• No results found

Maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy are associated with amygdala hyperresponsivity in children

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy are associated with amygdala hyperresponsivity in children"

Copied!
8
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-1015-x ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

Maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy are associated

with amygdala hyperresponsivity in children

Noortje J. F. van der Knaap1 · Floris Klumpers1,2 · Hanan El Marroun3 ·

Sabine Mous3 · Dirk Schubert1 · Vincent Jaddoe4,7 · Albert Hofman4,5 ·

Judith R. Homberg1 · Henning Tiemeier3,4 · Tonya White3,6 · Guillén Fernández1

Received: 21 June 2016 / Accepted: 8 June 2017 / Published online: 30 June 2017 © The Author(s) 2017. This article is an open access publication

birth, did not explain this relation. Our findings are in line with a model in which prenatal depressive symptoms of the mother are associated with amygdala hyperresponsivity in her offspring, which may represent a risk factor for later-life psychopathology.

Keywords Depression · Prenatal · Amygdala · fMRI ·

Child

Introduction

In 7–15% of all pregnancies, the mother encounters depres-sive symptoms, which may increase some risks of her unborn child [1]. Indeed, children prenatally exposed to maternal depressive symptoms show more often prema-turity, anxiety, impulsive behavior and sleep problems at young ages than non-exposed peers [2–4]. Moreover, as they mature, these children have more affective problems and antisocial behavior than children of euthymic moth-ers [3–5]. However, the mechanistic link between maternal depression during pregnancy and the child’s mental health is not well understood. The amygdala is a core structure in emotional processing [6, 7]. Altered amygdala function is thought to underlie affective symptoms and increased risk of psychopathology [4, 8–11]. A study on newborns exposed to maternal depression shows altered amygdala microstructure as represented by lower fractional anisot-ropy and axial diffusivity and altered functional connectiv-ity of the amygdala [12, 13]. Initial studies on school-aged children revealed lasting differences in cortical thickness associated with exposure to prenatal maternal depression [14, 15]. While these studies might suggest lasting struc-tural effects, it is currently unclear whether amygdala func-tionality is affected at older ages. Probing this question

Abstract Depression during pregnancy is highly

preva-lent and has a multitude of potential risks of the offspring. Among confirmed consequences is a higher risk of psycho-pathology. However, it is unknown how maternal depres-sion may impact the child’s brain to mediate this vulner-ability. Here we studied amygdala functioning, using task-based functional MRI, in children aged 6–9 years as a function of prenatal maternal depressive symptoms selected from a prospective population-based sample (The Generation R Study). We show that children exposed to clinically relevant maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy (N = 19) have increased amygdala responses to negative emotional faces compared to control children (N = 20) [F(1,36) 7.02, p = 0.022]. Strikingly, postnatal maternal depressive symptoms, obtained at 3 years after

* Noortje J. F. van der Knaap n.vanderknaap@fcdonders.ru.nl

1 Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, P.O. Box 9101, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands

2 Experimental Psychopathology and Treatment Section, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

3 Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Erasmus Medical Centre, 3000 CB Rotterdam, The Netherlands 4 Department of Epidemiology, Erasmus Medical Centre, 3000

CB Rotterdam, The Netherlands

5 Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA

6 Department of Radiology, Erasmus Medical Centre, 3000 CB Rotterdam, The Netherlands

7 Department of Paediatrics, Erasmus Medical Centre, 3000 CB Rotterdam, The Netherlands

(2)

could provide further insight into the elevated risk of men-tal disorders, which typically becomes apparent later in life. Thus, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, using an emotional face matching task, in a prospective community sample of children aged 6–9 years [16]. We hypothesized that exposure to maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy is associated with amygdala hyperresponsivity for emotional faces in school-aged children.

