• No results found

An empirical test of Goal-framing theory applied to collective performance : the mediating role of social well-being and joint production motivation

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "An empirical test of Goal-framing theory applied to collective performance : the mediating role of social well-being and joint production motivation"

Copied!
66
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

An empirical test of Goal-framing theory applied to collective performance: the mediating role of social well-being and joint production motivation

Master Thesis

Student: Lina María Bernal Fuentes / Student No. 11710497

MSc. Business Administration, Strategy track

University of Amsterdam, Faculty of Economics and Business

Supervisor: Siri Boe-Lillegraven

University of Amsterdam, Faculty of Economics and Business

(2)

1 Statement of Originality

This document is written by the student Lina María Bernal Fuentes, who declares to take full responsibility for the contents of this document.

I declare that the text and the work presented in this document are original and that no sources other than those mentioned in the text and its references have been used in creating it.

The Faculty of Economics and Business is responsible solely for the supervision of completion of the work, not for the contents.

(3)

2 TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract ... 4 Introduction ... 5 Literature review ... 7 Goal-framing theory ... 7

Three overarching frames: hedonic, gain and normative ... 9

Goal-framing theory application to pro-social, pro-environmental and collective behaviours ... 10

Pro-social behaviours ... 11

Pro-environmental behaviours ... 11

Collective behaviours and organizational applications ... 13

Well-being ... 16

General definition ... 16

The appearance of social well-being ... 17

The conceptual framework ... 20

Hypotheses development ... 22

Part 1. Normative frame, joint production motivation and performance. ... 22

Part 2. Normative frame, social well-being, joint production motivation, and collective performance... 24

Methods ... 27

Participants ... 28

Materials and task ... 29

Operationalization of variables... 30

Independent variable – normative frame ... 30

Manipulation check ... 32

Mediator 1 – social well-being ... 33

Mediator 2 – goal commitment ... 34

Dependent variable – Contribution to collective performance ... 37

(4)

3

Experiment 1 – control condition ... 38

Experiment 2 – treatment condition.... 39

Pilot ... 40

Data collection ... 41

Results ... 42

Part 1. Normative frame, goal commitment and contribution to collective performance ... 45

Part 2. Normative frame, social well-being, goal commitment and contribution to collective performance... 47 Part 3. Observations ... 52 Discussion ... 54 Theoretical implications ... 54 Practical implications ... 57 Future research ... 58

Limitations of the study ... 59

Conclusion ... 60

(5)

4

Abstract

This thesis tests the application of goal-framing theory in a collective setting and the mediating

mechanisms of social well-being and joint production motivation. As proposed by Lindenberg

and Foss (2011), first I test if there is a positive relationship between the normative frame and

performance, and the mediating role of joint production motivation. Second, I propose using

social well-being as a second mediating mechanism that appears from the normative frame and

increases joint production motivation and performance. Through a between-subjects

experiment (N= 84) implemented with students from the faculty of economics and business at

the University of Amsterdam, I found that the normative frame did not lead to higher

performance. However, the examination of the normative frame antecedents explains this

unexpected result and provides empirical support for Lindenberg and Foss’s (2011) suggested

steps to create collective production situations, which is consistent with goal-framing theory.

Furthermore, social well-being proved to be a tool that can support or diminish motivation and

performance, and its effect largely depends on managers’ ability to manipulate it.

Keywords: goal-framing theory; normative frame; joint production motivation; social

(6)

5

Introduction

Organizations’ ability to pursue goals is one of the most important factors determining

high performance. This pursuit is a composite of individual-level goals and collective goals

that coexist within organizations and should remain aligned to guarantee success (Gottschalg

& Zollo, 2007). Problems arise when people prioritize their individual goals over the firm’s

interests. In this matter, goal-framing theory (Lindenberg & Frey, 1993; Lindenberg, 2000;

Lindenberg, 2001; Lindenberg & Steg, 2007; Lindenberg, 2008) states that managers can

trigger or design frames (environments) within organizations that increase awareness about

collective production and drive people’s motivation towards organizational goals achievement.

Goal-framing theory states that the normative frame, which focuses on safeguard

collective benefits through goals like ‘behaving in the right way’, ‘doing the right thing’, and ‘contributing to collective benefits’ (Lindenberg, 2008), is the most adequate frame to

guarantee value creation (Foss & Lindenberg, 2013). This is because it prompts the appearance

of pro-social behaviours, interest alignment, and, especially, joint production motivation (Foss

& Lindenberg, 2013; 2011). However, its stability can be easily threatened by the influences

of alternative forces like the hedonic frame and the gain frame, which are focused on

individual-level goals like “feeling good” or “improving one’s resource position”, respectively.

Lindenberg and Foss (2011) call manager’s attention to the importance of this issue and suggest

different forms of goals alignment to maintain people’s motivation focused on collective

production. In this matter, the hedonic frame is treated with special caution, because it is related

with low levels of value creation and is recognized to be especially capable of displacing the

normative frame (Foss & Lindenberg, 2013). Therefore, the authors limit the supporting role

of the hedonic frame to the normative frame to the use of incentives in a modest, non-contingent

(7)

6

However, in the application of goal-framing theory to social behaviours and

pro-environmental behaviours, authors find that social influences and values coming from the

hedonic frame can actually support the normative frame and make people endorse pro-social

and pro-environmental behaviours (Keizer, Lindenberg, & Steg, 2011; Keizer, Lindenberg, &

Steg, 2013; Keizer, Lindenberg, & Steg, 2008; Lindenberg, 2006; Lindenberg & Steg, 2007;

Lindenberg & Steg, 2013; Steg & Vlek, 2009). These alternative ideas of using the hedonic

frame to support the normative frame have not been considered yet in the ground of collective

performance, as the theoretical and empirical works in this field are still very limited. In fact, in firm’s contexts, the main positive relation between the normative frame and the levels of

performance still lacks empirical proof that will ensure the effectiveness of the goal-framing

theory in competitive environments.

Therefore, this thesis aims to contribute to goal-framing theory by testing the effect of

the normative frame on performance, and by suggesting an alternative way in which the

alignment of the hedonic and the normative frame can generate a positive impact on

performance. According to Lindenberg and Foss (2011), the normative frame is recreated by

applying two of the four antecedents proposed: task transparency and teams structure and

symbolic management. In a laboratory experiment, a frame manipulation is implemented to

increase people’s motivation for collective production and therefore their levels of contribution

to collective goals. Additionally, social well-being, as a kind of hedonic force, is proposed as

an alternative mechanism to impact motivation for collective production. In opposition to what

is claimed by goal-framing theory, social well-being as a hedonic element is expected to appear

from the normative frame and cause a positive effect in people’s joint production motivation

and performance.

