• No results found

Sharing is caring : an experimental study on the impact of stress on the knowledge sharing

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Sharing is caring : an experimental study on the impact of stress on the knowledge sharing"

Copied!
60
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

 

 

“Sharing  is  Caring”  

An  experimental  study  on  the  impact  of  stress  on  knowledge  sharing  

                       

Master  thesis    

Behavioural  Economics  and  Game  Theory  

15  ECTS           By   Dafne  Schröer   9854223   ***          

The  University  of  Amsterdam   Amsterdam  School  of  Economics  

Supervisor:  dr.  T.    Buser  

Second  corrector:  dr.  J.J.  van  der  Weele    

July  2016  

(2)

This  document  is  written  by  Student  Dafne  Schröer  who  declares  to  take  full  responsibility   for  the  contents  of  this  document.  

 

I  declare  that  the  text  and  the  work  presented  in  this  document  is  original  and  that  no   sources  other  than  those  mentioned  in  the  text  and  its  references  have  been  used  in   creating  it.  

 

The  Faculty  of  Economics  and  Business  is  responsible  solely  for  the  supervision  of   completion  of  the  work,  not  for  the  contents.  

(3)

Alchemists turned into chemists when they stopped keeping secrets.

Eric Raymond

(*1957, American computer programmer, and author)

(4)

Abstract    

Knowledge  sharing  within  an  organization  has  become  increasingly  more  important  in  the   fast  moving  world  we  live  in.  Most  organizations  use  shared  databases  to  store  and  reuse   knowledge.  Experience  and  previous  research  have  shown  that  without  intervention,  people   will  not  automatically  share  the  knowledge  they  have  via  a  shared  database.    This  makes  it   important  to  get  a  better  understanding  of  all  factors  that  have  an  impact  on  the  decision  to   share.  This  paper  examines  an  experiment  to  find  the  impact  of  stress  on  knowledge  

sharing.  Stress  is  seen  as  one  of  the  most  significant  work-­‐related  health  risks  and  has  an   impact  on  behaviour,  decision  making  and  mental  functioning.  Because  of  this  I  hypothesise   that  stress  could  be  expected  to  have  an  impact  on  the  decision  to  share  knowledge  as  well.   This  is  why  in  this  paper  the  knowledge  sharing  and  stress  are  connected.  A  negative  

relationship  between  being  stressed  and  sharing  was  found  and  some  of  the  findings  

provided  weak  statistical  evidence  in  favour  of  the  hypothesis,  but  future  research  is  needed   to  draw  any  definite  conclusions.    

(5)

Contents  

 

1   Introduction                   1  

 

2   Literature  review                 4    

2.1       Previous  literature  on  knowledge  sharing     4  

2.1.1     The  impact  of  knowledge  sharing         4  

2.1.2     Possible  issues  with  knowledge  sharing       4  

2.1.3     Possible  solutions  for  knowledge  sharing  issues     6  

2.2     Previous  literature  on  stress         4  

2.2.1     Stress  and  some  of  it’s  effects         8  

2.2.2     Stress  and  decision  making           9  

2.2.3     Stress  and  human  interaction         10  

2.3     Connecting  knowledge  sharing  and  stress     12  

 

3   Hypothesis                   13  

 

4   Method                   13  

  4.1     Treatment  group             14  

4.1.1     Execution  of  the  TSST-­‐G             15  

4.1.1.1   TSST-­‐G  phase  1             15  

4.1.1.2   TSST-­‐G  phase  2             16  

4.1.2       Knowledge  sharing  part  of  the  experiment     17  

4.1.2.1     Knowledge  sharing  phase  1         17  

4.1.2.2     Knowledge  sharing  phase  2         18  

  4.2     Control  group             19  

 

4   Results                   20  

 

6   Discussion                     26  

  6.1     Discussion  of  the  results           26  

  6.2     Limitations  of  the  study           26  

  6.3     Suggestions  for  future  research         29  

 

7   Summary  and  concluding  remarks           31  

 

Appendix                     33  

(6)

1.  Introduction    

Humans  have  always  shared  their  knowledge  and  knowhow  by  telling  each  other  stories   about  their  thoughts  and  experiences  (Smith,  2001).  Despite  this  innate  capacity  for  sharing   and  storytelling,  capital,  labour  and  raw  materials  were  historically  seen  as  much  more   valuable  than  creating  and  applying  knowledge  (Smith,  2001).  Indeed,  within  the  field  of   commerce,  organizations  used  to  focus  mainly  on  processing  information  to  solve  problems   (Nonaka,  1994).  But  this  is  changing,  and  today  it  is  widely  recognised  as  more  important  to   manage  human  intellect  and  convert  it  into  new  and  innovative  products  and  services   (Smith,  2001).    

 

It  has  become  a  main  focus  for  organizations  to  prevent  valuable  human  knowledge  

resources  from  being  wasted.  In  particular,  knowledge  in  the  form  of  knowhow  is  often  lost   by  outsourcing,  mergers  and  downsizing  (Smith,  2001).  Suppiah  and  Manjit  (2011)  

estimated  that  90%  of  all  knowledge  within  an  organization  resides  in  the  minds  of  its  staff.   This  makes  it  all  the  more  important  for  organizations  to  find  ways  to  store,  sort  and  use  the   knowledge  they  have  at  their  disposal  (Smith,  2001).  

 

Organizations  often  use  electronic  databases  to  share  and  (re)use  corporate  knowledge   (Cress  and  Hesse,  2004).  To  be  able  to  create  knowledge,  it  has  to  be  understood  that  this   process  begins  its  life  as  data.  Data  are  initially  transformed  into  information  and  only  then   can  this  information  be  transformed  to  knowledge.  Information  alone  has  little  value  until  it   has  been  given  meaning  or  is  being  used  on  the  job  (Smith,  2001).  This  shows  the  

importance  of  well  functioning  knowledge  sharing  systems.    

Economics  is  interested  in  human  decision  making.  The  question  “what  decisions  do  people   make?”  is  important,  but  equally  important  is  “why  do  people  make  the  decisions  they   make?”  What  drives  people  to  do  what  they  do?  And  how  can  we  influence  that?  In  the   case  of  knowledge  sharing  one  could  ask  why  would  people  share  knowledge,  and  why  do   some  people  share  their  knowledge  with  others,  while  others  do  not.  

