• No results found

How the international public view Palestinians : does the visual frame matter?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "How the international public view Palestinians : does the visual frame matter?"

Copied!
48
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

How the International Public View Palestinians:

Does the Visual Frame Matter?

Ruba Mimi 10583750

Master’s Thesis

Graduate School of Communication

Master’s Program Communication Science: Political Communication Supervisor: Dr. Linda Bos

(2)

“Photography is a system of visual editing. At bottom, it is a matter of surrounding with a frame a portion of one’s cone of vision. Like chess, or writing, it is a matter of choosing from

among given possibilities, but in the case of photography the number of possibilities is not finite but infinite”

(3)

Abstract

Due to the rising use of visuals in media, visual framing studies are gaining a greater

importance nowadays. In response to the calls for more empirical research on visual frames, this study was conducted with a focus on the Israeli-Palestinian conflict, and specifically photographs of Palestinians. This experimental study aims to investigate the effect of two visual frames on the emotions and opinions of the international public. Using two

photographs from Gaza war 2014, the Human-interest frame and the Militant frame were examined. A total of 216 respondents from 36 different countries participated in the

experiment. The Human-interest frame resulted in stronger influence on individual’s positive emotions toward Palestinians, while the Militant frame resulted in negative opinions about the Palestinian in the photograph. However, no effect was found on opinions about

Palestinians in general.

(4)

"Still photographs are the most powerful weapon in the world," AP photojournalist Eddie Adams said. Adams won the Pulitzer Prize for his best- known photograph “General Nguyen Ngoc Loan executing a Viet Cong prisoner in Saigon” (see appendix A) taken during his coverage of the Vietnam War. The photograph shows the South Vietnamese General Nguyen Ngoc Loan executing a prisoner with a single shot to the head. This photo was one of the most influential photos taken during the Vietnam War, Adams commented on the

photograph:“The general killed the Vietcong; I killed the general with my camera” (Rare Historical Photos, 2014, para. 4). The anti-war movement adopted the photo as a symbol of war’s brutality, and it was to change the public’s perception of the war by showing the ruthless picture of the South Vietnamese (Reed, 2004). However, what this photograph does not tell is the whole truth. That prisoner in the picture had just murdered a South Vietnamese colonel, his wife and their six children, which is what led Gen Loan to carry out the execution (Pozgar, MBA, & Pozgar, 2012). Adams said: “Photographs are half truths. You have to put yourself in the situation, it is a war” (Pozgar et al., 2012, p.52). In this photo the general is framed violently, but what if he was framed differently? Would it have changed what the public thought about the war?

According to Abraham and Appiah (2006), visuals are effective in shaping ideological messages, and can change the way people think about the events. Despite the importance of visuals in the field of political communication, empirical evidence about their effect remains scarce (Fahmy & Eakin, 2014). They are the least studied in comparison with the verbal focus (Schill, 2012). A study found that 5 out of 600 studies in political

communication focus on visuals (Johnston, 1990).

There are historical events that have received attention in framing studies; one of these events is the American war on Iraq (e.g., Fahmy & Kim, 2008; Schwalbe, 2006; Schwalbe, Silcock, & Keith, 2008). But surprisingly one of the most important ongoing

(5)

events of the 20th century that hasn’t received enough attention is the Israeli-Palestinian

conflict (Fahmy & Eakin, 2014). The only available studies that have tackled the conflict were content analysis studies, which focused on the representation of Palestinians and Israelis in foreign newspapers (Deprez & Raeymaeckers, 2010; Fahmy & Eakin, 2014; Ross, 2003).

Despite the importance of this conflict to people from different countries who feel connected to it more than any other dramatic events in the world (Smith, 2009), almost none of the empirical studies have investigated the effects of using different frames on

international’s emotions judgments, or opinions. Quite recently Brantner, Lobinger, & Wetzstein (2011) conducted an experimental study related to this topic that will be reviewed in the next section, in which they focus on a comparative approach between perceiving Palestinians and Israelis in different events. However, going back to the question attached to the Vietnam example above, this study has another dimension and aims to investigating the effect of presenting the same side of the conflict, focusing on Gaza war in 2014, using two photographs with two opposite frames, in order to answer the following research question:

Which of the two visual frames (Human-interest and Militant) is stronger in influencing the opinions and emotions toward Palestinians internationally?

Theoretical framework

This part of the paper will be divided into four different sections; it will start with an overview of the foreign news effect, followed by the theory of framing where I will go in depth into the concept of visual framing. After that, I will examine visual framing effects on public opinion in times of conflict. Finally, I will define the two frames that are going to be used in the experimental study.

Foreign News

(6)

(Besova & Cooley, 2009). However, for national news other channels can possibly be used such as interpersonal communication (Robinson & Levy, 1986). This is much less the case with foreign news, where the only way for people to know about conflicts that happen overseas is through the media (Wu, 2003). McNelly and Izcaray (1986) found a strong relationship between the way the country is exposed to foreign news and the public opinion formed about it. This finding is not surprising and has been supported by many theorists and scholars (e.g., Semetko, Brzinski, Weaver, & Willnat, 1992; Wanta, Golan, & Lee, 2004). In other words, what gives people an opinion about a place they do not live in is the “mental images” they create out from the media (Lippmann, 1922). This leads us to the obtrusiveness of the issue (Walgrave & Van Aelst, 2006), which can give an explanation of how foreign news can be influential. When the topic in the news is related to a distant event from the public and does not directly affect them, it is called an unobtrusive issue (Yagade & Dozier, 1990). Research suggests that media coverage of such issues, can have greater influence on public opinion than the coverage of obtrusive issues, which people have direct personal experience with (Walgrave & Van Aelst, 2006; Yagade & Dozier, 1990).

One of the main reasons why foreign news is important nowadays is that the world is becoming more globalized (Besova & Cooley, 2009). Consequently, the average citizen is becoming more concerned about what happens in foreign countries. Many studies (e.g., McNelly & Izcaray, 1986; Wanta et al., 2004; Wu, 2003) have researched the coverage of foreign news and their impact on the audience. The findings of these studies demonstrate different effects on people’s attitudes towards other countries (Semetko et al., 1992), agenda setting effects (Wanta et al., 2004), effects on foreign policy (Strobel, 1997), and educational effects: educating the public about the world (Wanta et al., 2004; Wu, 2003).

Moreover, foreign audiences have a special interest in international news that relate to wars and conflicts, specifically when the event deals with matters of life and death in extreme

(7)

conflicts (Griffin, 2010). They capture the public’s attention due to the life-threatening element (Messaris & Abraham, 2001) and the high level of drama involved in it (Schill, 2012). This emotional identification attached to the events is essential in developing the public’s point of view towards those events (Rodriguez & Dimitrova, 2011). It makes them think about the conflict and the sides fighting in it, allowing them to make a decision on whom to sympathize with (Sheafer & Dvir-Gvirsman, 2010). As a result, through foreign news, audiences construct a reality of their own about the events (Lippmann, 1922). They translate the media messages they perceive, which eventually forms the public opinion (Besova & Cooley, 2009; Lippmann, 1922).