The experiment delivered contradictory results in the relationship between the

(8)

7

affect motivation neither production. However, overall this thesis contributed to goal-framing

theory with evidence supporting Lindenberg and Foss’s (2011) propositions about the how task

transparency, team structures and symbolic management configure the normative frame and

interact between each other to preserve or not its stability. Symbolic management proved to be

especially powerful in directing individual’s attention, and it can seriously hamper the stability

of the normative frame if it deviates from collective goals. Social well-being proved to be a

source of joint production motivation and indirectly affected the levels of performance, but

managers should control it carefully if they want that this element positively affect the levels

of collective production. Individual preference for a task, as an alternative variable, was able

to positively affect the model and the levels of motivation and performance. Finally, some

practical implications and paths of future research are proposed.

Literature review

Goal-framing theory

Goal-framing theory was developed in first place by Siegwart Lindenberg and was later

elaborated and applied to organizational, prosocial and environmental contexts by Nicolai Foss

and Linda Steg (Foss & Lindenberg, 2013; Lindenberg, 2000; Lindenberg, 2006; Lindenberg

& Steg, 2007; Lindenberg, 2008; Lindenberg & Foss, 2011; Steg & Vlek, 2009). With this

theory, Lindenberg (2000) wants to give an explanation of the rationales behind people’s

decisions. He states that people’s actions respond to a collection of inner beliefs and immediate

conditions in which decisions are made. Those conditions affect people’s selective attention

and determine what ideas influence people’s final actions the most (Lindenberg, 2000). The

immediate conditions that influence actions are defined as frames (Lindenberg, 2000). Frames

are packs of ‘circumstantial conditions’ driving people’s attention, beliefs, and knowledge

(9)

8

different frames, which in turn allow the prevalence of different beliefs and therefore different

actions. This is defined as framing process.

According to the theory, different frames can coexist and interact with each other

depending on the external cues that are present in the environment (Lindenberg, 2000;

Lindenberg, 2008). The framing process creates two dimensions where frames operate: the

foreground and the background. The foreground is the dominant dimension where a specific

frame leads people’s attention, priorities and actions, and drives them towards a specific set of

goals and related sub-goals (Lindenberg, 2000). The background dimension remains in the

environment but becomes weaker than the foreground. It is still important because it contains

a set of goals that can be relevant for the foreground and can deteriorate or enhance the

prominence of a frame in the foreground if the environmental cues change (Lindenberg, 2000).

There are two characteristics of frames that allow them to operate in the two different dimensions, the salience and the porosity. The ‘salience of a frame’ refers to the level of

strength that the frame has in order to be in the foreground or in the background (Lindenberg,

2000). The salience is lower the greater the distance of the product of a behaviour from the

behaviour itself (Lindenberg, 2000). That means that the more time people need to wait to see

the effect of an action, the less strong the frame leading the action is.

The second characteristic, the ‘porosity’, refers to the capacity of all frames to affect

each other. Being porous means that each frame is exposed to the influence of the others

(Lindenberg, 2008). So, when a goal frame dominates the foreground, it pushes the other two

to the background but without making them fade. Instead, the porosity allows the frames to

remain active in the background and acquire relative strength depending on the situational

triggers appearing in the environment. This idea suggests that mixed goals are present in people’s motivations and they affect constantly their willingness to engage in certain

(10)

9

Lindenberg (2000) states that frames are composed of two types of goals, higher level

goals and lower level goals. The high-level goal governs people’s beliefs and actions and has

under its dominance a set of more concrete and related low-level goals. When the high-level

goal becomes ‘focal’ or an ‘overarching frame’ (Lindenberg, 2008), the lower-level goals point

to the same direction as the higher-level goal does. Those overarching frames are capable to

prompt automatic reactions in people when facing specific external circumstances.

Specifically, in an organization, an overarching frame is able to organize people’s cognitions

and evaluations, and they guide people’s responses in different types of situations.

Three overarching frames: hedonic, gain and normative

Lindenberg (2008) recognizes three main overarching frames: the hedonic goal-frame,

the gain goal-frame, and the normative goal-frame. The hedonic goal-frame has lower-level

goals focused on improving the way one feels at a specific moment. This frame has a high level

of salience, operates in the short term and is focused on chasing opportunities for satisfaction.

Satisfaction is understood as part of individual’s physical well-being, like avoiding pain and seeking physical pleasure, or as part of individual’s social well-being, like improving

self-esteem, looking for work motivation and behavioural approval (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011;

Ormel, Lindenberg, Steverink, & Verbrugge, 1999). The gain goal-frame drives people’s

attention towards sub-goals that improve people’s resources. This frame has a medium level of

salience and seeks to safeguard scarce resources by paying attention to opportunities to improve

the resource position. Pro-competition incentives focused on personal gains can trigger this

frame. Finally, the normative goal-frame has a low level of salience and has to do with the idea of ‘appropriateness’, like behaving in the right way, caring about group interests, and

complying with social norms (Lindenberg, 2000; Lindenberg, 2008). This frame has a low

(11)

10

The fact that each frame is associated with a different level of salience creates a sort of

hierarchy. On the top of it is the hedonic goal-frame, then it follows the gain goal-frame, and

finally, the normative goal-frame is at the bottom of the hierarchy. This means that if people

want to focus on normative goals, they have to displace hedonistic and lucrative goals to the

background. It also implies that people need to wait long periods of time to experience the

positive or negative effects of this decision (Lindenberg, 2000). Therefore, the sustainability

of the normative frame is weak and vulnerable to the effect of environmental forces.

The fact the frames are porous implies that they can influence each other while being

in the foreground or in the background. These interactions and the possible alignment between

frames can serve different purposes, for example in stimulating pro-social behaviours or

group-oriented behaviours. The following section will explain the different applications of the

goal-framing theory and the ways in which mutual support or alignments between frames have been

proposed so far.