Methods Participants

Subjects took part in ‘The Generation R Study’, a prospec-tive population-based birth cohort study investigating the health and development of children in the Netherlands [17]. At the age of 6 years, a brain MRI study began within a subsample of the Generation R participants. This pilot con-tained an MRI session with a structural scan, diffusion ten-sor imaging and resting state functional imaging. A total of 1070 children were scanned focusing on studies inves-tigating prenatal exposure to potentially harmful agents (e.g., nicotine, maternal depression or alcohol), or specific behavioral problems (e.g., ADHD or antisocial behavior). fMRI tasks were added to the imaging pipeline for spe-cific studies whenever possible. Within this context, tasks for emotional face matching task and cognitive flexibility task were employed. Further information on the entire neu-roimaging pilot is provided elsewhere [16]. Forty-seven children with mothers who displayed prenatal depressive symptoms (PDS) during pregnancy were invited for the current study. Control subjects (n = 37) were matched with the target population on age, gender, handedness and eth-nicity and also invited to take part in the fMRI study. The Medical Ethical Committee of the Erasmus Medical Cen-tre approved the study, and informed consent was obtained from a parent or a legal guardian prior to participation.

Maternal depressive symptoms and child behavioral assessment

Maternal depressive symptoms were assessed using the Brief Symptoms Inventory (BSI) between 20 and 25 weeks of gestation and when the child was 3 years of age, referred to in the manuscript as prenatal depressive symptoms and postnatal depressive symptoms, respectively. This validated self-report questionnaire defines a spectrum of psychiatric symptoms, of which the six-item depression scale was used [18]. Mothers with scores higher than 0.75 display clini-cally relevant depressive symptoms, according to Dutch normative data [19, 20], which was used as a cutoff score.

The control group had low BSI scores (0–0.67), compared to the PDS group (0.83–2.33). When children were around 6 years of age, parents completed the standardized Child Behavior Checklist 1.5–5 (CBCL; [21]). The CBCL for toddlers was used to obtain standardized parental reports of children’s internalizing and externalizing problems. This questionnaire contains 99 problem items, which are scored with regard to seven empirically based syndromes that were derived by factor analyses: emotionally reactive, anxious/ depressed, somatic complaints, withdrawn, sleep problems, attention problems, and aggressive behavior. The summary internalizing scale is a summary score for items on the first four syndrome scales, and the externalizing scale is a sum-mary score for attention problems and aggressive behavior. Each item is scored 0, 1 or 2 (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, 2 = very true or often true) on the basis of the child’s behavior during the preceding 2 months. Higher scores indicated more problems. Good reliability and validity have been reported for the CBCL [22]. In the current study, the weighted sum scores of the internalizing and externalizing problem scales were used to check for potential behavioral differences between groups.

Experimental paradigm

Children performed an emotional face matching task (Fig. 1), consisting of two blocks of an emotion condi-tion and three blocks of a control condicondi-tion, which were presented in alternating order starting with a control block [23, 24]. Each block contained six trials of 5 s each. Three stimuli were presented simultaneously at each trail, either fearful and angry female face (emotion condition) or hori-zontally and vertically oriented ovals (control condition). Faces were of mixed ethnicities in line with our study pop-ulation. For the emotion condition, children were instructed to identify the emotion of the cue face at the top of the screen and indicate which of the two target faces shown below matched in terms of emotional expression by press-ing one of two buttons. Within the control condition, the geometric orientation of the ovals had to be matched. Two versions of the task were used equally often in each group, and response time and accuracy were monitored. Prior to the neuroimaging procedure, children were familiarized with the procedure in a mock scanner.

MRI data acquisition

Scanning was carried out on a GE Discovery MR750 3 Tesla whole body MRI system (General Electric, Milwau-kee, USA) using an 8-channel head coil as described previ-ously [16]. Preceding the fMRI task, a structural scan was obtained. The high-resolution structural scan was acquired using a whole brain T1 inversion recovery fast spoiled

(3)

gradient recalled (IF-FSPGR) sequence with the following parameters: TR = 10.3 ms, TE = 4, 2 ms, flip angle = 16°, matrix = 256 × 256, 186 contiguous sagittal slices with an isotropic voxel size of 0.9 mm3. Functional MRI

was recorded using a gradient-echo blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) EPI sequence with a TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 85°, matrix × 64 × 64.

fMRI behavioral data analysis

In five children, no behavioral data were recorded (control

n = 3; PDS n = 2) due to a technical failure leaving us with thirty-four children that had correctly recorded behavioral data on task performance (response time and accuracy) (control n = 17; PDS n = 17). Additionally, datasets were excluded from behavioral analysis if children performed at chance level (control = 1, PDS = 2), giving us the final number used for analysis with 15 PDS children and 16 control children. Behavioral results were analyzed in SPSS

19.0 (SPSS, Chicago, Illinois, USA) using a repeated measures ANOVA, comparing between PDS children and controls. We did include these excluded children without recorded behavioral data, but with visual inspection of non-random engagement to the task at hand.

