• No results found

When the Visuals Speak Louder Than Words : effects of Environmental Visuals on Public Perceptions and Behavioral Change

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "When the Visuals Speak Louder Than Words : effects of Environmental Visuals on Public Perceptions and Behavioral Change"

Copied!
50
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Erasmus Mundus Masters

Journalism, Media and Globalisation

Joint degree

When the Visuals Speak Louder Than Words:

Effects of Environmental Visuals on Public

Perceptions and Behavioral Change

by

Pei Yun Lo

Student number: 11896639 Master’s Thesis

Graduate School of Communication Master’s Programme Communication Science

Supervisor/Examiner: Dr. Andreas Schuck

University of Amsterdam

(2)

Abstract

Engaging the public is considered to be a crucial task for the governments and

pro-environment organizations to tackle pro-environmental problems. Aiming to raise awareness and

encourage behavioral change regarding environmental issues, campaigners are now adopting

a persuasive approach by applying visual communication on social media to attract public’s

attention and promote public engagement. Several studies have been conducted to explore the

public perceptions and engagement around climate change issue by analyzing climate-related

photographic imagery used in mass media. Yet, little research has been done to evaluate the

effects of different types of visual representations on public perceptions and behavioral

change regarding the environmental issues, especially plastic pollution. This study therefore

aims to test, based on an experiment (N=228), whether any differences in effects on public

perceptions and behavioral change are associated with different visual formats including

video, cartoon, infographic and photograph. In line with the previous studies, the results

reveal that the solution-oriented visuals depicting plastic pollution increase perceptions of

self-efficacy, especially if the visuals pair with the clear messages discussing actions to

address plastic pollution. Secondly, the videos are found to be the most effective

communication tools to enhance the perceptions of issue importance. Thirdly, perceived

self-efficacy and issue importance also mediate the relations between the visuals and behavioral

(3)

that the impact-oriented visuals have the potential to cause boomerang effect on perceived

self-efficacy. Through the findings shown in this study, we offer insights into the influence of

visual communication regarding environmental issues on public perception and behavioral

change.

Key words: visual communication, environmental communication, plastic pollution, public

(4)

Introduction

In March, 2018, an upsetting two and a half minutes footage video by a scuba diver

depicting the wave of plastic pollution in Indonesia’s water evoked concern and discussion

over the growing threat of ocean plastic pollution on social media overnight (Roughneen,

2018). Plastic pollution is considered to be the most urgent environmental issue in the 21st

century. As research has shown, nearly 300 million tons of plastics are produced each year

(Wassener, 2011). And rather than being properly recycled, more than 8 million tons of

plastics end up in the oceans which ultimately endanger marine ecology (Wassener, 2011).

Moreover, once plastics are dumped into the oceans, approximately it would take more than

450 years for plastic bottles, for instance, to completely decompose (Evans, 2017). What’s

worse, scientists have found that plastics would slowly break up into microplastics which

make it even harder to clean from the oceans (Carrington, 2018). Lately, a considerable

amount of microplastics have been discovered trapped inside Artic sea ice and would

ultimately be released back to oceans when ice melts due to climate change (Briggs, 2018).

Scientists warms that microplastics have been mistakenly ingested by sea life and would

someday end up in human’s stomach through seafood and even drinking water (Carrington,

2018). Although the risk to people’s health has yet to be proved, plastic pollution has already

become a global environmental concern. As this kind of environmental pollution accelerates

(5)

immediate reactions to tackle the problems. In 2017, Unite Nations Environment declared the

war on plastic pollution and launched the #CleanSeas campaign aiming to urge global actions

against plastic pollution at policy level (UN News, 2017). It is estimated that by 2050 the

number of plastics will exceed the populations of fish in the global oceans if people still

heavily rely on single-used plastic products such as plastic bags and single-used straws (UN

News, 2017).

