• No results found

Medicalization in a world of creolization : clinicians' perspectives on categorizing children's behavioral difficulties as ADHD in Pune, India

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Medicalization in a world of creolization : clinicians' perspectives on categorizing children's behavioral difficulties as ADHD in Pune, India"

Copied!
63
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

 

 

 

 

Clinicians’  perspectives  on  categorizing  children’s    

                                             behavioral  difficulties    as  ADHD  in  Pune,  India  

                                                       

 

   

 

               

Supervisor:  Dr.  Christian  Bröer    -­‐      Second  reader:  Dr.  Maarten  Bode  

                         

Medical  Anthropology  and  Sociology  

                 

University  of  Amsterdam  

                 

               GSSS  

(2)

Inhoudsopgave  

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS  ...  3  

INTRODUCTION  ...  4  

PROBLEM  STATEMENT  ...  5  

RELEVANCE  ...  7  

1.   SCALES  OF  ATTENTION  ...  8  

THEORETICAL  FRAMEWORK  ...  8  

Globalization  and  creolization  ...  8  

Ecological  niche  ...  11  

METHODOLOGY  ...  12  

Research  location  ...  13  

Respondents  ...  13  

Sampling  and  sample  ...  13  

Data  collection  ...  15  

Data  analyses  ...  16  

Ethical  consideration  ...  16  

Reflection  and  limitation  ...  17  

2.  PRESENTING  ISSUES  ...  19  

HELPSEEKING  ROUTES  ...  19  

THE  PRESENTING  QUEST  ...  21  

WHAT  IS  AT  STAKE  ...  27  

3.  NEGOTIATING  ADHD  ...  30  

WEBS  OF  MEANING  ...  30  

TO  LABEL  OR  NOT  TO  LABEL  ...  36  

TOWARDS  A  MULTIPLICY  OF  UNDERSTANDINGS  ...  41  

PARTIAL  CONNECTIONS  ...  42  

APPENDIX  ...  46  

APPENDIX  1  SAMPLE  RESPONDENTS  ...  47  

APPENDIX  2  INTERVIEW  GUIDELINE  ...  49  

APPENDIX  3  COMPARISON  PERSPECTIVES  CLINICIANS  ...  52  

APPENDIX  4  INFORMED  CONSENT  ...  53  

APPENDIX  5  NEWSPAPER  ARTICLE  RISE  ADHD  ...  54  

APPENDIX  6    PHARMA-­‐  SPONSORED  POSTERS  ADHD  ...  55  

APPENDIX  7  NEWSPAPER  ARTICLE  STIMULANT  DRUGS  ...  56  

                 

(3)

Acknowledgments  

 

This  thesis  is  the  product  of  a  ten-­‐week  study  in  Pune,  India  and-­‐a  six  month  process  of  

constructing  and  deconstructing  the  quest  for  categorization  of  childhood  behavior  as  ADHD  in   creolizing  the  world.  This  result  would  not  have  been  possible  without  a  range  of  people,  to  whom  I   wish  to  express  my  gratitude.    

 

On  my  mission  to  learn  more  about  professional’s  perceptions  of  challenging  behavior,  I  spent  most   of  my  time  knocking  on  doors  of  hospitals,  clinics  and  universities  in  the  city  of  Pune.  Luckily,  those   doors  most  often  opened  up,  and  so  did  the  professionals  to  whom  I  spoke.  I  hereby  want  to  thank   all  of  those  who  gave  time  to  participate  in  this  study.    I  would  not  have  knocked  on  so  many  doors   if  it  wasn’t  for  the  help  of  Hedyeh  and  Iman  -­‐  I  owe  my  pleasant  stay  in  Pune  to  your  hospitality,   friendship  and  handwritten  maps  of  the  city.  

 

On  my  mission  to  write  up  the  findings,  my  family  and  friends  were  most  valuable.  I  wish  to  thank   them  for  their  continued  encouragement  in  pursuing  this  study,  and  for  tolerating  my  attention   deficit  in  the  last  few  months.  Hannah,  Marlies  &  Sebas,  Elles,  Lotte;  who  sat  through  numerous   deja-­‐vu’s  of  academic  worries,  and  were  kind  enough  to  hear  me  out  and  take  me  out  of  my  thesis   bubble  at  the  right  time.  Margriet,  my  mom,  who  hosted  a  number  of  thesis-­‐camps  and  reminded   me  that  learning  has  no  set  route  nor  an  expiration  date.  I  owe  a  special  thanks  to  Rochana,  who   masters  the  art  of  changing  perspectives  and  encouragement  like  no  one  else.  

 

On  my  mission  to  pursue  the  art  of  doing  research  in  Medical  Anthropology  and  Sociology,  I  met   several  inspiring  teachers  who  I  wish  to  thank  for  their  contribution  to  my  process  of  learning.  

To  Christian  Bröer,  who  guided  me  through  this  study  in  a  series  of  thought  provoking  

conversations  -­‐  your  style  of  supervision  encouraged  me  not  to  take  for  granted  what  seemed  self-­‐ evident,  and  allowed  me  to  build  on  my  pre-­‐  excising  knowledge  of  mental  health  –  and  Maarten   Bode,  who  was  kind  enough  to  make  time  to  review  my  research  plans  prior  to  fieldwork  and  to   discuss  my  findings  during  the  stage  of  data  analyses.  

(4)

 

Introduction  

 

Ever  since  I  began  working  as  a  therapist  in  Dutch  mental  health  care,  I  have  been  interested  in  the   controversial  area  of  classifying  childhood  struggles  as  psychiatric  concern.  

In  my  experience,  childhood  struggles  might  be  presented  as  ‘he  throws  with  chairs’,  ‘she  quit   school’  or  ‘he  deals  drugs’.  It  is  upon  the  clinician,  partly  in  collaboration  with  parents,  to  decide  if   these  difficulties  should  be  addressed  as  a  psychiatric  condition.  With  these  experiences  in  mind,   the  issue  of  medicalization  caught  my  interest  during  the  master’s  program  Medical  Anthropology   and  Sociology    (Felipe,  2011:5;  Rafalovich,  2013;  Singh  et  al.,  2013).    

In  my  work  in  the  Netherlands,  I  noticed  great  differences  in  the  way  clinicians  approached   children’s  behavioral  struggles.  In  community-­‐based  work  in  impoverished  areas  these  struggles   were  often  not  perceived  as  psychiatric  concerns,  but  instead  were  framed  as  a  social  problem.   Mental  health,  in  this  community,  was  often  perceived  as  a  luxury  ‘for  famous  people’  or  suitable  for   ‘lunatics’  (Horton,  2007:806).  In  a  clinical  setting,  however,  similar  struggles  were  more  likely  to  be   perceived  as  mental  health  problem  and  categorized  accordingly.  This  is  how  I  became  interested   in  the  empirical  variations  of  medicalization,  as  mediated  by  the  context  in  which  clinicians  practice   and  caretakers  seek  help.  

