• No results found

The role of state anxiety in placebo analgesia within a conditioning paradigm: An open-label placebo study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The role of state anxiety in placebo analgesia within a conditioning paradigm: An open-label placebo study"

Copied!
30
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Leiden University

Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences

Master Thesis

The Role of State Anxiety in Placebo Analgesia within a Conditioning Paradigm: an Open-Label Placebo Study

A. Agbaria S2251876

Master’s Thesis Health and Medical Psychology Supervisor: R. M. Smits MSc

Principal Investigator: Prof. Dr. A. W. M. Evers Institute of Psychology, Leiden University Date: January 17, 2020

(2)

Abstract

Anxiety, expectancy induction, and conditioning have been shown to influence placebo analgesic responses. This study aimed to investigate the role that state anxiety has in placebo analgesia in a conditioning paradigm. The researchers conditioned 28 healthy individuals to relate personalized heat pain intensities with color cues representing the effectiveness of a sham external neural stimulator device. At the beginning of the study, state anxiety levels were measured. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three groups, two experimental placebo groups or a control group (CG). Participants in the placebo groups were conditioned and received verbal suggestion, either by learning about the nature of the placebo device (open-label placebo; OLP) or not (closed placebo; CP). The CG was not conditioned and was given neutral verbal suggestions about the ENS device. We hypothesized that a placebo effect would take place in the OLP and CP groups, and that the effect would be comparable between the two. We further hypothesized that individuals with high state anxiety will experience less placebo effects, and that they would have higher overall pain ratings. Lastly, we controlled for anxiety increase. Results and statistical analyses were inconclusive, mainly due to the small sample size. The current study, however, is the first in this field to incorporate a protocol that takes into account the role of state anxiety in placebo analgesia within a conditioning paradigm, while incorporating OLPs and heat pain. This study can thus be considered a pilot study. Future research based on the current paper should have a larger sample size and make minor modifications.

Keywords: anxiety, expectancy, conditioning, placebo analgesia, heat pain, ENS, state anxiety, placebo, verbal suggestion, open-label placebo, pilot study

(3)

The Role of State Anxiety in Placebo Analgesia within a Conditioning Paradigm

The term ‘placebo effect’ can be considered as the response of an individual after the administration of a placebo. Evidence has demonstrated the magnitude and significance of placebo effects in pain studies (Schedlowski, Enck, Rief, & Bingel, 2015). Placebo analgesia is the subjective pain relief following the application of a treatment that is not directly caused by the pharmacological aspects of that treatment, but rather by placebo aspects. The placebo effect is mediated by the specific psychosocial context surrounding that administration (Benedetti, 2009). For example, positive or negative expectations can significantly affect the experience of pain (Colloca & Benedetti, 2005). Vase, Robinson, Verne, and Price (2003) argue that the expectation of the therapeutic effect under a placebo paradigm may be a fundamental concept underlying the placebo analgesic effect, equal to the expectancy theory of placebo effects, which implies that belief in the functionality of an administered placebo is crucial in order for placebo analgesic effects to take place. In experimental placebo paradigms, a sham treatment (e.g. a pill or a device) is usually paired with pain reduction in order to create the expectations of analgesic effects (Price et al., 1999).

Manipulations of both verbal suggestions and conditioning show to positively affect placebo analgesia (Finniss, Nicholas, & Benedetti, 2009). A considerable number of placebo analgesia studies include conditioning paradigms, which entail pairing a sham treatment with pain intensity changes to create an expectation of analgesic effects (Voudouris, Peck, & Coleman, 1985). These paradigms, along with manipulations in verbal instructions, have shown to be crucial processes for eliciting a placebo effect by decreasing symptoms when administering a placebo treatment (Colloca, Klinger, Flor, & Bingel, 2013). Such paradigms consist of acquisition and evocation phases. During the acquisition phase, the pairing between an unconditioned stimulus (UCS; e.g., heat intensity) and a conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., visual cue) takes place, leading to a conditioned response (CR; e.g., altered level of experienced pain after the presentation of a visual cue corresponding with varied heat intensities). In the evocation phase, the CR can be evoked by the mere presentation of the CS without the UCS. The result of a conditioning paradigm in the context of placebo analgesia would be the conditioned placebo analgesic response.

The roles of conditioning and expectancy induction through verbal suggestion in the field of placebo analgesia show mixed results (e.g., Bąbel et al., 2017; Bartels et al., 2014). For example, a study by Voudouris, Peck, and Coleman (1990) indicates that conditioning alone is a powerful

(4)

method to induce placebo effects, compared to verbal expectancy induction in placebo analgesia. However, various studies (e.g., Carlino et al., 2014; Bartels et al., 2014) suggest that it is the combined effect of both verbal suggestions and conditioning that has more significant placebo effects than sole verbal suggestion and sole conditioning. For example, the pure pairing between pain reduction and visual cues combined with verbal suggestions can generate conditioned (placebo) analgesic responses. Thus, to increase the magnitude of placebo analgesia, the employment of both specific conditioning paradigms and manipulations in verbal instructions can be used (Finniss, et al., 2009).

A variety of cognitive and biological factors have been found to have a moderating role in pain perception, such as age (Cook & Chastain, 2001). Alongside (verbal) manipulations and conditioning paradigms to affect one’s outcome expectancies, levels of anxiety have also been hypothesized to have a role in placebo analgesia. Namely, briefing individuals about impending pain has been shown to increase negative emotions, including anxiety, and in turn increase one’s perception of pain (Flaten, Alaksen, Lyby, & Bjørkedal, 2011). Researchers differentiate between trait anxiety (a person’s general disposition to be anxious) and state anxiety (a person’s current level of anxiety, which can be modulated by situational factors) (Spielberger & Krasner, 1988). In the current study, we will mainly focus on the relationship between state anxiety and placebo analgesia, as state anxiety can affect a variety of aspects of pain perception. For example, Staats, Staats, and Hekmat (2001) suggest that state anxiety affects the induction of placebo effects, in which low state anxiety decreases pain perception (Evans, 1985). Moreover, heightened levels of state anxiety have been shown to lead to a reduction in pain tolerance (Carter et al., 2002) and to increased self-reported pain intensities (Jones, Spindler, Jorgensen, & Zachariae, 2002). We will study the effects of state anxiety in the situational context of an experimental pain procedure

making use of conditioning and expectancy induction. 1.1 Open-Label Placebo

The expectancy theory of placebo analgesia is challenged by demonstrations that placebo treatments can still elicit analgesic effects even when individuals are aware of the nature of the treatment being technically ineffective (Benedetti et al., 2003). Typically, placebos are delivered in a deceptive manner (i.e., closed placebos; CPs), making placebo receivers (e.g., patients of chronic pain) unaware of the nature of the placebo treatment. However, a number of studies suggest that deception is not required in order to elicit placebo effects (e.g., Carvalho et al., 2016). Evidence