Functional MRI data processing and statistical analysis

We were stringent by only including subjects with limited movement artifacts (less than 3 mm absolute movement for each direction vector; or any other related movement arti-facts) leaving us with good quality data from 19 PDS chil-dren and 20 matched control chilchil-dren. Two chilchil-dren were included in the PDS group, who were exposed to prenatal maternal depressive symptoms based on self-report of the mother but for whom BSI scores were missing (Table 1). For the analysis of the functional scans, we used Statisti-cal Parametric Mapping (SPM 8.0; http://www.fil.ion.ucl. ac.uk/spm). Groups were checked for differences in overall

Fig. 1 Overview of experimental task conditions. The task consisted

of two emotional face blocks and three control blocks. Each block contained six trials of 5 s with a total time of 30 s per block.

Partici-pants were instructed to indicate which of the two items at the bottom matched the item at the top

Table 1 Descriptive statistics

of the participants

a Child Behavioral Checklist b Brief Symptoms Inventory

Control PDS Statistics Age [mean (SD)] 7.77 (0.95) 7.75 (0.76) F(37;0.007), p = 0.95 Gender (% boys) 40 63 X2 = 1.6, p = 0.206 Ethnicity (%) X2 = 0.219, p = 0.896 Dutch 70 63 Non-Dutch, western 5 6 Non-Dutch, non-western 25 31 CBCLa score at 6 years

Internalizing [mean (SEM)] 7.9 (1.4) 9.3 (2.3) t(37, −0.49), p = 0.623 Externalizing [mean (SEM)] 8.3 (1.5) 10.6 (2.4) t(37, −0.24), p = 0.810 Prenatal BSIb score [mean (SD)] 0.13 (0.22) 1.14 (0.4) U = 340, p = 0.001

(4)

movement during scanning, assessed by independent t tests of mean scan-by-scan displacement for all six movement directions. Functional scans of participants were rigid body transformation aligned to their T1 image. The first four vol-umes were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration. Struc-tural scans were segmented using an age appropriate (age of 7) tissue probability map toolbox (the Template-O-Matic (TOM8) [25]. The segmentation parameters were subse-quently used to normalize functional and structural scans to Montreal Neurological Institute space (MNI 152) with the unified segmentation procedure implemented in SPM8. The normalized images were smoothed with a full width at half maximum kernel of 6 × 6 × 6 mm.

Statistical analysis was performed using general linear models, in which the experimental blocks were modeled with 30-s boxcars based on condition (emotion or control) and convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Seven covariates were included to remove any signal unrelated to the task (six movement parameters and whole brain mean signal of the functional scans). A one sample student t test was used to check voxel-wise for significant differences in the contrasts emotion > control. MarsBaR toolbox in SPM [26] and an anatomical amyg-dala mask from the Automated Anatomical Labeling Atlas (AAL) was utilized to extract overall contrast estimates for the bilateral amygdala. Univariate ANCOVA was used using SPSS to test for group differences in activity. We included gender as a covariate, because it was numerically imbalanced among the two groups. Results were robust also upon removal of influential values (more than 2.5 SD from mean).

To overcome missing values within the CBCL scales and the BSI postnatal values, multiple imputations were performed (Markov chain Monte Carlo; five imputations and ten iterations, using the prior CBCL assessment at 3 years of age, prenatal BSI assessments, gender, maternal characteristics and paternal emotional state as predictors). The imputed internalizing and externalizing scores were log transformed and were compared between groups using an independent student t test. Finally, a linear regression analysis was performed on the mean BOLD signal from the bilateral amygdala for the emotion > control contrast as dependent value with maternal PDS as predictor. To control for postnatal depressive symptoms of the mother, a second model included postnatal maternal depressive symptoms as predictor in addition to the PDS. In addition, a nonparamet-ric spearman correlation analysis was performed to com-pare prenatal depressive symptoms and postnatal depres-sive symptoms with the BOLD signal from the bilateral amygdala for the emotion > control contrast as dependent value, to verify no bimodality was influencing our results.

Results

Children of the PDS and control group did not differ signif-icantly in age, ethnicity or gender (Table 1). Furthermore, groups did not differ significantly in externalizing or inter-nalizing problems at age 6 years (Table 1).