 

I  found  a  particular  interest  in  readings  diagnosing  children’s  mental  health  categories  across  the   world.  This  is  how  I  got  introduced  to  writings  on  the  ‘globalization  of  psychiatry’,  and  this  is  how  I   became  acquainted  with  critical  voices  in  this  field  of  study  and  got  drawn  into  ongoing  debates  on   diagnosing  Attention  Deficit  Hyperactive  Disorder  (ADHD)  in  ‘non  Western’  settings.  These  debates,   I  noticed,  often  expressed  concerns  over  the  homogenizing  effects  of  introducing  ‘Western  based   psychiatry’  in  a  context  where  these  categories  had  not  originated  (Timimi  &  Leo,  2009a).  I  also   noticed  that  these  writings  often  approached  medicalization  ‘after  it  has  taken  place’,  in  other   words  once  a  diagnosis  was  granted.  When  and  how  and  which  childhood  difficulties  are  

medicalized,  I  suggest,  can  differ  from  context  to  context.  The  clinician,  and  the  context  he  or  she   operates  in,  is  key  in  this  process  of  translating  or  not  translating  struggles  as  a  mental  health   concern.  The  way  that  professionals  give  meaning  to  behavioral  problems,  I  suggest,  is  partly   embedded  in  and  shaped  by  the  context  within  which  they  work.  By  doing  so,  he  or  she  mediates  

(5)

between  local  knowledge  on  the  one  hand  and  universalistic  knowledge  on  the  other.  Local,  in  this   case,  can  refer  to  knowledge  about  needs,  resources  and  beliefs  in  the  given  context  -­‐  and  universal   can  refer  to  knowledge  from  western-­‐based  psychiatry  and  classification  systems  (Watters,  2010).    

This  is  how  I  became  interested  in  the  process  of  categorizing  behavioral  difficulties  as  ADHD   through  the  eyes  of  psychiatrists,  psychologists  and  paediatricians  in  Pune,  India  (Singh  et  al.,   2013).    A  body  of  literature  suggested  a  worrisome  impact  of  the  spread  of  the  ‘Western’     categorisation  of  ADHD  in  developing  countries  due  to  globalization.  However,  there  are  few   writings  from  the  emic  perceptions  of  the  designated  ‘medicalizers’  and  ‘globalizers’  themselves:   the  clinicians  in  India  who  asses  reported  behavioral  difficulties  and  categorize  these  as  ADHD,  or   not.      

Problem  statement      

A  range  of  studies  has  indicated  a  sharp  rise  of  ADHD  diagnosis  worldwide  (Singh,  Filipe,  Bard,   Bergey,  &  Baker,  2013:385).  India  is  one  of  the  non-­‐Western  settings  were  this  rise  has  been   reported  (David,  2013;  Hasan  &  Tripathi,  2014).    This  global  rise  has  been  an  object  of  study  for   medical  anthropology  and  sociology:  Singh  (2011:889)  described  these  studies  ‘as  fueled  with   worries  and  concerns  over  medicalization  of  childhood  behavior’.  Timimi  argued  that  ADHD  is   wrongfully  explained  as  universal  psychiatric  disorder.  The  rise  of  ADHD  should  rather  be   explained  as  a  result  of  globalization,  which  creates  the  possibility  of  categorizing  deviant   behaviour  as  a  mental  health  concern  on  a  global  scale  (Rafalovich,  2013,  Timimi,  2005,  2010).    The  spread  of  this  category  is  facilitated  through  the  work  of  psychiatrists,  psychologists  and   pediatricians  in  low-­‐income  settings.  This  spread  is  argued  to  be  worrisome,  as  the  adoption  of   ADHD  as  a  concept  might  go  hand  in  hand  with  adopting  built-­‐in  Western  notions  and  the  built-­‐in   Western  approach.  This  is  argued  as  challenging,  as  there  are  always  local  ideas  and  rules  about   acceptable  and  deviant  child  behavior  and  how  this  should  be  addressed  in  addition  to  local  ideas   about  health,  illness,  causation  and  healing  (Timimi,  2011).  Using  the  category  of  ADHD,  therefore,   might  shape  which  children’s  behaviours  are  perceived  as  deviant  and  could  replace  local  ways  of   interpreting  and  handling  these.  These  critiques  of  cultural  imperialism  and  homogenization  are   echoed  in  Ethan  Watters’  book  Crazy  like  us,  the  globalization  of  an  American  psyche.  (Watters,   2010).      

 

Amaral  (2007)  argued  that  ’a  consistency  of  prevalence  of  ADHD,  does  not  necessarily  mean  that   the  that  phenomenon  is  interpreted  the  same  way  around  the  world’  (in:Singh  et  al.,  2013:385).  

(6)

This  suggests  that  there  are  multiple  approaches  and  pathways  to  ADHD,  each  shaped  by  the   context  in  which  caretakers  live  and  clinicians  practice.  This  was  shown  in  a  small-­‐scale  study  in   Goa,  India,  where  parents  expressed  concerns  such  as  ‘ear  wax’,  ‘physical  weakness’,  ‘headaches’   and  educational  difficulties  upon  seeing  a  psychiatrist  (Wilcox  et  al,  2007:  1605).  In  cases  where   these  difficulties  were  categorized  according  to  the  DSM  category  ADHD,  parents  did  not  adopt  its   biomedical  meaning  and  did  not  perceive  the  difficulties  as  a  mental  health  disorder  (Wilcox  et  al,   2007:  1609).  Instead  parents  framed  children’s  struggles  differently  (Wilcox  et  al,  2007:  1608).   These  findings  indicate  that  clinicians  might  need  to  take  living  conditions,  explanatory  models,   class,  stigma,  local  healing  traditions,  ideas  about  childhood  and  perceptions  concerning  body  and   mind  into  account  in  their  work  (Wilcox  et  al.,  2007;  Patel  et  al.,  1998).  More  so,  these  findings   indicate  a  need  to  re-­‐evaluate  the  current  dominant  frameworks  to  interpret  the  global  rise  of   ADHD.  To  my  best  knowledge  there  has  been  no  research  on  ADHD  from  a  creolization  point  of   view.    

 

Philosopher  Annemarie  Mol  (2002:23)  argued  that  a  doctor  does  not  bring  a  disease  into  being;   instead  ‘there  must  be  a  patient  who  worries  or  wonders  about  something  and  a  clinician  who  is   willing  to  attend  to  it’.  Present  research  has  looked  into  parents,  nurses,  and  teachers’  explanatory   models  for  ADHD  in  India  (David,  2013;  Hasan  &  Tripathi,  2014;  Wilcox,  Washburn,  &  Patel,  2007).   To  my  best  knowledge  there  has  been  no  research  on  categorizing  behavioral  difficulties  as  ADHD   in  India  from  a  clinicians  point  of  view.  

