• No results found

Profiling Compulsive Buyers at Risk: Demographics, Behavioral Buying Characteristics and Buyer Satisfaction

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Profiling Compulsive Buyers at Risk: Demographics, Behavioral Buying Characteristics and Buyer Satisfaction"

Copied!
51
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Profiling Compulsive Buyers at Risk:

Demographics, Behavioral Buying

Characteristics and Buyer Satisfaction

Abstract

The current study adopts a consumer welfare perspective with the purpose of profiling and

identifying compulsive buyers before their behavior spins out of control. While building forward

on existing literature on demographical variables, buying behavior characteristics and buyer

satisfaction measures are introduced as potential new identifiers for compulsive buyers.

Demographical variables illustrate limited usefulness while both behavioral buying characteristics

and buyer satisfaction show potential. Compulsive buyers have a high frequency of buying

episodes, while the time and amount of money spend per episode seems irrelevant. Furthermore,

compulsive buyers have lower levels of satisfaction towards the good/service consumed and its

provider.

Lars de Goede

(2)

Introduction

“It’s almost like you’re on a drunk. You’re so intoxicated;… I got this great high. It was like you couldn’t have given me more of a rush”.

A response from one of O’Guinn and Faber’s (1989, p 153) interviewees on the seemingly innocent

topic of compulsive buying. However, one with a striking resemblance to the topic of substance

abuse, where the term innocence is be far from applicable. Some more of the experiences and

emotions from O’Guinn and Faber (1989, p. 154) interviewees’ illustrate the extent to which

compulsive buying manifests itself in consumers’ minds.

“I couldn’t tell you what I bought or where I bought it. It was like I was on automatic”. “I never bought one of anything, I always buy at least two. I still do. I can never go even

to the Jewel and buy one quart of milk. I’ve always got to buy two.”

The bottom line of these quotes is that compulsive consumer behaviors are more extreme forms of

regular consumer behavior, driven by more extreme and emotional motivations (Hirschman,

1992). They also illustrate that compulsivity in decision making goes way beyond the generally

assumed rational acting consumer, the consumer making decisions based on costs and benefits.

The question is when normal motivations and afterwards experienced positive feelings and

gratification become so intense that they can be labelled abnormal (Faber, O’Guinn & Krych,

1987). The following and final two quote’s shine a light into the topic of the potential practical

implications of compulsive buying (O’Guinn and Faber, 1989 p. 155).

(3)

“I didn’t have one person in the world I could talk to. I don’t drink. I don’t smoke. I don’t do dope. But I can’t stop. I can’t control it. I said I can’t go on like this…. My husband

hates me. My kids hate me. I’ve destroyed everything. I was ashamed and I just wanted to die.”

Although the first of these two quotes also has a laughable aspect, the second one assures the

jocular part of the story wears in a scenario where consumers start losing control and the severe

potential implications on an individual’s life become apparent. Looking at the opening quote in

hindsight, for consumers suffering from heavy compulsiveness their buying behavior indeed has

an abuse potential similar to substance addiction (Faber, O’Guinn & Krych, 1987). Koran et. al.

(2006, p. 1806) provides a brief summarizing list of possible consequences of compulsive buying: “guilt or remorse, excessive debt, bankruptcy, family conflict, divorce, illegal activities, such as

writing bad checks and embezzlement, and even suicide attempts”.

Ever since Faber and O’Guinn started to quantify the magnitude of compulsive consumption in

1989, considerable parts of studied populations are found to be represented by people that consume

compulsively in the way described before. At the time, they found the incidence of compulsive

buying to be 5.9%. Over twenty years of research afterwards lead to both more and less

conservative conclusions about the incidence, or prevalence as others name it. Summarizing the

results creates a range of a compulsive buying incidence between 1.8% and 16% (Dittmar, 2005; Faber & O’Guinn, 1992; Hassay & Smith, 1996; Koran et. al., 2006; Magee, 1994; Mueller et al.,

2010). The most moderate estimate translates into over three hundred thousand compulsive buyers

in The Netherlands and around 6 million in the United States. “Clearly, even the most restrictive

(4)

behavior” (Dittmar, 2005 p. 469). Thus, next to its severe consequences, compulsive buying is also

widespread, making the phenomenon far from innocent.

The paper by Kraepelin (1915) that is often seen as the first contribution to academic research on

the topic of irrational overconsumption dates from over 100 years ago, indicating the presence of

the topic of on scholars’ agendas for a considerable period of time. During the late 80’s begin 90’s,

when the stream of literature started to increase, scholars realized the implications of a considerable amount of compulsive buyers amongst us: “it is in the interest of both people who

suffer from compulsive spending and society in general to try to reduce the incidence of this problem” (Faber, O’Guinn & Krych, 1987 p. 135). However, a “chief unaddressed question” to

achieve this reduction, asked by Faber and O’guinn (1992 p. 460) nearly 25 years ago; “how one

can best identify those at risk for compulsive buying”, still has not been answered since literature

on this topic is limited to a range of papers looking into a restricted selection of aspects on which

to profile and identify compulsive buyers (e.g.: a selection of demographics). The bulk of literature

focusses on psychological aspects in order to unravel the black box of compulsive buying.

Although that aspect is important in creating a clear and complete image of what compulsive

buying entails and what goes on in compulsive buyers’ minds, psychological processes are not

readily observable and are often hard to grasp. They thereby do not practically assist in the

identification of those buyers that are at risk.

To address this unanswered question, this paper adopts a consumer welfare perspective, moving

the focus from the psychological to the identification part of compulsive buying research. Thereby

the timely identification of risk category buyers through observable aspects is of focal attention

here. “The dimensions which seem most important to include in building these typologies are

(5)

what types of things are bought, when they are bought and for whom they are purchased” (Faber,

O’Guinn & Krych, 1987 p. 134). So far no groundbreaking additions have been made to this list.

Therefore, the earlier mentioned demographics will be exploited further, but moreover, two novel

dimensions for profiling and identifying compulsive buyers are introduced. Multiple behavioral

buying characteristics as well as several buyer satisfaction measures will serve as new potential

anchors for the profiling and identification of those compulsive buyers at risk. Most importantly,

the exterior nature of these three dimensions of profiling provides the opportunity of an

identification process which is possible during buying episodes and inside buying environments,

something vital for consumer welfare in the topic of compulsive buying. Their potential thus

allows for timely identification and assistance of those buyers at risk before debt becomes too high,

social relationships become damaged beyond repair and physical or any other harm is done. Taking

this a step further, successful identification eventually allows for compulsive buyer assistance in

managing their behavior. Even more importantly, it provides clues for clear managerial

implications towards the creation of less provoking marketing designs (Prentice & Woodside,

2013) aimed at the prevention of compulsive buying escalation.

Lastly, in contrast to virtually all research concerning the topic of compulsive buying, the data

gathered for this research is gathered inside a retail environment. Seven of the largest casinos in

Macau, by far the largest gaming destination in the world, participated in the collection of data

from respondents in an actual consumption setting. In comparison, selft-reported mail surveys,

(6)

Literature

Discussing compulsivity in the marketplace in the light of consumer welfare implies discussing a

number of subtopics. Next to defining the phenomenon itself, its (partly) malignant nature, its

potential dangerous consequences, and its prevalence, the dimensions on which profiling

consumers at risk is done will be discussed. These topics will be discussed in order of appearance

in the following section.