Furthermore, media make the choice of what to inform the audiences about, which is known as first level agenda setting (McCombs & Shaw, 1972). However, Kiousis &

McComb (2004) argue that media do not reflect reality as is. Instead they shape the agenda and tell the audience how to think about it. This is what they called attribute agenda or second-level agenda setting, which some scholars argue has similarities with framing, in the sense that both are concerned with how issues or events are depicted in media (Weaver, 2007).

Framing  and  “Misframing”  of  News  Photographs    

The importance of foreign news in affecting the way audiences take sides in conflicts makes the close investigation of its effects vital. To do so, I am using the framing theory as the foundation of this study. It will help us to understand how visuals in foreign news affect the way the international audience perceive Palestinians.

Framing is about the construction and presentation of a fact from a particular aspect to promote a certain interpretation (Reese, 2001; Schwalbe, 2006). There are different

(8)

help journalists organize the information they have about certain events effectively for audiences. However, others see it as a process that includes making a side of the event more salient than the other (Scheufele, 1999). In this paper, I will use a more comprehensive definition of framing suggested by Entman (2003): “Framing is selecting and highlighting some facets of events or issues, and making connections among them so as to promote a particular interpretation, evaluation, and/or solution” (p. 417). Entman (1993) also argues that frames call the audience’s attention to a certain aspect of reality, which leads the audiences to have different reactions.

However, what the news reflects does not necessarily represent what the journalist intended to deliver. This happens when the media message is “misframed”. Misframing is a new concept that was introduced for the first time in communication literature in 2004 by the philosopher Nancer Fraser (Ginsburg, 2009). It is about the message being framed in a way that diverges from the creator’s intentions. According to Fraser (2004), this happens as a consequence of globalization, and other scientists explain this misinterpretation with the notion of cultural relativism: “that human perception is culturally influenced” (Segall, Campbell, & Herskovits, 1966, p. 1). For this reason, it is important to know what the photographer’s perception is about the photograph and its effects on a foreign audience. Visual Framing

News organizations use different tools to construct issues or events and present it to their audiences (Rodriguez & Dimitrova, 2011), through which they shape people’s reasoning about different issues (Nelson, Clawson, & Oxley, 1997).

The framing of a photograph can be more effective than verbal framing (Coleman, 2006; Entman, 1993; Scheufele, 1999). The accessibility theory, according to Gibson & Zillmann (2000), can best explain the effects of photos; when a decision needs to be made, the mind retrieves photos out of the memory more quickly than text (Entman, 1993; Nelson

(9)

et al., 1997; Rodriguez & Dimitrova, 2011). This happens because audiences process visuals faster than they do texts (Schill, 2012).

There are several explanations why visuals are more influential than any other format of news; presenting the information in photographs makes the events more realistic and touching (Graber, 1988). In addition to that, photographs are seen as credible evidences of the events (Pfau et al., 2006) especially because audiences believe in what they see more than in what they hear or read (Schweiger & Adami, 1999). They rely on the photograph’s content in forming short-term and long-term impressions (Zillmann, Gibson, & Sargent, 1999). Another reason why photographs are more influential is their closeness to reality and their power in creating strong emotional and instant cues (Rodriguez & Dimitrova, 2011). Fahmy & Wanta (2007) concluded in their study that media do not only report the events to their audiences, instead they report visuals that become an important part of the event itself. For these reasons, photographs should be used carefully to avoid the gross misperceptions of the issues it represents (Zillmann et al., 1999), especially since the readers highly depend on photographs to form impressions about what happens around them (Zillmann, Knobloch, & Yu, 2001).

For the aforementioned reasons, this study will investigate the effects of photographs solely, especially that there is an empirical evidence that it is difficult for people to remember the verbal messages that are accompanied with the photos (Drew & Grimes, 1987).

Visual Framing in Times of Conflict: A Literature Review

Framing photographs during times of war can be even more influential. Shah, Watts, Domke & Fan (2002) argue that by selecting aspects of war and ignoring others, the media constrains the reader’s interpretation. Fahmy & Wanta (2007, p. 20) added: “At times of war, visuals become an effective tool for creating persuasion and gaining public support for government,

(10)

national security, and military actions.”

Many visual framing studies (e.g., Dimitrova & Strömbäck, 2005; Fahmy & Kim, 2008; Pfau et al., 2006; Schwalbe et al., 2008) took a content analysis approach, particularly those that studied the framing of American wars like the wars in Vietnam, Iraq and

Afghanistan. The findings of these studies overlap in the sense that the conflict frame (which is sometimes called the military frame) and the Human-interest frame were the most

dominant in the newspapers.

The literature of framing studies that were relevant to the “Israeli-Palestinian conflict” is scarce. Fahmy & Eakin (2014) mentioned in their study that there is a deficiency in

examining media frames within this conflict. Instead, some studies take the content analysis comparative approach to investigate the unbalanced representation of both sides: the Israeli and the Palestinian, in international newspapers (e.g., Deprez & Raeymaeckers, 2010). Quite recently, Brantner et al.,(2011) conducted a comparative study in which they used an

experiment to test the effects of two Palestinian and Israeli photographs on reader’s emotional response, evaluation of communicative quality, journalistic credibility and objectivity. The study used two frames; the Human-interest and the political frame, and concluded that visual Human-interest framing elicited stronger emotional responses, higher values concerning the communicative quality, and had an impact on the perceived actor representation. However, this study did not look at the directional effects of the frames, nor at what kind of feelings the Human-interest frame elects for instance. Instead, the comparison focused on two visuals vs. text with no visuals. Additionally Brantner et al. (2011) did not look at the effects of the frames on the respondents opinions, but rather on the journalistic values of the stimuli.

(11)

2011; Carpenter, 2007; Dimitrova & Strömbäck, 2005; Schwalbe et al., 2008), and taking into consideration that foreign news is often visually framed through photographs showing disaster and violence (Greenwood & Jenkins, 2013), this study will focus on two war frames: Human-interest frame and Militant frame. These two frames were chosen because they are both the most prominent in framing studies on wars (Carpenter, 2007; Schwalbe et al., 2008). This study will investigate the effects of the two frames in news photographs from Gaza war between Palestinians in Gaza and Israel. The war took place between the 7th of July and 26th of August 2014, resulting in the death of over 2,200 Palestinians and 72 Israelis.

Frames Definitions: Human-interest vs. Militant Frame

There are various definitions to the frames that were chosen. The Human-interest frame usually refers to personalizing, dramatizing or emotionalizing the news (Semetko &

Valkenburg, 2000). In this study, the definitions will be derived from the study of Schwalbe et al. (2008): in this frame, civilians are depicted as victims of war/conflict and focus on the emotional presentation of people. In their study, this frame is composed of photos of Iraqi citizens, journalists, or the reactions of U.S. citizens. In this study, the Human-interest frame reflects Palestinians as victims of the conflict. For example, a mother crying about the killing of her son, or a school student being arrested, etc. The other frame in this study is equal to what Schwalbe et al. (2008) called “the conflict frame”, in which war machines, troops, weapons, and soldiers are pictured. In this study, the frame is named “Militant frame”, and it depicts Palestinians as fighters. Examples for this frame are a Palestinian holding a machine gun, or civilian burning tires.

Visual Framing Effects

As mentioned earlier, visual frames do not only highlight a certain aspect of an event, but they play an important role in affecting the receiver’s emotions (Brantner et al., 2011),

(12)

behaviors (Chong and Drukman, 2007), beliefs and opinions (Slothuus, 2008). However, the effect depends on the type of frame and the receiver (Slothuus, 2008).