Goal-framing theory application to pro-social, pro-environmental and collective behaviours

Goal framing theory has been used to understand the roots and functioning of pro-social

behaviours, pro-environmental behaviours, and collective (organizational) behaviours (Foss &

Lindenberg, 2013; Lindenberg, 2000; Lindenberg & Foss, 2011; Lindenberg & Steg, 2013;

Steg & Vlek, 2009). All these works highlight the role of the normative frame as the glue to

stick people to productive behaviours and to reinforce a collective orientation even when

(12)

11 Pro-social behaviours

Lindenberg (2006) explains how pro-social behaviours are strongly linked to the

creation of mental models, cognitive process and goals. Pro-social behaviours include actions

as sharing knowledge, helping others, expressing solidarity, among others (Lindenberg, 2006). Individuals’ actions are the result of a framing and cognitive process that connect the motives

to realize an action with the final pro-social behaviour. The motives can be influenced by the person’s and other people’s expectations of the action or by predictions over the effect of an

action. Therefore, all frames can lead to pro-social behaviours; however, the motives behind

the behaviours are different and are related to a different level of social support (Lindenberg,

2006).

The two most remarkable contributions of the application of goal-framing theory to

pro-social behaviours are first the recognition that the normative frame requires a strong level

of relational signalling that approves or disapproves an action; and second, the recognition on

that the hedonic frame can support the normative frame, appealing to the fear of not being

socially approved (Lindenberg, 2006). In this case, the hedonic support takes the shape of social

sanctions that individuals try to avoid and lead them to endorse a collective orientation.

Pro-environmental behaviours

In the field of pro-environmental behaviour, Lindenberg and Steg (2007) proved that

mixed motives governed environmental actions. People’s motivations to act

pro-environmentally are heterogeneous and not constant. In the hedonic frame, Lindenberg and

Steg (2007) found that people engage in pro-environmental behaviours when they derive

pleasure and satisfaction from it, and also when endorsing the behaviour is congruent with a

personal value. Commonly this is called ‘having an emotional affinity with the environment’.

(13)

12

behaviours when it brings positive consequences for themselves, for example saving money

(Lindenberg & Steg, 2007). But even though the gain frame has a role in conditioning

pro-environmental behaviours, people are more willing to engage in them if they find an affective

relation with the cause (hedonic reason). That suggests a stronger effect from the hedonic frame than from the gain frame in people’s environmental behaviours. Finally, regarding the

normative frame, the main reason people acted pro-environmentally is due to the desire of behaving ‘appropriately’ and according to certain standards (Lindenberg & Steg, 2007). This

work is important because the authors conclude that environmental behaviours are mainly

based and prompted by the normative frame, but it is fundamental to get the hedonic and the gain frames aligned in the background, so they don’t displace the normative frame from the

focus.

Lindenberg and Steg (2013) propose two mechanisms to consolidate the influence of

the normative frame and align the gain and hedonic frames with it. The first one is the signalling

from leader figures and cross-norm activation, which states that other’s attitudes towards a norm affect one’s behaviour. The second mechanism is the moralization of behaviours which

defines how important is to comply with a social norm or not (Lindenberg & Steg, 2013). The

proposal of these two mechanisms is important because they highlight the role of social forces

in reinforcing the strength of the normative frame and therefore in the adoption of

pro-environmental behaviours.

Social values can indirectly support the feeling of joint production and moral obligation

(Lindenberg & Steg, 2013). Interestingly, the authors did not deepen on the idea that

individuals can also experience a sense of well-being when they act in a normative frame. This

well-being can be the result of complying with social norms or from behaving accordingly to

(14)

13 Collective behaviours and organizational applications

Regarding collective behaviours, Lindenberg & Foss (2011) introduce and elaborate on

the concept of ‘joint production motivation’. Joint production motivation is defined as the “human capacity to actively engage in collaborative activities, and it is based on the insight

that the motivation to engage in them is strongly related with cognitions about tasks, interdependencies and common goals” (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011, 502). The authors categorize

joint production motivation situations as settings with high a heterogeneity of efforts, that

imply complementary resources and interdependencies (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). The

perception that a common endeavour exists for a group of people triggers pro-social behaviours

as sharing cognitions and information, exerting efforts to coordinate, willing to support peers,

among others (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). As stated by the authors, the collaborative character

of joint production motivation makes it appealing to organizations and a tool to effectively

manage group-oriented behaviours.

Lindenberg & Foss (2011) explain that the interdependencies among tasks and people’s

orientation towards a collective long run imply that joint production requires a strong degree

of interest alignment (Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Trötschel, 2001; Lindenberg

& Foss, 2011). Then, individuals’ decisions to work for a collective goal can be prompted by

a proper normative frame that emphasizes collective goal achievement.

Lindenberg & Foss (2011) proposed four mechanisms to establish that normative frame

and therefore increase joint production motivation. The first one creates the setting in which people recognize that the situation is one of joint production motivation. This is ‘transparency

of task and team interdependencies’. When employees perceive that tasks are designed to

contribute to common goals and that everyone’s efforts are needed to succeed in the collective

(15)

14

The second aspect, ‘cognitive and symbolic management’, directly support the

normative frame and it refers to the importance of having a clear and shared vision on what is the goal, and to the role that leaders and peer’s play when promoting fair treatment and ensuring

productive behaviours. The clearer the vision, and the more normative-directed the peers’ and leaders’ behaviours, the higher the support to the normative frame and therefore to joint

production motivation (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011).

Third, ‘reward structures’ indirectly support the normative frame as they determine the

set of productive or negative incentives that conduce people towards collective goals or

individual goals. Rewards must be linked to joint outcomes and must be given in recognition

instead of monetary rewards. They also must be non-contingent, otherwise, the gain or hedonic

frame are triggered, and people perform tasks in the pursuit of personal interests (Lindenberg

& Foss, 2011).

Finally, a ‘knowledge-based authority structure’ is an organizational design that

legitimizes the task and teams’ structures and interdependencies. This is a hierarchy that serves

joint production purposes and is able to offer suitable solutions and guidance to achieve

organizational goals. Therefore, an authority structure based on knowledge and legitimate

expertise will strengthen the normative frame and therefore joint production motivation

(Lindenberg & Foss, 2011).

The benefits of raising joint production motivation in a collective context can be seen

in terms of value creation (Foss & Lindenberg, 2013). Joint production motivation promotes

the appearance of pro-social behaviours like knowledge sharing (Lindenberg, 2006) and affects

the types of activities people are willing to engage in, the amount of effort they are willing to

exert in a task, and how people strive to achieve coordination (Foss & Lindenberg, 2013). All

of this spurs productivity and innovation (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). In ambiguous situations,

(16)

15

respond faster and in a more coordinated manner, which reduces costs of planning and

formalization (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). The fact that people align themselves to others’ behaviours through peer’s signalling also reduces opportunism and the need to exert control

mechanisms over employees (Foss & Lindenberg, 2013). Therefore, the management of joint

production motivation becomes relevant as it can increase goals attainment and therefore

collective performance (Foss & Lindenberg, 2013).