However, while tackling such environmental issues, pro-environmental campaigners,

especially campaigners from the minority groups, found it difficult to get their voices covered

by mainstream media. As Segerberg (2017) pointed out, advocators such as minority groups

who are trying to bring up environmental issues relevant to local impacted communities into

the national or international level have difficulty attracting main stream media’s attention. It

might be mainly because the environmental issues might not be “newsworthy” enough for

market-driven media to inform as well as entertain the audience. Therefore, to immediately catch the media and people’s attention, visual communication might come in handy for campaigners and marketers. (Gioglio & Walter, 2014). Visual representations have been

considered to be the effective communication tools to covey affective and cognitive

information at the first glance since it is found that human brain can process visual content

60,000 faster than text content (Gioglio & Walter, 2014; Messaris, 1997; Lazard & Atkinson,

(6)

communication “immediate visceral understanding” (Green & Myers, 2010, p.340), and further evoke attitude formation and change in the way text cannot, especially in persuasive

campaign messages (Griffin, 2008; Lazard & Atkinson, 2014). After all, “a picture tells a thousand words” might not be merely an old saying.

The usage of visual representations have been widely used by the governments and

organizations in their pro-environmental campaigns to raise awareness and promote

behavioral change. Sheppard (2005) supported that visualization theoretically affects public

perceptions and behavioral change regarding environmental issues. So far a body of studies

have been conducted to explore the public perceptions and engagement regarding climate

change by analyzing photographic imagery on climate change impact, cause and solution used

in mass media. However, besides photographic imagery, environmental visuals include a wide

range of visual representations, from three-dimensional visualizations to infographics,

cartoons, graphs and videos. (O'Neill & Smith, 2014). It is notable that there is still limited

knowledge built around the usage of different types of visual representations and their

potential to influence public perceptions and encourage behavioral change regarding

environmental issues. Hence, aiming to find out how different types of visual representations

can influence public perceptions and behavioral change through the mediators of perceived

self-efficacy and perceived issue importance, the current study extends from the previous

(7)

four types of visual representations that are commonly presented on social media. This study

particularly focuses on plastic pollution as it is so far the most recent and urgent global crisis

that requires governments and individuals to take immidiate action. Against this backdrop of

visual communication, this study aims to answer the following questions: RQ: How do

different types of visual representations influence public perceptions and behavioral change

regarding environmental issues?

Theoretical Framework

Public perceptions of environmental visuals

Sheppard (2005, p.638) defined public perception as “the process of seeing or otherwise

perceiving phenomena, leading to particular responses or states which include both cognitive

and affective outcomes”. Sheppard further suggested when people perceive environmental

visuals, they mainly go through three mental stages, which can be broadly categorized “as

cognitive (related to knowledge and understanding), affective (related to feelings, attitudes,

and emotions), behavioral (related to changes in behavior of the viewer)” (Sheppard, 2005,

p.638; Zube, Sell & Taylor, 1982). As studies has found, visual representations are powerful

communication tools to covey cognitive and affective information, and could further evoke

attitude and behavioral change (Messaris, 1997; Griffin, 2008; Lazard & Atkinson, 2014). It is

expected that exposing people to various environmental visual representations would help

(8)

develop positive and negative feelings and ultimately act to it.

Based on previous study (Fernandez, Piccolo, Maynard, Wippoo, Meili, &Alani, 2016)

on the investigation of perceptions and behaviors of participants on two climate change

movements from 100 non-visual text-based tweets, Fernandez et al. (2016) suggested that

most of the social media participants were remaining at the desirability stage where people

showed only the desire to act. On the contrary, if the visual-based posts were being evaluated

in the study, the result might be different. Earlier visualization study (Sheppard, 2005)

examing the impact of imagery proved that “the cognitive advantages of visual information

over written or verbal information have been widely documented, for example, when mentally

visualising conditions that cannot be seen directly in the real world” (p.638). To further

understand people perceptions of environmental visuals and their intention to behavioral

change, a wide range of recent studies using qualitative methodologies (e.g. Q-sort) and

quantitative methodologies (e.g. experiments) have been carried out to investigate how people

response to photographic imageries depicting climate change impact, cause and solution. In a

series of papers, O'Neill and Nicholson-Cole (2009) conducted Q-sort method study to

investigate public perceptions of climate change photographic imageries. They found that

extreme impact imagery, especially the ones picturing dramatic weather of climate change

impacts are more captivating for the public and could lead to the increased perceptions of