Empirical studies found stronger emotional effects of the Human-interest frame compared to the non-human interest frames (Brantner et al., 2011). Cho and Gower(2006) found that the Human-interest frame resulted in a strong impact on participants’ emotional response. According to Konstantinidou (2008) photographing victims with this frame, generally results in an emotional connection with the victim, evoking empathy for the

suffering. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence of the Human-interest effect on war photos. Therefore, I will build on the aforementioned studies to investigate the effect of the Human-interest frame on public emotions and opinions in comparison to the Militant frame. Therefore, the following hypothesis is formulated:

Hypothesis 1: The photograph with Human-interest frame will lead to more positive emotions about Palestinians than the Militant frame.

Scholars have found evidence of framing effects on people’s judgments/opinions and attitudes (e.g., Brantner et al., 2011; Sheafer & Dvir-Gvirsman, 2010). According to Griffin (2010) the photographs of the Vietnam War, which can be categorized as a conflict frame, affected the American people’s opinions about the war negatively. I expect the same effect when using similar photographs from the Israeli-Palestinian conflict, hence, the second hypothesis is:

Hypothesis 2: The photograph with a Militant frame will lead to more negative opinions about Palestinians than the Human-interest frame.

According to Geise & Baden (2014), to decode a photograph, the recipients use their intuitive interpretation and their knowledge. But scholars’ opinions about the knowledge effect on people’s opinion vary. For example, Krosnick and Brannon (1993) argue that the

(13)

more knowledge the recipients have about politics, the more quickly they can act on what they receive and accordingly form opinions. This argument was also confirmed by Lecheler & de Vreese (2012), who tested political knowledge as a moderator between frames and opinions. On the other hand, Kinder and Sanders (1990) found that media frames generally have greater effect on people with less knowledge about the topic. The explanation of this finding might be that people with less knowledge lack counter-arguments, hence they believe in what they see or read. For this reason, this study will also try to answer the following research question:

RQ1: In which way does the level of knowledge about the conflict moderate the relationship between the type of frame and the opinions?

H1

H2

RQ +/-

Figure 1. This conceptual model shows the frames effects on international’s opinions and

emotions.

Method

In this section, I will introduce the data employed in the study, as well as how data was collected, followed by a description of how the independent, dependent and control variables were measured.

Sample

In December 2014, 216 people were recruited to participate in this study within 14 days. The

Type of frame Human-interest Militant Opinions Knowledge about IP conflict Emotions

(14)

recruitment process focused on international organizations, aiming for a diverse sample from different countries and beliefs. For this reason, the participants were recruited from: 1) International media organization in Hilversum: Radio Netherlands Worldwide, 2)

International computer company in Amsterdam: AerData, 3) Facebook groups (See appendix B), 4) Networks of my international friends. To avoid biased responses, the recruitment on social media was done through non-Palestinian or Israeli people, who forwarded the experiment link with the poster (see appendix C) to their network and posted it in different Facebook groups. From the recruited population, 70 percent (n=151) fully completed the experiment, which made the drop rate 30 percent, 22 percent of the total respondents (n=47) were excluded from the study for the following reasons: 1) respondents who dropped out before getting assigned to an experimental condition (24 respondents) 2) those who were assigned but did not fully answer the questions related to the experiment manipulation (21 respondents). 3) Respondents who are from Palestine or Israel (2 respondents). Therefore, the sample consisted of 169 respondents (37 percent men and 63 percent women), with an

average age of 31 years of age (SD= 9.46). The respondents were from 36 different countries from the Americas, Asia, Australia and Europe, with various educational levels between “high school and lower (n=7) to post-graduate university degrees (n=77)”, 71 percent of the respondents have university degrees. Remarkably, 59 percent of the respondents indicated having no religion, while 29.5 percent were from different Christian religious groups and 8 percent were Hindus. The sample included very few Muslims 3 percent and no Jews.

Design and Procedure

The study used a quantitative research design and administered an online survey experiment, to measure the effect of two visual frames on the emotions and opinions of international public about Palestinians. Using a between-subjects experimental design, participants were randomly exposed to one of the three different conditions: (1) Human-interest condition, (2)

(15)

Militant condition, (3) Control group (no photograph). Less people were assigned to the control group as shown in table 1, because the study aimed at comparing the effects of the two photographs. The control group was created to look for further differences between those who received a photograph and those who did not. The gender distribution among the three groups was nearly equal (see table 1). A randomization check of the sample showed that it was successfully done: the participants’ mean age, gender and religion do not significantly differ between the groups.

Table 1

The Respondents Gender Distribution on the Three Groups in Percentages and Counts (N= 169)

Note. 7.2% (n=12) of the respondents did not indicate their gender.

At the beginning of the experiment, participants completed a (pre-test) questionnaire asking them about their foreign news interest, news habits, and interest in the Israeli-

Palestinian conflict (see appendix D for the full questionnaire). In order to make sure that respondents do not reveal the topic of the experiment, they were also asked about other foreign news: Ebola, Ukraine conflict, and Global warming. After that, the respondents were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions. In the experimental conditions, one

photograph was shown on the screen for at least 5 seconds. After that, they were asked to answer a manipulation question to make sure they saw the photograph. An example of the

Condition Participants Gender

Female (n= 99) Male (n=58) Experimental condition 1: Human-interest frame 36.7% (n = 62) 59.6% (n= 37) 37% (n= 23) Experimental condition 2: Militant frame 36.1% (n= 61) 59% (n= 36) 27.8% (n=17) Control group 27.2% (n = 46) 56.6% (n = 26) 39.1% (n = 18)

(16)

manipulation question is as follows: “The girl in the photograph is holding …” and they were given multiple answer choices. 93.5 percent of the respondents who were exposed to the Militant frame photograph answered the manipulation question correctly, while 98 percent of the respondents of the Human-interest question gave the right answer. Following that in the post-test, the participants were asked to complete a series of discrete and dimensional self-report questions measuring: 1) their opinions about the person in the photograph, 2) their emotions, 3) their general opinion about Palestinians and Israelis, 4) and their knowledge about the Israeli-Palestinian conflict. The knowledge questions were asked after the manipulation to avoid priming the participants about the topic. The respondents who were assigned to the control group received only general opinion questions and the knowledge set of questions.

Stimulus Material

The visual stimuli were chosen following the definitions of the two frames used in this study. The selection process was as follows: First, it started with an advanced search of photographs from the last Gaza war between the 7th of July 2014 and the 26th of August 2014. This event was selected because it was the most recent big event in relation to the Israeli- Palestinian conflict, which gathered the attention of most of the International media. Hence, the possibility that people were exposed to similar photographs is higher, which increases the validity of this study.