 

This  choice  of  study  was  informed  by  a  multitude  of  recommendations  for  future  empirical   research  on  ADHD  in  low  income  and  non-­‐western  settings.  Timimi(Timimi  &  Leo,  2009b)   proposed  to  study  ‘what  different  behavioral  norms  and  ideals  are  found  in  different  settings?     (Timimi  &  Leo,  2009b:214).  Singh(2011:890)  proposed  to  explore  ‘  what  is  done  prior  to  an  ADHD   diagnosis’  and  to  study  ‘  how  physical  spaces,  social  spaces  and  national  spaces  help  to  create  and   to  constrain  an  ADHD  diagnosis?’  Lastly,  Felipe  suggested  that  future  research  on  ADHD  ‘should   reconstruct  the  pathway  to  an  ADHD  diagnosis’.  This  research  project  aims  to  take  up  a  small  part   of  these  quests.  This  resulted  in  the  following  question  for  this  study:  

“How  and  when  do  psychiatrists,  psychologists  and  pediatricians  in  Pune  categorize   reported  challenging  behavior  as  ADHD?  

   

(7)

Relevance  

The  study  situates  itself  in  the  social  science  debate  concerning  medicalization  in  relation  to  

globalization  (Watters,  2010)  and  cross-­‐cultural  validity  of  mental  health  diagnosis  (Timimi,  2005;   Watters,  2010).  Specifically,  it  positions  itself  within  ongoing  debates  on  the  impact  of  ‘  

globalization  of  the  Psy  science’  in  low-­‐income  settings  (Watters,  2010).  Creolization  theory   (Hannerz,  1987)  and  ecological  niche  theory  (Singh,  2011;  2013)  were  used  as  a  theoretical   frameworks  to  approach  this  research.    This  study  aims  to  contribute  to  ‘diversification  and   localization  of  ADHD  analyses’  (Singh,  2011:889).  

 

The  first  chapter,  Scales  of  attention,  reviews  theoretical  frameworks  that  have  informed  current   studies  on  ADHD  in  non-­‐western  settings.  Creolization  theory  and  ecological  systems  theory  are   proposed  as  fitting  analytical  frameworks  for  this  study.  An  insight  is  provided  into  how  this   framework  shaped  the  methodological  approach  to  this  research.  

The  second  chapter,  Presenting  issues,  looks  into  clinician’s  perceptions  of  help-­‐seeking  routes  and   help-­‐seeking  reasons.    Building  on  the  creolization  and  ecological  niche  perspectives,  delaying   factors  for  help  seeking  are  distinguished  and  interpreted.  It  is  argued  that  reasons  to  seek  help   have  to  outrank  reasons  not  to  do  so;  and  what  the  presented  complaints  represent  in  the   industrializing  city  of  Pune  is  explored.    

Chapter  three,  Negotiating  ADHD,  provides  an  insight  into  how  clinicians  explain  and  approach   behavioral  difficulties  associated  with  ADHD.    It  is  argued  that  ADHD  is  not  a  fixed  diagnosis  -­‐   ADHD  ‘happens’  in  negotiation  and  in  dialogue  with  what  is  most  valued  among  parents  and   clinicians.  This  study  distinguishes  four  strategies  for  categorization  of  behavioral  difficulties  and   explores  how  these  empirical  variations  of  categorization  can  be  explained  through  creolization  and   ecological  niche  theory.    

                 

(8)

 

1. Scales  of  attention  

 

Undertaking  research  requires  choosing  from  a  number  of  theoretical  and  methodological   approaches.    

Theory  was  used  in  four  ways  in  this  study.  Firstly,  theory  was  used  to  map  current  debates  on   categorizing  behavioral  challenges  as  ADHD.  Secondly,  it  was  used  to  deconstruct  and  problematize   existing  paradigms  on  the  ‘globalization  of  medicalization’.  Thirdly,  it  served  as  a  tool  to  inform  and   shape  data  collection.  Lastly,  theory  was  used  as  an  analytic  tool  to  explain  and  interpret  the  

empirical  findings.    

Different  scales  of  attention  allow  for  different  analyses  of  data,  and  this  chapter  presents  the   theory  and  methodology  that  informed  this  study.  

Theoretical  framework  

The  central  concepts  in  this  study  –namely  globalization,  medicalization  and  categorization–  are  all   part  of  ongoing  scholarly  debates.  Following  Mol  (2002:41),  I  chose  to    avoid  words  that  ‘are  central   to  raging  controversies  in  the  literature’,  and  followed  the  suggestion  to  find  newer  terms  ‘that   resonate  fewer  agendas’  instead.    I  noticed  that  the  words  diagnosis,  globalization  and  

medicalization  in  the  literature  were  often  loaded  words  -­‐  each  had  a  great  explanatory  power   connected  to  them.    These  terms  were  often  accompanied  by  the  connotation  of  a  ‘significant  

ethical  problem’  (Singh  et  al.,  2013:385).  Following  Annemarie  Mol’s  example,  this  research  aims  to   approach  these  concepts    ‘minus  the  negative  connotations  attributed  to  them’.    

Globalization  and  creolization  

The  practice  of  diagnosis  in  cross-­‐cultural  psychiatry  has  been  an  area  of  interest  for  

anthropologists  and  sociologists  for  a  long  time.  Studies  on  idioms  of  distress  in  non-­‐Western   settings,  as  well  as  studies  on  mental  health  care  for  migrant  or  refugee  populations  are  well   publicized  (Kleinman,  1987;  Kohrt  et  al.,  2014;  Nichter,  1981).    These  studies  looked  into  ‘cultural   differences  in  mode  of  onset,  symptomatology,  and  help-­‐seeking’  (Kleinman,  1987).  The  study  of   globalization  of  the  ‘psy  sciences’  is  a  relatively  new  area  of  debate  and  study.    Theorist  Robertson   (1992)  described  globalization  as  a  concept  that  refers  to  ‘both  the  compression  of  the  world  and   the  intensification  of  consciousness  about  the  world  as  a  whole’  (in:  Eriksen,  2007:4).  

(9)

trade  and  transnational  economic  activity,  faster  and  denser  communications  networks  and  

increased  tensions  in  -­‐and  between-­‐  groups  due  to  intensified  exposure”.  The  study  of  globalization   has  many  well  publicizedfaces;  though  this  thesis  will  only  review  those  positions  that  are  relevant   for  this  study(Eriksen,  2007:4).  