(5)

shows that long-term placebo effects can be elicited under “open-label” conditions, in which individuals are fully informed about the administration of an inert placebo treatment (Blease, Colloca, & Kaptchuk, 2016; Charlesworth et al., 2017). These cases allow for the use of placebo treatments without ethical constraints such as those that come with deception, and they promote patient involvement in clinical settings. Kaptchuk and Miller (2018) hypothesize that open-label placebos (OLPs) are valuable for conditions in which placebo responses are substantial and rival the active treatment. For example, Hoenemeyer, Kaptchuk, Mehta, and Fontaine (2018), who studied OLP treatment for cancer-related fatigue, conclude that OLPs may reduce fatigue symptom severity and quality of life in cancer survivors. Promising results arise from research concerning OLP in clinical implementation settings, including chronic pain in the lower back (Carvalho et al., 2016) and irritable bowel syndrome (Kaptchuk et al., 2010). Furthermore, Locher and colleagues (2017) compared the effects of CP, OLP, and not receiving any treatment in an experimental heat pain paradigm. They conclude that OLP treatments are as effective as CP treatments when accompanied by a rationale. It is thus worthy to investigate whether further implementing a conditioning paradigm along with differences in knowledge levels over the (placebo) nature of the treatment (OLP versus CP) would contribute to the increase of placebo analgesia, taking into account individuals’ state anxiety levels.

1.2 Relevance

Further exploration of OLPs are of great theoretical and clinical importance. The use of placebo treatments is a point of controversy because placebos do not include an active medication to treat the condition at hand, because they are thought to be less effective than other treatments, and due to the above-mentioned ethical reasons (Colloca & Howick, 2018). This is where OLPs could take part, through bypassing a part of the ethical barriers to the use of placebo treatments. In addition, it is important to study the potential of OLPs as a therapeutic method, as physicians use placebo treatments in routine care (Linde et al., 2018). OLPs can be exploited to treat pain conditions, such as fibromyalgia and chronic pain, while diminishing the administration of pharmacological substances and the adverse effects accompanying them. Thus, a thorough understanding of the best conditions to apply them is of great value.

Moreover, it is important to identify the ways in which patients’ anxiety levels at the time of the treatment (i.e., state anxiety) may affect their responsivity to placebo treatments in order to facilitate treatment effectiveness. This can be done through studying the effects of state anxiety on

(6)

placebo responsivity, taking into account the patients’ knowledge of the nature of the treatment (OLP or CP). Understanding the response nature of highly anxious individuals to different types of placebo treatments can enhance our knowledge over the most appropriate treatments to be used in medical settings and over the best forms of communication methods about placebo treatments to this subgroup of patients. While conducting experimental studies with OLPs, an explanation about what placebo effects entail are of utmost importance. Providing participants with placebo information can function as a simulation between a medical professional and their patients, allowing direct translation of the results of the study into clinical practice using (open-label) placebo treatments, taking into account the patients’ state anxiety levels during treatment application.

In the present study, we aim to condition healthy individuals to associate pain intensities with neutral visual cues (i.e., colors with high or low pain intensities), leading to changes in their pain expectancies while receiving a placebo treatment, taking in consideration knowledge over the nature of the treatment (open-label or closed) and their personal levels of state anxiety.

1.3 Hypotheses

We investigate whether OLPs are as beneficial as CPs in alleviating the intensity of healthy individuals’ ratings of their experienced pain. Furthermore, we study the effects of state anxiety on individuals’ pain perception and placebo analgesia in a conditioning paradigm, inducing changes in pain intensity expectancies. Thus, firstly, we hypothesize that individuals from the placebo groups (i.e., OLP and CP) will experience more placebo analgesia when compared to the control group (CG). Secondly, we hypothesize that individuals who are informed about the placebo nature of the treatment (i.e., OLP group) will experience similar placebo analgesia levels to those who are not educated about it (i.e., CP group). Furthermore, we hypothesize that individuals scoring high on state anxiety will experience less placebo effects across the three groups. Moreover, we expect that overall pain ratings will be higher for individuals scoring high on state anxiety than individuals scoring low on state anxiety. Lastly, we control whether state anxiety levels increase at the end of the experiment in comparison to the beginning of the testing.

2. Methods 2.1 Participants

Our study included 28 participants (60.7% females) between the ages of 18 and 35 (M = 23.14, SD = 2.578), of 16 different nationalities (Figure 1), 17.9% of whom were of German nationality.

(7)

Individuals were not eligible to participate if they had participated in heat pain-related studies in the behavioral and social sciences faculty before (i.e., “Aan de slag!! Pijn studie” and/or “ENS on pain and itch”); were experiencing severe physical morbidity; were diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder; were suffering from chronic pain; were suffering from injuries on hands/arms; were regular users of recreational drugs; and/or were pregnant or breastfeeding. Participants who had consumed alcohol and/or pain medication 24 hours prior to the experiment, caffeine and/or nicotine three hours prior to the experiment, and/or recreational drugs one week prior to the experiment, were exempted from the study. All participants were randomly assigned to one of three groups, and were compensated for their participation either financially or with study credits.

Figure 1. Pie chart of participant nationalities. 2.2 Study Design

This study was a randomized, single-blinded, controlled trial, that included two experimental groups (i.e., closed placebo (CL; n = 9) and open-label placebo (OLP; n = 9)) and a control group (CG; n = 10). Both the CL and the OLP groups were under a conditioned pain paradigm, in which they were conditioned to associate personalized heat temperatures with a sham device being ON or OFF, and they both received verbal suggestions about the nature and the effectiveness of the treatment. The CG was not conditioned, nor did they receive verbal suggestions that could lead to associating personalized temperatures with the sham device being ON or OFF. The CG underwent

(8)

sham conditioning to control for the same visual input as the experimental groups, but the colors were unmatched to intensities. The number of participants beginning with the sham device being ON or OFF was counterbalanced between the groups. Furthermore, stratification based on gender was applied for randomization purposes with a 75% versus 25% ratio for females and males respectively.

Researchers followed validated and standard heat pain study protocols (Corsi & Colloca, 2017; Skvortsova, Veldhuijzen, van Middendorp, van den Bergh, & Evers, 2018) through all the sessions. The experiment lasted approximately two hours per participant, including 20 (sham) conditioning trials and six testing trials, through which two experimenters in white lab coats were there to conduct it at all times (Exp A and Exp B) in a standard neat room with closed windows in a room temperature between 20 and 25 degrees Celsius. This study was approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee (CEP19-1010/497). An overall study design is summarized in Figure 2.