(7)

In  the  context  of  knowledge  sharing  within  institutions,  for  knowledge  to  be  stored  in   databases,  employees  often  need  to  make  an  active  decision  to  contribute  their  knowledge.   The  decision  to  share  has  been  studied  in  depth  and  the  problems  that  arise  with  sharing  via   databases  have  been  described  in  multiple  studies.  Understanding  how  to  address  these   problems  to  create  well-­‐functioning  knowledge  management  systems  is  also  a  focus  of   existing  literature.    

 

At  the  outset,  human  inertia  is  thought  to  be  one  of  the  biggest  obstacles  to  the  creation  of   well-­‐functioning  knowledge  management  systems  (Smith,  2001).  This  is  exacerbated  by  the   “free  rider”  problem  that  is  inherent  in  common  pool  resources  such  as  knowledge  

databases.  A  solution  to  the  free-­‐rider  problem  that  ensures  most  knowledge  will  be  shared   has  yet  to  be  found,  however  prior  research  on  knowledge  sharing  and  possible  

interventions  and  their  results  are  discussed  in  the  first  part  of  section  2  of  this  paper.    

One  possible  reason  for  the  lack  of  knowledge  being  shared  in  organizations  that  has  not  yet   been  studied  is  stress.  Stress  has  been  found  to  change  behaviour  and  cognition  by  

changing  neuronal  activity  (Joëls  and  Barams  2009;  Sandy  and  Haller  2015;  Dawans  et  al.   2012).  It  creates  social  withdrawal  (Sandy  and  Haller  2015),  impaired  physical  and  mental   functioning  (Kalia  2002)  and  the  majority  of  the  brain  regions  affected  by  stress  response   are  also  involved  in  the  decision  making  process.  

 

Apart  from  knowledge  sharing  problems,  organizations  also  have  to  deal  with  stress  related   issues  on  a  daily  basis.  In  2001  the  World  Health  Organization  noted  that  stress  is  one  of  the   most  significant  health  risks  in  the  twenty-­‐first  century  (Kudielka  et  al.  2007).  In  2014  the   European  Agency  for  Safety  and  Health  at  Work  described  a  project  carried  out  by  Matrix   (2012)  on  the  estimated  cost  of  work-­‐related  stress  and  mental  health  issues  in  the  

European  Union  in  2012.  Matrix  (2012)  found  that  the  cost  of  work-­‐related  depression  was   estimated  at  €620  billion.  Although  the  authors  are  not  clear  on  what  part  of  the  work-­‐ related  depression  is  actually  caused  by  stress,  the  findings  make  it  clear  that  work  stress  is   not  just  a  health  problem,  but  also  an  economic  problem.  Further,  TNO  (2014)  found  in  their   study  on  labour  market  stress  that  one  in  eight  workers  in  the  Netherlands  is  suffering  from   work-­‐related  stress.  In  the  United  States  these  numbers  are  even  higher,  with  40%  of  

(8)

workers  reporting  their  job  as  very  or  extremely  stressful  and  25%  of  workers  viewing  their   job  as  the  number  one  stressor  in  their  lives  (Sauter  et  al.  2014).  These  complementary   findings  indicate  that  most  organizations  will  have  to  deal  with  stress  in  one  way  or  another.      

Taken  together,  stress  and  knowledge  sharing  are  two  extremely  important  issues  for  most   organizations,  and  both  have  been  researched  extensively.  Given  all  these  findings,  one   could  expect  stress  to  have  an  impact  on  knowledge  sharing.  However,  until  now,  this   impact  has  not  been  studied.  This  paper  seeks  to  fill  the  gap  in  the  existing  literature  by   examining  the  impact  of  stress  on  knowledge  sharing.  Could  (part  of)  the  lack  of  knowledge   sharing  possibly  be  explained  by  stress?  Based  on  the  theoretical  view  of  stress  and  

knowledge  sharing  described  in  section  2,  this  paper  describes  an  experimental  setting  that   investigates  people’s  sharing  behaviour  when  influenced  by  stress  (and  benchmarked   against  a  non-­‐stressed  treatment)  as  discussed  in  section  3.  

The  experimental  design  used  to  study  the  impact  of  stress  on  knowledge  sharing  follows   the  well  proven  Trier  Social  Stress  Test  for  Groups  (Dawans  et  al.  2010),  followed  by  a   knowledge  sharing  task.  The  complete  design  of  the  experiment  used  is  discussed  in  more   detail  in  section  4.  Results  are  presented  in  section  5  and  discussed  in  section  6.  Although   the  data  from  this  experiment  came  from  a  very  small  and  noisy  sample  (meaning  that  in   most  cases,  the  results  were  either  not  statistically  significant,  or  provided  only  weak   evidence  in  favour  of  the  tested  hypothesis),  this  paper  does  find  a  negative  relationship   between  stress  and  sharing.    

 

Section  6  will  finish  with  a  discussion  on  some  limitations  of  this  study  that  should  be  taken   into  account  when  interpreting  the  results,  followed  by  some  implications  for  future  

research.  This  paper  will  end  with  a  summary  and  some  concluding  remarks  in  section  7.    

 

(9)

2.  Literature  review    

2.1  Previous  literature  on  knowledge  sharing    

2.1.1  The  importance  of  knowledge  sharing  

Cress  and  Kimmerle  (2009)  describe  a  vision  of  a  world  that  allows  people  to  share  and   combine  knowledge  all  over  the  world,  regardless  of  the  place  and  time.    This  vision  was   spurred  by  the  emergence  of  Web  2.0  websites  that  emphasize  user-­‐generated  content,   interoperability  and  usability,  and  social  software  (DiNucci,  1999).  The  new  breed  of   software  makes  it  possible  for  people  to  start  conversations  with  each  other  and  create  a   comprehensive  pool  of  knowledge  which  is  kept  up  to  date  by  users  from  all  over  the  world.   The  fact  that  everyone  has  access  to  this  knowledge  and  can  change  it  means  that  the   quality  of  the  common  knowledge  pool  will  be  very  high.  The  first  to  make  this  vision  real  is   the  online  encyclopaedia  Wikipedia.  Because  millions  of  people  use  this  encyclopaedia  and   can  change  and  add  knowledge  as  they  wish  (as  long  as  it  is  objective  and  supported  by   references)  (Cress  and  Kimmerle,  2009),  Wikipedia  has  successfully  become  a  high  quality   source  of  information,  as  shown  by  Giles‘  (2005)  research  which  compares  Wikipedia  and   Encyclopaedia  Britannica.    