The gain and the hedonic frame play an important role in maintaining the strength of

the normative frame (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). The gain frame is associated with a medium

level of value creation because contains strategic goals that look for economic benefit and

improving the resource position (Lindenberg, 2000). In a competitive context, the gain frame

plays a role in directing attention towards profit opportunities and performance improvement

(Lindenberg & Steg, 2013). For these reasons, firms should find ways to ‘obliquely’ align the

gain frame with the normative frame, while taking care that the gain frame does not become

more relevant than the normative frame (Lindenberg & Steg, 2013). In contrast, the hedonic

frame is associated with the lowest levels of value creation and with dubious effects on

performance. Lindenberg and Foss (2013) state that “an organization which members take a

myopic perspective and are predominantly hedonically oriented is not conducive to value creation” (Lindenberg & Steg, 2013, 92-93). The consequences of letting the hedonic frame

comes to the foreground are that the “investments in human capital are not undertaken, helping

behaviours do not thrive, and rewards that are not directly linked to efforts [become] useless”

(Lindenberg & Steg, 2013, 93).

Lindenberg and Foss (2011) propose few ways to make the hedonic frame a useful tool

to support the normative frame. Individual rewards, for example, can be used to increase

motivation, but they have to be non-contingent, modest and directed to group goals and not

(17)

16

and not only due to the final output (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). In the same way, sanctions are

effective to monitor behaviour and ensure individuals’ contribution to collective goals, because

people are going to avoid the unpleasant penalties by contributing to the group. In the

application of this theory to value creation, Foss & Lindenberg (2013) recognize that the

hedonic frame also includes goals as improving the self-esteem, reacting to perceived

unfairness and seeking excitement. These goals can also increase joint production motivation

while the person operates in a normative frame; however, this aspect was not further studied in

their theoretical paper.

In conclusion, the review of the different applications of goal-framing theory in

pro-environmental behaviours and pro-social behaviours offers ideas in which the hedonic frame

turns to be beneficial for collective contexts, in contrast with the limited role assigned to this

frame by Lindenberg and Foss (2013; 2011). More specifically, the role of social values and

group-related feelings can have a positive effect on people’s willingness to engage in pro-social

behaviours. Therefore, this brings up the idea that a different conception of the hedonic frame

based on the idea of social well-being can increase people’s level of joint production motivation

and therefore can positively affect collective performance. The following section explores the

different conceptions of hedonism, and how they can be applied positively to goal-framing

theory and collective performance.

Well-being General definition

The concept of well-being has been broadly studied and deeply developed by many

theorists (Deci & Ryan, 2008; Diener, 1984; Keyes, 1998; Reis, Sheldon, Gable, Roscoe, &

Ryan, 2000; Ryff, 1995; Wright & Cropanzano, 2000). Warner Wilson (1967) was one of the

(18)

17

defines major categories and subfactors that contribute to well-being including aspects such as:

intelligence, socio-economic status, personality, values, environment and health. This

definition of well-being as a function of certain external and internal conditions is later

questioned by Diener (1984) and Houston (1981), who develop a different idea of well-being

based on ideas of pleasure and pain. The fulfilment of needs lead to high levels of satisfaction

and therefore to well-being (Diener, 1984; Houston, 1981). In the same line of thought, telic

theories try to explain subjective well-being as the fulfilment of needs, goals and desires.

Diener (1984) and Austin & Vancouver (1996) state an idea of well-being based on people’s

sensation of progress toward goals (Austin & Vancouver, 1996; Diener, 1984).

These theories are important for the development of the concept of well-being because

they find goals achievement to be strongly linked to the affection of individuals and therefore

to their levels of satisfaction. Ed Diener (1984) finally defines well-being as a subjective

construct with an important categorization: first, well-being as a normative external criterion

(as a virtue or quality) that defines what is a desirable thing; second, as a subjective idea defined

by people that allow them to evaluate life in positive terms; and third, as an everyday speech

about an ongoing dominance of positive affects over negative affects that entails pleasantness

experiences (Diener, 1984). Thus, well-being is a subjective concept; it is a positive measure;

and its measurement includes an assessment of the global state of an individual’s life (Diener,

1984).

The appearance of social well-being

Carol Ryff (1995) and Corey Lee Keyes (1995) make a step forward in this matter and

define well-being as multidimensional construct composed of six elements: self-acceptance,

positive relations, autonomy, environmental mastery, purpose in life, and personal growth. This

(19)

18

(positive relations) and based on aspects related with feeling self-competent and skilful when

doing tasks (environmental mastery and personal growth). Building on this idea, Corey Lee M.

Keyes (1998) advances a model of social well-being defined as a conscious self-evaluation

taking into account that individuals understand themselves as part of social structures that have

collective goals. He narrows this construct to five main dimensions: social acceptance, social

integration, social contribution, social actualization, and social coherence. Social acceptance is

analogue to self-acceptance and refers to what idea of society someone forms in her mind from

the character and qualities of general people. For example, if someone thinks that others are

capable of good actions and kindness. People with high levels of social acceptance have a

positive view of human nature (Keyes, 1998). Social integration refers to the quality of the individual’s relationship with the community. A person who normally presents a good level of

well-being has a good relationship with the immediate community (Keyes, 1998). Social

contribution is the assessment of the individual’s social value. If the person feels valuable to

society and has something to offer, then she experiences a good level of social contribution.

This category is analogue to the idea of self-efficacy and the capacity to achieve objectives

(Keyes, 1998). Social actualization refers to the individual’s belief that society has a potential

to be realized in the future and that she is going to be a beneficiary of that growth (Keyes,

1998). Finally, social coherence refers to people’s understanding of how society operates, how

it is organized and what happens at a certain point in time. According to Keyes (1998), high

measures of these aspects lead to a good social well-being.

The major distinction of Keyes’ approach (1998) is that it establishes that well-being is related with the individuals’ interrelations with a society. This social well-being can be applied

to smaller scenarios like groups within organizations, and it is possible to argue that a big part of individual’s well-being and motivation is linked to healthy social relationships between the

(20)

19

person and the colleagues (social integration), and between the person and the task under her

responsibility (social contribution).

Ryan and Deci (2000) through the Self-determination theory also explore how the sense

of well-being is linked with the satisfaction of the psychological needs of competence,

relatedness and autonomy (Ryan & Deci, 2000), which are similar to Keyes’s categories of

social contribution and social integration. Reis, Sheldon, Gable, Roscoe, & Ryan (2000)

empirically show that the fulfilment of the needs of relatedness and competence lead to higher

levels of daily well-being, which in turn affect positively the levels of long-term well-being

(Reis et al., 2000). Therefore, in scenarios of daily social interaction, the satisfaction of the

needs of competence, relatedness and autonomy, leads to the realization of people’s well-being

(Reis et al., 2000).