Rational Consumer vs. Compulsive Consumer: the Reasonability of the Assumption of Rationality

As touched upon in the introduction, “Over the last 50 years, the theory of rational choice has

emerged as the dominant paradigm of quantitative research in the social and economic sciences.

The idea that individuals make choices by rationally weighing values and uncertainties (or that

they ought to, or at least act as if they do) is central to Bayesian methods of statistical inference

and decision analysis; the theory of games of strategy; theories of competitive markets, industrial

organization, and asset pricing; the theory of social choice; and a host of rational actor models in political science, sociology, law, philosophy, and management science” (Nau, 1999 p. 1). Rational

choice models are derived from utilitarian theory (Cook & Levi, 1990) and therefore assume the

weighing of costs and benefits of available alternatives (Hoch & Loewenstein, 1991). Consumers

evaluate and process the attributes of these alternatives, make trade-offs on their importance and

finally choose an alternative based on self-developed decision rules (Bettman, 1979).

Solely leaning on the philosophy of rationality neglects another important aspect in the human

decision-making process. As Hall (2002 p. 23-24) notes, “the weakness of these approaches is in

(7)

Or, as Poels and Dewitte (2006 p. 3) frame it, the study on emotions “has led to the general

conception that emotions are not a useless by-product but are essential for rational thinking and

behavior”. Hoch and Loewenstein (1991 p. 492) justly recognize that “a more complete

understanding of consumer behavior must recognize that people are influenced by both long-term

rational concerns and more short-term emotional factors.”

The above mentioned authors try to emphasize the existence of an interplay between rational and

emotional motives in consumer decision making, an interplay in which emotions possibly become

powerful enough to override cognitional reasoning in the decision making process (Zajonc, 1980).

Especially those emotional factors play an important role in the decision making process in the

mind of the compulsive consumer, illustrated by the irrationality of their decisions.

Via Emotions to Compulsivity

Defining compulsive buying is leaned on relatively recent work by Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney and

Monroe (2008) supported by a stream of roughly 15 years of research by Hollander and colleagues

(1993; 1993; 1995; 2005; 2006) and McElroy, Phillips and Keck (2004) on the

obsessive-compulsive spectrum. This stream of research incorporates aspects of both obsessive-obsessive-compulsive

(OCD) and impulse-control disorders (ICD) into the concept of compulsive buying. It is therefore formulated as “a consumers tendency to be preoccupied with buying that is revealed through

repetitive buying and lack of impulse control over buying” (Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney & Monroe,

2008, p. 622).

Consumers with compulsive inclinations feel emotionally compelled to act in a certain manner

(Robbins et. al. 2012). They experience obsessive thoughts caused by anxiety or distress

(8)

of the associated tension through short term pleasurable behavior (Black, Shaw & Blum, 2010)

that is sometimes experienced as a “high”, a term familiarized in the field of narcotics but also

found in testimonials on related phenomena like compulsive shopping (Dittmar & Drury, 2000).

Taking the concept of experiencing a “high” a step further, a share of respondents of 11% in

Schlosser et. al. (1994) experience shopping as sexually stimulating. Faber, O’Guinn and Krych,

(1987) and Hirschman (1992) both mention the effectiveness of these experienced positive

emotions as a self-treatment for unhappiness.

While compulsive behaviors are seemingly purposeful in providing relief, they are in fact highly

ego-dystonic (American Psychiatric Association, 1985 p. 234, 1994, 2000) since consumers

suffering from compulsions logically realize their behaviors or rituals are harmful but they still

feel emotionally compelled to pursue them (Robbins et. al. 2012). The numbers from Schlosser et.

al. (1994) underpin this, 72% of their respondents are concerned with their frequency of buying,

85% with the debt related to their compulsive buying behavior and 74% with the fact that they

experience a loss of control while shopping. Another point illustrating the awareness of the

negative effects of compulsive buying is the higher interest in store return policies and the higher

frequency and volume of returned products under compulsive buyers compared to non-compulsive

buyers as found by Hassay and Smith (1996).

Eventually compulsive consumers find themselves in a situation of inner conflicts between the

presence of a drive, an impulse or an urge, which create a (sudden) desire to act in a certain way

on the one side, and the willpower to override this desire on the other side. Typically, the

compulsive consumer falls short in employing self-control and starts the process of simultaneous

augmentation of the short term benefits and downplaying or denying the long term (harmful)

(9)

& Blum, 2010; Faber, O’Guinn & Krych, 1987; O’Guinn & Faber 1989; Hoch & Loewenstein 91;

Hirschman, 1992; Nataraajan & Goff, 1991). Often it is only the afterwards confrontation with the

harmful consequences of made compulsive decisions that feeds motivation to (temporary) stop

behaving compulsively (Black, Shaw & Blum, 2010).

Once behaving compulsively, it is perhaps the consciousness within the compulsive

decision-making process that ensures that consumers attempt to set up clear boundaries to limit their own

compulsive behavior (Hoch & Loewenstein, 1991), creating a process in which decisions are made ritualistically “performed according to certain rules or in a stereotyped fashion” (American

Psychiatric Association, 1985 p. 234, 1994, 2000). Experiencing the negative downside of

compulsivity however adds emotional distress to the already existing tension eventually causing

rules and limits to be broken and compulsive behavior to become repetitive and uncontrollable (Hirschman, 1992). “Thus, it appears that it is extremely difficult for compulsive consumers to

become noncompulsive” (Hirschman, 1992 p. 166). When eventually the outcome of the process

becomes chronically compulsive and consumers develop a physically or psychologically

dependency on the substance or activity relevant in their case, the compulsive character of the

individuals’ pattern of consumption fully manifests itself (Faber, O’Guinn & Krych, 1987;

Hirschman, 1992; O’Guinn & Faber, 1989).

Summing up, the difference between the normal and the compulsive consumer is far more than an

alarmingly higher frequency of consumption observed on the surface, but rather lies in divergent,

tension related motivations that go beyond the desire for material acquisition (O’Guinn & Faber,

(10)

Compulsive Buying, the Buying Part

This paper uses a wide perspective in discussing compulsive buying, a phenomenon occurring in

the goods as well as the services industry, and which can be triggered in, or by all stages of the

process that lead to and follow a transaction. In other words, the pre-buying phase: anticipation,

preparation and shopping (Black, 2007), the eventual spending/buying phase, and/or the

post-buying phase: owning, using or experiencing (O’Guinn & Faber, 1989) all potentially play a role

in triggering compulsivity.

How Bad Can it Get?