(9)

which is known as boomerang effect, and possibly demotivate people from taking actions in

response to the risks portrayed (the imagery of impact might decrease the perceptions of

self-efficacy) (O'Neill &Nicholson-Cole, 2009). Later, O'Neill et al. (2013) conducted another

Q-sort study on the same topic but with the participants from the Unite States, Unite Kingdom

and Australian and found the similar pattern of results (O'Neill, Boykoff, Niemeyer & Day,

2013). Furthermore, they also found that images of solutions and actions evoke people’s

willingness to do something about climate change (participants tend to have higher

perceptions of self-efficacy). The recent study by Metag et al. (2016) using the same method

and imageries in a cross-national sample from Germany, Austria and Switzerland also backed

up O'Neill et al.’s results. However, the results from another cross-national paper using mixed

methods and participant samples from the UK, Germany and the US appeared to contradict

the previous studies (Chapman, Corner, Webster & Markowitz, 2016). Chapman et al. (2016)

indicated that images of climate solutions generate positive emotional responses, but they

perform poorly in motivating personal behavioral change. Images of climate change impacts,

on the other hand, provoke negative emotional responses and turn out to be the most

motivating for the audience (Chapman et al., 2016). To further authenticate O'Neill and his colleagues’ Q-sort studies, Hart and Feldman (2016) used experimental methodology to look into the direct and indirect effects imagery and text have on public perceptions and behavioral

(10)

Their results replicated O'Neill et al.’s studies, suggesting that solution-oriented visuals

increase the perceptions of self-efficacy, and further promote behavioral change around

climate change issue through the mediator of perceived self-efficacy, especially if the imagery

coupled with the text discussing actions. However, there is no significant boomerang effect

regarding the images of impact as well as no significant indirect effect on behavioral change

through the mediator of perceived issue importance. Moreover, in contrast to Chapman et al.’s

study (2016), Hart and Feldman (2016) did find that the images of climate solution have the

potential to indirectly increase behavioral change through the mediator of self-efficacy.

Taken together, all the studies suggested that different climate change photographic

imageries depicting impact and solution have a greater association with public perceptions

and further evoke behavioral change. Firstly, images of action and solution generate positive

responses while images of impact generate rather negative responses. Secondly, images of

action and solution lead to greater perceived self-efficacy, and further improve engagement,

especially when the images pair with the text messages discussing actions through the

mediator of perceived self-efficacy. However, the fact that impact-oriented visuals or

pollution-oriented visuals would generate perceived issue importance and promote

engagement, and also would reduce public perceptions of self-efficacy are still in need of

further investigation.

(11)

Visual representations are categorized in several genres such as (1) photograph, (2)

video, (3) infographic, (4) cartoon. Photograph is one of the well-used visual representations

on social media. A number of pro-environmental organizations apply photographs as their

crucial visual communication strategies to present the actual evidence of climate change

impact. Since 1997, the environmental campaign group Greenpeace has been using

comparative photographs to document the melting glaciers caused by climate change (Doyle,

2007). By demonstrating the actual and potential effects of climate change upon the

landscape, photographs “call attention to the problems associated with trying to communicate

the reality of environmental issues that are both temporal and unseen” to skeptical

governments and the disinterested public (Doyle, 2007, p.129). With photographic evidences

of climate change impact, Greenpeace has successfully increased public consciousness

(Doyle, 2007). As mentioned above, photographic imagery has been researched by a wide

range of studies (O'Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009; O'Neill et al., 2013; Metag et al., 2016;

Chapman et al., 2016; Hart & Feldman, 2016). The current study will therefore test whether

the previous results hold true by using experimental method that compares other three types

of visuals, namely video, infographic and cartoon to photographic image on plastic pollution

impact and plastic pollution solution.

