits for both men and women, bringing some support to Hypothesis 1. Two additional findings appeared: Firstly, while both perceived costs and benefits played a role in the mediation effect, perceived benefits mediated a lion’s share of the effect. This suggests that religions mostly encourage their followers to have children by emphasizing benefits of parenthood, for example by promoting children as a blessing and stressing their emotional values (Catechism 2366-2379). Those findings were generally in line with the two previous studies on fertility behaviour in Germany (Brose, 2006; Arránz Becker and Lois, 2017).

Secondly, a substantial direct effect of religiosity on fertility intentions remained which was not explained through the perceived benefits and costs of having children. On the one hand, it is

(21)

112

possible that the selection of items on perceived costs and benefits of children used in the Polish survey did not cover all relevant aspects of parenthood. For example, fulfilling expectations of significant others or continuing the family among the benefits, and increased instability of partnership or possible hardships for the partner among the costs were not part of the questionnaire used in this study (Miller, 1995; Guedes et al., 2015). If religious people attach more importance to those missed aspects, part of the mediation effect could have remained hidden in the presented analyses. Another possible explanation is that the remaining direct effect stands for the influence of the particularized theology, which represents direct pronatalist religious teachings eminent in the Catholic Church. Those pronatalist teachings may exert an independent normative pressure on religious individuals, prompting them to have children regardless of their personal beliefs on positive or negative aspects of parenthood. That way, the findings gave support for the notion that particularized theology still plays a non-negligible role in the overall impact of religiosity on fertility.

As for the second examined mechanism, the study brings only limited support to Hypothesis 2. It was found that religiosity moderated the impact of the perceived costs of childbearing on fertility intentions only for women. In line with the expectations, the results revealed that the negative effect of perceived costs on fertility intentions becomes weaker as religiosity increases. Among very religious women, perceived costs of children had only a small effect on fertility intentions, while for the least religious women, perceived costs were a strong negative determinant of intentions. In other words, highly religious women seem to be inclined to have children regardless of any costs children may carry for them. No such effect was found for men. Several possible explanations may be applied here. For instance, it is possible that women either benefit or believe that they can benefit more than men from practical and emotional support in religious communities when a child is born (e.g. by family members, friends, religious groups and institutions). It is also possible that the overall negative impact of perceived costs on intentions is weaker for men, thus leaving fewer possibilities for likely interactions. Some of the items on costs included in the Polish survey are also more important for women (pregnancy is strenuous, difficult to combine work and childbearing for women) and thus, they impact men’s fertility intentions indirectly and probably to a lower degree. Furthermore, even the items that apply to men directly (e.g., having less time for leisure or for a spouse) may not impact them as much as they impact women in a country like Poland, where traditional gender roles are still widespread and women are expected to provide most of the care and time for children (Mills, 2010; Szelewa, 2017). In that situation, more religious women may count on help from

(22)

113 their religions and social networks that support them in their decision to have large families regardless of the costs. In a more egalitarian gender context like Germany on the other hand, evidence of a moderating effect was found for both men and women (Arránz Becker and Lois, 2017).

The picture that emerged in this study is that the effect of religiosity on fertility is mostly mediated through perceived benefits of parenthood, while the moderating effect of religiosity relates to the perceived costs. Specifically, religions seem to amplify perceived benefits of having children and at the same time limit the impact of costs related to childbearing. Furthermore, the study demonstrated that these mechanisms are visible for fertility intentions already, pointing to the role of religiosity at the early stage of the fertility decision making process. A future avenue of research may thus be to examine the fertility decision making process in more detail. First, fertility intentions could be analysed together with their realization in order to verify whether high religiosity leads to having more children by helping people to cope with high costs regarding childbearing (the second part of the mechanism). Second, the process could also be examined by parity, since previous research on determinants of fertility intentions suggested some parity-specific effects (Philipov and Berghammer, 2007). Due to limitations in the sample size, it was not possible to carry out the analysis by parity in this study.

In future and more detailed analyses, it would also be crucial to consider different categories of perceived benefits and costs of children. While costs and benefits were modelled here as single dimensions, it is possible that different categories, like direct economic consequences, opportunity costs or non-economic costs and benefits might have varying effects for religious and non-religious people. Making a distinction between different types of costs and benefits of children could allow depicting more refined mechanisms in which religiosity shapes fertility. Importantly, these mechanisms might play out differently in various economic contexts. While the current study took place during a relatively good economic situation in Poland, the monetary and indirect costs of children may be perceived in a different way under more challenging economic conditions.

Overall, future studies need to investigate whether the relationships described here hold true across time and space. Longitudinal studies and cross-country comparisons would also shed light on the question whether the identified mechanisms can be applied across other cultural settings. In very secular countries for instance, where highly religious people are a more

(23)

114

selected group and religion has less influence on society, the nature of these mechanisms may be different than in Poland or Germany.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Het aantal geoogste vruchten in de proefkas stijgt tot week 22 ten opzichte van de referentiekas (Figuur 27). Daarna blijft het verschil stabiel, terwijl na week 29 het

Religiosity and Reproductive Decisions in Europe Copyright © Christoph Bein 2020.. All

While the first one examines whether cross-country differences in the strength of the association between religiosity and fertility intentions could be explained by the

We construct models on the influence of religiosity, gender attitudes and the gender distribution of household tasks on the intention to have a(nother) child as well as

And finally, the gender regime might be more relevant in determining the role of religiosity for fertility intentions among men due to differences between egalitarian

Hieruit kan afgelei word dat middelvlakbestuurders (i.e. skoolhoofde) meer sires ervaar. • Slegs die manlike geslag ervaar sires. Op die huidige stadium is dit 'n

3-left shows a typical unfiltered 2D velocity distri- bution of a single orbitally-shaken particle tracked in the reactor, characterised by a mean of 33.42(3) cm s −1 and.. a

First we deploy a focused literature study into the specific problem setting, but combine and share our findings with an extensive research project case study in the logistic