Figure 2. General study design. 2.3 Materials

2.3.1. Questionnaires

2.3.1.1. The short version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (S-STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983) was used in order to measure state anxiety levels of individuals. The reliability of the six-item version of the S-STAI is 0.82 (Marteau & Bekker, 1992). Scores of state anxiety were measured at the beginning and the end of the study for each participant.

2.3.1.2. The Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R) (Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994) measured optimism by assessing a total score formed by 10 items, each with a 5-point Likert scale (0 – strongly disagree, 4 – strongly agree). The higher the score, the higher the level of optimism per participant. This questionnaire was used for purposes exceeding the scope of this study.

2.3.1.3. The Numerical Rating Scale (NRS) for pain is a subjective pain measure on which individuals rate their perceived pain intensities on an eleven-point numerical scale (0 = no pain at all; 10 = worst imaginable pain). The NRS provides sufficient discriminative power for chronic pain patients describing their pain intensities (Jensen, Turner, & Romano, 1994). Scores of 0.5-2

(9)

were considered low, 3-4.5 were considered moderate, and scores of 5.5-7 were considered high. During the calibration phase, we did not exceed temperatures that scored a 7 or higher on the NRS.

2.3.2. Devices

2.3.2.1. The PATHWAY Pain & Sensory Evaluation System was provided by the University of Leiden in order to induce thermal pain. The PATHWAY system allows for programmable control of temperature in a precise manner, using the Advanced Thermal Stimulator thermode. This thermode has a 30x30 mm contact with the skin and can produce temperatures between 0°C and 55°C, with the possible change rate of 8°C per second.

2.3.2.2. The Beurer EM80 was used as a sham device. In this study, it was referred to as the “(sham) external neural stimulator (ENS) device”. ENS is a lightweight programmable device with up to eight electrodes and four separately adjusted channels. It is a non-invasive electrical stimulation device that is normally used for pain therapy, muscle stimulation, and relaxation purposes.

2.3.2.3. Three computers: (A) PATHWAY laptop, operated by Exp A; (B) documents computer (i.e., Excel sheets for NRS, eligibility sheets, questionnaire operating system), operated by Exp B; and (C) participant computer (for questionnaires and Psychology Software Tools, which were used for conditioning purposes).

2.4 Procedure

2.4.1. Recruitment and Participant Contact. Participants were recruited through Sona Systems, research recruiters, and flyers. Registration was only possible through Sona Systems, under the study name “Pain Personality Study”, after which participants received a confirmation email with an information letter about the study. All participants were sent standard email reminders 24 hours prior to their appointments.

2.4.2. Randomization. An independent researcher prepared randomization envelopes, which were stratified by sex (female/male), to allocate each participant to one of the 6 conditions: three groups (CL/OLP/CG) * ENS (ON/OFF). This procedure was used in order to maximize blinding during testing.

2.4.3 Study

2.4.3.1. Introduction and eligibility requirements (15 mins): participants were first required to sign an informed consent form, after which demographic and eligibility information were

(10)

collected along with the S-STAI at the beginning of the study. Only participants who qualified for the study were able to carry on with the rest of the phases.

2.4.3.2. Threshold and Calibration (40 mins): In this phase, three levels of heat stimulation were determined for each participant (i.e., low pain, moderate pain, and high -but bearable- pain) through using the PATHWAY Pain & Sensory Evaluation System and the NRS rating scale. Heat stimuli were applied on the dorsal side of the non-dominant arm, placing the PATHWAY thermode 11cm from the wrist in the elbow direction with its full surface touching the skin. Tattoos and damaged skin were avoided. This phase was identical for all participants, as they were not yet assigned to specific groups. In the first part of this phase, warmth thresholds were repeatedly measured by asking each participant to stop the heat simulation when they first felt a change in temperature on the thermode, which was done by clicking the PATHWAY mouse button. Pain threshold measurements were conducted in a similar fashion, in which participants stopped the heat stimulation when they first felt pain. These procedures were conducted through one practice trial and three measurements each. After each measurement, participants were asked to rate their pain threshold on a scale from 1-10 on the NRS, which were filled in by Exp B. The thermode was removed after this part of the calibration phase. The second part of this phase included brief stimuli of different, gradually rising intensities (starting at 36°C till 50°C), after re-attaching the thermode on the non-dominant arm, and participants were then asked to rate their pain immediately after the temperature was back to baseline after each stimulus. At the end of this part the thermode was detached again and Exp A filled in the corresponding temperatures that the participant rated as low, medium, and high pain intensities in the PATHWAY program to prepare for the upcoming phase. In the final part of the phase, the thermode was attached to the dominant arm. This part of the calibration phase included applying the different personalized temperatures and checking for the pain ratings on the NRS for finalizing the personalized temperatures to be used in the conditioning and testing phase. At the end of this stage, the thermode was removed and participants were instructed to take a short break while the researchers set up for the next phase of the experiment.

Each temperature peak lasted for 4 seconds, after which participants were asked to rate their pain intensities from 1 to 10 on NRS. During the calibration phase, any pain rating above 7 was considered too high and out of the scope of this study.

2.4.3.3. ENS Instructions and Calibration (15 mins): Depending on the group they were assigned to, participants received different information about the ENS device and its functionality,

(11)

after which Exp A gave participants verbal suggestions according to their group. The OLP group was openly told the ENS device would be used as a sham device, and that placebo effects can have a large impact on pain perception. They were also told that hundreds of studies have demonstrated that placebo effects can influence chemical processes in the body to successfully alter pain perception and received further information about the placebo effect through an information sheet (for more details view Appendix B). Furthermore, the CP group was not told that the ENS device will be switched off during the experiment, but that the device can send light electrical pulses that can affect nerve conduction, that it decreases pain in about 92% of the people, and that they will probably feel less pain from the thermode (Appendix C). They further received an information sheet stating information about the effectivity of the ENS device. Lastly, in order to control for expectations, the CG was told that the ENS device will sometimes by turned on and some others will be turned off, and that some patients have reported to benefit from the effects of the ENS, but some others say that it does not alter their experience of pain. Control participants also received a handout containing this information (Appendix D).

The sham device was attached to the dominant arm through two electrodes two cm from the elbow in the direction of the palm, and they were four cm apart. The device was set to only cause minor pulsations when activated to pulsation level four on electrodes CH2 in order to convince the participants that it was working, after which the participants, regardless of their assigned group, were told that researchers must find a suitable and effective ENS mode below their perception level, lowering the intensity until participants could not feel the pulsation anymore. In reality, the ENS device was then switched off. Only participants of the OLP were aware that the device was not on.