H1a: Solution-oriented visuals depicting plastic pollution will generate positive emotional

(12)

negative emotional reactions.

H1b: Solution-oriented photographs depicting plastic pollution will lead to higher

perceptions of self-efficacy.

H1c: Impact-oriented photographs depicting plastic pollution will lead to higher perceptions

of issue importance.

H1d: Impact-oriented photographs depicting plastic pollution will lower perception of

self-efficacy.

In terms of the usage of videos in visual communication, the power of videos cannot be

underestimated. Visual communication experts, Gioglio and Walter (2014) suggested that

videos have the ability to attract immediate attention and resonate with the viewers in a way

other mediums cannot. Facebook CEO Mark Zuckerberg announced in his first 2016

Facebook Live Q & a session that the usage of videos apparently outperforms the other types

of content formats on Facebook (Hutchinson, 2016). According to BuzzSumo, “video content is driving significantly higher shares over time” (Hutchinson, 2016). Moreover, it has been widely discussed for decades that videos have the potential to influence issue awareness and

perceptions of self-efficacy that normally related to behavioral change (Acierno, Rheingold,

Resnick, & Stark-Riemer, 2004; Occa & Suggs, 2016). In previous study on post

victimization assessment and treatment, Acierno et al. (2004) reported that related-knowledge

(13)

and Suggs (2015) reaffirmed in their communicating breast cancer screening study that videos

turn out to be more effective than infographics as they were considered to be more engaging

and clear. Videos are believed to be the useful tools to communicate complicated concepts in

a visual manner and could further provoke behavioral change (Occa & Suggs, 2016;

Ferguson, 2012). The current study hereby expects that videos addressing plastic pollution

would lead to higher perceptions of self-efficacy as well as higher perceptions of issue

importance, and further provoke behavior change.

H2a: Solution-oriented videos depicting plastic pollution will lead to higher perceptions of

self-efficacy

H2b: Impact-oriented videos depicting plastic pollution will lead to higher perceptions of

issue importance

Another well-known visual communication format depicting environmental issue is

infographic. Infographics are believed to be the key methods to present issue to the public in

an understandable way (de Haan, Kruikemeier, Lecheler, Smit & van der Nat, 2017).

Infographics present the combination of visual language (e.g. pictures, texts, graphics and

charts) that could quickly attract people’s attention (Occa & Suggs, 2016). The early reported

application of infographics was found in the healthcare domain. For instance, infographics

were used to promote the usage of sunscreen to reduce skin cancer (Stephenson & Witte,

(14)

studies indicated that infographics could not only catch people’s attention but also simplify

the complex information (Severtson & Henriques, 2009). As climate change and

environmental issue are often seen to be the most complex issues for the public, infographics

are emerging to be the popular communication tools among scientists and pro-environmental

activists to help break down the complex environmental information. Lazard and Atkinson

(2014) suggested that infographics can prompt audience to evaluate pro-environmental

information and further increase audience engagement. Furthermore, a variety of studies

(O'Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009; O'Neill et al. ,2013; Metag et al, 2016; Chapman et al.,

2016; Hart & Feldman, 2016) confirmed that pairing solution-oriented visuals with text

highlighting actions that can be taken to address climate change issue have a greater

association with self-efficacy and further promote engagement in individual and political

spheres. Therefore, it is expected that combining text information and visual components in

an infographic format could result in the same outcomes and even leverage the power of

environmental visual communication. Hereby, the current study proposes the following

hypotheses:

H3a: Infographics could be easily understood and convey the issue of plastic pollution better

than the other three types of visual representations

H3b: Solution-oriented infographics depicting plastic pollution will lead to higher