To find the suitable photographs, I used four online image suppliers: 1) ANP photo: The Dutch news agency, 2) AP images: the Associated press photographs library, 3)

Reporters agency: a platform for photographers to sell their news and artistic photos, 4) Several Palestinian photographers’ pages and websites. After this advanced search, 10 photographs were downloaded. Photographs that can cause confusion due to the

(17)

representation of several objects were excluded, and only portrait photographs were taken to the next step to more adequately capture the effects. The remaining 6 photographs (see

appendix E) were tested in a pilot study, in which a group of international university students, computer engineers, and researchers participated (N=19). Each of them, randomly received three of the six photographs, and was asked to answer a few questions to measure the effect of the photographs on their emotions and opinions. To measure the participants’ emotional response, the following terms were used: “anger”, “sympathy”, “pride”, “inspiration”, and “disgust”. For measuring their opinion, the following terms were used: “terrorist”, “victim”, “hero”, “criminal”, “Militant”, “Nationalist”, “violent”, “peaceful”. Both sets of measures used a 7 points scale (1 = not at all, and 7 = very much). The emotions questions gave the respondents the possibility to add any feelings they had when they saw each of the photos that were not on the answer list. Finally, the pilot responses were analyzed manually and the means from both the negative emotions and descriptions were summed1. The photograph with the highest negative emotions (M=4.15, SD=1.48) and descriptions (M=4.13, SD= 2.100) was used to represent the Militant frame, while the photograph that was found the most positive on emotions (M= 4.00, SD = 1.27) and on descriptions (M=6.22, SD= .972) was used to represent the Human-interest frame in the experiment. Worth mentioning is that after I excluded the photographs that had lower scores, three photographs remained; two were for Palestinian children, and one for a funeral of a Palestinian male. Therefore, to make the comparison possible, I picked the photographs that had children almost in the same age (see figure 2) but with different settings and frames (see appendix F for photographs context and descriptions). One is depicted as a victim of the conflict (for the Human-interest frame), and the second is holing a weapon (for the Militant frame). This will make the comparison                                                                                                                

1 Since the pilot study recruited 19 respondents only, no factor analysis were conducted on

the items, instead each positive items and negative items were summed and computed creating four items measuring: positive emotions, negative emotions, positive opinions, negative opinions.

(18)

between the two frames in the photographs possible.

Human-interest frame photograph Militant frame photograph

Figure 2. The photographs used in the experiment.

Measures

Type of frame: Militant vs. Human-interest. The type of frame is the independent variable in this study. Two photographs were used in the experimental conditions. To measure the effect of the frames, the participants were asked to look carefully at one of the photographs, and answer several questions about it.

Emotions. To measure the respondents’ emotions, the participants were asked to self-report how they felt when they first saw the photograph. This question was measured using the terms in table 2 that were derived from an earlier visual study about the emotional response toward political visual messages (Bucy, 2003). The items were measured using a 7 points scale, where 1= not at all, and 7= very much.

To create a new variable measuring emotions, a principle component analysis (PCA) was conducted on the 6 items with orthogonal rotation (Varimax). The analysis shows that two components have an eigenvalue above 1, and the scree plot confirmed that with two clear points of inflexion. Three items loaded under each of the two factors and all the items have strong positive associations with the factor it loaded under (see table 2). The first factor

(19)

represents positive emotions (α= .56) while the second factor represents negative emotions

(α= .58). Although the two factors resulted in a weak reliability of the scales, they were still

accepted and used in this study, because they loaded moderately to strongly with their factors. Additionally, they were derived and adapted from a reliable scale from a previous study: (Bucy, 2003). The items which loaded under the first factor were computed and formed a new dependent variable measuring positive emotions (M= 2.72, SD= 1.28) , whereas the items which loaded under the second factor were computed forming another dependent variable measuring negative emotions (M= 4.53, SD= 1.41)(see table 2) .

Table 2

Descriptive Statistics and Exploratory Factor Analysis With Varimax Rotation of Emotions (N= 123)

Items

Descriptive Rotated Factor Loadings

M SD Positive emotions Negative emotions

Angry 4.41 2.03 .80 Sympathetic 4.31 2.18 .68 Proud 1.64 1.23 .73 Inspired 2.24 1.68 .81 Disgusted 3.39 2.13 .66 Sad 5.81 1.60 .71 Eigenvalues % of variance α 1.75 28.82 .56 1.60 27.27 .58 Note. Factor loadings < .40 were not shown.

Opinions about Palestinians. To measure the respondent’s opinions about

Palestinians, the participants were asked two questions. In the first question, the respondents were asked to self-report what they think of the child. This variable was measured using the

(20)

following terms: “terrorist”, “victim”, “hero”, “criminal”, “Militant”, “nationalist”, “violent”, “peaceful”, and “brave” (see table 3 for descriptive statistics). The items were also measured with 7 points scales where 1= not at all, and 7= very much.

A new variable was formed from the nine items using principle component analysis with orthogonal rotation. The analysis resulted in two components with eigenvalues above 1. To improve the reliability of the scales, the analysis recommended deleting two items: “victim” (M=6.30, SD= 1.27) and “Peaceful” (M= 3.54, SD= 2.21). The factor analysis was conducted again with the remaining seven items. Five of the items loaded under the first component and two items loaded under the second. All the items correlate strongly and positively with the factor in which it loaded (see table 3). The first factor represented the negative opinions about the child, whereas the items that loaded under the second factor represented positive opinions.

Table 3

Descriptive Statistics and Exploratory Factor Analysis With Varimax Rotation of Opinions (N= 123)

Items

Rotated Factor Loadings M SD Perceiving the child

negatively

Perceiving the child positively Terrorist 1.67 1.27 .79 Hero 2.31 1.68 .76 Criminal 1.67 1.24 .72 Militant 2.33 1.84 .82 Nationalist 2.67 2.00 .80 Violent 2.20 1.65 .83 Brave 3.50 2.07 .78 Eigenvalues % of variance α 3.44 42.01 .85 1.68 22.11 .63 Notes. Factor loadings less than .40 were not shown.

The reliability of the scale of the first factor was strong (α = .85), while the reliability of the second factor was moderate (α = .63). The items of the first factor that was named

(21)

perceiving the child negatively were computed and formed a new variable measuring

negative opinions (M= 2.10, SD=1.30), and the two items which loaded under the second factor named perceiving the child positively were also calculated, forming a new variable measuring positive opinions (M= 2.90, SD= 1.61).

While the question above measured the opinion of the participants about the children in the photographs, two other questions were asked to measure their opinions about

Palestinians. One of these questions was asked to measure their level of agreement with 6 statements derived and adapted from an earlier study that focused on visuals’ effects on respondents’ attitude toward Palestinians and Israelis (Brantner et al., 2011). 7 point Likert scales were used to measure how strongly the participant agreed or disagreed with each statement. Three of the statements were biased to the Israeli side, for example: “The Israeli

military attacks on Palestinians are justified” (M= 2.88, SD= 1.63) and 3 were biased to the

Palestinian side, for example: “Palestinians have the right to defend themselves in every

possible way”(M= 3.90, SD= 1.67) (see appendix E for the full wordings). A principle

component analysis (PCA) was conducted on the six items and resulted in two components with eigenvalues above 1: (factor 1= 2.2, factor 2 = 1.08) with three items loaded under each of them. The first factor was named Palestine attitudes” (α = .53) and the second “Pro-Israel attitude” (α= .57). However, the reliability of the scales of the two factors was weak, hence, the measure’s of both components were not used. 2

The second question measured the respondents’ attitudes towards Palestinians and Israelis in general. This question “How do you feel about: 1) Palestinians (M= 4.39, SD= 1.09), 2) Israelis (M=3.95, SD= 1.23). The respondents were asked to report their feelings toward each from 1-7, ranging from strongly dislike to strongly like. Additionally, a semantic                                                                                                                

2 The two scales were still tested as dependent variables with the two frames, but resulted in

(22)

differential 7 points scale was used to measure if the participants consider themselves “Pro- Palestine=1” or “Pro- Israel= 7”, (M=2.84, SD= 8.08).