In concrete terms, chronically compulsive behavior leads to excessive acquisition and/or use of

goods or services. Depending on the nature of the good or service consumed, considerable negative

economic, psychological and societal effects (O’guinn & Faber, 1989) threaten and interrupt the

day to day life of the consumer (Faber, O’Guinn & Krych, 1987; Nataraajan & Goff, 1991). The

snowball effect starts when consumers spend beyond their disposable income and debt starts to

accumulate. Practices like overextending credit lines, an increasing number of credit cards used

and forced sales of property become necessary to free up financial space (Faber, O’Guinn & Krych, 1987; O’guinn & Faber, 1989). Simultaneously, feelings of guilt, remorse, anxiety, lower

self-esteem and the fear of justification by others increase psychological pressure (Faber, O’Guinn &

Krych, 1987; Hirschman, 1992; O’guinn & Faber, 1989). In a more extreme manner this leads to

the hiding of compulsive purchases or activities and criminal undertakings like theft and

embezzlement to be able to support compulsive behavior (Hirschman, 1992). The harmful

consequences extend beyond the consumers’ self when his or hers dysfunctioning economically

and socially puts pressure on an individual’s family, friends and occupational environment too, as

(11)

care system and crime control (Krych, 1989). In the worst case, compulsive behavior evolves into

situations described by either suicidal intentions due to feelings of loneliness and disintegration as

a consequence of social isolation, or heavy physical consequences of substance abuse, both

possibly causing death (Hirschman, 1992).

How Common is Compulsive Buying?

To illustrate the extent to which the phenomenon of compulsive buying spreads among society, a

brief chronological summary on findings about its prevalence is presented. Faber and O’Guinn

(1989; 1992) found the incidence of compulsive buying to be 5.9% and later between 1.8% and

8.1%. Magee (1994), using the same classification, came to the conclusion of a fraction of 15% to

16% of respondents that she labeled as compulsive consumers. Hassay and Smith (1996)

determined the incidence to be 12.2%. Dittmar (2005) draws a similar conclusion with a

percentage of 13.4 of individuals that can be labeled compulsive buyers. Koran and colleagues

(2006) were the first to employ a large sample of 2513 respondents. It lead them to the conclusion

of a prevalence of compulsive buying of 5.8%, more in line with the first insights provided by Faber and O’Guinn (1989; 1992). Most recently, Mueller et. al. (2010) joined this range of

incidences with their point prevalence of 6.9% within a German population.

Note that the epidemiological terms incidence and prevalence are used intertwined by the authors

referred to here, both simply denoting to the fraction of carriers within the population at the given

moment of measurement.

Profiling and Identifying the Compulsive Buyer

Since the theoretical purpose of this paper lies in profiling and identifying compulsive buyers in

(12)

demographics, behavioral buying characteristics and buyer satisfaction measures. The specific

aspects under each of these dimensions, as well as their hypothesized effect will be discussed in

the following section. With a consumer welfare point of view, the profiling aspects have been

chosen so that they share an exterior nature. They are publically observable to a large extend,

which makes identification possible in a stage early enough to ensure potential harmful damage to

the compulsive buyer in question can be prevented.

Demographics

Age

O’guinn and Faber (1989) are the first that quantitatively investigated the relationship between age

and compulsive buying. In comparing their general sample against their abnormal compulsive

group, age was found to be significantly lower in the latter, indicating a negative relationship. In

follow up research in the years after similar results are found, e.g., negative correlation between age and compulsive buying (d’Astous, 1990; d’Astous, Maltais & Roberge, 1990; Magee, 1994).

However, Sherhorn, Reisch and Raab (1990), failing to find a significant age difference in

comparing their compulsive with their normal sample, and Roberts (1998), producing a lack of

correlation between age and compulsive buying, couldn’t confirm these results within their

samples. A more recent stream of research joins the earliest and confirms a negative relationship

between age and compulsive buying, either by correlational analysis or comparing means (Dittmar,

2005; Koran et. al., 2006; Mueller et. al., 2010; Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney & Monroe, 2008).

Summarizing, the first hypothesis is proposed as follows:

(13)

Gender

One of the most vivid stereotypes about compulsive buying is that it’s a female matter in particular (Faber, O’Guinn & Krych, 1987). Except for a couple of authors concluding an insignificant

difference between men and women (Koran et. al., 2006; Magee, 1994; Mueller et. al., 2010), most

literature tends to support the basis for this stereotype in proving the higher compulsive buying

tendencies of females in contrast to males either by comparing groups or applying correlational

analysis (d’Astous, 1990; d’Astous, Maltais & Roberge, 1990; Christenson et. al., 1994; Dittmar, 2005; Faber & O’Guinn, 1992; O’Guinn & Faber, 1989; Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney & Monroe,

2008; Roberts, 1998; Scherhorn, Reisch & Raab, 1990).

H2: Females have higher compulsive buying tendencies than males.

An important side note considering gender is the fact that the data for analysis used in this paper

finds its origin in a casino environment. Within the gambling literature results in relation to gender

generally are the opposite as proposed above as it is said to be a male thing, although this

supposedly is a diminishing trend (Black, Shaw & Blum, 2010). Thus, this could influence results

towards the opposite than hypothesized direction.

Education and Income

Literature concerning these two demographics is relatively less abundant as for the preceding two

within the field of compulsive buying behavior. Generally, higher education ensures better job

opportunities, which in turn should go accompanied by a higher income. Thus, multicollinearity

in connecting them to the compulsive buying spectrum could arise. Different theories exist

however, in which they do not necessarily affect compulsive buying in the same direction. Higher

(14)

compulsive buying. Income, however, could positively drive compulsive buying as higher income

provides room to spend more without experiencing the negative downsides of it.

When it comes to education two contradictory results show up in existing (relatively recent)

scientific literature. Dittmar (2005) found an absence of a significant relationship between

education and compulsive buying. Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney and Monroe (2008), on the other hand,

confirm a negative relationship as sketched before. Following the latter, hypothesis three is

formulated as follows:

H3: Education drives compulsive buying negatively.

In contrast to education, income gets considerably more attention. Results are pointing in different

directions. Ironically, most research in the past decades agrees on the absence of a relationship

between income and compulsive buying (Christenson et. al., 1994; Dittmar, 2005; O’Guinn &

Faber, 1989; Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney & Monroe, 2008; Roberts, 1998; Scherhorn, Reisch & Raab,

1990). Contradictory results have been found however. Black et. al. (2001) note that greater

severity of compulsive buying is associated with lower income. Koran et. al. (2006), in the same

philosophy, claim that compulsive buyers have lower income compared to other respondents. In contrast to all former mentioned results, d’Astous (1990) found an inverted U-shape relationship

between income and compulsive buying, pointing at middle incomes to be driving compulsivity.

In formulating hypothesis four, the base is formed by the more recent results by Black et. al. (2001)

and Koran et. al. (2006) that indicate:

(15)

Behavioral Buying Characteristics

The idea behind the concept of behavioral buying characteristics and the connection to compulsive

buying lies in the higher buying frequency/severity observed for compulsive buyers. They are

argued to drive compulsive buying in the sense that for consumers that have experienced that their

buying behavior provides them a high (Dittmar and Drury, 2000) or emotional lift (O’guinn and

Faber, 1989, Christenson et. al., 1994), the fact that they more easily become loyalty/reward

program members or find themselves in buying situations more frequently, for longer periods and

spend an increasing amount of money, indicates that they are deteriorating into a compulsive

buying habit. The behavioral buying characteristics therefore serve as signals that consumers are

(becoming) compulsive buyers.