As expected, the emotion condition was more challeng-ing than the control condition, leadchalleng-ing to slower responses [mean (SD) RT emotion condition = 2, 9 (0.5) s versus RT control condition = 1.7 (0.6) s]. FGreenhouse–Geisser(1, 29)

(158; p < 0.001); and lower accuracy (mean percentage correct (SD) emotion condition = 81% (12%) compared to the control condition = 90% (12%) FGreenhouse–Geisser (1,

29) (12.9, p = 0.001). Group identity did not affect over-all accuracy [F(1,29) = 0.22, p = 0.64] or response times

[F(1,29) = 0.41, p = 0.53]. Also, there were no significant group by condition interactions [accuracy F(1,29) = 0.0,

p = 0.99; reaction time F(1,29) = 0.631, p = 0.43]. Thus,

PDS and control children showed comparable task per-formance during fMRI, enabling us to compare neural responses between groups without evidence for potentially confounding behavioral differences.

In all children, emotional face matching resulted in sig-nificantly increased activity in the amygdala, hippocampus, fusiform gyrus and occipital lobe, compared to the control condition (Table 2). We extracted the mean bilateral amyg-dala BOLD signal in the emotion condition relative to the control condition for each group separately. Critically, the analysis showed that PDS children [mean (SD) = 0.49 (0.67)] had a stronger differential amygdala response [F(1,36) 7.02, p = 0.022; Fig. 2] compared to control chil-dren [mean (SD) = 0.02 (0.49)].

Linear regression analysis confirmed the positive rela-tion between PDS and activity to emorela-tional faces within the bilateral amygdala B = 38.5, 95% Confidence Inter-val (CI) 5.4–71.1, p = 0.023). Pre- and postnatal mater-nal depressive symptoms scores were positively correlated (n = 37; rs = 0.63 p < 0.001), suggesting group differences

were potentially driven by maternal depressive symptoms postnatally. However, when including maternal postna-tal depressive symptoms within the regression model the positive relation between PDS and amygdala reactiv-ity remained, although just missing formal significance (B = 43.7, 95% CI −1.2 to 88.5, p = 0.056). Interestingly, maternal postnatal depressive symptoms were not reliably associated with amygdala reactivity (B = −12.5, 95% CI

−80.1 to 55.2, p = 0.717). Therefore, it does seem that

pre-natal depressive symptoms predict amygdala responsivity over and above any shared variance. Nonparametric cor-relation analysis confirmed this finding, prenatal depres-sive symptoms show a positive correlation with amygdala

(5)

reactivity (Rs = 342 p = 0.038), while postnatal depressive symptoms did not (Rs = 0.236 p = 0.16).

Discussion

We investigated the association between prenatal mater-nal depressive symptoms and brain activity in 6- to 9-year-old children. We provide initial evidence of an elevated amygdala response to negative emotional stim-uli in children exposed to maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy. While these findings by themselves do not permit firm conclusions on causality, they suggest that being exposed to maternal depressive symptoms pre-natally may be related to enduring changes in later-life by affecting neural processing in young children.

An early and substantially larger study of the same prospective cohort, at a younger age, reported an asso-ciation between prenatal maternal depressive symptoms and reduced head and body growth during fetal life and increased affective problems in early childhood [4, 20]. Prenatal maternal depression is also related to differences in the offspring’s physical health, social functioning and stress levels [10]. Here we show the association between PDS exposure and amygdala functionality in school-age children, at an school-age much younger than the typical age of onset of clinical relevant affective psychopathol-ogy. While we did not find any differences in internaliz-ing or externalizinternaliz-ing problems as a function of maternal

Table 2 Main effect of the

emotional matching task

Depicted are the FWE corrected active regions for the emotion > control contrast *** p < 0.001 ** p < 0.01 * p = < 0.05

Hemisphere Number of voxels Peak MNI coordinates Peak t value

x y z

Emotion > control

Fusiform gyrus R 38 40 −50 −24 9.24***

Fusiform gyrus L 61 −41 −50 −24 8.68***

Inferior occipital lobe R 175 33 −87 −8 8.16***

Medial Temporal lobe R 57 48 −36 0 9.54***

Inferior frontal gyrus L 62 −56 20 20 9.89***

Inferior fontal gyrus R 87 59 23 12 7.93***

Hippocampus and amygdala L 15 −19 −10 −12 6.24**

Hippocampus R 1 22 −32 0 5.54** Lingual L 9 −19 −91 −16 5.19** Lingual R 3 11 −32 −1 6.15** Precentral lobe L 4 −41 1 40 5.81** Precentral lobe R 1 40 5 60 5.79* Precuneus R 3 3 −61 32 5.64*