Knowledge about the Israeli-Palestinian conflict. The participants’ level of

knowledge about the Israeli- Palestinian conflict was measured with six knowledge questions on a nominal scale. The questions were with different levels of difficulty; some were related to recent news, and some to historical events. To provide further insight into how this was measured, here is an example of one of the questions used: “The Israeli- Palestinian conflict dates back to the year” the answers options were: 1) 1887, 2) 2000, 3) 1973, 4) 1948, 5) Can’t say. The six questions (see appendix E) formed the moderator variable of this study and the following steps were taken to use this variable in analysis: 1) the questions were recoded into a different variable and the correct answer of each question was coded as 1 and a wrong answer was coded as 0, 2) the six questions were computed and one new variable was created. The new variable was measured through a 6 points scale (M= 3.15, SD= 1.86). Only 9 percent of the respondents answered all the questions correctly and over 11 percent of the participants failed to answer any of the questions correctly.

Results

As explained in the methods section, the principle component analysis resulted in two variables for measuring each of the dependents. Hence to test the two frames’ effects on the respondents’ emotions (H1) and opinions (H2), I used the Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA). The reason behind using MANOVA is to enable testing the effect of the frames on the composite of the two dependent variables (Field, 2009) and to be able to compare the two frames’ effects on each of the dependent variables. All the analyses were carried out using the IBM SPSS software.

(23)

more positive emotions toward Palestinians than the Militant frame. To test this hypothesis, the positive and the negative emotions variables were used as dependent variables in the MANOVA. The Box’s M value of 1.12 was associated with a p value of .776, F (3, 2658137) = .37, which is interpreted as non-significant, thus the null hypothesis that observed

covariance matrices of the dependent variables are equal across the frames was accepted, meaning that the robustness of our MANOVA test is guaranteed (Rimarcik, 2007). The Pillai’s trace analysis showed a significant effect of the frame on the respondents’ positive and negative emotions, V = 0.10, F (2, 120) = 6.83, p < .01.

Moreover, a separate univariate ANOVA on the outcome variables, using Pillai’s trace, showed significant frame effects again on the positive emotions, F (1, 121) = 12.46, p < .001. However, this analysis showed a non-significant frame effect on the negative

emotions, F (1, 121) = 1.35, p > .05.

The Levene’s test of equality of variances assumes that error variance of the dependent variables is equal across the frames. This assumption was accepted for both the negative emotions (p > .05), and the positive emotions (p > .05). This means that the ANOVA applied above is valid and the homogeneity of variances is guaranteed (Field, 2009).

Looking at the mean differences of the two frames in relation to the positive and negative emotions variables, we can see that the Human-interest frame photograph resulted in more positive emotions than those exposed to the photograph with the Militant frame. On the other hand, participants who saw the photograph with the Human-interest frame had slightly more negative emotions than those who were exposed to the Militant frame. However since hypothesis 1 is investigating the effect of the Human-interest frame on positive emotions solely, the effect of the frames on negative emotions were ignored. Therefore the significant

(24)

results of the frames on positive emotions, in addition to the significant difference between the two frames on it and based on the mean difference between the Human-interest frame and the Militant frame (see figure 3). The Human-interest frame lead to more positive emotions than the Militant frame, therefore hypothesis 1 is fully supported.

Figure 3. The Means of the Positive and Negative emotions in the Two Frames.

To test whether the Militant frame would elicit more negative opinions towards Palestinians than the Human-interest frame (Hypothesis 2), the same analyses as in

hypothesis one were conducted. The two variables “Perceiving Palestinians positively” and “perceiving Palestinians negatively” were used as dependent variables in the MANOVA. The Box’s M value of 11.21 with a significant p value of .012, F (3, 2658137)= 3.67, caused the null hypothesis of equality of covariance matrices of the dependent variables to be rejected. But we can ignore this figure because the number of participants of each framing group is equal (Rimarcik, 2007).

Using Pillai’s trace, the MANOVA showed a significant effect of the frame on the composite of the two variables regarding opinions, V = 0.20, F (2, 120) = 15.06, p < .001.

3.12    (SD  =  1.25)   4.39   (SD  =  1.45)   2.33     (SD  =  1.20)   4.69   (SD  =  1.37)  

Positive  emotions   Negative  emotions   2   3   4   5   M ean of p ar ti ci p an ts e moti on s

(25)

The separate univariate ANOVA showed a significant effect of the type of frames on

perceiving the child negatively F (1, 121) = 28.67, p < .001. However, this analysis showed

that the frame had a non-significant effect on perceiving Palestinians positively, F (1, 121) = .25, p > .05. The Levene’s test had a significant difference for the negative opinions (p < .05), however the positive opinions had a non-significant one (p > .05). Thus, the null hypothesis of equality of the independent variables’ error variance across the groups, is violated for negative opinions.

On average, participants who were exposed to the Militant frame photograph had more negative opinions (M = 2.68, SE = 0.15), than those from the Human-interest frame (M = 1.55, SE = 0.15). However, there was no difference regarding the positive opinions in the Militant frame (M = 2.98, SE = 0.21), compared to those exposed to the Human-interest frame (M = 2.83, SE = 0.21).

Additionally, a one-way ANOVA was conducted to measure the respondents’ general opinions about Palestinians, (measured with 7 points scale between strongly dislike and strongly like). However the analysis resulted in insignificant differences between the 3 groups: Human-interest frame (M = 4.55, SD = 1.19), Militant frame (M= 4.29, SD = 1.06) and the control group (M= 4.30, SD= 1.00), F (2,161)= 1, p >.05. Therefore, there is no effect of the frame on general opinions about Palestinians 3.

To find out the effect of the frames on the general attitudes respondents have toward the conflict, a one way ANOVA was conducted on the variable “where do you stand from the conflict” which was measured in a semantic differential scale (Pro-Palestine=1, Pro- Israel= 7). However, the frames appeared to have no significant effect on the attitude (F (2,161)= .98, p > .05). On the contrary, the difference between the groups (Militant, Human-interest,                                                                                                                

3 The same analyses were conducted between the two groups with the manipulation leaving

out the control group, however the results did not change.  

(26)

and no photo group) was opposite to the expectations; the Militant frame resulted in a lower mean (M= 1.67, SD= 13.48) compared to the Human-interest frame (M= 3.28, SD= 1.342) and the control group (M= 3.74, SD= .99).

Therefore, hypothesis 2: the Militant frame will lead to more negative opinions about Palestinians, is partially accepted, because the effect of the frames on the respondents’ direct opinion about the child was significant, but, the effect on the general opinions result in insignificant differences between the experiment groups.