 

Corporate  knowledge  is  one  of  the  most  critical  assets  in  modern  organizations.  With  the   rise  of  internet-­‐based  disruptive  technologies,  it  has  become  increasingly  difficult  for   organizations  to  sustain  their  competitive  advantage  even  with  more  advanced  technology,   innovative  products,  and  better  services.  Perhaps  the  only  thing  that  is  sustainable  is  a   knowledge  advantage,  because  it  is  usually  difficult  to  imitate  and  is  embedded  in  multiple   aspects  of  an  organization,  including  its  history  (Cress  and  Kimmerle,  2009;  Alavi  and  Leitner   2001;  Armistead  and  Meakins,  2002),  culture  and  identity,  routines,  policies,  systems  and   documents,  as  well  as  individual  employees.  This  is  why  organizations  are  constantly  trying   to  find  ways  to  maintain  their  own  corporate  knowledge  (Wang,  2005).      

 

2.1.2  Possible  issues  with  knowledge  sharing  

Knowledge  sharing  is  about  maintaining  the  knowledge  flows  in  an  organization  while   knowledge  pools  (such  as  databases)  promote  preservation,  sharing  and  reuse  of  

(10)

knowledge.  Knowledge  pools  allow  many  people  to  search  for  knowledge  without  having  to   contact  the  person  who  originally  developed  the  knowledge  (Wang  and  Ahmed,  2003).  This   gives  more  people  access  to  information.  It  also  helps  to  create  a  better  use  of  employees’   steadily  growing  wealth  of  knowledge  to  solve  problems  and  to  achieve  goals  (Smith,  2001).   Although  very  important,  technology  is  only  a  facilitator  in  knowledge  management.  The   human  aspect  is  still  vitally  important;  and  human  behaviours,  like  sharing,  do  not  always   occur  naturally,  however  they  are  of  huge  importance  for  knowledge  management  systems   to  work  properly  (Armistead  and  Meakins,  2002).  

 

For  knowledge  pools  to  function  as  an  effective  way  to  store  organizational  knowledge,  it  is   not  only  important  that  stored  knowledge  is  used  by  others,  but  maybe  even  more  essential   that  people  share  the  knowledge  they  have  (Wang  and  Ahmed,  2003).  The  problem  –  that   people  will  very  gladly  use  the  information  from  a  pool  of  jointly  compiled  knowledge,  but  in   many  instances  those  same  people  will  not  help  to  grow  the  pool  by  adding  knowledge  of   their  own  –  is  seen  in  many  situations  where  shared  knowledge  pools  are  created.  This  is   also  the  case  for  Wikipedia  –  millions  of  people  use  Wikipedia,  but  only  a  small  percentage   actually  contributes  to  the  knowledge  pool  (Cress  and  Kimmerle,  2009).  

 

This  same  problem  arises  in  organizations  where  people  need  to  share  knowledge  through   databases,  such  as  file  servers  or  document  management  systems.  When  people  have  to   make  a  decision  to  share  knowledge  with  others  by  entering  it  in  a  shared  database,  a  social   dilemma  arises.  A  social  dilemma  is  a  situation  in  which  the  decision  that  gives  the  best   result  for  the  individual  is  not  necessary  also  the  best  decision  for  the  group  as  a  whole.  One   reason  for  not  sharing  information  could  be  that  knowledge  is  often  seen  as  a  kind  of  power   that  people  do  not  want  to  give  away  (Cress  and  Hesse,  2004).  This  gives  rise  to  a  conflict   between  the  interests  of  individual  group  members  and  the  interests  of  the  group  as  a   whole  (Cress  and  Kimmerle,  2009).  The  knowledge  sharer  has  private  costs  associated  with   sharing  knowledge  (she  must  expend  time  and  effort  to  put  knowledge  into  the  database)   but  receives  no  private  benefit  from  this  effort.  She  already  has  access  to  her  own  

knowledge,  whether  or  not  she  contributes  any  of  the  knowledge  to  the  shared  database   (Cress  and  Kimmerle,  2009).  Hence,  while  everyone  else  can  benefit  if  the  knowledge  sharer   puts  her  knowledge  into  the  database,  the  knowledge  sharer  herself  receives  no  benefit  at  

(11)

all,  and  indeed  bears  a  personal  cost.    Without  intervention,  this  situation  also  means  that   the  knowledge  sharer  has  no  reason  to  expect  any  reciprocity  from  the  people  that  use  her   information  (Cress  and  Hesse,  2004).  Indeed,  the  knowledge  sharer  only  has  a  slight  hope  to   benefit  from  the  knowledge  contributed  by  others  (Fulk  et  al.  1996).  The  cost-­‐benefit  

analysis  therefore  means  that  the  most  efficient  strategy  for  every  individual,  independent   of  what  everyone  else  is  doing,  is  to  withhold  information.  But  the  group  as  a  whole  will   have  a  lower  benefit  if  members  withhold  information  from  the  shared  database.  This   makes  the  most  efficient  strategy  of  withholding  information  an  Inferior-­‐Nash  Equilibrium   (Cress  and  Hesse,  2004).    

 

The  explanation  above  shows  that  we  can  look  at  the  exchange  of  knowledge  via  a  database   as  a  public  goods  dilemma.  Characteristics  of  a  public  good  are  non-­‐rivalry  and  non-­‐

excludability.  In  the  case  of  knowledge  sharing  via  a  database,  the  database  can  be  seen  as   the  public  good.  The  value  of  the  database  will  not  diminish  if  people  use  it,  so  it  is  non-­‐rival,   and  it  does  not  matter  if  a  group  member  contributed  to  the  database  or  not,  everyone  in   the  group  can  use  it,  making  it  non-­‐excludable  (Cress  and  Hesse,  2004;  Cress  and  Kimmerle,   2009).  

 

2.1.3  Possible  solutions  for  knowledge  sharing  issues  

The  free-­‐riding  problem  described  here  –  that  people  will  use  the  available  information   shared  by  others  without  contributing  themselves  –    raises  the  question  of  how  to  motivate   individuals  to  behave  in  the  group’s  interest  and  share  their  knowledge.  This  means  that,   given  the  described  dilemma,  people  need  to  behave  contrary  to  the  individually  rational   option  (Cress  and  Hesse,  2004).      