2.4.3.4. Conditioning and Formal Testing (15 mins): In the conditioning part of this phase, 20 visual cues (10 purple lights with the word ON for low pain, and 10 yellow lights with the word OFF for medium pain) were paired with the activation of the ENS device in the OLP and the CP groups. In the CG, stimuli were presented in a semi-random order along with the two colors paired with ON and OFF words, leading to sham conditioning. These sham trials were of 3:1 ratio of random color-cue allocation and planned pairing. Personalized temperatures were administered; namely, 10 relatively moderate pain intensities and 10 relatively low intensities. All participants were required to rate their pain levels on the NRS after each stimulus. Furthermore, the testing part of this phase included six visual cues, three yellow and three purple, three of which included the

(12)

word ON and three that included the word OFF (in the OLP and CP groups they corresponded with the colors; in the CG they were semi-randomized). However, all heat intensities were the same personalized medium intensity across the six cues. Differences between pain ratings in this part of the experiment were used as a measure for placebo effects.

2.4.3.5. Finishing (15 mins): The ENS electrodes and the PATHWAY thermode were removed, and the participants were asked to fill-in the S-STAI questionnaire again as well as the LOT-R (which was used for an extended research project) on the participants’ computer. Participants were further debriefed about the actual purpose of the study, and asked to sign a confidentiality form necessary for further research purposes. Lastly, individuals were rewarded for their participation, either through study credits or a through a financial reward.

2.5 Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using the IBM Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). To answer the research questions mentioned above, two mixed analyses of variance (mixed ANOVAs) were conducted. The first was to answer the questions of whether the placebo effect took place, and if so, whether it was specific to the placebo groups (i.e., OLP and CP). Based on this model, we further assessed if the OLP group had comparable results to the CP group regarding placebo analgesia. In the second model, we added the results of state anxiety levels through the use of the S-STAI results to study the effects of state anxiety on the placebo effect. We used post hoc tests with Bonferroni multiple testing correction for groups. Furthermore, a regression analysis was conducted to test the effects of state anxiety on the overall pain ratings (i.e., NRS scores). Moreover, a paired samples t-test was conducted to compare anxiety levels from the beginning and the end of the experiment to control for any increase in participants’ anxiousness levels at the end of the experiment. For all analyses, the level of statistical significance was set at p < .05.

3. Results

Twenty-eight individuals participated in this study, however four were excluded, three of whom were excluded due to program issues during the conditioning phase, and one due to personal response inconsistency. The OLP and the CP groups had nine participants each, and the CG had six. Prior to the completion of mixed analysis of variance (Mixed ANOVA), assumptions of sphericity, homoscedasticity, and normality were checked for, and none of them were fulfilled. However, statistical analyses still took place for exploratory purposes.

(13)

3.1 Hypotheses Testing

Firstly, we tested whether a placebo effect took place in the study, we compared the pain ratings on the ENS OFF and ENS ON. Results show a significant difference between the two (p = .01; ηp2

= .26). Furthermore, we compared groups to answer whether this was due to our placebo manipulation (i.e., the verbal suggestions and the conditioning) and whether this placebo effect is comparable between the OLP and the CP groups. Results show no overall significant group differences (p = .88; ηp2 = .01). Results of the two placebo hypotheses are reported in Table 1, and

a visualization of ON and OFF scores across groups are demonstrated in Figure 2. Table 1. A Summary of Placebo Effects and Group Comparability.

Type III SS df MS F Sig. (p) ηp2

Differences between ON and OFF 3.61 1 3.61 7.33 .01 .26

Group Differences 0.13 2 .06 .13 .88 .01

Error (Difference ON and OFF) 10.33 21 .49

(14)

In addition, to study the third hypothesis over the effects of low state anxiety on the placebo effect, we incorporated another between subject factor, namely the S-STAI, in the mixed ANOVA model stated above. Results show a non-significant effect (p = .76; ηp2 = .21), and that

by adding the S-STAI to the model, the placebo effect in general also became non-significant (p = .96; ηp2 = .01).

Furthermore, to study whether state anxiety levels had an effect on individuals’ overall pain ratings, namely whether highly anxious individuals scored higher on the NRS than people low on anxiety, we conducted a regression analysis. Results show non-significant differences, as further shown in Table 2.

Table 2. Model and Coefficients Summary including S-STAI total on NRS scores.

Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients t Sig. (p) Variance B SE β R2 Adjusted R2 (Constant) 3.94 1.42 2.77 .01 Total of S-STAI -0.03 0.04 -.14 -0.65 .53 Full Model .53 0.02 -0.03

3.2 Control for Anxiety Levels

A paired samples t-test was conducted to compare means of the S-STAI levels at the beginning and at the end of the experiment to control for any increase in participants’ anxiety levels. Results show no significant differences between the first S-STAI and the second (t(21) = 0.48, p = .64) with the mean difference of 0.76; as stated in Table 3.

Table 3. Summary of the Differences in Anxiety Levels at the Beginning and the End of the Study.

Paired Differences

M SD SE Mean t df Sig. (p) (2-tailed)

Difference in S-STAI 0.76 7.41 1.58 0.48 21 .64

4. Discussion

The current study investigated the role of state anxiety on placebo analgesia in a conditioning paradigm. Previous research suggests that expectancies can significantly affect the experience of pain either positively or negatively (Colloca & Benedetti, 2005), and expectancies of a therapeutic

(15)

effect in a placebo setting can be fundamental in underlying the placebo analgesic effect (Vase et al., 2003). Furthermore, the combined use of verbal manipulation and conditioning positively affects placebo analgesia. In this study, participants were subjected to thermal pain and were conditioned to believe that the ENS device was functional in the reduction of pain. The actual placebo nature of the device was only disclosed with a subgroup of the participants, namely OLPs, as it has been shown that long-term placebo effects can be elicited under these conditions (Blease et al., 2016; Charlesworth et al., 2017). Moreover, state anxiety levels of participants were measured prior to the experiment, as it has been hypothesized that heightened levels of state anxiety can lead to an increase in self-reported pain intensities (Jones et al., 2002); state anxiety was measured in the end once more to investigate how the experiment affected participants’ psychological states. In sum, we aimed to condition healthy individuals to connect pain intensities with color cues representing the claimed effectiveness of the ENS device, taking into consideration the state anxiety and nature-of-treatment awareness levels of the participants.