Konstantinidou (2008) argued that the Human-interest frame evokes empathy with the victim. For further interesting investigations, two more independent t-tests were conducted; the first was to find the effect of the frames on the respondents description of the children in the photographs as victim. The analysis resulted in significant deference between the frames, in describing the children in the photographs as victims, T (121) = 3.45, p< .01. The Human-interest frame resulted in higher mean (M= 6.68, SD= .76) than the Militant frame (M= 5.92,

SD= 1.55). The second analysis was to investing the effect of the frames on the respondents’

feeling of. On average, participants felt more sympathy for the child in the photograph with the Human-interest frame (M = 4.97, SE=. 25) than for the child in the photograph with Militant frame (M = 3.64, SE = .28). Significant difference was detected in feeling sympathy between the Human-interest frame and the Militant frame T (121) = 3.52, p < .01. The results above confirm Konstantinidou argument in this regard.

A more detailed MANOVA has been conducted to investigate whether the

participants’ knowledge about the Israeli-Palestinian conflict moderates the relation between the type of frame and the opinion of the participants. However, the analysis showed that knowledge does not affect the participants opinions, and does not moderate the

aforementioned relationship, V = 0.02, F (2, 108) = 1.15, p > .05. In addition to that, univariate analysis was conducted on two other dependent variables to test the moderation

(27)

effect of knowledge. However no significant effect was found on: 1) the respondents’ attitude toward the conflict (Pro- Palestine, Pro- Israel), F (2, 151)= .82, p >.05, 2) the respondents’ general opinions about Palestinians F (2, 151) = .08, p > .05.

Worth mentioning that both hypotheses and the research question were also tested using MANCOVA controlling for gender, age, and the participants’ interest in the conflict, but no significant effect was found.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate which of the Palestinians’ photographs have a stronger influence on international audiences’ opinions and emotions toward Palestinians. Building on previous framing studies (Brantner et al., 2011; Carpenter, 2007; Dimitrova & Strömbäck, 2005; Schwalbe et al., 2008) two frames were used: the Human-interest and the Militant frame. It was hypothesized that the Human-interest frame will lead to more positive emotions about Palestinians than the Militant frame (H1) whereas the Militant frame will lead to more negative opinions than the Human-interest frame (H2). Furthermore, the way

knowledge about the conflict moderates the relationship between the frames used and the respondents’ opinions was also explored. The experiment found that the Human-interest frame made people feel more positive about Palestinians, so hypothesis 1 was supported. However, the effect of the frames on the internationals’ opinions about Palestinians was only partially accepted. The respondents who viewed the photograph with the Militant frame, had relatively more negative opinions about the child in the photo, nevertheless, their opinions about Palestinians in general did not differ in comparison with the opinions of respondents who viewed the photo with the Human-interest frame. Furthermore, the study found that knowledge does not affect the relationship between the type of frame and their opinions of Palestinians.

(28)

The findings of this study show that the Human-interest frame influenced the

individual’s emotions more strongly, while the Militant frame was stronger on opinions. The first finding of the experiment regarding the Human-interest frame is in line with the findings of Brantner et al. (2011) and Cho and Gower (2006) that the Human-interest is a strong frame and has an emotional power in affecting peoples’ feelings. However, in this study we can see that the Militant frame also resulted in high negative emotions. The reason for this might be that the subject photographed is a child, and still has a strong human side. The weapon in the photograph might be responsible for the negative emotions but further investigation is needed to state this definitively, creating a need for a future study on this topic.

In line with previous studies (Brantner et al., 2011; Griffin, 2010; Sheafer & Dvir-Gvirsman, 2010), the second finding of the experiment showed that individual opinions about the people in the photograph is affected in the way the subject is framed, resulting in more negative opinions of the child captured in the Militant frame. However the effect stops here, and does not go beyond that to the general opinions/attitudes about Palestinians. The

explanation of this can be summarized in two main points; the study focuses on an ongoing 67 years old conflict and individuals might have a connection to it that can hinder such strong attitudinal changes. The second point is related to the first; people are “particularly sensitive to changes in a positive or negative direction” (Boettcher, 2004, p. 333), and according to Sheafer & Dvir-Gvirsman (2010) the case of the Israeli-Palestinian conflict falls under this sensitivity which makes taking sides in such a conflict a complex decision making process.

Surprisingly, the fact that the two photographs are of children, did not prevent negative effects from the individuals toward the Palestinians, the Militant photograph still reflected negative effects on the individual’ opinions and emotions about Palestinians. Also, it was found that respondents sympathized more with the child in the Human-interest frame, which confirms Konstantinidou (2008) argument that this frame evokes empathy with the

(29)

suffering. If we look closely at the results we can see that to some extent both children in the two photographs were seen as victims. This indicates that visual frames with photographs of children might have special effects on people’s emotions and opinions. This is partially in line with the argument of Höijer (2004) who states that visuals of children, women and old people as victims, move our emotions, adding that children are the most effective in the perspective of compassion. Nevertheless, empirical evidence on this remains scarce, so these findings present aspects of framing that need to be further investigated. For future empirical studies, it would be valuable to test if the visual frames with children have different effects of the perception of the people about the event, than those without children.

On the contrary, of several studies (Kinder & Sanders, 1990; Lecheler & de Vreese, 2012) that found a moderation effect of the knowledge on the relationship between the frame and the opinion, this study found no effect of knowledge. The reason could be that the level of knowledge of the respondents about the conflict was generally low, potentially due to the large geographic distance between respondents and the conflict, and a lack of interest in the conflict.

The two photographs used in this study coincidently happened to be taken by the same Palestinian photographer “Mohammed Saber”, whom I interviewed after conducting the experiment to see what his expectations of the audience’s perception are about Palestinians. The words he mentioned when talking about the Human-interest photo were more or less similar to how it was perceived by the respondents, however, he was reluctant about the Militant photo and said: “the way this child with a weapon is perceived depends on the people and their experience with wars. If they ever experienced wars they would know this is normal and judge us positively” and added: “She is a strong brave child, and reflects the strength of our people”. For Saber, the girl is celebrating victory in Gaza after the war ended, where the Palestinian children were exposed to extreme violence. This indicates signs of

(30)

misframing, the child was not perceived positively as the photographer expects, but he referred to something interesting to be considered as a moderator for future studies, which is the individual’s experience with wars/ conflicts.

One of the interesting stories Saber mentioned during the interview was that: “weapons are trendy among children because of the successive wars on Gaza in the past few years, my 5 year old child wants a tattoo with a Kalashnikov4 on his arm, and in holidays you see most

of the children here playing with fake weapons”. Obviously, the meaning of the Militant photo in the Palestinian context differs from that of the Internationals as the study shows, and this can be due to notions of cultural/political relativism. This confirms what Segall et al. (1966) said about the human perception and how it is influenced by their culture and inspired by Saber I would add that the perception might also be influenced by the person’s history with wars. A recommendation for future research is to replicate this study experimenting the same stimuli and frames on a Palestinian and an Israeli sample, to investigate how those involved in the conflict perceive the frames, in comparison with the findings of this study.

Scholars are not alone in their call for careful use of photographs to avoid the misperception of its meanings (Zillmann et al., 1999). Palestinian politicians in the 1990’s also warned photographers not to photograph children with weapon according to Saber, and those who published such photos used to receive a fine from the Palestinian committee of journalists in Gaza.