Fig. 2 Amygdala hyperresponsiveness in children of mothers with

clinically relevant prenatal depressive symptoms compared to con-trol children. Displayed are mean BOLD fMRI contrast estimates extracted from the anatomically defined bilateral amygdala across all voxels for the contrast emotion > control condition separately for each group F(1,36) 7.02, p = 0.022 (error bars represent standard error of the mean, AU arbitrary units, PDS prenatal depressive symp-toms, *p < 0.05)

(6)

depression, our sample was likely too small to reveal these effects [4]. To the best of our knowledge, altered amygdala functionality related to prenatal maternal depressive symptoms has not been investigated before. However, there are data on postnatal early life exposure to stressors, such as maltreatment or confinement to an orphanage on functional brain activity. These early life events are similarly related to higher amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli, in both children and adults, corre-sponding to our current findings [27–29].

Pre- and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms are positively correlated [30]. Studies investigating the effects of pre- and postnatal exposure on psychopathology later in life have shown that prenatal exposure and postnatal exposure are differentially associated with family risk factors. Prenatal exposure to depression increases risk of depression at age 18 independent of exposure to maternal postnatal depression [9]. Although both prenatal mater-nal depression and postnatal matermater-nal depression have been associated with altered amygdala structure in the offspring [12, 31], this previous study underlined that the two periods could influence child development in different ways [9]. In our study, adding maternal postnatal depres-sive symptoms, acquired at the age of 3 years postpartum, in the regression model resulted only in a slight attenua-tion of the associaattenua-tion of PDS with amygdala reactivity. Indeed, postnatal depressive symptoms at this time point appeared not to impact amygdala reactivity. Seemingly, and perhaps surprisingly, prenatal rather than postna-tal depressive symptoms of the mother are more strongly associated with amygdala hyperresponsivity, at least at the time points of our assessments. This would also suggest that the observed amygdala hyperresponsivity is not sim-ply associated with an inherited vulnerability for depres-sion [32] but related to an additional environmental cause within a critical prenatal period of development. It has to be noted, however, that we controlled for postnatal depres-sive symptoms only at the moment when the children were 3 years of age, meaning that transient changes in postnatal depressive symptoms, for instance immediately postpar-tum, are not accounted for.

The ability of humans to regulate their emotions is a key element of affective fitness. The amygdala is at the core of this regulatory process [6]. Failure to properly regulate emotional responses, as reflected in heightened amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli, has been related to affec-tive psychopathology or risk thereof [33, 34]. Children and young adults with affective disorders, for instance, show increased amygdala responsivity [35–37]. In addition, amygdala hyperactivity is associated with disease risk of psychopathology, even when individuals are currently in a disease-free state [38–40]. These findings indicate that amygdala hyperresponsivity could reflect a predisposition

to psychopathology in later-life for this specific group of children, and follow-up studies would be necessary to con-firm this line of thought.

Interestingly, in our study we found heightened amyg-dala responsivity to emotional stimuli in children exposed to PDS, whereas control children displayed a limited signal change in response to the same stimuli. In line with this, previous studies have shown an overall reduced responsiv-ity of the amygdala among children, as compared to adoles-cents or adults, with the level of amygdala activity not dis-criminating between neutral and negative emotional stimuli [41–43]. Moreover, in a similar emotional face matching study in children, the healthy control group shows no sig-nificant amygdala activity increase associated with fearful faces compared to baseline [27]. Hence, it is interesting that we observed enhanced amygdala reactivity to negative emotional faces in the PDS exposed children at this young age, potentially suggesting a premature response compared to the general child population.