 

There  are  some  interesting  aspects  to  this  dilemma.  In  1994  Constant  et  al.  found  that   people  react  differently  depending  on  the  knowledge  to  be  shared.  They  suggested  that  to   be  able  to  better  understand  the  process  of  knowledge  sharing  in  an  organization,  the   attitude  and  perceptions  of  employees  need  to  be  measured.  Cress  and  Kimmerle  (2009)   described  this  phenomenon  from  a  psychological  perspective,  including  a  focus  on  the   motivational  situation  of  a  user  of  knowledge  management  systems.  They  show  how  this   motivational  situation  leads  to  free-­‐riding  and  explain  why  the  readiness  to  cooperate  will  

(12)

always  be  a  fundamental  part  of  a  successful  knowledge  process.    

Many  researchers  note  that  expected  rewards  are  one  of  the  most  important  factors  for   people  to  share  their  knowledge  with  others  (Wang,  2005).  Cress  and  Hesse  (2004)  did  a   number  of  experiments  to  test  the  impact  of  different  motivational  situations  on  the   amount  and  quality  of  knowledge  shared.  They  found  that  providing  metaknowledge  about   relative  importance,  implementing  a  use-­‐related  bonus  (a  bonus  received  if  the  shared   information  is  used  by  others)    and  reducing  the  cost  of  adding  information  to  the  database   all  resulted  in  a  rise  of  the  quality  of  information  shared.  Cress  and  Hesse  (2004)  also  tested   prescriptive  rules  (that  is,  rules  that  prescribed  subjects  should  share  a  given  number  of   contributions  via  the  shared  database)  and  provided  subjects  with  feedback  about  the  mean   contributions  of  their  teammates.  Although  there  were  some  positive  effects,  none  of  these   motivational  situations  was  able  to  stop  the  free-­‐riding  problem  completely.  1    

 

It  is  not  only  motivation  that  stimulates  people  to  contribute  their  knowledge  –  culture  can   also  play  a  vital  role.  A  good  knowledge  sharing  culture  in  an  organization  has  been  

prescribed  as  one  of  the  most  important  issues  in  a  well  functioning  knowledge  

management  system  (Wang,  2005;  Chow  et  al.  2000).    Tong  et  al.  (2015)  use  a  field  study  in   the  ICT  sector  to  show  that  organizational  culture  is  positively  related  to  knowledge  sharing   and  impacts  job  satisfaction.  Further,  Cropanzano  et  al.  (2003)  executed  two  field  studies  in   which  they  found  that  emotional  exhaustion  has  negative  consequences  for  both  individual   employees  and  their  employers.  Emotional  exhaustion  negatively  affected  job  performance,   organizational  commitment  and  turnover  intentions.    

 

Further,  according  to  Wang  (2005),  individual  differences  are  also  an  important  explanatory   factor  for  knowledge  sharing.    To  be  willing  to  share  one’s  knowledge  with  others,  one  must   be  willing  to  interact  with  others.  If  and  how  people  share  knowledge  could  even  go  beyond   individual  differences  as  described  by  Wang  (2005).  Chow  et  al.  (2000)  looked  into  the                                                                                                                  

1  As  an  aside,  it  is  worth  pointing  out  from  an  experimental  economics  perspective  that  Cress  and  Hesse  (2004)  

use  a  form  of  deception  in  this  experiment  –  telling  subjects  that  they  would  be  sharing  via  a  database  with   their  team  members,  but  these  team  members  were  in  fact  made  up.  This  may  have  some  impact  on  the   validity  of  the  findings.  

(13)

importance  of  cultural  differences  in  knowledge  sharing.  They  argue  that  because  of  the   globalization  of  economic  activity  it  is  getting  more  important  to  take  cultural  differences   into  account,  because  people  from  different  countries  and  cultures  often  differ  in  the  way   they  react  to  work  related  conditions  (Triandis  et  al.  1988;  Chow  et  al.  2000).  Members  of   different  cultural  groups  are  often  distinguished  by  the  relative  emphasis  they  place  on  their   self-­‐interest.  Hence  we  can  distinguish  cultural  groups  from  one  another  using  two  basic   values:  individualism  and  collectivism.  When  there  is  a  conflict  of  needs,  people  from   collectivist  cultures  are  more  inclined  to  give  up  their  needs  to  benefit  the  group.  These   cultures  tend  to  cooperate  more.  In  contrast,  people  from  individualistic  cultures  tend  to  be   more  competitive  and  place  their  own  needs  above  those  of  the  group.  In  a  situation  of   knowledge  sharing  a  difference  could  evolve  between  people  from  a  collective  culture  and   those  from  an  individualistic  culture,  especially  when  there  is  a  conflict  between  collective   interest  and  self  interest  in  the  knowledge  sharing  situation.  Given  all  these  findings  the   study  of  Chow  et  al.  (2000)  shows  that  the  extent  and  motivation  in  knowledge  sharing  may   differ  across  countries  and  should  be  taken  into  account  in  the  search  for  possible  solutions.  

 

2.2  Previous  literature  on  Stress    

2.2.1  Stress  and  some  of  it’s  effects  

In  the  past  several  decades  there  has  been  a  great  deal  of  interest  in  the  sociological  study   of  stress  (Perlin  and  Bierman  2013).  In  this  paper,  stress  will  be  defined  as  the  activation  of   the  neurophysiological  stress  response  (Sandy  and  Haller  2015).  Stress  changes  behaviour   and  cognition  by  changing  neuronal  activity.  This  happens  in  situations  that  are  threatening,   and  it  is  crucial  for  survival  that  this  happens  rapidly  and  enduringly  (Joëls  and  Baram  2009;   Sandy  and  Haller  2015;  Dawans  et  al.  2012).    

 

Sandy  and  Haller  (2015)  discuss  a  range  of  research  on  coping  behaviours  in  response  to   stress.  Social  withdrawal  is  one  of  the  coping  behaviours  found  in  response  to  stress.    Stress   and  related  stress  disorders  have  a  big  impact  on  the  way  people  function  (Kalia  et  al.  2002).   Apart  from  social  withdrawal,  studies  have  also  shown  an  increase  in  irritability  and  displays   of  anger  as  other  patterns  of  short-­‐term  social  stress  responses  (Repetti  and  Wang  2016).   Kalia  et  al.  (2002)  discuss  the  fact  that  it  has  consistently  been  shown  that  the  negative  

(14)

impacts  caused  by  stress  are  as  severe  as  those  caused  by  other  medical  conditions  such  as   arthritis,  hypertension  and  diabetes.  