For the first aspect under this dimension, loyalty/reward program membership, there is a much

discussed endogeneity problem potentially leading to overestimation of its effect (Bolton, Lemon

& Verhoef, 2004). E.g.: compulsive buyers could be theorized to become loyalty/reward program

members more often or quicker than normal buyers because they are more interested in the benefits

the program offers. However, being a loyalty/reward program member, as proposed here, could

also drive compulsive buying in the sense that the benefits the program offers triggers buying

behavior. Theorizing similarly, the endogeneity problem could also hold for the other three aspects

under the topic of behavioral buying characteristics; duration, frequency and expenditure height.

Where being a compulsive buyer could drive buying more often, spending more money and shop

for longer periods, these three aspects could also cause consumers to lapse into a compulsive

buying habit. The existence of the endogeneity issue and the ongoing discussion is noted. Here it

(16)

Therefore, all four behavioral buying characteristics are treated as antecedents and modeled as

such.

Loyalty/Reward Program Membership

Loyalty as a concept has been virtually unanimously approached as a means aimed at generating

more profit from consumers. From the standpoint of consumer welfare, loyalty programs are

absent in literature to date. Theorizing therefore is inferred from other branches of research.

Loyalty or rewards programs are aimed at increasing the perceived value of the relationship for

the consumer though the provision of free benefits by the product or service provider (Bolton,

Lemon & Verhoef, 2004), often as a reward for their repeated purchases in the past and present

(Meyer-Waarden, 2008). It is this surplus that should ensure repeated visitations and eventual

loyalty. Indeed, authors find support for this theory. Positive connections have been found between

loyalty programs and consumers share of wallet/expenditures (Leenheer et. al., 2007;

Meyer-Waarden, 2007), between loyalty programs and customer retention/lifetime (Meyer-Meyer-Waarden,

2007; Verhoef, 2003), and between loyalty programs and purchase frequencies, basket value and

lower inter-purchase time (Meyer-Waarden, 2008). This teaches us that loyalty or reward program

membership could drive consumers to become heavier, potentially compulsive, buyers.

H5: Loyalty/reward program membership positively drives compulsive buying

Buying Severity: Duration, Frequency and Expenditure Height

The three concepts between brackets, although separately measured, are closely linked together

and therefore theoretically captured together under the buying severity flag. They describe the

amount of hours spend during a buying episode, the yearly amount of buying episodes and the

(17)

Defining compulsive buying before touched upon the more frequent or severe shape of

consumption of compulsive buyers compared to a more normal consumption pattern, when

profiling compulsive buyers this is a potential important factor in play. A key takeaway from

definitions found in literature is the repetitive nature of the consumers buying pattern (Hirschman,

1992; O’guinn & Faber, 1989; Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney & Monroe, 2008). Considering the level of expenditures, the repetitiveness aligns with findings by Faber, O’Guinn and Krych, (1987) and

O’guinn and Faber (1989) indicating overextended credit lines, a higher amount of credit cards

used and forced sales of property. Koran et. al., 2006 indeed indicated a positive relationship

between duration (the number of hours spend during a buying episode) and compulsive buying.

Supplementary, Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney and Monroe (2008) found a significant relationship

between frequency of buying (the yearly amount of buying episodes) and expenditure height

(amount of money spend per episode) on the one side and compulsive buying on the other.

Logically, the following hypotheses concerning buying severity are therefore proposed.

H6: Buying duration per episode positively drives compulsive buying

H7: Buying episode frequency positively drives compulsive buying

H8: Expenditure height per episode positively drives compulsive buying

Buyer Satisfaction

A third dimension of aspects on which profiling is based is the level of satisfaction with the

contextual service or good bought and the corresponding provider. The concept of buyer

satisfaction is divided into four aspects; overall service/product quality satisfaction, feelings of

service/product provider loyalty, propensity of positive word-of-mouth and the propensity to

(18)

In case of a normal consumer, satisfaction is driven by the price and general quality of the good or

service bought (Anderson, Fornell & Lehmann, 2004). High perceived quality and a good price

lead to higher satisfaction, which in turn is proposed to lead to a higher amount of consumption as

buyers consciously realize spending their money provides them with a satisfactory level of

benefits. The status quo describes a satisfied frequently visiting consumer spending a fair amount

of money.

For compulsive buyers however, the benefits of consumption have become outweighed by its

severe negative consequences mentioned earlier. As consumers either consciously or

unconsciously experience the downside of their compulsive buying behavior on their daily life,

their attitude towards the service or good compulsively consumed is proposed to become worse

and the satisfaction with the one providing it to them therefore is proposed to drop, as the

degeneration of their quality of life is attributed to both. Although their compulsivity drives them

to maintain a high level of consumption, their level of satisfaction is thus proposed to drop. The

status quo here describes a dissatisfied frequently visiting consumer spending a fair amount of

money.

Theorizing in this sense implies approaching buying satisfaction as an outcome of compulsive

buying. The corresponding hypotheses are therefore formulated as follows:

H9: compulsive buying drives overall service/product quality negatively

H10: compulsive buying drive feelings of service/product provider loyalty negatively

H11: compulsive buying drives the propensity of positive word-of-mouth negatively

(19)

See figure 1 for an overview of all variables used for profiling in the proposed hypotheses.

Figure 1: Hypothesized Profiling Antecedents and Outcomes

Methodology

This section sheds light into the methodological part. All topics concerning the data and its analysis

will be discussed here. First of all, the measurement, scaling, sampling survey and data collection

will be discussed, after which the method of analysis concludes this section. The data analyzed is

acquired through Professor Arch G. Woodside (Boston College) used in his coproduction with

Catherine Prentice (Swinburne University); Problem Gamblers’ Harsh Gaze on Casino Services

(2013). It finds its origin in the popular gaming destination of Macau. Although still underestimated by many, this Chinese gambling Valhalla established itself as the “heavyweight

(20)

At first glance there might seem to be a gap between the topic of compulsive buying and the

gambling related data used to examine it. However, as touched upon in the introduction, both

topics are share a great amount of commonality. As illustrated in figure 2, gambling and

compulsive buying, among other similar disorders, both fall on the obsessive-compulsive spectrum

(Hollander, 1999). The spectrum consists of a family of multiple disorders sharing symptoms,

cause, effect and other commonalities. The spectrum thus represents the similarities between the

compulsive and impulsive behaviors that belong to it.

Obsessive- Compulsive Disorder Impulse- Control Disorder

OCD AN Trich IIU PG

Binge Eating Klep

Compulsive Buying

Figure 2: Obsessive-Compulsive Spectrum Disorders

Note: Adapted from Hollander (1999, p.40). The figure is not scaled, however, the position on the scale indicates closer alignment with either OCD or ICD. OCD: obsessive-compulsive disorder; AN: anorexia; Trich: trichotillomania; Klep: kleptomania; IIU: impulsive internet usage; PG: pathological gambling.

Furthermore, individuals exhibiting serial and simultaneous compulsive behaviors are far from an

exceptions to the rule; “… drug addiction was part of a larger pattern of compulsive behavior that

was manifested in multiple ways in the consumer's life. For example, I did not just take stimulants

consistently. I also drank alcohol almost daily, exercised ritualistically, worked in binges, ate

certain foods repeatedly, and would go on shopping "splurges" during which I would buy large quantities of one item” (Hirschman, 1992 p. 163). An overview on numerical findings on

(21)

the perspective that compulsive buying condition extends beyond retail shopping for products” and adopting the idea that “problem gambling is regarded as a subcategory of compulsive buying”

(Prentice & Woodside, 2013 p. 1110), advocates the suitability of the use of gambling data as a

case-setting within the broader framework of compulsive buying, a setting not limited to any

product or service.