 

One  position  in  the  debate  is  that  globalization  is  a  one-­‐directional  process  that  ‘entails  global   uniformity’  (Eriksen,  2007:8)  and  ‘happens  at  an  unprecedented  speed’.  In  these  writings,  the   terms  ‘global1’  and  ‘local’  are  often  used  to  indicate  two  opposing  places,  and  two  opposing  types  of   knowledge.  I  refer  to  this  concern  as  ‘globalization-­‐as-­‐hegemony’.  It  is  argued  that  traditional  ways   of  interpreting  complaints  can  be  rapidly  replaced  by  interpretations  informed  by  a  biomedical,   Western  based  concepts.  The  spread  of  these  concepts  is  facilitated  by  the  global  use  of  -­‐  and   academic  instruction  in  -­‐  diagnostic  and  classification  manuals  DSM  V  and  ICD10.  These  manuals   provide  benchmarks  for  ‘mental  illness’,  and  thereby  a  ‘shared  language  for  comparison’  (Eriksen,   2007:65).  Consequently,  a  psychiatrist  in  India  and  the  Netherlands  can  diagnose  ADHD  following   the  same  descriptive  indicators.    This  is  often  problematized  as  a  case  of  ‘category  fallacy’  

(Kleinman,  1987):  suggesting  that  the  category  of  ADHD  lacks  coherence  and  validity  in  a  low-­‐ income  setting.    It  is  argued  that  ‘while  this  category  might  be  valid  in  its  original  –  Western  -­‐   context,  it  presents  medicalization  of  social  problems’  in  a  context  such  as  India  (Timimi  &  Leo,   2009a).  This  critique  is  prevalent  in  a  multitude  of  studies  on  globalizing  mental  health  

(Summerfield,  2012;  Timimi  &  Leo,  2009a).  

This  is  one  connotation  of  ‘globalization’  in  writings  in  medical  anthropology  and  sociology  on  the   rise  of  mental  health  diagnoses  worldwide.  

 

Another  position  the  debate  contests  is  this  connotation  of  globalization:  arguing  that  ‘at  the  very   best,  this  is  a  truth  with  serious  modifications’  (Eriksen,  2007).  The  counter  voices  in  this  debate   argue  that  ‘it  is  wrongfully  assumed  that  globalization  implies  westernization  and  standardization’   (Eriksen,  2007:5).  Hannerz    (1990)  argued  that  ‘  no  total  homogenization  of  systems  of  meaning   and  expression  has  occurred,  nor  does  it  appear  like  that  there  will  be  one  any  time  soon’(Eriksen,   2007:112).  A  need  for  a  new  concept  was  suggested;  a  concept  that  allows  room  to  analyse  ‘how   apparent  identical  products  are  perceived  in  distinctly  local  ways’  (Eriksen,  2007:59).    Creolization                                                                                                                  

1  Or  universalistic    

2  This  interviewguide  in  included  in  appendix  2.     3  See  appendix  4  

(10)

(Hannerz,  1987)  was  proposed  as  an  accurate  term  to  refer  to  this  phenomenon  (Eriksen,  2007:6).  

Creolization  refers  to  ‘the  intermingling  and  mixing  of  two,  or  several,  formerly  discrete  traditions   or  cultures’.  Hannerz  argued  that  concepts  and  services  are  always  adapted  to  a  degree,  so  that  they   fit  the  local  context.  The  concept  of  creolization  emphasizes  the  malleability  of  meanings,  and  

allows  us  to  see  that  people  relate  to  global  diversity  in  different  ways.  Eriksen  argued  that  this   process  goes  on  nearly  everywhere  -­‐  ‘  but  there  are  important  differences  as  to  the  degrees,  forms   and  speed  of  mixing’  (Eriksen,  2007:112).  The  concept  of  creolization  contributes  to  the  

understanding  that  adopting  and  adapting  ‘foreign’  flows  is  not  a  straightforward  process  of   translating.    This  is  echoed  in  the  following  definition  of  creolization  by  Glissant  (2007):  ‘I  call   creolization  the  meeting,  interference,  shock,  harmonies  and  dis-­‐  harmonies  between  the  cultures  of   the  world  .  .  .  [it]  has  the  following  characteristics:  the  lightening  speed  of  interaction  among  its   elements;  the  awareness  of  awareness:  thus  provoked  in  us;  the  reevaluation  of  the  various  elements   brought  into  contact  (for  creolization  has  no  presupposed  scale  of  values);  unforeseeable  results.   Creolization  is  not  a  simple  cross  breeding  that  would  produce  easily  anticipated  results’  (Kirmayer,   2006:  163).  

 

Following  Eriksen,  I  suggest  that  the  criticism  of  the  ‘global  psy-­‐skeptics’  is    ‘at  best  is  a  truth  with   serious  modifications’.  I  suggest  that  the  current  frameworks  to  study  globalization  of  mental   health  do  not  allow  enough  room  for  interconnectedness  and  partial  connections  (Strathern,  2005).     These  paradigms  of  the  ‘global  psy-­‐skeptics’  might  not  have  moved  along  with  new  ways  of  

conceptualizing  globalization.    Psychiatrist  Kirmayer  (2006)  proposed  a  creolization  perspective  as   a  paradigm  to  study  ‘the  new  cross  cultural  psychology’.  Anthropologist  Pinto  (Pinto,  2011:483)   argued  that  studying  mental  health  in  India  needs  a  concept  that  allows  room  to  analyze  how  ‘the   plurality  of  forms  of  healing  and  multiple  ideas  about  the  self’  are  in  constant  negotiation.  This   framework  should  go  beyond  emphasizing  differences  between  ‘Western  psychiatry’  and  ‘Indian   culture.’      

 

A  creolization  perspective  informed  this  study  in  two  ways.  Firstly,  it  shaped  the  aim  to  not  look  for   ‘how  clinicians  categorize  ADHD  differently’,  but  instead  to  study    ‘how  clinicians  categorize  ADHD   adaptably’.  The  need  to  study  negotiation  and  adaptation  processes  is  stressed  by  medical  

anthropologist  Kienzler  (2012:227),  who  found  that  the  category  of  Post  Traumatic  Stress  Disorder   in  Kosovo  was  not  “implemented  in  cultural  voids  but  are  appropriated  by  local  experts  and  lay   people  who  change  and  adapt  them  to  fit  their  respective  local  realities”.    

(11)

Secondly,  this  concept  contributed  to  problematizing  and  dividing  clinicians  into  contrasting  roles   of  being  both  ‘Indian’  and  ‘trained-­‐in-­‐western-­‐psychiatry’.  Strathern,  a  feminist  as  well  as  an  

anthropologist,  contested  that  people  ‘take  turns’  to  play  different  roles.    She  argued  that  ‘there  are   no  ‘opposing  roles’,  instead  there  are  partial  connections’.  Though  clinicians  are  ‘entry  points  to  an   international  flow  of  meaning  into  national  cultures’,  the  author  posed  that    ‘  there  is  no  walking   from  one  ‘place’  into  another  (Hannerz,  1987:556;  Strathern,  2005:35).  The  different  roles  do  not   constitute  half  or  whole’.  Following  Strathern,  I  propose  that  there  is  no  ‘Indian  side’  and  ‘western   side’(Strathern,  2005:35).  Instead  of  looking  for  differences,  or  essentializing  these  as  ‘Western’  or   ‘Indian’,  this  study  aims  to  look  for  partial  connections.    