 

Employee  stress  has  been  shown  to  have  significant  consequences  for  organizations.   Stressed  people  experience  impaired  physical  and  mental  functioning  and  more  work  days   are  lost.  Some  of  the  consequences  of  stress  found  are  absenteeism,  accidents  and  human   errors,  with  the  increase  in  human  error  in  particular  having  a  further  impact  on  the   occurrence  of  accidents  and  on  the  quality  of  production.  But  there  are  also  consequences   of  worker  stress  found  in  the  interpersonal  relationships  within  an  organization.  These   include  disputes  between  workers,  conflicts  and  interpersonal  problems;  but  also  violence,   the  resistance  to  change  and  loss  of  intellectual  capital  (Kalia,  2002).  It  is  becoming  clear   that  stress  is  a  phenomenon  that  negatively  affects  a  growing  number  of  people  in  the   workplace  (Brun  and  Lamarch,  2006).  

 

2.2.2  Stress  and  decision  making  

Decision  making  and  stress  are  intricately  connected.  Many  (economic)  decisions  have  to  be   made  under  stress.  When  a  stress  reaction  occurs,  it  originates  in  the  hypothalamus  which   offsets  a  physiological  and  endocrine  response.  This  stress  response  affects  different  brain   regions  including  the  orbitofrontal  cortex,  the  amygdala,  basal  ganglia,  limbic,  hippocampus   and  the  prefrontal  cortex.  These  regions  are  very  sensitive  to  stress  hormones,  due  to  the   fact  that  they  have  many  stress  hormone  receptors  (Starke  and  Brand,  2012).      

 

Complex  neural  networks  are  also  involved  in  decision  making,  with  a  majority  of  the  brain   regions  affected  by  stress  response  also  involved  in  the  decision  making  process.  For   example,  decisions  that  involve  moral  dilemmas,  reward  processing,  uncertainty  and   adjustments  from  automatic  response  are  all  decisions  that  take  place  in  one  or  more  of   these  brain  regions  with  many  stress  hormone  receptors.  These  findings  show  that  the  brain   regions  that  are  associated  with  decision  making  are  sensitive  to  stress  induced  changes   (Starke  and  Brand,  2012).  

 

Different  studies  on  decision  making  under  acute  laboratory  stress  have  shown  that  the   underlying  mechanisms  of  decision  making  may  be  altered  by  stress  (Bos  et  al.  2009;  Leder  

(15)

et  al.  2012;  Gathman  et  al.  2014).  Particularly  in  people  with  substantial  cortisol  responses,   the  brain  regions  involved  in  decision  making  also  show  changed  neural  activation  (Starke   and  Brand,  2012).  This  shows  that  stress  has  cognitive  consequences  (Tomova  et  al.  2014).   Some  of  the  underlying  mechanisms  affected  are  the  strategy  used  and  the  processing  of   feedback,  because  the  working  memory  is  sensitive  to  stress  induced  changes  (Starke  and   Brand  2012;  Leder  et  al.  2012).  In  addition,  the  adjustment  from  automated  response,  the   sensitivity  to  reward  and  punishment,  the  reduction  in  the  action  outcome  contingencies   and  the  explicit  knowledge  of  participants  in  these  studies  are  underlying  mechanisms   altered  by  stress  (Starke  and  Brand,  2012).    

 

Delaney  et  al.  (2014)  look  at  the  impact  of  stress  on  economic  decision  making.  They  find   that  subjects  that  are  exposed  to  stress  have  increased  subjective  discounting  rates,  exhibit   more  present-­‐focused  preferences  and  are  less  likely  to  explore  available  options.  Starke   and  Brand  (2012)  also  mention  the  way  stress  affects  fine-­‐tuned  decision  making.  Stress   likely  triggers  cardiovascular,  neural  and  hormonal  reactions  and  this  all  can  lead  to  a   performance  shift  in  decision  making.    

 

2.2.3  Stress  and  human  interaction  

More  recently,  there  has  been  continuing  research  on  the  impact  of  stress  on  the  ability  of   people  to  tune  into  others  (Tomova  et  al.  2014),  which  may  have  significant  consequences   for  social  interactions  in  a  workplace  environment.  This  recent  research  has  led  to  some   interesting  findings,  especially  with  regard  to  the  differences  between  men  and  women  in   social  stress  responses.  The  generally  regarded  social  stress  response  for  people  is  the   “fight-­‐or-­‐flight”  response  (Dawans  et  al.  2012).  Fight-­‐or-­‐flight  responses  are  less  recourse   demanding  and  more  automatic  strategies.  This  means  that  people  in  a  fight-­‐or-­‐flight   response  tend  to  fall  back  to  more  egocentric  processes  when  having  to  judge  emotions  or   perspectives  of  others,  since  paying  attention  to  feelings,  needs  and  intentions  of  others  is   resource  demanding  (Tomova  et  al.  2014).    

 

Research  has  shown  that  the  fight-­‐or-­‐flight  response  is  mainly  found  in  men  (Dawans  et  al.   2012),  with  research  by  Taylor  et  al.  (2000;  2006)  finding  a  different  strategy  in  women.  The   researchers  found  that  women  do  not  become  more  egocentric  under  stress,  but  become  

(16)

more  social.  They  use  the  metaphor  “tend-­‐and-­‐be-­‐friend”  to  describe  this  female  stress   response.  The  evolutionary  explanation  Taylor  et  al.  (2000;  2006)  provide  is  that  women   tend  to  create  and  maintain  their  social  network  to  protect  themselves  and  their  offspring   from  threats.  However,  it  should  be  noted  that  at  the  time  of  the  study  there  was  still  little   known  about  the  immediate  prosocial  and  antisocial  responses  following  stress,  particularly   in  men  (Dawans  et  al.  2012),  and  the  research  of  Taylor  et  al.  (2000;  2006)  was  mainly  based   on  research  done  on  rodents  (for  the  endocrine  response  to  stress)  and  primates  (for  the   behavioural  response  to  stress).  It  could  be  questioned  if  the  same  responses  would  apply   to  human  females.  Tomova  et  al.  (2014)  did  not  find  any  differences  in  psychological  and   physiological  stress  responses  between  men  and  women.  This  means  that  the  differences  in   self-­‐other  distinctions  between  men  and  women  may  not  be  due  to  gender  differences   (Tomova  et  al.  2014).  