Preliminary analyses show a violation of the assumptions, however we decided to continue with the analysis for exploratory purposes, as this the first study to use such a protocol. Researchers firstly checked whether the placebo effect took place or not, and it turned out that it did, as individuals from the placebo groups (i.e., OLP and CP) had lower pain intensity scores when the participant screen suggested that the sham ENS device was ON. This is in line with what we attempted to accomplish in order to be able to begin our analyses. Secondly, we wanted to study whether this effect was due to our placebo manipulation by comparing the placebo groups with the CG. However, we found that the placebo effect took also took place in the CG, meaning that the placebo effect was not solely due to the researchers’ manipulations (i.e., through conditioning and verbal manipulation). This could be due to the small sample size that we had in all the groups, leading to statistical restriction. It is also possible that we unintentionally conditioned the CG even though we used a 75% versus 25% ratio of screen randomization of OFF and ON color pairings; however, that is unlikely because there were 20 conditioning trials, which should be sufficient for our conditioning purposes, in line with previous studies that successfully conditioned individuals with similar trial numbers (Colloca, Petrovic, Wager, Ingvar, & Benedetti, 2010; Corsi & Colloca, 2017). Furthermore, we hypothesized that individuals aware of the nature of the ENS device (i.e., OLP group) will experience similar placebo effects to individuals of the CP group. Results show

(16)

there were no group differences in placebo effects, meaning that the two groups were comparable in their placebo analgesic responses.

Moreover, we tested whether anxiety levels influenced placebo analgesia in our sample, and results show that there were no effects. Interestingly, however, when anxiety levels were taken into consideration, the placebo effect was no longer evident in the statistical analysis, which could possibly be due to the small sample size, making the data statistically sensitive. Namely, the addition of an extra predictor in the statistical model without changing the sample size, especially when it is relatively small, makes the anxiety factor function as a positive confounder (Cohen, 1988). We further hypothesized that individuals scoring high on state anxiety would have higher overall pain ratings on the NRS than individuals scoring low on state anxiety. Results showed no differences between individuals high in state anxiety and individuals low in state anxiety. Again, the small sample size could account for such results. Lastly, we controlled for any increase in state anxiety at the end of the experiment by comparing individual scores to state anxiety levels from the beginning of the study, for ethical compliance reasons. Results showed no violation of the standard suggesting that participants should not leave the lab feeling more negatively than when they arrived.

This study’s limitations extend to its variables. Due to the small sample size, this study could be considered to have a highly underpowered design for the above-mentioned research purposes, and future research should collect further data in this field of study. The main reason behind such a small sample size was time limitations. Furthermore, not much information can be derived from this study, as statistical analyses were inconclusive due to assumption restrictions. Moreover, we cannot conclude much about whether conditioning fulfilled its main purpose, as it was coupled with verbal suggestion. This way, we could only derive whether the placebo effect took place without being able to distinguish which approach had more effect (conditioning or verbal suggestion). This makes it difficult to fully understand the reasons behind why the CG also demonstrated placebo analgesic effects. However, we chose this particular design as recent research (Bartels et al., 2014) suggests that the combination of verbal suggestion and conditioning has better effect on placebo effects than either of them alone, and that to increase the magnitude of placebo analgesic responses, it would be better to employ a combination of both (Finnis et al., 2009).

(17)

Moreover, during the calibration phase, personalized moderate and low temperatures were calculated to be used in the conditioning and testing phases. These individualized temperatures were, for some participants, of a small degree difference (e.g., low: 46 degrees; moderate: 47 degrees), and for others they were with a greater range (e.g., low: 42 degrees; high: 49.5 degrees). Namely, 11 participants had a temperature range less than three temperatures difference. Even though the scores are personalized, it is more likely that individuals experiencing a smaller range of temperatures would be habituated to the similar degrees, perceiving similar amounts of pain intensities throughout the conditioning and testing phases, affecting results of group differences.

Altogether, it is of great importance to understand the underlying mechanisms of OLP, as this approach does not violate the ethical standards of administering a health treatment; namely, deception. One of the main strengths of this study is that it is the first to investigate the role of anxiety in placebo analgesia in a conditioning paradigm using the open label placebo method. Furthermore, this research attempted to investigate how state anxiety affects one’s responsivity to the application of a placebo treatment in the purpose of simulating an actual patient-professional interaction. Even though the results were inconclusive due to statistical restrictions, the study is the first of its kind to incorporate such a model, in which researchers followed very strict validated protocols (Corsi & Colloca, 2017; Skvortsova et al., 2018). Moreover, state anxiety levels were measured at the end of the study for control purposes, order for the participants not to leave the lab feeling more anxious than when they arrived; results show that this ethical standard was not violated. A further strength of this research is the diversity of the sample. Even though it was a very small sample, the participants were of 16 different nationalities, making the sample relatively heterogeneous. This cultural diversity is interesting, as it allows us to look into the various ways in which individuals experience and cope with pain, in line with studies suggesting that people from diverse cultures experience pain differently (Hocking, 1996; Peacock & Patel, 2008). A final strength of this research is that there were two researchers in the lab with the participants at all times, easing the complex implementation of the protocol and eliminating researcher bias.

Due to the above-mentioned reasons, this experiment could be considered a pilot study to a more strongly developed design. The main aspect to be further developed is the sample size, which can be attained through having sufficient data collection time, financial support, and more efficient recruitment methods (e.g., online advertising). Future research using this as a pilot study could make use of expanding this research to look into how the effects of people’s general anxiety levels

(18)

(i.e., trait anxiety) are connected to their placebo and conditioning responsivity. Additionally, in this study, we attempted to compare individuals high in anxiety to ones low in anxiety due to the higher contrast and to make the sample more comparable to pain patients suffering from anxiety. This rationale is based on evidence suggesting that anxiety disorder affects 25% of pain populations (Polatin, Kinney, Gatchel, Lillo, & Mayer, 1993), whereas it affects 8.8% of the general population (Blazer, Kessler, McGonagle, & Swartz, 1994). However, it would be of interest to include moderate anxiety scores in the comparison, as the number of individuals scoring in the moderate range in this paper was comparable to the number of individuals in the high range. Future studies can also manipulate state levels of anxiety in order to have more balanced sample numbers within the state anxiety variable, which could achieve better statistical inferences by decreasing the rate of statistical assumption violations. Moreover, future research could look into the extent that verbal suggestion alone can influence placebo analgesia and pain perception in individuals scoring high on anxiety, as verbal suggestion alone is more applicable in a realistic clinical setting. Research based on this pilot study can explicitly measure the belief in the administered placebo for further control purposes. The limitations of this study were partly expected as it is the first of its kind to combine many different factors, such as state anxiety and conditioning, into one experiment in the field of placebo analgesia of thermal pain. Even though not many statistical inferences took place in this paper, it can still have a large positive impact on this field of study, as future research can enhance upon the limitations and can make use of the process implemented.