Limitations of our study have to be kept in mind. Most importantly, depression is a complex disease. Altered negative mood accompanies, for instance, dysregula-tion of the HPA-axis, altered immune funcdysregula-tion and nega-tive lifestyle habits including smoking, increased alcohol intake, changes in food intake or sleep disturbances [44–

46]. Therefore, prenatal maternal depression may expose the unborn child to a range of adverse conditions, but our sample size did not permit to elucidate their specific roles on amygdala hyperactivity. Secondly, we do not have spe-cific parent–child interaction measures within this current sample, which could be of influence in brain development of the child [47]. Moreover, the emotional face matching task used in this study is tapping in the face processing capabilities of the young participants next to emotional processing. These two constructs are indistinguishable in our study, and future studies are necessary to separate the involvement of both aspects within the activation of the amygdala.

In sum, we provide initial empirical evidence that prena-tal, clinically relevant depressive symptoms of mothers are associated with amygdala hyperresponsivity in their chil-dren at 6–9 years of age. Altered responsivity of the amyg-dala may serve as a risk factor for children’s mental health in later-life. Large-scale epidemiological long-term follow-up studies are necessary to confirm the link between prena-tal maternal depression, amygdala functioning and risk of psychiatric disease in offspring. Such findings should trig-ger research into potential prevention strategies to counter-act such long lasting consequences at an early stage. Our results are a first step forward in recognizing a mechanistic link between maternal mood during pregnancy, functional properties of the amygdala and later live vulnerability for psychopathology in offspring.

(7)

Acknowledgements This study was made possible with

finan-cial support from NWO Brain and Cognition grant (# 433-09-311), the Sophia Children’s Hospital Fund (SSWO-616), the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (No. 633595, DynaHealth) and the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMW Geestkracht Program 10.000.1003 and ZonMw TOP 40-00812-98-11021). MRI data acquisition was spon-sored in part by the European Community’s 7th Framework Pro-gramme (FP7/2008-2013, 212652). Supercomputing resources were supported by the NWO Physical Sciences Division (Exacte Weten-schappen) and SURFsara (Lisa compute cluster, www.surfsara.nl). The Generation R Study is conducted by the Erasmus Medical Center in close collaboration with the School of Law and Faculty of Social Sciences of the Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Municipal Health Service Rotterdam area, Rotterdam, the Rotterdam Homecare Foun-dation, Rotterdam and the Stichting Trombosedienst & Artsenlabora-torium Rijnmond (STAR-MDC), Rotterdam. We gratefully acknowl-edge the contribution of children and parents, general practitioners, hospitals, midwives and pharmacies in Rotterdam. We thank members of the neuroimaging team of the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry for their efforts in study coordination, data collection and technical support. The general design of Generation R Study is made possible by financial support from the Erasmus Medical Center, Rot-terdam, the Erasmus University RotRot-terdam, ZonMw, the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO), and the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest All authors declare no conflict of interest. The

manuscript does not contain clinical studies or patient data.

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the

Crea-tive Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( http://crea-tivecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

References

1. Evans J, Heron J, Francomb H et al (2001) Cohort study of depressed mood during pregnancy and after childbirth. BMJ 323:257–260

2. Gerardin P, Wendland J, Bodeau N et al (2010) Depression dur-ing pregnancy. J Clin Psychiatry 72:378–387. doi:10.4088/ JCP.09m05724blu

3. Van Batenburg-Eddes T, Brion MJ, Henrichs J et al (2013) Parental depressive and anxiety symptoms during pregnancy and attention problems in children: a cross-cohort consistency study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54:591–600. doi:10.1111/ jcpp.12023

4. El Marroun HE, White TJH, van der Knaap NJF et al (2014) Prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and social responsiveness symptoms of autism: population-based study of young children. Br J Psychiatry 205:95–102. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.113.127746

5. Hay DF, Pawlby S, Waters CS et al (2010) Mothers’ antenatal depression and their children’s antisocial outcomes. Child Dev 81:149–165. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01386.x

6. Phelps EA, LeDoux JE (2005) Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48:175–187. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025

7. Davis M, Whalen P (2001) The amygdala: vigilance and emo-tion. Mol Psychiatry 6:13–34

8. O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Jenkins J et al (2013) Prenatal mater-nal mood is associated with altered diurmater-nal cortisol in adoles-cence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:1630–1638. doi:10.1016/j. psyneuen.2013.01.008

9. Pearson RM, Evans J, Kounali D et al (2013) Maternal depression during pregnancy and the postnatal period: risks and possible mechanisms for offspring depression at age 18 years. JAMA Psychiatry 70:1312–1319. doi:10.1001/ jamapsychiatry.2013.2163