(15)

H3c: Impact-oriented infographics depicting plastic pollution will lead to higher perceptions

of issue importance

Last but not the least, cartoon, as “one of the best depiction that is hundred times more effective than the description in words” (Ashfaq, 2012), is also investigated in the current study. Since the writer Benjamin Franklin published the first political cartoon in American

since 1750s, cartoons have been commonly used to engage with the viewers with the

humorous messages (Green, 2008). The earlies study used cartoons to demonstrate emergency

department (ED) release instructions to patients and found out that cartoons effectively

convey information and increase the patients’ willingness to comply with the instructions

(Delp & Jones, 1996). Nowadays, cartoons have also been commonly used to portray

international issues in printed newspapers as a source of satirical representation of the reality

of the issues (Ashfaq, 2012). Newspaper cartoonist, Patrick Chappatte, once shared in

TEDGlobal 2010 that the jokes shown in cartoons are the best way to address the serious

issues (Chappatte, 2010). Moreover, cartoons are not just merely used to entertain viewers,

but more often serve as “a force of social and political change” that would point out the

absurdity of reality and challenge the state quo (Giarelli & Tulman, 2003: 947). “Cartoons

reflect what the public finds absurd, worrisome, and desirable without necessarily being able to say why” (Giarelli & Tulman, 2003, p.954). Today, the environmental issues have become the popular subjects among political cartoonists as cartoons are similar to the other visual

(16)

representations in its capacity to represent environmental issues in a more creative and

engaging way (Manzo, 2010). It is believed that cartoons could break down the often complex

information and get the abstract environmental message across (Gioglio & Walter, 2014). “After all,” said John Renard, the organizer of Earthworks 2008, a global cartoon competition, “humor is often a valuable key in the struggle to win hearts and minds” (Green, 2008). In sum, as cartoons provide the entertaining and amusing contents to keep the public engaged

and are often used to appeal to the public, the current study therefore expects that cartoons

have the potential to be the most captivating for the public in comparison with the other three

types of visual representations and could also generate higher perceived issue importance (see

table 1 below for the predicted effects of the conditions on perceived Self-efficacy and issue

importance).

H4a: Cartoons are the most appealing visuals compared to the other three types of visual

representations

H4b: Impact-oriented cartoons depicting plastic pollution will lead to higher issue

importance

Table 1. Predicted Effects of the Eight Conditions on Perceived Self-efficacy and Issue Importance

Experimental condition

Mediators

Self-Efficacy Issue Importance Infographic

Impact +

Solution +

(17)

Impact + Solution Video Impact + Solution + Photograph Impact - + Solution +

Note. + indicates a positive main impact. – indicates a negative impact. Blank spaces indicate no main impact. The main effects are predicted by comparing impact-oriented visuals with solution-oriented visuals.

Perceived self-efficacy and perceived issue importance as mediator

Looking at possible mediation indirect effect, the current study builds from the prior

researches (O'Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009; Hart & Feldman, 2016) to examine how visuals

may indirectly affect individual behavioral intentions through the mediators of perceived

self-efficacy and issue importance. The concept of perceived self-self-efficacy, first coined by

Bandura, are “beliefs in one’s capacity to organize and execute the courses of action required to produce given attainments” (Bandura, 1977, p.3). That is, individuals have high level beliefs in their capacity to do something about environmental problems. The influence of

self-efficacy on behavioral change has been widely documented in the health domain since 1970s.