Measurement and Scaling

This section describes the measurement and scaling of compulsive buying itself and the aspects

under each of the three dimension linked to compulsive buying (demographics, behavioral buying

characteristics and buyer satisfaction measures), which will be discussed in order of appearance.

Compulsive Buying

When reviewing the available definitions in literature, true compulsive behavior is described by

not in the least vague terms like: abnormal (Edwards, 1993; Nataraajan & Goff, 1991), beyond the

bounds of normalcy (Hirschman, 1992), quantitatively and qualitatively different from societal

norms, inappropriate, excessive and disruptive (Faber, O’Guinn & Krych, 1987), placing any compulsive consumer far outside the “normal” world. However, the question remains when the

point is reached at which one can be labeled as an abnormal and inappropriate compulsive

consumer. As with many conditions, severity differs highly across carriers, in this case from

occasionally to excessively compulsive. Also, the boundary between normal and abnormal

behavior is culture as well as context specific (Hassay & Smith, 1996). Therefore, instead of using

a dichotomous philosophy, compulsivity is approached as a continuum or spectrum (Hassay &

Smith, 1996; Nataraajan & Goff, 1991; Prentice & Woodside, 2013). On the one side of the

spectrum lies normal (occasionally compulsive) behavior, on the other, abnormal (excessive

(22)

Table 1: PGSI Items

I have… …wagered larger amounts to get the same feeling of excitement.

…tried to win back losses.

…borrowed money or sold something to get money for gambling. …felt a gambling problem existed.

…been criticized for betting or told a gambling problem exists. …felt guilty about gambling

…bet more than could be lost.

Has… …gambling caused health problems including stress and anxiety.

…gambling caused financial problems.

The gambling specific measure used as a proxy for compulsive buying here is the Problem

Gambling Severity Index (PGSI), also known as the Canadian Problem Gambling Index (Ferris &

Wynne, 2001). The construct is build up out of nine items (see table 1) on which a 4-point scale answer is requested: “1 = never, 2 = sometimes, 3 = most of the time and 4 = almost always”,

resulting in a summed PGSI rating ranging from 9 (normal) to 36 (abnormal).

Table 2: Item-to-Item/Item-to-Total Correlations for PGSI Scale

1 1 1 2 2 0,556** 1 3 3 0,263** 0,734** 1 4 4 0,424** 0,307** 0,280** 1 5 5 0,502** 0,595** 0,495** 0,472** 1 6 6 0,539** 0,605** 0,588** 0,432** 0,595** 1 7 7 0,429** 0,578** 0,627** 0,443** 0,555** 0,671** 1 8 8 0,493** 0,407** 0,342** 0,659** 0,565** 0,539** 0,538** 1 9 9 0,398** 0,560** 0,584** 0,387** 0,537** 0,566** 0,589** 0,358** 1 Total Total 0,650** 0,830** 0,790** 0,592** 0,780** 0,822** 0,815** 0,676** 0,744** 1 ** P < 0,001

The Cronbach’s Alpha for construct reliability on the gathered data for the nine items is 0.894,

meaning good reliability, nearly excellent. All item-to-item and item-to-total correlations are

(23)

Demographics

The four demographic variables are all scaled categorical. Gender is measured in two, both age

and education in three, and income in four categories. For an overview of the specific categories

per variable and the affix for the various variable category dummies see table 3. For age, education

and income categories are divided; low, middle and high, except for income which has a fourth

category that allows for very high income levels. Junior high/high school is regarded as the

breakpoint between low and middle education because of the intermediate nature of junior high

and the relatively higher equality with elementary school versus high school.

Table 3: Demographic Variable Scales

Scale Age Gender Education Income

0 18 to 35 Female Junior high or below $0 to $6000

1 36 to 55 Male High school $6001 to $15000

2 56+ Diploma $15001 to $25000

3 $25001 +

Behavioral Buying Characteristics

Of the four behavioral buying characteristic measures, two are categorical while the other two are

continuous scales. Loyalty/reward program membership is a yes/no question and both duration

(hours) and frequency (number of buying episodes) are open questions and continuous scaled.

Finally, expenditure height is measured in four categories. For the specifics see table 4 below.

Table 4: Demographic Variable Scales

Scale Loyalty

membership Duration Frequency Expenditure height

(24)

Buyer Satisfaction

To measure the four aspects of buyer satisfaction (overall service/product quality satisfaction,

feelings of service/product provider loyalty, propensity of positive word-of-mouth and the

propensity to switch to another service/product provider), seven items were used (see table 5), each

to be answered on a 7-point scale (from low/disagree to high/agree). Service/product provider

loyalty was measured as a single item construct, the other three are were measured on two items.

Table 5: Buyer Satisfaction Items

1. I think the overall service quality of the casino is…

2. Overall, how satisfied are you with the service provided by the casino? 3. I always consider this casino as my first choice to visit.

4. I say positive things about the casino to other people.

5. I recommend the casino to other players who haven’t been here. 6. I will switch to another casino that offers better services and deals.

7. I will switch to another casino if I experience a problem with the casino service.

Results for a factor analysis (n = 411) over all seven items and six out of seven (without item 3)

are provided in table 6 and 7. The corresponding Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measures for sampling

adequacy are 0.679 and 0,604, combined with the result of Bartlett’s test of sphericity, which is

highly significant in both cases (P < 0,001), this indicates appropriateness of the use of factor

analysis. Theoretically, a four factor solution is advocated, support for this approach is found in

the rule of thumb concerning the percentage of variance explained per factor (> 5%). The four

factor solutions on all items show mixed results, depending on the type of factor analysis (see table

6). Principal axis factoring includes the separate feelings of service/product provider loyalty item

within the word-of-mouth aspect of buyer satisfaction. The principal components analysis however

(25)

Table 6: Factor Analysis on All Buyer Satisfaction Items

Principal Axis Factoring – Varimax rotation Principal Components Analysis – Oblimin

rotation

Rotated matrix Pattern matrix

Item 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 0,214 0,159 0,809 0,088 -0,087 0,034 -0,912 0,129 2 0,206 0,119 0,805 -0,070 0,107 -0,019 -0,920 -0,111 3 0,692 0,042 0,170 0,203 0,068 -0,003 -0,015 0,946 4 0,911 0,073 0,238 -0,106 0,963 0,009 -0,040 -0,001 5 0,953 0,057 0,167 -0,077 0,957 0,001 0,029 0,051 6 0,089 0,870 0,140 0,062 -0,014 0,935 -0,009 0,059 7 0,025 0,872 0,121 -0,055 0,020 0,948 0,007 -0,063

When the service/product provider loyalty is isolated from the analysis, its one item nature makes

exclusion theoretically possible and statistically just, the factor analysis results concerning the

other three aspects show great similarity (see table 7). Using theoretical background and the

eigenvalue (>1) and percentage of variance explained per factor (>5%) rules, a three factor solution

is proposed.