10. Raposa E, Hammen C, Brennan P, Najman J (2014) The long-term effects of maternal depression: early childhood physical health as a pathway to offspring depression. J Adolesc Health 54:88–93. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.07.038

11. Pawlby S, Hay D, Sharp D et al (2011) Antenatal depression and offspring psychopathology: the influence of childhood maltreatment. Br J Psychiatry 199:106–112. doi:10.1192/bjp. bp.110.087734

12. Rifkin-Graboi A, Bai J, Chen H et al (2013) Prenatal maternal depression associates with microstructure of right amygdala in neonates at birth. Biol Psychiatry 74:837–844. doi:10.1016/j. biopsych.2013.06.019

13. Qiu A, Anh TT, Li Y et al (2015) Prenatal maternal depression alters amygdala functional connectivity in 6-month-old infants. Transl Psychiatry 5:e508. doi:10.1038/tp.2015.3

14. Sandman CA, Buss C, Head K, Davis EP (2015) Fetal expo-sure to maternal depressive symptoms is associated with corti-cal thickness in late childhood. Biol Psychiatry 77:324–334. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.025

15. El Marroun H, Tiemeier H, Muetzel RL et al (2016) Prenatal exposure to maternal and paternal depressive symptoms and brain morphology: a population-based prospective neuroim-aging study in young children. Depress Anxiety 33:658–666. doi:10.1002/da.22524

16. White T, El Marroun H, Nijs I et al (2013) Pediatric population-based neuroimaging and the Generation R Study: the intersection of developmental neuroscience and epidemiology. Eur J Epide-miol 28:99–111. doi:10.1007/s10654-013-9768-0

17. Jaddoe V, van Duijn CM, van der Heijden AJ et al (2010) The Generation R Study: design and cohort update 2010. Eur J Epi-demiol 25:823–841. doi:10.1007/s10654-010-9516-7

18. Derogatis L, Melisaratos N (1983) The brief symptom inventory: an introductory report. Psychol Med 3:595–605

19. de Beurs E (2004) Brief symptom inventory, handleiding (Dutch). doi:10.1037/t03829-000

20. El Marroun H, Jaddoe VW, Hudziak JJ et al (2012) Maternal use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, fetal growth, and risk of adverse birth outcomes. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69:706–714. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2333

21. Achenbach TM, Ruffle TM (2000) The child behavior checklist and related forms for assessing behavioral/emotional problems and competencies. Pediatr Rev 21:265–271

22. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LM (2000) Manual for the ASEBA preschool form and profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, Burlington

23. Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Weinberger DR (2006) Imaging genet-ics: perspectives from studies of genetically driven variation in serotonin function and corticolimbic affective processing. Biol Psychiatry 59:888–897. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.005

24. van Wingen GA, van Eijndhoven P, Tendolkar I et al (2011) Neural basis of emotion recognition deficits in first-episode

(8)

major depression. Psychol Med 41:1397–1405. doi:10.1017/ S0033291710002084

25. Wilke M, Schmithorst VJ, Holland SK (2003) Normative pedi-atric brain data for spatial normalization and segmentation dif-fers from standard adult data. Magn Reson Med 50:749–757. doi:10.1002/mrm.10606

26. Brett M, Anton J, Valabregue R, Poline J (2002) Region of inter-est analysis using an SPM toolbox. 8th Int Conferance Funct Mapp Hum Brain 16:abstract 497

27. Tottenham N, Hare TA, Millner A et al (2011) Elevated amyg-dala response to faces following early deprivation. Dev Sci 14:190–204. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00971.x

28. McCrory EJ, De Brito SA, Sebastian CL et al (2011) Heightened neural reactivity to threat in child victims of family violence. Curr Biol 21:R947–R948. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.015

29. Dannlowski U, Kugel H, Huber F et al (2013) Childhood mal-treatment is associated with an automatic negative emotion pro-cessing bias in the amygdala. Hum Brain Mapp 34:2899–2909. doi:10.1002/hbm.22112

30. Beck CT (2001) Predictors of postpartum depression. Nurs Res 50:275–285. doi:10.1097/00006199-200109000-00004

31. Moutsiana C, Johnstone T, Murray L et al (2015) Insecure attachment during infancy predicts greater amygdala volumes in early adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 56:540–548. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12317