In sum, this study investigated the influence of state anxiety on placebo analgesic responses within a conditioning paradigm, taking into account the level of knowledge that individuals have concerning the nature of the treatment that they received (open-label or not). This is the first study to incorporate variables within one protocol, thus its strengths and limitations cause one to consider it a pilot study, leading future research to be more conclusive of the collected data.

(19)

References

Bąbel, P., Bajcar, E. A., Adamczyk, W., Kicman, P., Lisińska, N., Świder, K., & Colloca, L. (2017). Classical conditioning without verbal suggestions elicits placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia. PLoS One, 12(7), e0181856

Bartels, D. J., van Laarhoven, A. I., Haverkamp, E. A., Wilder-Smith, O. H., Donders, A. R. T., van Middendorp, H., . . . Evers, A. W. (2014). Role of conditioning and verbal suggestion in placebo and nocebo effects on itch. PLoS One, 9(3), e91727.

Benedetti F. (2009). Placebo Effects: Understanding the mechanisms in health and disease: Oxford University Press.

Benedetti F., Pollo, A., Lopiano, L., Lanotte, M., Vighetti, S., & Rainero, I. (2003). Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 4315-4323.

Blazer DG, Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz MS. The prevalence and distribution of major depression in a national community sample: The National Comorbidity Survey. Am J Psychiatry 1994;151:979–86.

Blease, C., Colloca, L., & Kaptchuk, T. J. (2016). Are open-label placebos ethical? Informed consent and ethical equivocations. Bioethics, 30(6), 407–414. https://doi.org/10.1111/bioe.12245

Carlino, E., Torta, D., Piedimonte, A., Frisaldi, E., Vighetti, S., & Benedetti, F. (2014). Role of explicit verbal information in conditioned analgesia. European Journal of Pain, 19(4), 546-553. doi:10.1002/ejp.579

Carter, L.E., McNeil, D. W., Vowles, K. E., Sorrell, J. T., Turk, C. L., Ries, B. J., & Hopko, D.R. (2002). Effects of emotion on pain reports, tolerance and physiology. Pain Research and Management, 7, 21-30

Carvalho, C., Caetano, J. M., Cunha, L., Rebouta, P., Kaptchuk, T. J., & Kirsch, I. (2016). Open-label placebo treatment in chronic low back pain: A randomized controlled trial. Pain, 157(12), 2766–2772. https:// doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000700.

Charlesworth, J. E. G., Petkovic, G., Kelley, J. M., Hunter, M., Onakpoya, I., Roberts N., … Howick, J. (2017). Effects of placebos without deception compared with no treatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Evidence-Based Medicine, 10(2), 97–107

(20)

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: L.

Erlbaum Associates.

Colloca, L. & Benedetti, F. (2005). Placebos and painkillers: is mind as real as matter? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 545-552

Colloca, L. & Howick, J. (2018). Placebos without deception: outcomes, mechanisms, and ethics. International Review of Neurobiology, 138, 219-240. doi:10.1016/bs.irn.2018.01.005.

Colloca, L., Klinger, R., Flor, H., & Bingel, U. (2013). Placebo analgesia: Psychological and neurobiological mechanisms. Pain, 154, 511–514

Colloca, L., Petrovic, P., Wager, T. D., Ingvar, M., & Benedetti, F. (2010). How the number of learning trials affects placebo and nocebo responses. Pain, 151(2), 430–439. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.08.007

Cook, A. J. & Chastain, D. C. (2001). The classification of patients with chronic pain: Age and sex differences. Pain Research and Management , 6, 142-151.

Corsi, N., & Colloca, L. (2017). Placebo and nocebo effects: the advantage of measuring expectations and psychological factors. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 308.

Evans, F. J. (1985). Expectancy therapeutic instructions and the placebo response (pp. 215-228). New York: Guildford.

Finniss, D. G., Nicholas, M. K., & Benedetti, F. (2009). Placebo Analgesia—Understanding the Mechanisms and Implications for Clinical Practice. Reviews in Pain, 3(2), 15-19. doi:10.1177/204946370900300205

Flaten, M. A., Aslaksen, P. M., Lyby, P. S., & Bjørkedal, E. (2011). The relation of emotions to placebo responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1818 –1827 Hoenemeyer, T. W., Kaptchuk, T. J., Mehta, T. S., & Fontaine, K. R. (2018). Open-Label Placebo

Treatment for Cancer-Related Fatigue: A Randomized-Controlled Clinical Trial. Scientific Reports, 8(1). doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20993-y

Hocking, B. (1996). Some social and cultural anthropological aspects of musculoskeletal disorders as exemplified by the Telecom Australia RSI epidemic. In: Moon, S.D., Sauter, S. L., (Ed.), Beyond biomechanics: psychosocial aspects of musculoskeletal disorders in office work (p.145-158). Taylor Francis: London.

IBM Corp. Released 2017. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

(21)

Jensen, M. P., Turner, J. A., & Romano, J. M. (1994). What is the maximum number of levels needed in pain intensity measurement? Pain, 58, 387-392.

Jones, A., Spindler, H., Jorgensen, M. M., & Zachariae, R. (2002). The effect of situation-evoked anxiety and gender on pain report using the cold pressor test. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 307-313

Kaptchuk, T. J., & Miller, F. G. (2018). Open label placebo: Can honestly prescribed placebos evoke meaningful therapeutic benefits? Bmj. doi:10.1136/bmj.k3889

Kaptchuk, T. J., Friedlander, E., Kelley, J. M., Sanchez, M. N., Kokkotou, E., Singer, J. P., . . . Lembo, A. J. (2010). Placebos without deception: a randomized controlled trial in irritable bowel syndrome. PLoS One, 5(12), e15591.

Linde, K., Atmann, O., Meissner, K., Schneider, A., Meister, R., Kriston, L., & Werner, C. (2018). How often do general practitioners use placebos and non-specific interventions? Systematic review and meta-analysis of surveys. Plos One, 13(8). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0202211 Locher, C., Nascimento, A. F., Kirsch, I., Kossowsky, J., Meyer, A., & Gaab, J. (2017). Is the

rationale more important than deception? A randomized controlled trial of open-label placebo analgesia. Pain, 158, 2320–2328.

Marteau, T. & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British Journal of Psychology, 31(3):301–306.

Peacock, S. & Patel, S. (2008). Cultural influences on pain. Reviews in Pain, 1(2), 6-9.

Polatin PB, Kinney RK, Gatchel RJ, Lillo E, Mayer TG. Psychiatric illness and chronic low-back pain. The mind and the spine—Which goes first? Spine 1993;18:66–71

Price, D. D., Milling, L. S., Kirsch, I., Duff, A., Montgomery G. H., & Nicholls, S. S. (1999). An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm. Pain, 83, 147-156.