Table 7: Factor Analysis on Six Buyer Satisfaction Items

Principal Axis Factoring – Varimax rotation

Principal Components Analysis – Oblimin rotation

Rotated matrix Pattern matrix

Item 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 0,193 0,160 0,807 -0,003 0,027 -0,917 2 0,200 0,117 0,806 0,004 -0,025 -0,931 4 0,928 0,070 0,249 0,964 0,005 -0,036 5 0,937 0,055 0,187 0,994 -0,003 0,032 6 0,081 0,869 0,146 0,029 0,935 -0,008 7 0,031 0,868 0,121 -0,027 0,949 0,007

Combining the results, the items are joined as theoretically proposed: item 1 and 2, item 3, item 4

and 5, and item 6 and 7. Scales represent the average of the individual items (1 to 7). Item-to-item

and item-to-total correlations are shown in table 8, the corresponding Cronbach’s Alhpa values for

product/service quality, word-of-mouth propensity and switching propensity are: 0.828, 0,958 and

(26)

Table 8: Item-to-Item/Item-to-Total Correlations for Buyer Satisfaction Scales

Overall quality satisfaction Word-of-mouth propensity Switch propensity

1 2 Total 4 5 Total 6 7 Total

1 1 4 1 6 1

2 0,708** 1 5 0,920** 1 7 0,775** 1

Total 0,929** 0,919** 1 Total 0,979** 0,981** 1 Total 0,941** 0,943** 1

** P < 0,001

Sampling, Survey and Data Collection Procedures

The participants of this study are Chinese residents on a holiday trip to one of the seven largest casino’s (in terms of total annual revenues) in the popular Chinese gaming destination of Macau.

With support of the Macau Secretariat for Economy and Finance, the seven casinos were

approached for data collection, after which all seven agreed.

Data was gathered using a complete survey written in the Chinese language. Before final data

gathering in situ, the survey was piloted two times in order to improve clarity. Firstly eight

questions, secondly two were revised to come to the final instrument that required four minutes of

time to complete.

Final data collection was done with assistance of three master-level students. They approached the

casino visitors on their way departing the partaking casinos’ table gaming area to request

participation in the survey, using a seated area near these gaming areas. Participating visitors were

all confirmed to be gambling. This procedure was repeated three days a week, alternately between

14:00 and 17:00, and 19:00 and 22:00 for the month of February 2013. Upon agreement to

participate they were offered a food voucher worth USD 25 or an umbrella. Eventually, 50 percent

of the visitors approached in this way participated in the survey. The percentage ranged from 40

(27)

Analysis - Ordinary Least Squares and Seemingly Unrelated Regression

This section describes the methods of estimation used to test the hypotheses. The analysis is spread

into two parts to account for the antecedent and outcome approach to the three dimensions on

which compulsive buying profiling is based. The first part of the analysis relies on ordinary least

squares regression (OLS), where the antecedents are modelled into one regression model with

compulsive buying as the dependent variable. The second part uses seemingly unrelated regression

(SUR) to account for the expected correlation of the error terms for the different one on one models

between the four satisfaction measures (dependent) and compulsive buying. Both analyses will be

preceded by pooling-tests to confirm the (in-) appropriateness of pooling the data from the

different casinos. Rosenthal’s (1991) meta-analytic method of added Z’s will be used to generalize

over the per casino models in case pooling is not allowed.

Results

This section will discuss the results of all analyses in order to answer to the proposed hypotheses.

The final results section is divided into two parts, one for each of the methodologies described

earlier. A brief data description will precede the final results to shed light into the structure of the

data.

Data description

Table 9 shows the total and per casino amount of observations. Descriptive statistics concerning

all the variables used in the analyses are provided in table 10. For the categorical variables the

(28)

minimum and maximum values are shown to provide insight into the structure of the responses.

The number of missing values is shown in the last column. Next

to four variables with a small number, two variables have a more

serious number of missing values (income with 44 and frequency

with 63). Missing values have been remedied in three ways. For

the demographical variables, observations for age and education

have been removed prior to analysis since saving two degrees of

freedom that “category unknown” dummies would have cost outweighs the small sacrifice of

losing ten observations here. Since income has 44 missing values, those have been replaced with

a dummy variable for category unknown. For the behavioral buying characteristics, missing values

for the categorical variable loyalty/reward program membership are replaced by a missing

category dummy variable while the continuous nature of duration and frequency allowed for

replacement of the missing values by mean imputation.

Table 10: Descriptive Statistics of Full Dataset

Variable Average (µ) / frequency St. Dev. (σ) Min Max # Missing values

Compulsive buying 11,09 3,14 9 24 - Demographics Age 0=151, 1=212, 2=41 7 Gender 0=161, 1=250 - Education 0=161, 1=163, 2=84 3 Income 0=282, 1=58, 2=16, 3=11 44

Behavioral buying characteristics

Loyalty progr. memb. 0=171, 1=234 6

Duration 3,03 1,54 1 10 5

Frequency 2,66 1,79 1 7 63

Expenditure height 0=112, 1=214, 2=68, 3= 17 -

Buyer satisfaction

Overall quality satisfaction 4,83 0,78 2,5 7 -

(29)

Analysis – OLS and SUR

The first part in this section will discuss pooling issues raised by the various sources used to gather

the data. The second and main section will discuss the results from both the models that link the

demographical variables and behavioral buying characteristics (treated as antecedents) to

compulsive buying, and the models that link compulsive buying to the buying satisfaction

measures (treated as outcomes). The latter is done in separate models as buyer satisfaction is

theoretically proposed as an outcome of compulsive buying and thus requires compulsive buying

to become the independent instead of dependent variable.

Pooling

Since the data is gathered in seven different casinos, regular (OLS) and separate intercept

(OLSDV) pooling tests verify the (in-) appropriateness of pooling all data together. Table 11 shows

the results for the antecedent models, table 12 for the outcome models. As can be seen from the

p-values in the lower row of table 11, both single and separate intercept pooling of the data is not

allowed (p<0,05). Therefore, models will be estimated per-casino and results will be generalized through Rosenthal’s (1991) meta-analytic method of added Z’s.

Table 11: Pooling Tests for Antecedent Models

OLS OLSDV dfpooled 384 278 dfunpooled 286 286 SSRpooled 2571,93 2445,33 SSRunpooled 1543,93 1543,93 F value 1,94 1,82 P value 0,000 0,000

The p-values for the outcome models in table 12 indicate separate intersect pooling of the data is

(30)

propensity models (p>0,05). The SUR models will therefore be estimated in a pooled manner, for

matters of uniformity all models will be estimated with per casino intercepts.

Table 12: Pooling Tests for Outcome Models

Overall quality

satisfaction Loyalty

Word-of-mouth

propensity Switch propensity

OLS OLSDV OLS OLSDV OLS OLSDV OLS OLSDV

dfpooled 399 393 399 393 399 393 399 393 dfunpooled 387 387 387 387 387 387 387 387 SSRpooled 235,63 220,25 681,66 659,90 583,18 559,07 243,16 337,99 SSRunpooled 215,07 215,07 653,41 653,41 551,88 551,88 331,85 331,85 F value 3,083 1,553 1,395 0,444 1,829 0,840 1,099 1,195 P value 0.000 0.159 0,166 0,949 0,042 0,609 0,359 0,284

Findings – part one: demographics and behavioral buying characteristics

Table 13 provides an overview of the results for the separate OLS models for the demographics

and behavioral buying characteristics. Even though the limited number of per casino observations,

with 38 for casino six as the lowest, six out of seven models show to be significant (p<0,05) with

adjusted R2 values ranging from 0,091 to 0,715. Explanatory power thus highly variates, not

surprisingly the number of significant (p<0,05) variables also highly variates from zero to six over

the different casinos. Since casino thee is insignificant, it will be excluded from the meta-analysis.