For this thesis, the most remarkable contribution of this empirical work is that it proves

that some individuals are more responsive than others to the variations of the factors that

contribute to the daily well-being. The sensitivity of each person to the circumstances that

affect well-being determines to what extent environmental clues make someone more (or less)

inclined to behave accordingly to them. Therefore, the way people experience well-being is not

homogeneous and can be affected by the overarching frame that dominates an environment.

This aspect is relevant because it determines that people can experience well-being in

organizations from being in a normative frame similar to if they were in a hedonic frame.

Finally, Harter, Schmidt & Keyes (2003) explore in detail the role of well-being and its

potential positive effect on organizational performance. The authors proved that workers that

experience positive feelings (i.e. motivation or commitment) and have their social needs and

affective needs satisfied, translate these emotions in higher business performance. The study

(21)

20 The conceptual framework

In goal-framing theory, well-being is present in the hedonic frame (Lindenberg, 2000), and its strength is such that it can be “involved in almost everything people do” (Lindenberg,

2000, 187). Hedonic well-being can take the form of the realization of pleasures and sensorial

goals like seeking satisfaction, avoiding pain, avoiding effort, among others (Foss &

Lindenberg, 2013). But, it can also appear from reactions in the normative or gain frame that

derive in contentment or unpleasantness. The role of the hedonic frame proposed in this thesis

is not a reinforcement in forms of incentives or rewards as proposed by Lindenberg and Foss

(2011), but it is a consequence of the normative frame. The normative frame entails the

existence of a shared purpose within teams, and it is based in strong relational signalling and

social forces (Lindenberg & Steg, 2013). Therefore, the common purpose, the social regulation

and the opportunity to reinforce interpersonal relationships, make the normative frame a fertile

ground for social well-being to raise. People can experience social well-being because they

belong to a group and because they feel capable to contribute to collective outcomes (Keyes,

1998), or more precisely because they are immersed in a normative frame. The possibility of

improving the social well-being in different frames allows the hedonic frame to co-exist in a

normative situation without necessarily resulting prejudicial for performance. In such situation,

the frames are not completely differentiated, and their different effects on people behaviours

do not have to be incompatible with the idea of value creation.

As social well-being is based on the idea of being able to contribute to a group and

being part of a collective, a high level of social well-being will increase the chances that people

engage in collaborative activities inside their group (Keyes, 1998). This is because the group

is configured around a shared goal, and because collaborating with others to achieve the goal improves and strengthen group members’ interrelationships. Goal-framing theory conceives

(22)

21

the interrelated activities. Therefore, an individual’s good level of social well-being can

positively affect the levels of joint production motivation.

In this way, an individual that operates in a normative frame that belongs to a group

and works with others for a collective outcome can find in the normative frame an ongoing

source of satisfaction. In turn, the social environment can incentivize people to stay in the

normative frame and find pleasure from it while they possibly also express this motivation in

forms of cooperation, adaptability, extra efforts and proactivity. All of these are expressions of

joint production motivation (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011), and these behaviours can positively

impact organizational performance. These ideas can be better understood in the following

conceptual model (figure 1).

Figure 1. Conceptual framework

It is important to clarify that the effect of this social well-being is not going to diminish

directly the negative effects of hedonic goals proposed by Lindenberg and Foss (2011). The authors’ ideas of hedonic well-being are more focused on short-term sensorial satisfaction. The

social well-being proposed here is focused on providing joy based on relationships and

interactions which can go beyond instant pleasure.

Social well-being *Social contribution: self-efficacy, being able to contribute to a team *Social integration – feeling part of a group, having good relationships

Joint production motivation *engage in collaborative tasks with others because of the existence of a common goal and team interdependencies Normative frame *Common goal *Interrelated tasks (collaborative tasks) *team existence (teams structures) Better collective performance *The result of coordinate efforts, mutual support, cooperation, coordination, less opportunism

(23)

22

I expect that individuals in a normative frame will get a satisfactory experience derived

from the appraisal of themselves immersed in a collective context and contributing to a

collective endeavour. This satisfaction raising from their social context will make them feel

more motivated to participate in the joint production activities. The high motivation for

performing collaborative activities will make them cooperate, be proactive, share knowledge,

coordinate with each other, be helpful and exert extra efforts. These attitudes then will produce

a higher collective performance.

Social well-being, analogue to the concept of employee “engagement” that is based on

cognitive and motivational antecedents, can also contribute to improving collective

performance. Finally, the total effect of the normative frame on social well-being and joint

production motivation is expected to cause a better collective performance. The following

section will establish the hypotheses of this conceptual framework.

Hypotheses development

This section is divided into two parts. The first part presents the model to test the

relationships proposed by goal-framing theory, regarding the effect of the normative frame on

collective performance, and the mediating effect of joint production motivation. The second

part presents a model to test the effect of the normative frame on collective performance

through the serial mediation effect of social well-being and joint production motivation.

Part 1. Normative frame, joint production motivation and performance.

In the context of collective production, the normative frame focuses on the efforts to

particularly contribute to others and seek to the best result for collective goals (Lindenberg &

Foss, 2011). This frame is built on the cognition of a common purpose and in the interrelation

(24)

23

focused on collective production. This frame establishes the rules and procedures that people

are supposed to follow and prevents them to pursue individual goals like improving the

resources position and obtaining pleasure from a given situation (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011).

Therefore, the normative frame limits the appearance of non-productive practices like

opportunism, while it reinforces the right type of cooperation to achieve joint endeavours

(Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). Therefore:

Hypothesis 1a. The normative frame has a positive effect on collective performance. The normative frame creates the conditions in which collective production situations

can appear. The cognition of being in a collective production situation and the behavioural

reinforcement towards collective performance lead people to experience higher levels of

motivation towards joint production (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). These high levels of

motivation incentivize people to endorse behaviours like knowledge sharing, cooperation,

altruism, proactiveness, willingness to help others, and additional efforts to find solutions to

collective problems (Lindenberg, 2006; Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). At an organizational level,

the normative frame and join production motivation could conditionate the tasks that

individuals are willing to engage in and the effort and coordination that they exert to make

those tasks. The net effect of this prosocial behaviours will be noted as higher collective

performance. Therefore:

Hypothesis 1b. The normative frame has a positive effect on joint production motivation

Hypothesis 1c. Joint production motivation has a positive effect on collective performance

Hypothesis 1d. The normative frame has an indirect and positive effect on collective performance, and this relation is mediated by joint production motivation.