 

In  short,  a  creolization  perspective  contributes  to  analyzing  categorization  of  ADHD  as  an  

interactive  process  that  is  shaped  by  multiple  flows  and  multiple  localities,  and  effects  groups  in   society  in  multiple  ways.    

 

Ecological  niche  

A  core  question  in  the  globalizing-­‐ADHD  debate  is  ‘whether  ADHD  fits  in  the  local  culture’.    

Culture,  in  one  of  many  definitions,  is  described  as  ‘  a  shared  system  of  beliefs  and  practices’  (Singh   et  al.,  2013:385).    For  this  study,  I  have  been  hesitant  to  use  ‘culture’  as  an  analytic  concept,  as  it   might  refer  to  an  endless  amount  of  variables  within  one  nation  state.    This  complexity  is  

heightened  as  for  India’s  ‘multi  every  thingness’  -­‐  with  it’s  multiplicity  of  religions,  languages,   ethnicity,  kinship  structures  in  a  country  of  1.4  billion  people,  India  is  deeply  stratified  along  the   lines  of  class,  caste,  gender,  language  and  religion.  Living  conditions  in  rural  and  urban  areas  might   differ  greatly,  and  what  might  be  true  for  a  child  growing  up  rural  Bihar  might  not  be  true  for  a   child  growing  up  in  urban  Bombay.    Or,  what  might  be  true  for  a  male  child  might  not  be  true  for  a   female.    

Due  to  this  complexity  I  suggest  that  culture,  in  this  research,  is  not  a  fitting  analytical  concept  to   study  ‘how  and  when  challenging  behavior  is  categorized  as  ADHD’  industrializing  urban  Pune.      

I  have  argued  that  the  concept  of  culture  in  India’s  ‘multi  everythingness’  and  industrializing  Pune   might  not  be  a  sufficient  analytical  concept  for  this  study.  I  chose  to  adopt  Singh’s  ecological  niche   model  as  a  less  ambiguous  concept,  that  allows  more  room  for  differentiation  (Singh,  2011;  Singh  et   al.,  2013:385).    Singh  uses  the  concept  of  the  ‘ecological  niche’  to  indicate  a  dominant,  shared,   preoccupation  towards  what  is  valued  in  a  child(Singh,  2013).    

(12)

An  ecological  niche  model  allows  the  highlighting  of  macro  and  micro  factors  that  influence  the   process  of  defining  behavior  as  problematic,  the  process  of  presenting  these  complaints  in  a  

biomedical  setting  and  the  process  of  categorizing  complaints  as  ADHD.    Mol  (2002:26)  argued  that   there  are  ‘endless  lists  of  heterogeneous  elements  that  can  be  either  highlighted  or  left  in  the   background’  in  a  study.  What  is  highlighted  ‘depends  on  the  purpose  and  character  of  the  

description’  (Mol,  2002:26).  Of  the  extensive  number  of  layers  that  make  up  India,  I  have  chosen  to   highlight    ‘government  policy’,  ‘mental  health  infrastructure’,  ‘  family  structure’,  ‘notions  of  

childhood’,    ‘gender’,  ‘class’  and  ‘school’  in  order  to  analyze  the  empirical  findings.  As  Singh’s   current  model  is  implicitly  built  on  the  concept  of  the  nation-­‐state,  I  propose  a  slight  modification   to  the  model  by  adding  by  adding    ‘modernization’  and  ‘globalization’  as  macro  layers  for  analysis.                  

   

 

   

Figure  1  ecological  niche  model     Methodology  

Based  on  a  review  of  literature,  I  have  suggested  that  current  analyses  of  globalizing  ADHD  have   not  sufficiently  integrated  experiences  of  clinicians’  practicing  in  low-­‐income  settings.    Therefore,   this  research  sets  out  to  empirically  explore  categorization  of  behavioral-­‐difficulties-­‐as  -­‐ADHD  from   a  clinician’s  perspective,  in  relation  to  well-­‐publicized  concerns  over  globalization  of  medicalization   (Filipe,  2011;  Singh  et  al.,  2013).      

reporting  behavior   and  diagnosing  ADHD  

school  

family  structure,  class,   gender,    notions  of   childhood   government  policy,   health  infrastructure   Globalization  of   mental  health,   modernization  

(13)

 

An  exploratory,  qualitative  research  strategy  was  considered  the  best  fit  to  approach  the  research   questions  and  aims.  By  collecting  and  analyzing  detailed  accounts  of  clinicians  perceptions,  this   study  seeks  to  unbracket  (Mol,  2002)  and  ‘de-­‐mystify  ‘(Barbour,  2013:17)  categorization  of   behavioral  difficulties  as  ADHD  in  India.    

 

Research  location  

Literature  on  mental  healthcare  in  India  indicated  that  psychiatrists,  psychologists  and  

pediatricians  mostly  practice  in  urban,  economically  prosperous  areas  (Carson  &  Chowdhury,   2000:394).  A  preliminary  exploration  on  the  Internet  confirmed  that  there  were  both  hospitals  and   private  practices  specializing  in  children’s  mental  health  in  Pune.    Due  to  the  number  of  practicing   clinicians,  and  thus  reasonable  hope  of  getting  access  to  these  professionals,  Pune  was  chosen  as  a   research  site.    

Pune  is  in  the  state  of  Maharashtra,  and  is  India’s  eighth  largest  metropolis  -­‐  with  a  population  of   2.4  million  people.  It  is  one  of  the  most  developed  areas  in  India,  with  the  6th  highest  income  per   capita  in  the  country.  It  is  known  for  its  high  concentration  of  software  companies,  auto  

manufacturers,  government  organizations  and  public  sector  organisations.  Moreover  it  is  known   for  its  educational  facilities  and  is  sometimes  referred  to  as  ‘Oxford  of  the  East’.    

Respondents    

The  choice  to  study  clinician’s  perceptions  of  the  issue  was  twofold.  Firstly,  I  have  argued  the  need   to  study  categorization  of  behavioral-­‐difficulties-­‐as-­‐ADHD  from  the  point  of  view  of  ‘the  designated   medicalizers’.    In  this  respect,  I  followed  Annemarie  Mol(2002:27),  who  argued  that  clinicians   ‘might  be  listened  to  as  if  they  were  their  own  ethnographers’.    This  research  approaches  clinicians-­‐ as-­‐ethnographers  in  collecting  their  –subjective  -­‐  views  on  the  research  questions.    

Sampling  and  sample  

Purposive  sampling  was  used  to  create  a  diverse  sample  (Barbour,  2013:23).  Following  Rafalovich   (2005:309),  I  defined  clinicians  as  ‘persons  with  the  accredited  authority  to  make  ADHD  diagnoses,   outline  methods  of  treatment  and  administer  such  treatments’.  After  an  initial  exploration,  I  found   that  ADHD-­‐clinicians  in  Pune  were  predominantly  psychiatrists,  pediatricians  and  

(school)psychologists.      