For matters of completeness, pooling tests have been rerun to look into the consequences of

exclusion of casino three, results indicate no change in conclusions with p-values of 0,000 pooling

still not allowed. Another quick glance at table 13 shows the absence of beta’s for three variables

in three casinos (after exclusion of casino three) due to a lack of presence of observations for those

categories, those will thus not be available for meta-analysis which for those three variables will

(31)

Table 13: Results for OLS Models per Casino

Model Cas 1*** Cas 2*** Cas 3 Cas 4*** Cas 5*** Cas 6** Cas 7**

R2 0,435 0,566 0,326 0.671 0,795 0.636 0,471 Adj. R2 0,264 0,436 0,091 0,529 0,715 0,388 0,269 Intercept 9,670*** 8,638*** 7,725*** 7,843*** 8,322*** 5,848* 6,263*** Unstandardized β Age1 1,266* 0,619 1,136 1,082 1,117 0,151 1,303 Age2 -0,405 0,03 2,122 -0,201 1,574 1,951 1,896 Gender 1,241* 0,805 0,376 2,503*** 0,307 -2,466 0,721 Education1 0,26 -0,292 -0,109 -1,082 -0,135 -1,293 1,076 Education2 -0,81 0,124 0,554 -1,288 -0,373 -0,789 1,09 Income1 -0,209 -0,849 0,469 0,285 1,135 -1,607 -0,316 Income2 4,074** -0,506 2,447 -5,224** 3,412 -0,819 Income3 8,068*** 2,563 3,311 0,276 5,77* 4,24 Income Unknown -0,282 1,165 -0,702 2,076 -2,607** -0,722 -2,066

Loyalty progr. memb. 0,564 -0,201 1,846* 2,463** 4,87*** 2,675* 2,065*

Loyalty progr. memb. Unknown 0,595 3,592** -0,498 3,685 8,014*** 1,369

Duration -0,219 0,326* 0,332 -0,515 -0,578** 0,515 -0,083 Frequency -0,017 0,093 -0,074 0,697** 1,045*** 1,527*** 0,895*** Expenditure height1 -0,091 -1,002** -0,036 1,238 -0,749 0,21 -0,273 Expenditure height2 0,368 1,003 -0,676 -1,253 -2,607** -3,248 0,263 Expenditure height3 -2,774* 0,254 0,015 -2,427 1,22 -1,497 * p < 0,10 ** p < 0,05 *** p < 0,01

Durbin Watson values for the 6 remaining models are in-between 1,584 and 1,975, indicating no

reason to suspect autocorrelation (dL, < d < dU at p=0,05). P-values for the Sharpiro-Wilk test of

normality ranging from 0,058 to 0,979 indicate normal distributions (p>0,05), except for casino

five which has a p-value of 0,049, considering its minimal insignificance it is not assumed to cause

any severe misestimation. Finally, multicollinearity is excluded as a potential issue since the

maximum VIF value per casino model range between 2,833 and 4,426 (VIF<10).

As mentioned earlier, Rosenthal’s (1991) meta-analytic method of added Z’s will be used in order

(32)

Heerde et. al. (2013), to arrive at the effect sizes of the meta-analytic beta’s, a weighted mean of separate casino beta’s is calculated in which the weight is based on the inverse of the beta’s

corresponding standard errors, normalized to one.

Table 14: Results Added-Z Meta-Analysis

Variable Weighted mean β Meta-Analytic Z Meta-Analytic P Age1 0,940*** 2,724 0,005 Age2 0,504 0,852 0,139 Gender 0,832** 2,294 0,014 Education1 -0,226 -0,641 0,162 Education2 -0,352 -0,768 0,148 Income1 -0,168 -0,389 0,185 Income2 0,476 0,506 0,175 Income3 3,901*** 3,358 0,001 Income Unknown -0,313 -0,616 0,165

Loyalty progr. memb. 1,732*** 4,519 0,000

Loyalty progr. memb. Unknown 3,492*** 3,210 0,001

Duration -0,097 -0,817 0,143 Frequency 0,539*** 5,191 0,000 Expenditure height1 -0,273 -0,851 0,139 Expenditure height2 -0,530 -1,058 0,114 Expenditure height3 -1,161* -1,232 0,093 * p < 0.10 ** p < 0.05 *** p < 0.01

Looking at table 14 that provides an overview of the meta-analysis results, six variables show to

be significant at a 0,05 cutoff level, one more at a cutoff level of 0,1. Each of them will be discussed

in relation to the proposed hypotheses.

Hypothesis one, proposing age to negatively drive compulsive buying is rejected since the middle

age category (36 to 55) scores significantly higher (p<0,01) than the lowest category (reference).

(33)

results are in support of an inverted U-shape relationship between age and compulsive buying,

with middle incomes positively driving compulsive buying.

As already pointed out, hypothesis two is particularly vulnerable to the casino origin of the data.

Indeed this hypothesis needs to be rejected considering the positive significant result (p<0,05)

indicating males are more prone to be compulsive buyers (or gamblers) than females. Results

clearly indicate the caution that has to be taken when generalizing over different types of

compulsive behaviors.

Concluding the demographical variables, hypotheses three and four also need to be rejected.

Hypothesis three due to a complete lack of significance, education does not influence compulsive

buying. Hypothesis four due to insignificance of the middle two income categories and a

significant high loading for the highest category beta (p<0,01) in contrast to the reference level,

indicating high incomes strongly positively drive compulsive buying.

Considering the behavioral buying characteristics, support for hypotheses five and seven (p<0,01)

and limited significant (p<0,1) support for hypothesis eight is found, while hypothesis six needs

to be rejected. Both loyalty/reward program membership and the frequency of buying episodes

indeed positively drive people towards the compulsive buying category. The time spend on these

buying episodes seems to be irrelevant, just as the amount of money spend per episode, as there is

only limited evidence that solely extreme high levels of expenditure positively drive compulsive

buying while the middle two categories do not significantly differ from the reference category.

Findings – part two: buyer satisfaction

For matters of uniformity the seemingly unrelated regression models are also run without casino

(34)

model allowing for single intercept pooling, the only uniform manner of analysis remains separate

intercept pooling.

Table 15 shows the overview of the outcomes from the analysis for the separate intercept system

of SUR models linking compulsive buying to the satisfaction measures. Although effect sizes and

adjusted R2 values are considerably low, three out of four satisfaction measures are significant

(p<0,01). This signifies that hypotheses nine, ten and eleven find support indicating overall

satisfaction, feelings of loyalty and propensity of word-of-mouth are negatively driven by

compulsive buying. However, this does not mean compulsive buying positively drives the

propensity to switch good/service provider, rejecting hypothesis twelve.