(25)

24 Figure 2. Model Part 1

Part 2. Normative frame, social well-being, joint production motivation, and collective performance

In goal-framing theory, the hedonic frame is the frame that relates to people’s emotions

and well-being. Goals in this frame are especially salient given that they impact people’s

affections instantly. Lindenberg and Foss (2011) identify situations in which the hedonic frame

can displace the normative frame to the background leading to negative consequences for the

organization. For example, reward structures can look arbitrary if not managed properly, which

lead people to feel mistreated and develop retaliation towards the organization (Lindenberg &

Foss, 2011). Codes of conduct and sanctions can be signified as unfair and have negative

consequences that have to be avoided, pushing people towards a hedonic frame (Lindenberg &

Foss, 2011). Additionally, ambiguous authority structures can be understood an impetus of

superiority which deteriorates relational bonds and undermines joint production motivation.

Taking into consideration this evidence, the authors suggest using those mechanisms with

special care of not incentivising people to satisfy their hedonic goals.

However, the literature review also suggests that there are alternative ways to make people’s hedonic goals and well-being a positive support for the sustainability of the normative

frame. Well-being is increased with the achievement of goals and progress in tasks (Austin &

Vancouver, 1996; Diener, 1984) which is why the normative frame is endorsed by

Normative frame Joint Production motivation Collective performance

(26)

25

organizations. Those achievements generate pleasure and satisfaction at the same time (Austin

& Vancouver, 1996; Diener, 1984). Well-being is also the result of social interactions, which

can be found in social settings but also in collective production situations based on a normative

frame. Social interactions and peers’ relationships increase well-being in people (Ryff &

Keyes, 1995), which is also linked to the enjoyment that people experience when their

contributions are qualified as useful and become relevant for a collective goal (Keyes, 1998).

Two aspects from the normative frame may have an effect on individuals’ levels of

social well-being. First, the task transparency and team interrelations allow team members to

recognized themselves as part of a collective effort with an objective of social contribution.

Therefore, individuals acting under a normative frame can experience higher social well-being

because they feel useful and capable of contributing to a collective task. Second, by recognizing

themselves as part of a collectively, individuals experience higher social well-being because

the feel integrated within a team. In the same way, peer’s signalling and symbolic management

provide a sense of approval and social support. Besides, the ongoing joint work towards the

collective work produces social interactions that can strengthen the relationships between

peers. Therefore:

Hypothesis 2a. A normative frame will have a positive effect on individuals’ social well-being

Social well-being rises with the awareness of oneself being involved in a social setting

(Keyes, 1998). The relationships with others, with the context, with oneself and with collective

endeavours, determine the level of social well-being of one individual. Therefore, feeling confident of one’s role within a group, and being confident of one’s capacity to contribute to

others and be useful, will produce a high level of well-being. The capacity to recognize oneself in such situation can affect individuals’ willingness and motivation to engage in collaborative

(27)

26

activities that are proper from joint production motivation (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011).

Therefore:

Hypothesis 2b. Social well-being will have a positive effect on joint production motivation.

As It was stated before, joint production motivation will make people focus on

collective goals and will allow them to create a shared representation of actions, which will

reduce their need of coordination (Foss & Lindenberg, 2013). People will engage in creative

activities to achieve collective goals and will assist peers in dealing with ambiguous situations

(Foss & Lindenberg, 2013). Therefore, joint production motivation facilitates the appearance

of actions that contribute to collective goal attainment:

Hypothesis 2c. Joint production motivation has a positive effect on collective performance.

Danna & Griffin’s (1999) review of the literature on well-being and productivity, found

a link between peers’ working relationships with the existence of positive affects and

well-being, which turns into better performance. This relation also works in the opposite sense, poor

communication or negative stimuli from peers derives in poor well-being and this impacts

negatively productivity. Therefore:

Hypothesis 2d. Social well-being has a positive effect on collective performance Finally, according to Harter, Schmidt & Keyes (Harter et al., 2003; Ryff & Keyes,

1995), high levels of well-being leads to first higher motivation towards tasks, and second, it

turns in higher collective performance due to better individual performance and overall better

collective goal attainment.

Social well-being has an effect on motivation and productivity. Harter, Schmidt &

Keyes (2003) found that the presence of well-being in employees in form of positive

(28)

27

charge of tasks that allow them to grow and feel well and productive show higher performance.

The authors state that employees emotional antecedent generates a positive affect, as

commitment or motivation, and this leads to efficiency and creativity, and ultimately to better

performance (Harter et al., 2003). Therefore:

Hypothesis 2e. The normative frame has an indirect and positive effect on collective performance, and this relation is mediated first by social well-being and second by joint production motivation.

Figure 3. Model Part 2

Thus, this model, represented in Figure 3, can be understood as a sequential mediation

model, where social well-being and joint production motivation act as mediators of the

relationship between the normative frame and collective performance.

Methods

The experimental study was a lab experiment using a one-way between-subjects design.

Due to the use of frames and priming, the between-subjects form is the best way to ensure the results in the treatment condition won’t be biased by participants' learning curves. The control

condition was used to capture the level of joint production motivation in a neutral frame (where

at least I could control that the elements of the normative frame were not present) and later it

was used to contrast the changes in the motivation in the treatment condition due to the Normative frame Social well-being Joint Production motivation Collective Performance

(29)

28

intervention. The treatment condition consisted of implementing two of the four aspects that

create the normative frame proposed by Lindenberg and Foss (2011), which were the peer’s

signalling and the task transparency and team structures. This chapter explains in detail the

experiment design for each of the conditions, the operationalization of the variables, and the

procedures.

Participants

Participants in this experiment were master students from the Faculty of Economics

and Business of the University of Amsterdam. Given that goal-framing theory is concerned

with goal attainment and is applied to organizational performance and value creation (Foss &

Lindenberg, 2013), I focused on this type of population because they typically occupy the job

vacancies in firms after graduation, and it is expected that business students and business

professionals share a common background and have similar mental orientations. I decided not

to approach firms directly because I had very limited access to contacts that could facilitate the

experiment implementation and due to limited time for the data collection. Students were not

offered any kind of individual incentive to participate in the experiment, since I did not want

to activate any gain or hedonic frame in them before and during the experiment. Only a general

Amazon voucher of 30 euros was offered to be raffled among all participants (84 people).