Previous study (Rimal, 2000) indicated that perceived self-efficacy mediates the relations

between health knowledge and healthful habits. A number of theories in social psychology,

including social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1977) further strengthened the finding that those

who have high degree of self-efficacy are more likely to adopt issue-related behaviors or

(18)

self-efficacy. Later in several environmental studies, researchers have concluded the similar

results. The stronger sense of perceived self-efficacy people believe they have, the higher

possibility they would act effectively in environmentally responsible way (Taylor & Todd,

1995; Chen, 2014). Self-efficacy as the mediator variable is also proved to be the potential

mechanism for enhancing further environmental behaviors, known as spillover effect (Lauren,

Smith, Louis & Dean, 2017). While researches in health domain and environmental domain

mainly discussed the critical role of perceived self-efficacy on individual sphere, several

studies (Rosenstone & Hansen, 2003; Hart & Feldman, 2016) suggested that people would as

well have the tendency to perform behaviors such as signing petition and participating in rally

or protest in political sphere.

Issue important, on the other hand, also plays a vital contributor to people’s

pro-environmental behaviors in political sphere. The effects of issue important on behavioral

change have been prominently discussed in American political domain (Epstein & Segal,

2000). Earlier research on anti-nuclear war activity by Fox and Schofield (1989) suggested

that high perceived issue important significantly links to the increased anti-nuclear behavioral

intention and actual behavior. Later study (Weaver, 1991) further proved that increased

perceptions of issue important are found to have a significant correlation with participating in

political behavior related to the issue such as signing petition and voting. Overall, individuals’

(19)

themselves as highly efficacious as well as people perceive the relevant issue as highly

important.

Extending from the indirect effects from the previous studies, Hart and Feldman (2016)

reported a significant result that the solution-orient as well as text-based information with

action message does increase individuals’ intentions to be more inclined to engage in energy

conservation behaviors and political behaviors through the mediator of perceived

self-efficacy. Hereby, considering the previous discussion on each visual representation, the

current study expects that the solution-oriented visuals, especially infographics, videos and

photographs will have a significant indirect effect on not only individual behavior intention in

reducing the usage of plastic product but also their intention to take up political actions

through the mediator of perceived self-efficacy. As with issue importance, the results from Hart and Feldman’s research showed no significant effects. However, the previous Q-sort study carried out by O'Neill et al. (2013) suggested the impact-oriented images have a

positive association with greater perceived issue importance. Moreover, considering different

types of visuals are used in the current study, the study might lead to different results. Hence,

the current study predicts that all impact-oriented visuals will all have a significant indirect

effect on not only individual behavior intention in reducing the usage of plastic product but

also the intention to take up political action through the mediator of perceived issue

(20)

H5a: Solution-oriented visuals, especially infographics, videos and photographs have a

significant indirect effect on not only individual behavioral intention (i.e. reducing the usage

of plastic products) but also the intention to take up political actions through the mediator of

perceived self-efficacy

H5b: Impact-oriented visuals have a significant indirect effect on not only individual

behavioral intention (i.e. reducing the usage of plastic products) but also the intention to take

up political action through the mediator of perceived issue importance.

Method

A total of 228 participants were recruited using social media network by posting survey

hyperlink, and were invited to participate in a 10 minutes online survey experiment through

Qualtrics Panels. Qualtrics Panels provides opt-in online panels that allow researchers to

recruit more diverse samples (Peifer & Garrett, 2014). Gender, age and educational

background were collected to ensure the diversity of the participants.

Participants had an average age of 28.92 years old (SD=9.58). There were 71.1%

females and 26.3% males. Median education was bachelor’s degree (54.4%), and 33.8% of

participants had master’s degree. Regarding the understanding of plastic pollution and

perceived environmental identity, 33.3% of participants somewhat understand the issue of

(21)

of participants somewhat agree that they are environmental persons and 32% of participants

totally agree. 36% of participants agree that they are the type of person who would be

involved in environmental behaviors, 29.4% of participants somewhat agree. Moreover, the

survey performed randomization on Qualtrics so that each condition would be randomly and

evenly presented to each participant. The randomization was quite successful and eight

conditions were homogeneous regarding age, education and gender (see Table 2 below).