 

Research  by  Dawans  et  al.  (2012)  aimed  to  look  at  the  effect  of  a  psychosocial  laboratory   stressor  on  both  social  and  anti-­‐social  behaviour.  In  this  study,  the  researchers  focussed   specifically  on  the  stress  responses  of  males,  because  the  idea  is  that  the  tend-­‐and-­‐be-­‐friend   response  specifically  characterizes  women’s  stress  response.  Dawans  et  al.  (2012)  found   that  stress  exposure  increased  trust,  trustworthiness  and  even  sharing  behaviour  in  social   interactions  in  men.    It  should  be  noted  that  because  this  research  only  used  male  

participants,  one  cannot  compare  the  male  tend-­‐and-­‐be-­‐friend  response  to  the  female   tend-­‐and-­‐be-­‐friend  response  or  draw  any  conclusions  on  male  and  female  behaviour  in  a   mixed  sex  group.  To  do  that,  new  research  is  necessary  that  compares  both.  Dawans  et  al.   (2012)  also  found  that  non-­‐social  risk  taking  was  not  impacted,  but  stress  did  impact  the   willingness  to  accept  risks  arising  through  social  interactions.  This  finding  indicates  that  the   pro-­‐social  behaviour  following  stress  is  not  due  to  a  general  increase  in  the  readiness  to   bear  risks.  These  findings  show  that  the  social  approach  behaviour  found  for  females  in   response  to  stress  by  Taylor  et  al.  (2000;  2006)  also  holds  for  males.  Overall,  this  shows  that   humans  seem  to  have  the  tendency  to  provide  and  receive  protection  within  groups  in   threatening  times  (Dawans  et  al.  2012).  

 

(17)

2.3  Connecting  knowledge  sharing  and  stress  

One  question  left  to  ask  is:  what  causes  the  stress  to  occur?  As  with  knowledge  sharing,   research  has  shown  that  organizational  structure  and  culture  are  of  great  importance  in   stress  and  burnout  problems.  For  example,  Finney  et  al.  (2013)  find  that  organizational   structure  and  culture  are  the  most  important  predictors  of  stress  and  burnout  in  

correctional  officers.  Further,  in  terms  of  stress  and  burnout  in  healthcare  workers,  multiple   studies  have  found  that  the  organizational  structure  and  culture  have  a  large  impact  

(Fiabane  et  al.  2013;  Tennant,  2001;  Cooper  and  Cartwrite,  1994).      

Bringing  these  seemingly  disparate  areas  of  research  together,  we  know  that  knowledge   sharing  is  very  important  in  organizations,  and  that  stress  is  seen  as  one  of  the  most   significant  work-­‐related  health  risks  in  organizations.  Organizations  deal  with  knowledge   sharing  issues  and  stress  on  a  daily  basis.  If  we  look  at  the  research  done  on  both,  there   seems  to  be  a  number  of  similar  creators  and  consequences  in  both  stress  and  knowledge   sharing  problems.  Organizational  structure  has  a  significant  impact  on  both  stress  and  the   willingness  to  share  knowledge.  Interpersonal  relationships  are  also  an  important  influence   on  both,  and  since  stress  has  an  impact  on  decision  making  and  creates  more  present-­‐ focused  preferences,  the  decision  to  share  one’s  knowledge  with  others  might  be  impacted   by  stress  as  well.  

 

Given  all  the  results  described  above,  there  is  evidence  to  suggest  stress  will  have  a  negative   impact  on  the  willingness  of  people  to  share  their  knowledge  with  others.  While  the  tend-­‐ and-­‐be-­‐friend  response  described  by  Taylor  et  al.  (2000;  2006)  indicates  a  potential  positive   impact  of  stress,  most  of  the  evidence  came  from  studies  of  rodents  and  primates,  not  from   human  subjects.  Further,  the  study  of  Dawans  et  al.  (2012),  which  did  find  sharing  behaviour   in  response  to  stress,  used  experiments  in  which  human  participants  interacted  face  to  face   with  others.  Especially  in  a  situation  where  participants  share  information  in  an  anonymous   way  via  a  database  (as  is  the  case  in  the  experiment  studied  in  this  paper)  the  participants   should  not  expect  any  reciprocity.  This  means  there  would  be  no  reason  to  “tend-­‐and-­‐be-­‐ friend”  others.  This  is  why  in  this  study  a  negative  impact  of  stress  is  expected.  

(18)

As  far  as  I  am  aware,  there  has  not  yet  been  any  research  on  the  impact  of  stress  on   knowledge  sharing  via  databases.  Therefore,  this  study  sets  out  to  investigate  the  effect  of   psychosocial  stress  on  the  willingness  to  share  in  a  laboratory  experiment.  

   

3.  Hypothesis  

As  discussed  in  section  2.2  above,  stress  changes  people’s  behaviour  and  cognition  (Joëls   and  Baram,  2009;  Sandy  and  Haller,  2015;  Dawans  et  al.  2012)  and  can  create  a  response  of   social  withdrawal  (Sandy  and  Haller,  2015).  In  a  situation  where  one  is  asked  to  share   knowledge  with  others  via  a  shared  database,  social  withdrawal  may  make  subjects  less   empathetic  and  therefore  potentially  less  willing  to  share.    

 

Further,  the  impact  of  stress  on  interpersonal  relationships  (Kalia,  2002)  will  plausibly   influence  subjects’  willingness  to  share  knowledge  with  others.  When  a  person  has  negative   feelings  towards  people  she  has  to  share  knowledge  with,  she  might  be  less  willing  to  put  in   the  time  and  effort  required  to  share.  To  share  knowledge  via  a  database,  subjects  must   make  the  decision  to  put  in  the  time  and  effort  to  share  their  knowledge.  When  people  are   stressed,  their  fine-­‐tuned  decision  making  is  affected.  Therefore,  with  induced  stress,  it  is   expected  that  subjects  will  become  less  sensitive  to  feedback,  and  reward  and  punishment   measures  taken  to  try  to  stimulate  people  to  share  their  knowledge  (Starke  and  Brand,   2012).  In  addition,  the  resistance  to  change  as  an  effect  of  stress  might  have  a  negative   impact  on  subjects’  willingness  to  share  (Kalia,  2002).  

 

The  arguments  described  above  are  the  reason  why  the  hypothesis  of  this  paper  is  that   stressed  people  will  share  less  of  their  knowledge  with  others  via  a  shared  database.    