Students were approached during the breaks of tutorial sessions to increase the chances that

they were willing to participate. The classes were chosen randomly from general timetables of

the master program of business administration of the University of Amsterdam having between

20 or 25 people. Once the classes were chosen, the professors were approached and asked for

permission to perform the experiment. Participation was not mandatory, and students were

(30)

29 Materials and task

The task was designed to be performed in groups to simulate the collective setting in

which goal-framing theory is applied. It consisted of filling a matrix with the outcome of sums

of two-digits numbers just as the Figure 4 shows. Each piece of paper had three rows of

matrices. Participants had to decide whether they wanted to attain their individual goal by

filling one of the empty matrices, or if they also wanted to contribute to their team goal by

filling the three empty matrices.

All groups had a team goal and each person had an individual goal. All the participants

were informed about the two goals at the beginning of the experiment. The group goal consisted

of filling 15 matrices, which meant that, in order to reach the team goal, each team member

needed to fill the 3 matrices in his/her piece of paper. If this was not done for at least one team

member, then the team goal was not going to be achieved. The individual goal was achieved

by filling only 1 matrix.

Sums of two-digit numbers were chosen for the task in order to minimize effects of

increasing efficiency and learning behaviour when doing an activity many repeated times

(Benndorf, Rau, & Sölch, 2014). In this way, I wanted to avoid that people obtained satisfaction

for doing the task more efficiently and for getting used it. Two-digit sums were chosen for the

task because of two reasons. First, summing numbers was a basic mathematical calculation for

the level of education of the participants, therefore no previous knowledge was needed to

participate. Second, making sums with two-digit numbers required more time and mental effort

than just summing one-digit numbers, therefore the possibility that participants enjoyed the

task was lower. In that way, with that real-effort task, I intended to increase the level of external

(31)

30 Figure 4. Calculations

Besides the piece of paper with the task, participants required a pen to make the

calculations and an envelope only for the control condition. The envelope per person was given

to each student at the beginning of the experiment.

Operationalization of variables Independent variable – normative frame

The establishment of the normative frame was done using two of the four antecedents

of the normative frame proposed by Lindenberg and Foss (2011). More exactly, the antecedents of ‘task transparency and team interdependencies’ and ‘symbolic management’ in terms of

peer’s signalling were applied; the antecedents of ‘rewards structures’ and the

‘knowledge-based authority structure’ were not applied. The rationales for this decision are explained in

the following paragraphs.

I used task transparency and teams’ interdependencies to set the field where joint

production motivation can appear. Without this element, there cannot be a collective

production (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). This antecedent activated people’s cognition about the

existence of a collective effort, which is a fundamental part of goal-framing theory. Symbolic

management took the form of peer’s signalling which offers a direct support to the normative

frame and ensures that it does not decay easily. Symbolic management could also rise joint

production motivation by a sort of contagion effect (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). So, individuals

that express keen interest for the joint task influence others willingness to work and collaborate

(32)

31

Regarding the reward structures in goal-framing theory, these are advised to be used to

indirectly support the normative frame. However, there is always a high risk that they end up

strengthening the gain and the hedonic frame. Given the risk it represented and the fact that

their role was to provide indirect support to motivation, in this experiment I didn’t base the

normative frame in this antecedent.

Related to the knowledge-based structures, Lindenberg and Foss (2011) advice to use

them to compensate the top-down authority structures that can hamper the effect of the

normative frame. A top-down authority structure is reinforced by contractual agreements that

may lead people to shrink or skip rules, which indirectly increases the salience of the hedonic

or gain frame. Therefore, if organizations are too complex to prescind from the contractual

agreements and their authority structure cannot be simplified, knowledge-based structures can

compensate in favour of the normative frame (Lindenberg & Foss, 2011). If the organization

is small enough to avoid the top-down contractual authority structure, then flattening the

organization is sufficient to protect the salience of the normative frame (Lindenberg & Foss,

2011). Therefore, due to the moderate size and scope of the teams used in this experiment, the

knowledge-based antecedent was not applied.

The collective production situation was created by establishing a group setting and by

operationalizing the antecedents of tasks transparency and team structure and symbolic

management. First, groups were created to carry out a collective task. Knowing that group size

affects the effectiveness and performance of teams due to lack of coordination, lack of

leadership, loss of relational sense, problems of communication, among others, (Hackman &

Vidmar, 1970; Kozlowski & Bell, 2003; Mueller, 2012), the number of people per group was

chosen with the purpose of creating a minimum need of coordination and communication

among members without resulting in the inability to set an agreement. Hackman & Vidmar

(33)

32

five people because the more people were added to the group beyond that number, the less

comfortable people found them due to coordination and communication challenges.

Second, task transparency and team interrelations were achieved by asking participants

to collect all the pieces of paper with the individual calculation within the group. In this way,

people could observe if others decided to contribute to the team goal or not. Third, symbolic

management in form of peer signalling was achieved by telling participants that they were allowed to talk along the experiment. In that way, communication and people’s attitudes

created a contagion effect that was expected to impact each other’s frame, accordingly to

Lindenberg and Foss (2011). That implied that participants could agree on working all together

to achieve the team goal. These two last elements made people aware of the need for all

individual contributions in order to achieve the collective goal and allow them to influence each other’s decisions.

In this experiment, however, peer’s signalling was allowed to ‘run free’ in the treatment

condition. It was not controlled because that implied first bringing extra people to the

experiment that could exert the positive peer’s signalling, which was not feasible due to the people’s limited willingness to participate; or second, assigning to any team member the role

of creating a positive signalling, which was difficult to control and ensure uniformity across all

the groups. Still, even when the type of signalling was not controlled, I expected that the

collective production situation and the awareness of the collective goal were going to induce

the peer’s signalling towards the support of the collective goal.

Manipulation check

Six sentences of manipulation check were included to be sure that the operationalization

of the normative frame was effective. The sentences were written following the example of

(34)

33

performance. The measurement scale ranged from 1- completely disagree to 5- completely

agree. The sentences were:

• The instructions were clear to me and I was aware of the difference between the individual goal and the team goal.

• I felt that my effort was needed to achieve the team goal. • I felt like it is appropriate to contribute to the team goal.

• Someone in the group showed special interest in completing the team task.

• Overall, I perceived that the other team members were motivated to achieve the team goal.

• What I was doing was visible for the team.