(14)

The  sample  consisted  of  twenty-­‐one  clinicians:  seven  psychiatrists,  six  pediatricians,  six  

psychologists  and  two  school  counselors.  Sixteen  were  female,  and  five  male.  The  clinicians  were   aged  between  25  and  76  years  old.  Seventeen  clinicians  worked  at  private  hospitals  or  medical   colleges,  two  exclusively  in  private  practices  and  two  at  a  school.  The  majority  of  the  clinicians   worked  in  a  private  practice  alongside  their  jobs  in  hospitals  or  schools.  The  selected  clinicians   worked  with  patients  from  high,  middle  and  low  income  families.  

                             

For  triangulation,  I  interviewed  six  scholars  in  Anthropology,  Sociology,  Psychology  and  Education,   Ayurvedic  Medicine  and  one  director  of  an  NGO  for  Mental  Health.    

Further  details  on  clinicians,  scholars  and  stakeholders  in  NGOs  for  mental  health  are  presented  in   appendix  1.     psychiatrist,   7   psychologist,   6   pediatrician,   6   school   counselor:    2   Female,  16   Male,  5  

Figure  1.1  Sample  respondents    

(15)

 

Three  clinicians  and  scholars  were  initially  recruited  through  the  recommendations  of  a  faculty   member  of  the  University  of  Amsterdam,  and  by  using  my  own  professional  background.  These   contacts  were  established  upon  arrival  in  Pune,  and  resulted  in  personal  recommendations  and   contact  details  for  other  colleagues.  This  was  followed  by  a  four-­‐week  period  of  being  introduced  to   these  ‘gatekeeper’  clinicians.  After  these  initial  introductory  meetings,  all  of  the  contacted  clinicians   agreed  to  participate  in  the  study.  These  meetings  resulted  in  more  recommendations  and  contact   details:  the  sample  can  be  said  to  have  grown  through  the  ‘snowballing’  approach.  

A  second  strategy  for  obtaining  access  consisted  of  visiting  conferences  on  Public  health,  Inclusive   Education  and  visiting  faculty  members  of  the  Sociology,  Psychology,  Anthropology  and  Education   departments.  The  purpose  of  this  network  building  was  threefold.  The  first  reason  was  to  explore   whether  behavior  of  children  came  up  as  a  concern  during  the  conferences  and,  if  so,  which   behavior  was  mentioned  as  a  cause  for  concern.  The  second  reason  was  to  explore  the  language   used  to  indicate  behavioral  problems,  their  root  causes  and  preferred  ways  of  handling  them  by   non-­‐clinicians.  This  was  utilised  as  a  way  to  triangulate  data,  and  to  explore  in  which  existing   viewpoints  clinicians  perspectives  were  embedded.  Lastly,  I  discovered  that  personal  

recommendations  were  crucial  to  get  access  to  clinicians.  The  scholars  I  met  were  well  connected  in   Pune,  and  were  often  willing  to  recommend  me  to  a  clinician  in  their  network.  

 

Data  collection  

Data  was  collected  through  semi-­‐structured  interviews  and  informal  conversations.  All  of  the   formal  interviews  were  conducted  face-­‐to-­‐face,  and  structured  around  a  semi-­‐structured  interview   schedule2  that  allowed  the  taking  of  a  open-­‐ended  tone  if  required.  The  interviews  were  recorded   with  an  audio-­‐recorder  and  transcribed  at  a  later  stage.  Each  interview  lasted  for  between  40  and   60  minutes,  depending  on  the  clinician’s  schedule.  My  visits  were  often  much  more  elaborate  than   the  one  hour  time  slot  for  an  interview  -­‐  as  these  visits  included  waiting  time,  introduction  to   colleagues  and  being  shown  around  the  facilities.      

Every  interview  was  explained  as  consisting  of  two  parts;  the  first  asking  ‘factual  questions’,  and   the  second  asking  about  perspectives.  This  structure  was  chosen  due  to  my  noticing  that  some   respondents  were  hesitant  to  participate  owing  to  to  uncertainty  over  what  I  might  ask.    This   structure  also  proved  to  be  a  protective  measure  in  preventing  the  interview  being  an  open-­‐ended                                                                                                                  

(16)

conversation.  To  acquire  a  perspective  on  the  categorization  of  behavioral-­‐challenges-­‐as-­‐ADHD,  the   first  set  of  questions  asked  about  reported  complaints  in  general.  In  cases  where  behavioral  

complaints  were  mentioned,  I  then  asked  for  elaboration  on  this.  As  a  rule  of  thumb,  I  would  only   ask  questions  about  ADHD  after  this  term  had  been  used  by  the  clinician.    

Data  analyses  

The  process  of  data  analysis  was  characterized  by  connecting  ‘the  understood’  to  ‘the  need  to   understand’  (Strathern,  1999:6  in:  Hastrup,  2013:149).  The  data  were  analyzed  building  on  Glaser   and  Strauss’  grounded  theory  approach    (Green  &  Thorogood,  2004182-­‐184).    

 

Nvivo  10  was  used  as  a  tool  for  data  entry,  coding,  text  searching  and  ‘counting’  the  frequency  of   presentation  of  certain  perceptions.  An  example  of  comparing  multiple  perspectives  across  the   interviews  is  presented  in  appendix  3.  The  following  steps  characterized  the  data  analysis  process:    

• Code,  look  for  patterns  across  the  data,  conceptualize  what  these  patterns  might  indicate   and  pose  questions  on  what  is  difficult  to  understand.  Then  develop  questions  for  the  next   round  of  data  collection,  and  discuss  the  analyses  with  academic  supervisor    

• Look  for  ‘unusual  cases  or  viewpoints’  in  relation  to  commonly  expressed  viewpoints  and   conceptualize  what  these  might  point  at,  discuss  the  analyses  with  academic  supervisor.     • See  how  patterns,  unusual  cases  and  the  conceptualization  thereof  fit  or  contrast  with  

existing  theory,  discuss  the  analyses  with  academic  supervisor.    

• Look  for  data  that  cannot  be  explained  by  theory,  re-­‐read  interview  data,  search  for  new   theoretical  frameworks  to  understand  the  ‘gaps’,  discuss  the  analyses  with  academic   supervisor.    

 

Ethical  consideration  

The  ‘do  no  harm’  intent  was  central  to  the  process  of  research.  The  following  measures  were    taken   to  ensure  this.  Firstly,  I  disclosed  my  role  as  a  researcher  and  the  purpose  of  the  study  in  contacting   all  the  respondents.  I  asked  the  clinicians  to  read  the  consent  form  prior  to  interview,  in  order  to   make  sure  that  respondents  were  operating  with  full  understanding  of  the  implications  of  the   study.  After  showing  willingness  to  participate,  all  clinicians  were  asked  to  sign  for  consent.  This   form,  as  included  in  appendix  3,  emphasized  that  participation  was  anonymous  and  data  would  be   held  in  confidence.  This  information  was  repeated  verbally  before  starting  the  interview.  