32. Pilhatsch M, Vetter NC, Hübner T et al (2014) Amygdala-func-tion perturbaAmygdala-func-tions in healthy mid-adolescents with familial liabil-ity for depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 53(559– 568):e6. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2014.02.010

33. Victor TA, Furey ML, Fromm SJ et al (2010) Relationship between amygdala responses to masked faces and mood state and treatment in major depressive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:1128–1138

34. van Marle HJF, Hermans EJ, Qin S, Fernández G (2009) From specificity to sensitivity: how acute stress affects amygdala pro-cessing of biologically salient stimuli. Biol Psychiatry 66:649– 655. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.014

35. Gaffrey MS, Luby JL, Belden AC et al (2011) Association between depression severity and amygdala reactivity during sad face viewing in depressed preschoolers: an fMRI study. J Affect Disord 129:364–370. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2010.08.031

36. Yang TT, Simmons AN, Matthews SC et al (2010) Adolescents with major depression demonstrate increased amygdala activa-tion. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49:42–51

37. Van Den Bulk BG, Meens PHF, Van Lang NDJ et al (2014) Amygdala activation during emotional face processing in adoles-cents with affective disorders: the role of underlying depression and anxiety symptoms. Front Hum Neurosci 8:393. doi:10.3389/ fnhum.2014.00393

38. Monk CS, Klein RG et al (2008) Amygdala and nucleus accum-bens activation to emotional facial expressions in children and adolescents at risk for major depression. Am J Psychiatry 165:90–98

39. Zhong M, Wang X, Xiao J et al (2011) Amygdala hyperactivation and prefrontal hypoactivation in subjects with cognitive vulner-ability to depression. Biol Psychol 88:233–242. doi:10.1016/j. biopsycho.2011.08.007

40. Wolfensberger SPA, Veltman DJ, Hoogendijk WJG et al (2008) Amygdala responses to emotional faces in twins discordant or concordant for the risk for anxiety and depression. Neuroimage 41:544–552. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.053

41. Lobaugh NJ, Gibson E, Taylor MJ (2006) Children recruit dis-tinct neural systems for implicit emotional face processing. Neu-roReport 17:215–219

42. Thomas LA, De Bellis MD, Graham R, LaBar KS (2007) Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Dev Sci 10:547–558. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00614.x

43. Viding E, Sebastian CL, Dadds MR et al (2012) Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry 169:1109–1116

44. Patel A (2013) Review: the role of inflammation in depression. Psychiatr Danub 25(Suppl 2):S216–S223

45. Lopez-Duran NL, Kovacs M, George CJ (2009) Hypotha-lamic–pituitary–adrenal axis dysregulation in depressed children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:1272–1283. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.016

46. Palagini L, Baglioni C, Ciapparelli A et al (2013) REM sleep dysregulation in depression: state of the art. Sleep Med Rev 17:377–390. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2012.11.001

47. Tiemeier H, Velders FP, Szekely E et al (2012) The Generation R Study: a review of design, findings to date, and a study of the 5-HTTLPR by environmental interaction from fetal life onward. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51(1119–1135):e7. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.021

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

ternal free thyroid hormone (fT4 concentrations; ⬍10th percentile) at 12 weeks’ gestation (case patients) and 96 neonates who were born to women whose fT4 values were between the

Figure 2 presents a scatter diagram of the group of women with the lowest 10th percentile plasma fT4 concentrations at 12 weeks’ gestation and correlates maternal fT4

In conclusion, apart from depression and sleeping problems, women with low levels of thyroid hormone during early gestation are at risk for poor cognitive function

(2014) and previous PREMA re- search (Braeken et al., 2013; de Weerth et al., 2013; Henrichs et al., 2009; Monk et al., 2004), we hypothesized that: (1) 4- year- olds show

Given the expected link between adolescents’ depressive symptoms and maternal psychological control, and between maternal psychological control and maternal empathy, the aim of

The associations between maternal and paternal symptoms of depression and anxiety during preg- nancy and child attention problems were additionally adjusted for the respective

In this study, women with an EPDS score ≥10 were more likely to be born outside of Canada; to report having more chronic health conditions, more life stress and less social

Steeds nog klinkt het buitelend geluid van een Kievit of het jubelend fluiten van een Grutto zo nu en dan op in de val­ lende schemer die zich met de rijzende neve l vermengt.