Mediator 1 – social well-being

The concept of social well-being is defined by Keyes as “a self-appraisal of one’s circumstance and functioning with society” (Keyes, 1998, 122), and it is measured in different

dimensions including: social integration, social acceptance, social contribution, social

actualization and social coherence. Keyes (1998) proposes and test a social well-being scale to

demonstrate the social structural sources of well-being. The scale was applied and adapted to

different studies to test its validity, and in those occasions, its alpha reliability was 0.57, 0.84,

and 0.86 (Keyes, 1998). The same scale was used in this experiment to measure the two

dimensions pertinent to the context of this thesis. Those dimensions were social integration and social contribution. Social integration, defined as the “quality of one’s relationship with the

society” (Keyes, 1998, 122), gave a measure to which extent people felt they had

commonalities with others, and to what extent they felt a sense of belonging to the team. Social

contribution, defined as the evaluation of the individuals’ social value as vital members of a

(35)

34

individuals’ perception of their capability to perform tasks and contribute to collective goals

(Keyes, 1998). Both dimensions were measured in a scale from 1- strongly disagree to

7-strongly agree. The sentences were the following:

Social Integration

• You didn't feel you belong to anything you'd call a team. • You felt like you were an important part of your team.

• If you had something to say, you believe people in your team would have listened to you.

• You felt close to other people in your team. • You saw your team as a source of comfort.

• If you had something to say, you don't think your team would have taken you seriously.

Social contribution

• Your behaviour had some impact on other people in your team. • You think you had something valuable to give to the team.

• Your activities did not produce anything worthwhile for your team. • You didn't have the time or energy to give to your team.

• You thought that your work provided an important product for your team. • You felt you had nothing important to contribute to your team.

Mediator 2 – goal commitment

Giving the scarce pool of empirical work in goal-framing theory, there is not an

established scale that can be used to measure join production motivation as a dependent

variable. Lindenberg & Steg (2013) made an attempt to explain the effect of the normative

frame in terms of types of behaviour people adopted towards environmental issues. In Keiser,

(36)

35

were measured with factual observations of actions in a field experiment. None of these works

precisely focused on joint production motivation, but they focused on the which antecedent

frames lead to specific behaviour.

The literature of goal commitment and motivation offers an alternative way to

operationalize joint production motivation. Meyer, Becker & Vanderberghe (2004) analysed,

compared and integrated concepts of commitment and motivation. They defined work motivation as a “set of energetic forces that originates both within as well as beyond an

individual’s being, to initiate work-related behaviour, and to determine its form, direction,

intensity and duration” (Meyer et al., 2004, 992). The authors highlight two main characteristics of this concept: first, it is an ‘energizing force’ that drives actions, and second,

it has implications for the form, directions, intensity and duration of those actions.

Meyer et al. (2004) integrated the models of commitment and motivation and provided

explanations on how the motivational process happens and can be reinforced in a working field.

One particular model of commitment was relevant in this case, goal commitment. According

to the authors, goal commitment as many other forms of commitment appeared to have a

moderating effect when achieving work motivation (Meyer et al., 2004). It had the ability to

influence the attention people gave to the activities that served to achieve a goal, and therefore

the amount of effort put to attain it (Meyer et al., 2004).

By comparing the definitions and the theoretical frameworks behind those two

concepts, the authors came to the conclusion that both commitment and motivation were

energizing forces, but commitment was a type of force that linked an individual to a specific

sequence of behaviours. Therefore, motivation was a broader concept where commitment lied

within (Meyer et al., 2004).

The definition of goal commitment used in Meyers et al. (2004) paper is the traditional concept of “one's attachment to or determination to reach a goal, regardless of the goal's origin”

(37)

36

(Locke, Latham, & Erez, 1988, 24), except because in Meyer’s work the degree of commitment

is affected by additional factors that conditionate attention, intentions of effort, and willingness

to develop innovative solutions (Meyer et al., 2004). In this study, I focused on Meyer et al.

(2004) argument of this variable as a component of motivation, and how a positive or negative change in goal commitment will affect also the individuals’ motivation. Therefore, a proxy

way to measure joint production motivation is by assessing the commitment people feel

towards a collective goal.

There is not a unique way to measure goal commitment. Hollenbeck, Williams & Klein

(1989) developed a nine-item self-report measure of goal commitment that was widely used in

subsequent empirical works. The coefficient alpha of reliability for the scale in this study was

0.76. The items were: (1) I am strongly committed to pursuing this GPA goal; (2) I am willing

to put forth a great deal of effort beyond what I'd normally do to achieve this GPA goal; (3)

quite frankly, I don't care if I achieve this GPA goal or not; (4) there is not much to be gained

by trying to achieve this GPA goal; (5) it is quite likely that this GPA goal may need to be

revised, depending on how things go this quarter; (6) it wouldn't take much to make me

abandon this GPA goal; (7) it's unrealistic for me to expect to reach this GPA goal; (8) since

it's not always possible to tell how tough courses are until you've been in them a while, it's hard

to take this goal seriously; (9) I think this GPA goal is a good goal to shoot for (Hollenbeck et

al., 1989). For example, Sue-Chang & Ong (2002), in their work on goal assignment and

performance, measured goal commitment as a substitute of goal acceptance (Locke et al., 1988;

Sue-Chan & Ong, 2002) and adapted the seven points measure of goal commitment from

Hollenbeck et al. (1989). Sholihin, Pike, Mangena & Li (2011), in their study of goal-setting

participation and goal commitment, also referred to Hollenbeck et al. (1989) to measure goal

commitment. They used only three of the nine statements of the scale (items 1, 2 and 3).

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Results for the BILAG improvement component of the combined index in body systems for which a sufficient number of patients per treatment group ( ⱖ5) had baseline disease activity

Het lijkt erop dat deze veranderingen erop wijzen dat het Nederlandse kerk-staat model meer kenmerken van een kerk-staat model gaat vertonen waarbij niet pluriformiteit maar juist

Using acclimation to cold, average, or warm conditions in summer and winter, we measure the direction and magnitude of plasticity of resting metabolic rate (RMR), water loss rate

Queries are mapped to Wikipedia concepts and the corresponding translations of these concepts in the target language are used to create the final query.. WikiTranslate is

Research on searching spoken word collections using automated transcription dates to 1997 with the inception of the Spoken Document Retrieval track at the Text Retrieval

Instruction and Learning Situations with Aspects of Well-Being in Class for Dutch and Turkish/Moroccan Students Dutch students n = 189 Self-confidence Motivation School

In addition, this study showed that being motivated to avoid displaying one’s incompetence to others (high performance-avoidance goal orientation) was negatively related to

Research based on other variables did not yield any strong indications in favour of the existence of a significant relationship between the quality of social life and