Table 2. Sample characteristics

Age Education Gender

(M) (SD) Bachelor’s Master’s Females Males

Full sample (n=228) 28.92 9.58 54.4% 33.8% 71.1% 26.3% impact-oriented infographics (n=28) 32 13.04 57.14% 35.71% 75% 25% solution-oriented infographics (n=29) 27.79 8.45 51.72% 34.48% 68.97% 31.03% impact-oriented cartoons (n=29) 29.83 10.34 65.52% 17.24% 75.86% 17.24% solution-oriented cartoons (n=30) 28.17 7.16 53.33% 36.67% 70% 30% impact-oriented videos (n=26) 29.46 12.45 50% 34.61% 76.92% 23.08%

(22)

solution-oriented videos (n=25) 27.6 4.79 56% 32% 52% 40% impact-oriented photographs (n=31) 29.70 11.44 61.29% 32.26% 77.42% 19.35% solution-oriented photographs (n=30) 26.83 5.40 40% 46.67% 70% 26.67%

Note. The table only presents the highlighted data. “Rather not answer” in gender, and “less than high school”, “high school”, “doctoral degree” and “professional degree” in education are excluded in the table.

Stimuli

Eight experimental conditions were created by manipulating two different aspects of the

visuals: plastic pollution impact and plastic pollution solution. Four types of visuals, namely

infographic, cartoon, video and photograph were manipulated to focus on the impacts plastic

pollution have on the oceans and marine life and solutions that could be taken to minimize the

ocean plastic pollution. The solution-oriented visuals were chosen that mainly portrayed

beach and shorelines cleaning up. Furthermore, in order to make sure each visual is as

comparable as possible and minimize the possibility of bias that might have on the results, the

chosen visuals not only portrayed the same issue, but also offered the same information. Each

visual would couple with the same text that has already embedded in infographic, explaining

the impact of ocean plastic pollution and the solution that could be taken to address the issue.

(23)

Overall, there were eight conditions (infographics: impact, solution; cartoons: impact,

solution; videos: impact, solution; photographs: impact, solution).

Measurement and Procedure

The current study extended from the previous studies (O’Neil and Nicholson-Cole, 2009; O’Neill et al. ,2013; Metag et al, 2016; Chapman et al., 2016; Hart & Feldman, 2016) of public perceptions on visuals regarding plastic pollution across two variables: perceived issue

importance and perceived self-efficacy. Hence, the measurements used in the current study

for issue importance, self-efficacy and individual and political behavioral change are very

similar to the ones used in the prior studies.

Once consenting to participate in the survey, participants were first requested to answer

demographic questions, and then they were asked questions about their perceived level of

understanding of plastic pollution, and perceived level of environmental identity and

environmental behavior. Kollmuss and Agyeman (2010) defined environmental person and environmental behavior as a person “that consciously seeks to minimize the negative impact of his/her actions on the natural and built world (e.g. reduce plastic production or minimize

resource and energy consumption)” (p. 240). Based on the definition, participants were asked

to answer on a 7-point scale ranged from (1)strongly disagree to (7) strongly agree with the following statements: “I consider myself as an environmental person”(M=5.13,SD=1.26); “To

(24)

engage in environmental behaviors is an important part of who I am”(M=4.90, SD=1.47); “I am type of person who would be involved in environmental behaviors”(M=5.16, SD=1.36).

The scale used showed high reliability (α=.918).

Next step, participants were randomly assigned to view one type of the four visual

materials that portrayed either plastic pollution impact or solution. After exposing to the

visual material, participants were asked to answer the questions related to it and make the

evaluation on their affective level, perceived level of issue important, self-efficacy and

behavior change. To measure the mediator variable of affective level, issue important and

self-efficacy, along with the dependent variable of behavior change, the following measures

were applied.

Affective Level. Participants were ask to rate their affective level from (1) really negative to

(7) really positive on a 7-point scale (α=.562) to the following statement: “How does this visual material make you feel?”(M=3.23, SD=1.62). Likewise, participants were then asked to rate their feeling toward the visual on a 7-point scale from (1) very boring to (2) very

appealing to the following statement: “Do you find this visual material boring or appealing?”