Naturally, this study has several limitations; the first is that the sample was very diverse nationality wise, however, it included no Jewish people, hence the effect of the individual’s religious views in such a conflict that is highly associated with religion might have hindered interesting findings when controlling for religion. The second limitation is related to the

                                                                                                               

4 A type of military weapons, and the tattoo in the quote refers to body stickers that are trendy

(31)

stimuli of this study. Although the use of children photographs revealed interesting

indications for future studies, they also might have weakened the frames used, specifically the Militant frame. The last limitation to be addressed is regarding the generalizability of the findings. The convenient sample collected through an online experiment, decreased the external validity of the findings of the study, as it was not a truly random sample.

Despite the limitations, the findings of this study contribute to the limited but growing body of research on the effects of visual framing. This study might be the first of its kind that show how two different frames from the same event and the same side of a conflict can make a difference on the way the subjects of a photograph are perceived. In addition to that, it did not only look at the effects of the frames on emotions and opinions, but also looked closely at the direction of the effect (positive or negative). Technically, the items that were used to measure the opinions resulted in a reliable scale, which can be developed and used for future studies about the Middle East.

Eventually, the media content is becoming more and more visualized especially in such time when sharing a photo is made easy; e.g. Facebook members alone share 350 million photos daily (Crook, 2013). On the other hand, wars and conflicts do not seem about to diminish soon, which gives visual framing studies of wars a higher importance nowadays, and will continue to be of such importance for the foreseeable future.

References

Abraham, L., & Appiah, O. (2006). Framing news stories: The role of visual imagery in priming racial stereotypes. The Howard Journal of Communications, 17(3), 183-203.

Besova, A. A., & Cooley, S. C. (2009). Foreign news and public opinion: Attribute agenda-setting theory revisited. ECQUID NOVI, 30(2), 219-242.

(32)

Boettcher, W. A. (2004). The prospects for prospect theory: An empirical evaluation of international relations applications of framing and loss aversion. Political Psychology,

25(3), 331-362.

Brantner, C., Lobinger, K., & Wetzstein, I. (2011). Effects of visual framing on emotional responses and evaluations of news stories about the gaza conflict 2009. Journalism &

Mass Communication Quarterly, 88(3), 523-540.

Bucy, E. P. (2003). Emotion, presidential communication, and traumatic news processing the world trade center attacks. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 8(4), 76-96.

Carpenter, S. (2007). US elite and non-elite newspapers' portrayal of the iraq war: A comparison of frames and source use. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly,

84(4), 761-776.

Cho, S. H., & Gower, K. K. (2006). Framing effect on the public's response to crisis: Human interest frame and crisis type influencing responsibility and blame. Public Relations

Review, 32(4), 420-422.

Coleman, R. (2006). The effects of visuals on ethical reasoning: What's a photograph worth to journalists making moral decisions? Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly,

83(4), 835-850.

Crook, J. (2013). Snapchat Sees More Daily Photos Than Facebook. TechCrunch. Retrieved 21 January 2015, from http://techcrunch.com/2013/11/19/snapchat-reportedly-sees-more-daily-photos-than-facebook/

(33)

Deprez, A., & Raeymaeckers, K. (2010). Framing the first and second intifada: A

longitudinal quantitative research design applied to the flemish press. European Journal

of Communication, 25(1), 3-23.

Dimitrova, D. V., & Strömbäck, J. (2005). Mission accomplished? framing of the iraq war in the elite newspapers in sweden and the united states. Gazette, 67(5), 399-417.

Drew, D. G., & Grimes, T. (1987). Audio-visual redundancy and TV news recall.

Communication Research, 14(4), 452-461.

Entman, R. M. (1993). Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of

Communication, 43(4), 51-58.

Entman, R. M. (2003). Cascading activation: Contesting the white house's frame after 9/11.

Political Communication, 20(4), 415-432.

Fahmy, S., & Eakin, B. (2014). High drama on the high seas peace versus war journalism framing of an israeli/palestinian-related incident. International Communication Gazette,

76(1), 86-105.

Fahmy, S., & Kim, D. (2008). Picturing the iraq war constructing the image of war in the british and US press. International Communication Gazette, 70(6), 443-462.

Fahmy, S., & Wanta, W. (2007). What visual journalists think others think the perceived impact of news photographs on public opinion formation. Visual Communication

Quarterly, 14(1), 16-31.

(34)

Geise, S., & Baden, C. (2014). Putting the image back into the frame: Modeling the linkage between visual communication and Frame‐Processing theory. Communication Theory,

Gibson, R., & Zillmann, D. (2000). Reading between the photographs: The influence of incidental pictorial information on issue perception. Journalism & Mass Communication

Quarterly, 77(2), 355-366.

Ginsburg, R. (2009). Framing, misframing, and reframing. Civil Organizations and Protest

Movements in Israel: Mobilization Around the Israeli-Palestinian Conflict, , 91.

Gitlin, T. (1980). The whole world is watching: Mass media in the making & unmaking of the

new left Univ of California Press.

Graber, D. A. (1988). Processing the news: How people tame the information tide.

Greenwood, K., & Jenkins, J. (2013). Visual framing of the syrian conflict in news and public affairs magazines. Journalism Studies, (ahead-of-print), 1-21.

Griffin, M. (2010). Media images of war. Media, War & Conflict, 3(1), 7-41.

Höijer, B. (2004). The discourse of global compassion: The audience and media reporting of human suffering. Media, Culture & Society, 26(4), 513-531.

Johnston, A. (1990). Trends in political communication: A selective review of research in the 1980s. New Directions in Political Communication: A Resource Book, 337

Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1990). Mimicking political debate with survey questions: The case of white opinion on affirmative action for blacks. Social Cognition, 8(1), 73-103.

Kiousis, S., & McCombs, M. (2004). Agenda-setting effects and attitude strength political figures during the 1996 presidential election. Communication Research, 31(1), 36-57.

(35)

Konstantinidou, C. (2008). The spectacle of suffering and death: The photographic representation of war in greek newspapers. Visual Communication, 7(2), 143-169.

Krosnick, J. A., & Brannon, L. A. (1993). The impact of the gulf war on the ingredients of presidential evaluations: Multidimensional effects of political involvement. American

Political Science Review, 87(04), 963-975.

Lecheler, S., & de Vreese, C. H. (2012). News framing and public opinion A mediation analysis of framing effects on political attitudes. Journalism & Mass Communication

Quarterly, 89(2), 185-204.

Lippmann, W. (1922). The world outside and the pictures in our heads. Public Opinion, 4, 1-22.

McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. L. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public

Opinion Quarterly, 36(2), 176-187.

McNelly, J. T., & Izcaray, F. (1986). International news exposure and images of nations.

Journalism Quarterly, 63(3), 546-553.

Messaris, P., & Abraham, L. (2001). The role of images in framing news stories. Framing

Public Life, , 215-226.

Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing of a civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. American Political Science Review, , 567-583. Pfau, M., Haigh, M., Fifrick, A., Holl, D., Tedesco, A., Cope, J., . . . Roszkowski, P. (2006).

The effects of print news photographs of the casualties of war. Journalism & Mass

Communication Quarterly, 83(1), 150-168.

Pozgar, G. D., MBA, C., & Pozgar, G. D. (2012). Legal and ethical issues for health

professionals Jones & Bartlett Publishers.