(17)

anonymity,  all  clinicians  were  granted  a  code  (N1,  N2  etc.),  and  remained  anonymous  in  transcripts.   I  further  chose  not  to  list  these  codes  in  the  appendix  of  respondents.  As  the  community  of  

clinicians  in  Pune  is  relatively  small,  demographic  data  and  direct  quotes  in  this  thesis  could   otherwise  easily  be  linked  back  to  a  specific  person    

 

Reflection  and  limitation    

A  weakness  of  this  study  is  that  it  is  solely  built  on  reported  complaints  from  the  perception  of   clinicians,  and  recollections  of  approaches  to  categorization.  This  study  did  not  ask  clinicians   directly  about  their  definitions  of  ADHD,  but  instead  started  out  by  asking  about  behavioral  

difficulties  as  a  way  to  gain  insight  into  behaviors  that  are  associated  with  ADHD.    This  indirect  way   of  collecting  perspectives  on  the  categorization  of  ADHD  has  limitations  -­‐  suggested  links  between   ‘complaints’  and  ‘diagnosing  ADHD’  are  based  on  my  own  interpretation  of  the  clinicians’  answers.   Observation  during  consultation,  and  interviewing  parents  would  have  minimized  this  subjectivity.     My  decision  to  solely  talk  to  clinicians  was  shaped  by  restraints  of  not  speaking  Maharati  or  Hindi,   and  also  not  having  ethical  consent  to  do  participant  observation.    

I  have  tried  to  deal  with  this  limitation  by  talking  to  non-­‐  clinicians  about  their  conceptualization  of   ADHD,    by  discussing  my  analyses  with  clinicians  (after  the  interview  had  taken  place)  in  the  last   weeks  of  fieldwork  and  by  searching  literature  that  looks  into  helpseeking  routes  and  complaints   associated  with  ADHD.  

As  I  did  not  have  access  to  information  in  the  vernacular  languages  of  Maharathi  or  Hindi,  my  

interpretation  is  solely  based  on  studies  and  articles  in  the  English  language.  Eriksen  described  that     ‘content  on  the  internet  is  believed  to  be  English,  but  in  fact,  half  of  it  isnt’  (Eriksen,  2007:74).    Thus,   there  may  have  been  studies  on  ADHD  in  Hindi  or  Maharathi  that  I  did  not  have  access  to.  

 Lastly,  choosing  to  do  research  among  professional  colleagues  came  with  a  range  of  both  benefits   and  limitations.  My  professional  background  as  a  drama  therapist  in  children’s  mental  health  no   doubt  left  a  ‘footprint’  in  this  study  (Barbour,  2013:44).  Respondent’s  remarks  may  also  have  been   shaped  by  the  perception  that  they  were  talking  to  a  colleague.  This  reduced  the  chance  of  talking   about  categorization  of  ADHD  in  ‘laymen’s  terms’,  which  increased  the  chance  of  assuming  a  shared   meaning  for  shared  concept.  Wherever  possible,  I  tried  to  be  a  ‘professional  stranger’  and  to  reflect   on  my  role  after  each  interview.  One  measure  that  I  undertook  to  minimize  this  influence  was  to   postpone  conversations  on  mental  health  care  in  the  Netherlands  until  the  end  of  the  interview.    

(18)

The  choice  of  study  was  partly  informed  by  my  own  experiences,  as  described  in  the  introduction.   My  professional  background  might  have  influenced  and  emphasized  certain  statements  on  

categorization,  whilst  simaultaneously  de-­‐emphasizing  others  (Barbour,  2013:44).  I  recognized   Strathern’s  (2005:  35)  argument  that  ‘one  cannot  simply  take  turns  in  oneself’.  This  led  me  to   establish  that  it  is  indeed  impossible  to  be  an  aspirant  anthropologist/sociologist  without  being  a   therapist,  as  these  roles  are  partly  connected.  This  realization  led  me  to  present  different  ways  in   which  data  could  be  interpreted,  before  describing  my  own  interpretations  of  events  in  the  thesis.   This  encouraged  me  to  be  reflexive  while  writing,  and  to  provide  the  reader  insight  into  my  analysis   process  throughout  the  thesis.    

My  background  as  a  drama  therapist  also  came  with  benefits,  and  I  am  under  the  impression  that   my  professional  background  influenced  many  respondents’  willingness  to  talk  to  me.    

                                         

(19)

2.  Presenting  issues  

 

This  chapter  presents  a  detailed  account  of  referral  routes  and  presenting  issues,    as  reported  by     ‘the  clinician-­‐as-­‐ethnographer’.  Drawing  on  a  creolization  perspective,  I  will  argue  that  there  are  a   range  of  factors  that  can  delay  or  accelerate  consulting  a  clinician  for  behavioral  issues  (Erikson;   2007).  Presenting  behavioral  concerns  interacts  with  layers  such  as  mental  health  infrastructure,   policy,  education,  gender  and  class  in  specific  ways.  Drawing  on  an  ecological  niche  perspective,  I   will  argue  that  consulting  a  clinician  for  behavioral  issues  should  be  understood  in  relation  to  what   is  valued  most  in  a  child,  and  a  child’s  future.  

Helpseeking  routes  

The  city  of  Pune  is  decorated  with  advertisements  of  private  health  care  professionals;  varying   from  cosmetic  surgeons  to  gynecologists.  A  number  of  hospitals  in  the  city  have  a  Child  

Development  Center  (CDC)  and  employ  pediatricians,  psychologists  and  psychiatrists.  These   facilities  cater  to  parents  of  upper,  middle  and  lower  socioeconomic  class.  Prices  for  consultation   range  from  40  to  800  rupees.  Governmental  hospitals  and  NGO’s  in  slum  areas  offer  mental  health   services  at  low-­‐  or  no-­‐cost.    Six  hospitals  in  the  city  have  a  teaching  program  to  train  psychiatrists,   and  a  number  of  universities  and  colleges  offer  a  MA  program  in  clinical  psychology.  All  of  the   interviewed  clinicians  (N=21)  indicated  that  they  are  frequently  consulted  for  behavioral  issues  of   children,  and  used  the  term  ADHD  to  refer  to  a  pattern  of  behavioral  issues.    This  term  was  also   used  in  national  newspaper  articles3  and  on  pharma-­‐sponsered4  educational  posters  in  clinician’s   waiting  rooms.    

When  asked  about  referral  routes  in  Pune,  clinicians  indicated  that  children  primarily  come  to  them   following  a  referral  from  a  school  counselor,  or  from  a  pediatrician  or  general  practitioner.  A  small   number  of  patients  also  come  from  word-­‐of-­‐  mouth  or  self-­‐referral.  The  detailed  accounts5  on   referees  are  presented  in  table  2.1.  