Schedlowski, M., Enck, P., Rief, W., & Bingel, U. (2015). Neuro-bio-behavioral mechanisms of placebo and nocebo responses: implications for clinical trials and clinical practice. Pharmacological reviews, 67(3), 697-730.

Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S. & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A re-evaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063-1078.

(22)

Skvortsova, A., Veldhuijzen, J., van Middendorp, H., van den Bergh, O., & Evers, A. (2018). Placebo effects and conditioning of hormonal responses of oxytocin. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(3), A150-A151.

Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P.R. and Jacobs, G.A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto.

Spielberger, C. D. & Krasner, S. S. (1988). The assessment of state and trait anxiety, in Noyes, R. Jr., Roth, M., & Burrows, G. D. (eds): Handbook of Anxiety, Volume 2: Classification, Etiological Factors and Associated Disturbances (pp 31-51). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

Staats, P. S., Staats, A., & Hekmat, H. (2001). The additive impact of anxiety and a placebo on pain. Pain Medicine, 2, 267–279

Vase, L., Robinson, M. E., Verne, G. N., & Price, D. D. (2003). The contributions of suggestion, desire, and expectation to placebo effects in irritable bowel syndrome patients: an empirical investigation. Pain, 105, 17-25.

Voudouris, N. J., Peck, C. L., & Coleman, G. (1985). Conditioned placebo responses. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(1), 47-53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.1.47

Voudouris, N. J., Peck, C. L., & Coleman, G. (1990). The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response. Pain, 43(1), 121-128

(23)

Appendix A

The Short Version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983)

A number of statements which people have used to describe themselves are given below. Read each statement and then choose the most appropriate option to the right of the statement to indicate how you feel right now, at this moment. There are no right or wrong answers. Do not spend too much time on any one statement but give the answer which seems to describe you present feelings best.

Not at all Somewhat Moderately Very much I feel calm 1 2 3 4 I am tense 1 2 3 4 I feel upset 1 2 3 4 I am relaxed 1 2 3 4 I feel content 1 2 3 4 I am worried 1 2 3 4

(24)

Appendix B

Information Sheet Given to the OLP Group Electrical nerve stimulation (ENS) can decrease pain

What is ENS?

Electrical Nerve Stimulation (ENS) is the administration of an electric current produced by a special device that can affect nerve fibres. ENS decreases nerve conduction.

Scientific research found support for the effectiveness of ENS for chronic numbness of the lower limbs, lateral epicondylitis (tennis elbow), and carpal tunnel syndrome (Chesterton et al.,

2016; Koca, Boyaci, Tutoglu, Ucar, & Kocaturk, 2017; Mulvey, Fawkner, Radford, & Johnson, 2016).

How does ENS affect pain?

Heat pain is decreased by ENS through nerve fibre conductivity. Indeed, nerve fibres in the skin communicate what is perceived via electrical signals. When these signals are sent to the spinal cord and brain, we become aware of the pain sensations. ENS stimulation can influence this conductivity process by decreasing the intensity of incoming signals. This means that pain caused by stimuli applied on the skin, such as heat stimuli, can be decreased by this device (Chen & Johnson, 2017; Schliessbach, Klift, Arendt-nielsen, Curatolo, & Streitberger, 2016).

ENS decreases pain

ENS treatment has repeatedly been found to decrease pain from heat, as a side effect of decreased nerve conduction (Vance, Dailey, Rakel, & Sluka, 2017). A recent study showed that 92% of the participants reported substantially lower pain when they received heat stimuli during ENS (Ellrich & Lamp, 2017).

Sub perception stimulation

Some studies shown that slight electrical pulses, below the perception level, can modulate deep electrical signal conduction (Chen & Johnson, 2017, 2018). So clinically, a big advantage of ENS is that an (almost) imperceptible stimulation is sufficient to affect electrical conduction and thus reduce the perceived pain (Ellrich & Lamp, 2017).

(25)

References

Chen, C. C., & Johnson, M. I. (2017). An Investigation Into the Effects of Frequency-Modulated Electrical Nerve Stimulation (ENS) on Experimentally-Induced Pain in Healthy Human Participants. Journal of Pain, 10(10), 1029–1037. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2017.03.008 Chen, C. C., & Johnson, M. I. (2018). An Investigation Into the Hypoalgesic Effects of High- and Low-Frequency Electrical Nerve Stimulation (ENS) on Experimentally-Induced Blunt Pain in Healthy Human Participants. Journal of Pain, 11(1), 53–61.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2018.05.008

Chesterton, L. S., Lewis, A. M., Sim, J., Mallen, C. D., Mason, E. E., Hay, E. M., & Van Der Windt, D. A. (2016). Electrical nerve stimulation as adjunct to primary care management for tennis elbow: Pragmatic randomised controlled trial (TATE trial). British Journal of Sports Medicine, 48(19), 1458. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2016-f5160rep

Ellrich, J., & Lamp, S. (2017). Peripheral nerve stimulation decreases nociceptive processing: An electrophysiological study. Neuromodulation, 8(4), 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-1403.2005.00029.x

Koca, I., Boyaci, A., Tutoglu, A., Ucar, M., & Kocaturk, O. (2017). Assessment of the

effectiveness of interferential current therapy and ENS in the management of carpal tunnel syndrome: a randomized controlled study. Rheumatology International, 34(12), 1639–1645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00296-014-3005-3

Mulvey, M. R., Fawkner, H. J., Radford, H., & Johnson, M. I. (2016). The use of electrical nerve stimulation (ENS) to aid perceptual embodiment of prosthetic limbs. Medical Hypotheses, 72(2), 140–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2016.08.028

Schliessbach, J., Van Der Klift, E., Arendt-Nielsen, L., Curatolo, M., & Streitberger, K. (2016). The Effect of Brief Electrical and Manual Acupuncture Stimulation on Mechanical

Experimental Pain. Pain Medicine, 12(2), 268–275. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2010.01051.x

Vance, C. G. T., Dailey, D. L., Rakel, B. A., & Sluka, K. A. (2017). Using ENS for pain control: the state of the evidence. Pain Management, 4(3), 197–209.

https://doi.org/10.2217/pmt.14.13

(26)

PAIN PERCEPTION AND PLACEBO EFFECTS The Placebo Effect

Now, some more information follows about the purpose of this study. This study focuses on placebo effects. Placebo effects are a common phenomenon in scientific research and clinical practice.

In treatment studies, overall treatment effects are the result of two mechanisms:

The treatment effect itself (red bar in figure);

The placebo effect (blue bars in figure) which is dependent on factors such as previous experience, expectations and the physician-patient interaction.