 

4.  Method  

The  goal  of  this  research  is  to  look  at  the  impact  of  stress  on  knowledge  sharing.  The   hypothesis  was  studied  in  a  laboratory  study,  which  took  place  at  the  CREED  laboratory  at   the  University  of  Amsterdam.  Both  the  control  groups  and  the  treatment  groups  were  

(19)

tested  in  the  same  evening.  There  were  24  participants  of  which  18  were  master  students  in   Economics  at  the  University  of  Amsterdam  and  six  participants  were  from  outside  the   university.  Sixteen  participants  were  male  and  eight  participants  were  female.  The  Ethics   Committee  Economics  and  Business  (EBEC)  at  the  University  of  Amsterdam  approved  the   experimental  design.    

 

Participants  were  randomly  assigned  to  either  the  control  group  or  the  treatment  group.  A   control  group  was  included  to  ensure  the  observed  effects  in  the  treatment  group  were  a   direct  result  of  the  induced  stress.  There  were  two  control  groups  and  two  treatment   groups,  each  comprising  six  participants.  The  experiment  consisted  of  two  main  parts:  the   stress  test  part  and  the  knowledge  sharing  part.  The  control  group  only  participated  in  the   knowledge  sharing  part  of  the  experiment.    

 

4.1  Treatment  group  

To  induce  stress,  the  treatment  group  started  with  a  stress  test.  The  method  used  to  induce   stress  was  the  Trier  Social  Stress  Test  for  Groups  (TSST-­‐G)  developed  by  Dawans  et  al.  in   2010.  The  TSST-­‐G  is  a  standardized  motivated  performance  task  protocol  that  combines   high  levels  of  socio-­‐evaluative  threat  and  uncontrollability  in  a  group  format.  In  their   research,  Dawans  et  al.  (2010)  use  heart  rate  devices  to  test  autonomic  stress  responses.   Apart  from  the  autonomic  stress  response,  they  also  measure  the  endocrine  stress   responses,  because  studies  have  found  the  measurement  of  cortisol  as  an  indicator  for   adrenocortical  activity  to  be  of  high  predictive  value  for  psychosocial  stress  (Foley  and   Kirschbaum,  2010).  They  collect  saliva  samples  from  participants  before,  during  and  after   the  stress  test  to  measure  cortisol  levels.  Apart  from  the  biological  parameters,  the  authors   also  measure  different  psychological  parameters  such  as  discomfort  and  anxiety  using   questionnaires  such  as  the  trait  anxiety  scale  of  the  State-­‐Trait  Anxiety  Inventory  (STAI)   (Spielberger  et  al.  1970).  The  research  of  Dawans  et  al.  (2010)  and  others  has  shown  reliable   psychological  and  biological  (cortisol,  heart  rate)  stress  responses  (further  examples  in   Dawans  et  al.  2012;  Pabst  et  al.  2013;  Bos  et  al.  2009;  Gathman  et  al.  2014).  Primarily  due  to   financial  and  logistical  constraints,  the  TSST-­‐G  designed  by  Dawans  et  al.  2010  was  used  in   this  experiment,  without  actually  measuring  biological  and  psychological  stress  responses.      

(20)

4.1.1  Execution  of  the  TSST-­‐G  

The  TSST-­‐G  consists  of  two  phases:  a  preparation  phase  and  an  interview  phase  (see  figure   1).  At  the  start  of  the  experiment,  participants  entered  the  first  lab  and  were  randomly   allocated  a  seat  number.    Each  participant  had  a  pen,  some  paper  and  the  instructions  on   their  desk.  The  instructions  were  for  both  phases  of  the  experiment.  This  was  done  to  keep   the  time  between  the  stress  test  and  the  knowledge  sharing  test  as  short  as  possible.  After   reading  the  instructions,  participants  answered  some  control  questions  to  verify  they   understood  the  task.    

              4.1.1.1.  TSST-­‐G  phase  1  

After  the  control  questions,  the  preparation  phase  started.  Participants  had  10  minutes  to   prepare  a  2-­‐minute  speech  about  themselves  and  their  suitability  for  a  fictitious  sales  job.   After  10  minutes,  the  experimenter  asked  participants  to  take  their  second  seat  number  and   quietly  form  a  line  at  the  door.  Participants  were  not  permitted  to  bring  notes  into  the  next   phase  of  the  experiment.  

 

   

(21)

4.1.1.2  TSST-­‐G  phase  2  

The  second  phase  of  the  stress  test  consisted  of  two  parts.  For  the  first  part  of  the  second   phase,  participants  entered  a  second  lab  and  quietly  stood  in  front  of  two  interviewers,  a   man  and  a  woman  dressed  in  formal  business  clothes.  Participants  stood  next  to  each  other,   but  were  separated  by  mobile  dividing  walls  that  restricted  any  eye  contact  and  social   interaction  with  the  other  participants  (see  figure  2).  The  interviewers  gave  a  brief  verbal   summary  of  the  forthcoming  task  and  explained  to  the  participants  that  they  would  call   them  one  by  one  in  random  order  to  do  their  two-­‐minute  speech.  The  speeches  were  also   recorded  on  a  camera  that  stood  very  visible  on  the  interviewers’  desk.  This  was  done  to   further  increase  subjects’  stress.    

           

(22)

The  interviewers  withheld  any  form  of  verbal  or  non  verbal  feedback  while  participants   were  speaking.  If  a  participant  was  quiet  for  three  or  more  seconds,  the  interviewers  were   instructed  to  respond  in  a  standardized  way,  saying:  “Go  on,  there’s  still  some  time  left”   after  which  they  would  look  at  the  participant  in  silence.  If  a  participant  finished  their   speech  before  the  two  minutes  were  over,  the  interviewers  were  instructed  to  also  respond   in  the  standardized  way  saying:  “Go  on,  there’s  still  some  time  left”  after  which  they  would   look  at  the  participant  in  silence.    

 

After  all  participants  had  given  their  speech  (12  minutes  in  total),  the  second  part  of  the   second  phase  of  the  stress  test  started.  In  this  phase,  the  interviewers  asked  the  

participants  to  serially  subtract  the  number  13  from  a  given  number  (e.g.  1632,  1619,  1606,   etc.)  as  quickly  and  accurately  as  possible.  All  participants  received  an  individual  starting   number  to  avoid  learning  effects.  If  a  participant  made  a  mistake  the  interviewers  were   instructed  to  interrupt  them  saying:  “Sorry,  that’s  wrong.  Start  over  please”.  The  participant   had  to  go  back  to  his  or  her  individual  starting  number  and  start  again.  Each  participant  was   called  upon  multiple  times,  resulting  in  an  average  of  80  seconds  of  calculating  for  each   participant  (a  total  of  8  minutes).  After  the  calculations  finished,  the  interviewers  asked  the   participants  to  quietly  form  a  line  in  front  of  the  door  to  the  third  lab,  where  the  second   part  of  the  experiment  was  held.  