     

                                                                                                                3  See  appendix  4  

4  For  an  example    a  newspaper  article  on  the  rise  of  ADHD,  see  appendix  5  

5  In  the  table,  a  general  practitioner  and  pediatricans  are  referred  to  as  ‘MD’  as  they  are  both  medical  

(20)

Type  of  referral   Amount    (N=20)  

School  counselor     19  

School  counselor  and  pediatrician   13   School  counselor,  MD  and  self  referrals   10  

MD’s  only   1  

Table  2.1.  Referees    

The  central  role  of  schools  and  pediatricians  in  referring  is  supported  by  findings  from  previous   studies  on  ADHD  in  Goa  and  Kolkata  (Mukhopadhyay,  Misra,  Mitra,  &  Niyogi,  2003;  Wilcox  et  al.,   2007).  Clinicians  suggested  that  referrals  through  schools  are  due  to  an  increase  in  awareness  of   ADHD  amongst  teachers,  and  an  increase  in  the  number  of  school  counselors.  Under  the  Sarva   Shiksha  Abhiyan  (SSA)6  act  in  2000,  clinicians  were  invited  to  educate  schools  in  screening  for   developmental  issues,  including  ADHD  and  autism  (N15,  N27).  School  counselors  were  made  a   mandatory  requirement  in  2009,  under  the  Right  to  Education  Act  (RTE)  -­‐  partly  in  reaction  to  the   high  suicide  rate  amongst  students,  due  to  fear  of  educational  pressure  (Patel,  Flisher,  Hetrick,  &   McGorry,  2007:1303-­‐1304)  (N13).  Though  mandatory  by  law,  it  was  indicated  that  school   counselors  mostly  practice  at  private  English  or  Maharati  medium  schools.    

One  route  ,which  has  not  been  mentioned  in  the  literature  so  far,  is  that  of  parents  who  come  to   clinicians  as  a  self  -­‐referral  and  state  ‘  I  think  my  child  has  ADHD’.  Twelve  clinicians  remembered   seeing  parents  who  reported  this  after  consulting  ‘  Dr  Google’.  These  parents  were  referred  to  as  ‘   the  smaller  portion  of  the  parents’  .  For  examples  on  recollections  of  these  moments,  see  table  2.2.  ‘   Dr.  Google’.                                                                                                                                      

(21)

Table  2.2  Examples  Dr.  Google-­‐referrals     Examples  

“Yes,  they  do  that.  [After]  a  newspaper  article  or  so.  Mostly  they  are  individuals  who  are  working  in   software  companies.  Parents,  yes,  they  do  do  that...  we  have  a  lot  of  weekly  articles  in  newspapers  in  local   languages,  and  they  will  keep  a  paper  cutting  and  come  with  that.”.  (N4)  

“Parents  sometimes  collect  symptoms  on  the  Internet.  They  will  match  the  symptoms  with  the  kid.  They   will  come  in  and  say  “all  these  symptoms  match  with  my  kids”,  and  we  will  say:  'no,  no,  this  is  not  autism”   (N5).  

Parents  are  getting  a  lot  smarter  these  days,  they  read  up  a  lot  more  on  these  things.  They  do  read  up   and  they  do  come  up  with  diagnostic  terms,  asking  us:  is  he  this  or  is  he  this.  Especially  the  educated   parents,  they  do  that  They  have  paper  cuttings  and  show  us,  saying:  my  child  fits  this.  From  the  

newspapers  and  magazines  that  write  about  this.  A  lot  of  papers  now  write  about  ADHD,  about  autism.   They  read  this  and  think:  my  child  has  all  this  (N6).    

The  presenting  quest  

The  availability  of  mental  health  services  and  ‘referral  parties’  could  be  interpreted  in  different   ways.  Firstly,  it  might  indicate  the  presence  of  a  ‘market’  for  problematizing  childhood  behavior,   and  treating  it  as  a  medical  problem(Timimi,  2010).    In  this  line  of  reasoning,  the  market  is  not   exclusively  available  for  ‘the  elitist  population’  -­‐  as  it  was  suggested  that  the  facilities  to  assess   behavioral  difficulties  of  children  are  accessible  ‘for  every  pocket’.  This  might  indicate  that  an   awareness  of  behavioral-­‐problems-­‐as-­‐ADHD  is  spreading  across  the  different  classes  that  make  up   Pune.    Secondly,  the  phenomenon  of  self-­‐referral  on  the  basis  of  Internet-­‐gleaned  information   might  indicate  a  tendency  to  express  distress  in  ‘Western  ways’  (Watters,  2010).    These  

interpretations  would  be  in  line  with  Watters  thesis  that  globalizing  mental  health  ‘is  teaching  the   world  to  think  like  us’.    

Building  on  empirical  data  of  presenting  issues,  I  argue  for  a  different  interpretation  of  events.   Observations  from  this  study  suggest  that  though  the  context  of  Pune  is  not  necessarily  defined  by  a   shortage  of  facilities,  there  are  delaying  factors  for  help-­‐seeking.  These  factors  might  keep  a  child   from  being  consulted  by  a  clinician  altogether,  but  I  will  refer  to  these  factors  as  ‘delaying  factors’.   On  the  basis  of  empirical  data,  I  will  argue  that  ‘societies  are  unequally  affected  by  different  

tendencies  at  a  different  speed’  (Eriksen,  2007:9).  The  findings  from  this  study  indicate  that  notions   of  childhood,  the  role  of  extended  families,  stigma,  gender  and  class  are  all  factors  that  interact  with   the  decision  to  seek,  or  not  seek,  biomedical  help.    

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

In the present study, we examined the correspondence and discrepancy between parents on internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in two samples, namely a clinical sample

In addition to parental internalizing problems, externalizing behavior problems in parents may also be of influence on behavioral parent training outcome in

The main aim of the present study was to examine whether changes in discipline practices and parenting sense of competence of mothers’ and fathers’ of referred preschool

methylphenidate in preschool children with adhd symptoms and disruptive behaviors who had remaining significant behavior problems after previous behavioral parent training..

Subsequently, we investigated whether behavioral parent training under routine care conditions reduces disruptive behaviors in preschool children and improves parenting skills,

Meer ouderlijke stress bij moeders voorspelde grotere verschillen tussen ouders in de beoordeling van externaliserende gedragsproblemen van hun kind, terwijl meer ouderlijke

De onvoorwaardelijke steun van mijn promotor Pieter Hoekstra, heeft er in barre tijden zeker voor gezorgd dat ik door kon gaan.. Het is heel prettig een scherpzinnig denker

In 1993 verruilde Lianne deze baan voor een aanstelling bij de polikliniek van het Universitair Centrum voor Kinder- en Jeugdpsychiatrie (uckjp) in Groningen (nu Accare Groningen