Robustness: Fussy-Set Qualitative Comparative Analysis

Methodologically, another novel and potentially interesting aspect will be displayed here by means

of a robustness check of the OLS analysis. In the current paradigm of correlational analysis,

variability within data is not always exploited to a full extend. Especially in the focal case, with

multiple categorical variables (14 dummies) there is quite some potential for implicit interactions

impossible to estimate in OLS for example. Fuzzy-set qualitative comparative analysis (fsQCA)

provides an algorithmic solution easily allowing for any of those potential interactions. It provides

set-theoretic configurations of causes and conditions that affect the outcome (Ragin, 2008).

Namely, in configurational analysis the influence of the antecedent on the outcome condition is

Table 15: Results for separate intercept SUR Models

(35)

either negative, positive or absent depending on the context of the other antecedents. E.g., where

X1, X..., Xetc lead to higher or lower Y in correlational analyses, it is assumed that there are multiple

configurations, or causal recipes (Ragin, 2008) of high and/or low values of X1, X..., Xetc that lead

to high or low Y values in configurational analysis. For a complete background on fsQCA see

Ragin (2008).

Since algorithmic analysis implies combining antecedents to come to a certain outcome, and

interactions in one-on-on modes are impossible by definition, fsQCA will only be applied on the

demographical variables and the behavioral buying characteristics. Before analysis, two topics

need to be discussed for clarification of the methodology and interpretation of the results; the

calibration of the data and the measures of fit (consistency and coverage).

Calibration

Before analyzing the data a procedure similar to z-score transformation has to be performed, in

this case called calibration. Values are converted to a 0.00 to 1.00 scale indicating the degree of

membership on a certain antecedent condition, from full non-membership (0.00 to 0.05) to full

membership (0.95 to 1.00). Maximum ambiguity lies at 0.50. The philosophy behind this lies in

the irrelevancy of reaching scores on a ratio scale that lie beyond the principle of full (non-)

membership. Therefore, the foundation on which calibration is based lies in face validity more

than on sample descriptive statistics (mean/spread), and thus leans on general judgments, standards

or guidelines and researchers’ interpretation. More information can be found in Ragin (2008).

Table 16 provides an overview of the characteristics of the calibration of the original Macau dataset

with the first value being the 0.05 threshold of full non-membership, the middle being the 0.50

(36)

Table 16: Data Calibration For fsQCA Analysis

Variable Calibration score

(0.05, 0.50, 0.95) Compulsive buying (9, 11, 16) Age1 (0, 0.5, 1) Age2 (0, 0.5, 1) Gender (0, 0.5, 1) Education1 (0, 0.5, 1) Education2 (0, 0.5, 1) Income1 (0, 0.5, 1) Income2 (0, 0.5, 1) Income3 (0, 0.5, 1) Income Unknown (0, 0.5, 1)

Loyalty progr. memb. (0, 0.5, 1)

Loyalty progr. memb. Unknown (0, 0.5, 1)

Duration (1, 3, 5)

Frequency (1, 2, 3)

Expenditure height1 (0, 0.5, 1)

Expenditure height2 (0, 0.5, 1)

Expenditure height3 (0, 0.5, 1)

Consistency & Coverage

The measures of model fit/usefulness relied on in fsQCA analysis are consistency and coverage,

both sharing similarities with measures from more common statistical analyses. The first,

analogous to correlation, is a measure of the degree to which the data displays a set of common

antecedent conditions associated with an outcome condition. The second, describing the degree to

which this common set of antecedent conditions accounts for the instances of this outcome

condition, is analogous to the coefficient of determination (R2). Both measures range from zero to

one, the closer to one the better. For a full explanation and numerical examples see Ragin (2008).

Findings

Table 17 shows the fsQCA models investigating the relationship between the demographical

(37)

high score, empty dots represent a low score. All three models show very high consistency and

moderate coverage, indicating the strength of the models as such (nearly all respondents with this

configuration achieve this outcome) but their moderate explanatory power (not too many of these

configurations exist within the data).

Table 17: fsQCA Models for High Compulsive Buying

Model 1 2 3 Coverage (raw/unique) 0,223/0,084 0,155/0,016 0,172/0,033 Consistency 0,921 0,939 0,901 Age1 ● ● ● Age2 Gender ● ● ● Education1 ● Education2 Income1 ● ● Income2 Income3 Income Unknown

Loyalty progr, memb, ● ● ●

Loyalty progr, memb, Unknown

Duration ● ● ●

Frequency ● ● ●

Expenditure height1 ●

Expenditure height2 ●

Expenditure height3 ●

Fully in line with the results from the meta-analysis, all three algorithms consist of middle aged

males. Looking at education and income however, low and middle categories are present in the

algorithms while the role of education is insignificant and the role of income purely plays a role

when high in the meta-analysis. Both results are thus contesting the meta-analytical results in the

sense that low and middle categories of education and income can drive compulsive buying in the

(38)

Looking at the behavioral buying characteristics, results for loyalty/reward program membership

(being a member positively drives compulsive buying), frequency (buying episode frequency

positively drives compulsive buying) and expenditure height (the amount spend during a buying

episode is largely insignificant) are in line with the meta-analysis. In case of expenditure since all

categories pop up in the algorithms. Duration however also plays an important role in all the

algorithms, indicating a high amount of time spend during a buying episode positively drives

compulsive buying while it is insignificant in the meta-analysis.

Concluding, results from the fsQCA analysis are largely corresponding with the meta-analysis.

The allowance for implicit interactions however indicates that configurations with low or middle

levels of education and income and a high amount of time spend per buying episode positively

drive compulsive buying.

Discussion, Recommendations & Limitations

Discussion

The results for the demographical variables show mixed results in contrast to former literature.

The negative relationship between age and compulsive buying supported by the mass of the

authors only finds limited support here as the high age category does not significantly differ from

the low category benchmark but the middle category does score significantly higher. The negative

relationship thus only holds for middle to high age groups, while between low to middle age groups

age seems to drive compulsive buying positively. Perhaps this contrast arose due to the linear nature of some of the authors’ analysis in contrast to the nonlinearity that dummy coding here

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Traders purchase and sell shares in an experimental stock market on the basis of fundamental and technical information We find that as opposed to the linear scheme the threshold

When the scores on the schizotypal trait were classified into two categories (low versus high) based on the median, the high-schizotypal child molesters (H-ST)

focus on future expectations (i.e. I’m satisfied because I expect a lot of profit in the future), also figure out which antecedents of supplier satisfaction play an

 Charging a buyer a higher price (or giving a smaller discount) for products that the same buyer resells online than for products they sell offline (dual pricing) is an

Next, the univariate analysis shows that private equity buyers offer different deal terms; private equity buyers make less tender offers, and are involved in less

In addition, we extended previous investigations 11 Using Holm’s adjustment for multiple hypothesis testing (9 different measures of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities), the

te veel; te moeilijk; te veel stapeling; te weinig standaardvragen en vaar- digheden; te veel tekst; een meisjes- onvriendelijk examen; op zo’n exa- men zijn leerlingen niet te

De wiskunde in de profielen N&amp;G en N&amp;T wordt niet alleen bepaald door de vervolgopleidingen, maar ook door de grote overlap.. Kijken we naar de vervolgopleidingen van