As you can see in the picture, the placebo effect has a large influence in the total treatment effect. Placebo effects can thus increase therapeutic effects.

How do placebos work?

Expectations have a big impact on treatment outcomes. If you have positive expectations, they may develop a more positive treatment outcome and affect your pain experience in this present study. You may not only feel better from the procedure itself, but also because you expect to feel better.

Positive expectations also affect processes in your body. When you have positive expectations, the brain produces chemicals. These chemical substances are called

neurotransmitters and can make you feel better. Placebos can also trigger the release of neurotransmitters. In this study we will be making use of these processes to reduce pain.

(27)

In this study we want to induce placebo effects with the ENS device. The device will be switched off during the experiment, but it is definitely possible that you experience less pain because of placebo effects.

(28)

Appendix C

Information Sheet Given to the CP Group Electrical nerve stimulation (ENS) can decrease pain

What is ENS?

Electrical Nerve Stimulation (ENS) is the administration of an electric current produced by a special device that can affect nerve fibers. ENS decreases nerve conduction.

Scientific research found support for the effectiveness of ENS for chronic numbness of the lower limbs, lateral epicondylitis (tennis elbow), and carpal tunnel

syndrome (Chesterton et al., 2016; Koca, Boyaci, Tutoglu, Ucar, & Kocaturk, 2017; Mulvey, Fawkner, Radford, & Johnson, 2016).

How does ENS affect pain?

Heat pain is decreased by ENS through nerve fiber conductivity. Indeed, nerve fibers in the skin communicate what is perceived via electrical signals. When these signals are sent to the spinal cord and brain, we become aware of the pain sensations. ENS stimulation can influence this conductivity process by decreasing the intensity of incoming signals. This means that pain caused by stimuli applied on the skin, such as heat stimuli, can be decreased by this device (Chen & Johnson, 2017; Schliessbach, Klift, Arendt-nielsen, Curatolo, & Streitberger, 2016).

ENS decreases pain

ENS treatment has repeatedly been found to decrease pain from heat, as a side effect of decreased nerve conduction (Vance, Dailey, Rakel, & Sluka, 2017). A recent study showed that 92% of the participants reported substantially lower pain when they received heat stimuli during ENS (Ellrich & Lamp, 2017).

Sub perception stimulation

Some studies shown that slight electrical pulses, below the perception level, can modulate deep electrical signal conduction (Chen & Johnson, 2017, 2018). So, clinically, a big advantage of ENS is that an (almost) imperceptible stimulation is sufficient to affect electrical conduction and thus reduce the perceived pain (Ellrich & Lamp, 2017).

(29)

References

Chen, C. C., & Johnson, M. I. (2017). An Investigation Into the Effects of Frequency-Modulated Electrical Nerve Stimulation (ENS) on Experimentally-Induced Pain in Healthy Human Participants. Journal of Pain, 10(10), 1029–1037. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2017.03.008 Chen, C. C., & Johnson, M. I. (2018). An Investigation Into the Hypoalgesic Effects of High- and Low-Frequency Electrical Nerve Stimulation (ENS) on Experimentally-Induced Blunt Pain in Healthy Human Participants. Journal of Pain, 11(1), 53–61.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2018.05.008

Chesterton, L. S., Lewis, A. M., Sim, J., Mallen, C. D., Mason, E. E., Hay, E. M., & Van Der Windt, D. A. (2016). Electrical nerve stimulation as adjunct to primary care management for tennis elbow: Pragmatic randomised controlled trial (TATE trial). British Journal of Sports Medicine, 48(19), 1458. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2016-f5160rep

Ellrich, J., & Lamp, S. (2017). Peripheral nerve stimulation decreases nociceptive processing: An electrophysiological study. Neuromodulation, 8(4), 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-1403.2005.00029.x

Koca, I., Boyaci, A., Tutoglu, A., Ucar, M., & Kocaturk, O. (2017). Assessment of the

effectiveness of interferential current therapy and ENS in the management of carpal tunnel syndrome: a randomized controlled study. Rheumatology International, 34(12), 1639–1645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00296-014-3005-3

Mulvey, M. R., Fawkner, H. J., Radford, H., & Johnson, M. I. (2016). The use of electrical nerve stimulation (ENS) to aid perceptual embodiment of prosthetic limbs. Medical Hypotheses, 72(2), 140–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2016.08.028

Schliessbach, J., Van Der Klift, E., Arendt-Nielsen, L., Curatolo, M., & Streitberger, K. (2016). The Effect of Brief Electrical and Manual Acupuncture Stimulation on Mechanical

Experimental Pain. Pain Medicine, 12(2), 268–275. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2010.01051.x

Vance, C. G. T., Dailey, D. L., Rakel, B. A., & Sluka, K. A. (2017). Using ENS for pain control: the state of the evidence. Pain Management, 4(3), 197–209.

(30)

Appendix D

Information Sheet Given to the CG Electrical nerve stimulation (ENS)

What is ENS?

Electrical Nerve Stimulation (ENS) is the administration of an electric current produced by a special device that can affect nerve fibers. Can ENS affect pain?

Nerve fibers in the skin communicate what is perceived via electrical signals. When these signals are sent to the spinal cord and brain, we

become aware of the pain sensations. Because ENS induces stimulations, this may have an effect on the intensity of incoming pain signals. Few studies have examined this. Some patients have reported to benefit from the effects of ENS, but other patients report that it doesn’t make any difference in their experience of pain. With this study, we will investigate whether there is support or not for this conductivity theory.

Sub perception stimulation

Some studies show that slight electrical pulses, below the perception level, can modulate deep electrical signal conduction. This study will measure (almost) imperceptible stimulations.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Our main output was a modification of the existing Template for Intervention Description and Replication (TIDieR) checklist; this allows the key features of both active

The same procedures and analyses were used as in Study 1 to assess the effects of response imagery and of adding a verbal suggestion on pain (primary and secondary

In addition, it was expected that open-label positive ver- bal suggestions compared with the control group would reduce the severity of the participants’ skin condition, and

Participants were allocated to I) an open-label conditioned group (acquisition: CS + UCS with an explanation of conditioning and its expected effects; evocation: CS + placebo); II)

The propositio is concluded with the summary statement about the δικαιοσύνη of the disciples that should surpass that of the scribes and the Pharisees (Mt 5:20) This

This study focuses on the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) as a severity indicator for anxiety in primary care patients with different anxiety disorders (social phobia, panic disorder

Because no user (that is connected through a chain of friends to Bob) colludes with the OSN, the OSN does not have access to a private key and a matching public key encryption of S

The data retention on the other hand provides a legal framework for member states to conduct securitizing practices relating to telecommunications data. There