Rare Historical Photos,. (2014). Saigon execution: Murder of a Vietcong by Saigon Police

Chief, 1968. Retrieved 18 January 2015, from

(36)

Reed, C. (2004). Obituary: Eddie Adams. the Guardian. Retrieved 26 January 2015, from http://www.theguardian.com/news/2004/sep/22/guardianobituaries.artsobituaries

Reese, S. D. (2001). Prologue—Framing public life. Framing Public Life: Perspectives on

Media and our Understanding of the Social World, , 7-31.

Rimaric, M. MANOVA. Rimarcik.com. Retrieved 21 January 2015, from http://rimarcik.com/en/navigator/manova.html

Robinson, J. P., & Levy, M. R. (1986). Interpersonal communication and news comprehension. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50(2), 160-175.

Rodriguez, L., & Dimitrova, D. V. (2011). The levels of visual framing. Journal of Visual

Literacy, 30(1), 48-65.

Ross, S. D. (2003). Framing of the palestinian-israeli conflict in thirteen months of new york times editorials surrounding the attack of september 11, 2001. Conflict &

Communication Online, 2(2)

Scheufele, D. A. (1999). Framing as a theory of media effects. Journal of Communication,

49(1), 103-122.

Schill, D. (2012). The visual image and the political image: A review of visual communication research in the field of political communication. Review of

Communication, 12(2), 118-142.

Schwalbe, C. B. (2006). Remembering our shared past: Visually framing the iraq war on US news websites. Journal of Computer‐Mediated Communication, 12(1), 264-289.

Schwalbe, C. B., Silcock, B. W., & Keith, S. (2008). Visual framing of the early weeks of the US-led invasion of iraq: Applying the master war narrative to electronic and print

images. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 52(3), 448-465.

Schweiger, G., & Adami, M. (1999). The nonverbal image of politicians and political parties.

(37)

Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovits, M. J. (1966). The influence of culture on visual

perception Bobbs-Merrill Indianapolis.

Semetko, H. A., Brzinski, J. B., Weaver, D., & Willnat, L. (1992). TV news and US public opinion about foreign countries: The impact of exposure and attention. International

Journal of Public Opinion Research, 4(1), 18-36.

Semetko, H. A., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2000). Framing european politics: A content analysis of press and television news. Journal of Communication, 50(2), 93-109.

Shah, D. V., Watts, M. D., Domke, D., & Fan, D. P. (2002). News framing and cueing of issue regimes: Explaining clinton's public approval in spite of scandal. Public Opinion

Quarterly, 66(3), 339-370.

Sheafer, T., & Dvir-Gvirsman, S. (2010). The spoiler effect: Framing attitudes and expectations toward peace. Journal of Peace Research, 47(2), 205-215.

Slothuus, R. (2008). More than weighting cognitive importance: A Dual‐Process model of issue framing effects. Political Psychology, 29(1), 1-28.

Strobel, W. P. (1997). Late-breaking foreign policy: The news media's influence on peace

operations US Institute of Peace Press.

Walgrave, S., & Van Aelst, P. (2006). The contingency of the mass media's political agenda setting power: Toward a preliminary theory. Journal of Communication, 56(1), 88-109.

Wanta, W., Golan, G., & Lee, C. (2004). Agenda setting and international news: Media influence on public perceptions of foreign nations. Journalism & Mass Communication

Quarterly, 81(2), 364-377.

Weaver, D. H. (2007). Thoughts on agenda setting, framing, and priming. Journal of

Communication, 57(1), 142-147.

Wu, H. D. (2003). Homogeneity around the world? comparing the systemic determinants of international news flow between developed and developing countries. International

(38)

Yagade, A., & Dozier, D. M. (1990). The media agenda-setting effect of concrete versus abstract issues. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 67(1), 3-10.

Zillmann, D., Gibson, R., & Sargent, S. L. (1999). Effects of photographs in news-magazine reports on issue perception. Media Psychology, 1(3), 207-228.

(39)

APPENDICES

Appendix (A): General Nguyen Ngoc Loan executing a Viet Cong prisoner in Saigon

Appendix (B): Facebook groups where the lists where published

Facebook group Number of members

ISN Amsterdam online market 25,500

Expats in the Hague 19,900

Freundschaft Schweiz-Israel 2339 ISN ESN Utrecht online market 426 Utrecht international students 4,140

PuK Forum 1189

Expats Utrecht 4,056

Arts Students League of New York 2,740

(40)

Appendix D: The complete experiment questions in the order it was presented to the respondents:

Introduction: Dear participant,

You are kindly invited to participate in my master's thesis research. Your participation will take no longer than 10 minutes. In a form of a survey, you will be asked about foreign news and the way you perceive it.

The data gathered for this study are for academic purposes only. This means that your answers will remain completely anonymous, and will be used only for scientific research. You can quit the study at any time, for any reason, without any consequences.

Informed consent for participation, please read the terms below before proceeding.

I agree, fully and voluntarily, to participate in this research study. With this, I retain the right to withdraw my consent, without having to give a reason for doing so, I also have up to 24 hours after my participation to cancel my consent. I am aware that I may halt my

participation in the experiment at any time. If my research results are used in scientific publications or are made public in another way, this will be done such a way that my

anonymity is completely safeguarded. My personal data will not be passed on to third parties without my express permission. I am fully aware that I might be exposed to offensive

material, and I know I have the right to withdraw my participation if I feel disturbed. For more information about the research and the invitation to participate, you are welcome to contact me at ruba.mimi@student.uva.nl. Should you have any complaints or comments about the course of the research and the procedures it involves as a consequence of your participation in this research, you can contact the designated member of the Ethics Committee representing ASCoR, at the following address: ASCoR Secretariat, Ethics

Committee, University of Amsterdam, Postbus 15793, 1001 NG Amsterdam; 020‐525 3680; ascor-secr-fmg@uva.nl. Any complaints or comments will be treated in the strictest

confidence.

Thank you for taking part in the study, I greatly appreciate it. Kind regards,

Ruba Mimi

Graduate student, MSc Political Communication University of Amsterdam

PRE-TREATMENT

INSTRUCTIONS: I will ask you short questions about your news habits and interest in politics. Please note, you will not be able to go back within the questionnaire once you have moved to the next page.

Q1) How interested would you say you are in politics?

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Thus, it has been shown that HRM professionals may try to design HRM practices in alignment with the environmental context, the business strategy or already existing

In werklikheid was die kanoniseringsproses veel meer kompleks, ’n lang proses waarin sekere boeke deur Christelike groepe byvoorbeeld in die erediens gelees is, wat daartoe gelei

• You may use results proved in the lecture or in the exercises, unless this makes the question trivial.. When doing so, clearly state the results that

• How is dealt with this issue (change in organizational process, change in information system, extra training, etc.).. • Could the issue have

For aided recall we found the same results, except that for this form of recall audio-only brand exposure was not found to be a significantly stronger determinant than

Vermoedelijk verklaart dit de scheur op de 1 ste verdieping (trekt muurwerk mee omdat de toren niet gefundeerd is dmv versnijdingen). De traptoren is ook aangebouwd aan het

Gezien deze werken gepaard gaan met bodemverstorende activiteiten, werd door het Agentschap Onroerend Erfgoed een archeologische prospectie met ingreep in de

The size and complexity of global commons prevent actors from achieving successful collective action in single, world- spanning, governance systems.. In this chapter, we