 

4.1.2  Knowledge  sharing  part  of  the  experiment  

The  second  part  of  the  experiment  was  the  knowledge  sharing  part  (see  figure  1).  The   design  of  the  knowledge  sharing  part  is  a  modification  of  the  design  used  by  Cress  and   Hesse  (2004).  It  is  set  up  as  a  public  goods  dilemma  in  which  the  database  is  the  public  good   everyone  can  contribute  to.  This  second  part  of  the  experiment  also  consisted  of  two  

phases.  A  brief  summary  of  the  instructions  read  at  the  beginning  of  the  first  part  of  the   experiment  were  read  aloud.    

 

4.1.2.1  Knowledge  sharing  phase  1  

In  phase  1,  participants  were  asked  to  calculate  monthly  salaries  of  sales  personnel.  Each   participant  was  responsible  for  two  months  of  the  year,  meaning  that  in  each  group  of  six   participants,  a  full  year  of  information  was  available.  Participants  had  nine  minutes  to  

(23)

calculate  as  many  salaries  as  possible.  After  each  calculation  participants  had  to  make  a   choice  regarding  whether  they  would  share  their  calculation  by  putting  it  in  a  shared   database.  To  contribute  a  salary  calculation  to  the  database  would  take  approximately  15   seconds  of  time  per  calculation.  After  filling  in  all  fields  of  the  database  form,  the  participant   could  move  on  to  the  next  calculation  and  so  on.  Participants  could  also  choose  not  to  share   their  calculation,  in  which  case  they  could  move  straight  on  to  the  next  calculation.  The   decision  to  share  or  not  had  to  be  made  after  each  calculation.  It  is  important  to  note  that  it   was  made  clear  in  the  instructions  that  there  was  no  obligation  to  contribute  to  the  

database,  and  that  the  decision  to  contribute  calculations  to  the  database  had  no  direct   personal  benefit,  because  the  participant  would  have  access  to  her  own  phase  1  calculations   in  phase  2.  Putting  information  in  the  database  took  time  which  could  not  be  spent  on   calculating  more  salaries  -­‐  meaning  there  was  a  personal  cost  -­‐  but  it  would  help  the  other   participants  in  the  group,  because  those  participants  would  then  not  have  to  calculate  that   monthly  salary  in  phase  2.  Participants’  potential  payoff  in  both  phases  would  only  depend   on  how  many  calculations  they  got  right,  not  on  how  many  the  other  members  of  their   group  got  right.  Their  earnings  were  10  cents  for  every  monthly  salary  calculated  correctly  in   phase  1  and  in  phase  2  they  could  earn  30  cents  for  every  salary  calculated  correctly.  

 

4.1.2.2  Knowledge  sharing  phase  2  

At  the  beginning  of  phase  2,  all  participants  received  a  database  from  the  experimenter   which  showed  all  shared  phase  1  calculations.  In  phase  2,  participants  had  12  minutes  to   calculate  as  many  annual  salaries  as  possible.  To  do  so,  they  could  use  their  own  calculations   from  the  previous  phase  and  the  calculations  contributed  to  the  shared  database  by  their   group  members.  If  not  everything  was  contributed  to  the  shared  database,  participants  had   to  calculate  the  missing  monthly  salaries  themselves.    

 

It  is  clear  to  see  the  dilemma  that  arises  through  the  amount  of  money  people  can  earn   during  the  experiment:  each  participant  maximises  their  own  potential  earnings  by   withholding  information  from  the  shared  database.  Thus,  withholding  information  is  a   dominant  strategy.  However,  the  mean  payoff  for  all  group  members  is  higher  if  all  decide   to  contribute.  This  means  that  withholding  information  is  a  Pareto-­‐inferior  Nash  

(24)

At  the  end  of  the  last  phase  of  the  experiment,  a  random  draw  picked  one  group  member  to   be  paid  his  or  her  earnings  privately  in  cash.  This  meant  that  all  participants  had  a  one  in  six   chance  of  receiving  their  actual  payoff.  The  choice  to  pay  just  one  member  of  each  group   was  due  to  personal  financial  constraints.    

 

4.2  The  control  group  

The  control  group  did  not  participate  in  the  stress  test  part  of  the  experiment.  They  entered   the  third  lab  and  were  randomly  assigned  to  a  desk.  The  experimenter  read  the  instructions   for  the  knowledge  sharing  task  and  after  the  instructions  participants  answered  some   control  questions  to  show  they  understood  the  task.  After  the  instructions,  the  control   group  performed  the  same  knowledge  sharing  task  as  the  treatment  group,  described   above.  

 

Ideally,  the  experiment  would  control  for  possible  fatigue  effects.  The  participants  in  the   treatment  group  engaged  in  about  45  minutes  of  work  before  starting  the  knowledge   sharing  task.  This  could  have  made  them  more  tired  and  thus  might  have  affected  their   performance  on  the  knowledge  sharing  task.  Other  experiments  using  a  stress  test  

performed  a  similar  test  on  the  control  group  without  adding  the  social  stress  component  to   control  for  this  possible  fatigue  effect.  In  this  experiment  there  was  limited  lab  time  and  the   entire  experiment  had  to  be  executed  in  one  evening.  Because  of  this  limited  time,  the   control  group  participated  in  the  knowledge  sharing  part  of  the  experiment  only.  

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

It is not traditionally thought of as a type of outlier problem, but we believe that generalizing the problem into one which treats the data as being composed of an unknown number

This can explain the significant effect for store atmosphere and the lack of a direct effect for hedonic shopping value and brand heritage function on sharing content on

Results have shown that , even though all the dimensions of Humanness are present within the organizations, only the concept of social capital (which deals with the relationships

Therefore, questions arising concerning what role other informal mitigation mechanisms can play in mitigating the risk of knowledge leakage and if it can enhance

Maar omdat die omstandigheden voor de gehele organisatie worden bekeken en er geen toespitsing op de diverse groepen en categorieën plaatsvindt, wordt bij

The first is using the Facebook-group as a platform to share external health- related knowledge sources; the second is sharing internal knowledge through sharing patient-

This diameter may be obtained from models of non-cavitating vortices that describe the radial distribution of the azimuthal velocity, for given vortex strength and size of the

Objectives were to observe trends in type of foods consumed at two to three monthly intervals from ages six to 18 months; secondly to determine dietary intake in terms of