(M=4.68, SD=1.36).

Issue importance. Three 7-point scales were designed based on previous studies (Lazard &

(25)

level of perceptions, willingness and motivation from (1) strongly disagree to (7) strongly

agree to the following statements on the 7-point scales (α=.773): “Does this visual material make you feel the plastic pollution is an important issue?”(M=5.41, SD=1.37); “After viewing the visual material, how motivated do you feel to seek out more information about plastic pollution?” (M=4.24, SD=1.58); “How much more or less willing would you be to share this visual material with friends on social media?” (M=3.94, SD=1.69).

Self-efficacy. Three 7-point scales were designed based on previous studies (Hart & Feldman,

2016; Chapman et al., 2016). Participants were asked to rate their level of agreement from (1)

strongly disagree to (7) strongly agree (α=.450) with the following statements: “The visual

material make me feel that I can do something about plastic pollution (M=4.49, SD=1.71)”, “I feel confident that I could help reducing plastic pollution” (M=4.96, SD=1.66) and “I feel capable of reducing the usage of plastic products”(M=4.84, SD=1.58).

Behavioral Change. Individual behavioral change intentions were measured across two

domains: personal environmental behavior and personal political behavior. First, participants answered the question “does this visual material make you want to change your own behavior to reduce the usage of plastic products?” (M=4.82, SD=1.54) on a 7-point scale (α=.895)

ranged from (1) not at all to (7) very much. Then, participants were provided a list of actions that participants were informed would “minimize their impact on plastic pollution.”

(26)

strongly disagree to (7) strongly agree (α=.884) to each of the actions listed under the

following statement: how likely or unlikely it is that this visual material would trigger you to

engage in the following actions in the next 12 months: (1) bring your own shopping bag

(M=5.65, SD=1.57); (2) stop using straws (M=5.07, SD=1.74); (3) skip plastic produce bags

(M=5.03, SD=1.69); (4) buy/use less disposable products (M=4.95, SD=1.70); (5) recycle

(M=5.62, SD=1.64); (6) clean up beaches and shorelines (M=4.25, SD=1.93).

Personal political behavior were firstly measured by asking participants to answer the

question that whether the visual material would trigger them to support government policies

to tackle plastic pollution from a 7 point scale ranged from (1) much less supportive to (7)

much more supportive (α=.871). Later, as with personal environmental behavior, participants

were asked to rate their level of agreement to a list of actions in the political sphere that

participants could take to tackle the problem of plastic pollution. The statement went “how

likely or unlikely is it that the visual material would trigger you to engage in the following actions in the next 12 months?”. The lists of actions were (1) contact government officials to urge them to take action to ban plastic products (M=3.12, SD=1.75); (2) participate in a rally

or protest in support of action to ban plastic use (M=3.59, SD=1.90); (3) sign a petition in

support of taking action to ban plastic use (M=4.98, SD=1.76); (4) join or volunteer with an

organization working to reduce plastic use (M=4.11, SD=1.85); (5) donate money to an

(27)

Analysis

The current study conducted analysis in two steps. The first steps, for the main effects

analysis, consisted of a series of one-way ANOVA’s to investigate the main effects of

infographics, cartoons, videos and photographs depicting both plastic pollution impact and

plastic pollution solution on affective reactions, perceived self-efficacy, perceived issue

importance and understanding of images. Post Hoc Comparison using the LSD test and

independent sample T-Tests were conducted subsequently to compare the mean scores to test

for between-groups differences. The second step consisted of a mediation analysis to examine

the hypotheses that self-efficacy and issue importance mediate the effect of eight variables on

individual behavior and political behavior. The mediation analysis was conducted by using the

PROCESS macro for SPSS. To test the direct and indirect effects, regression coefficients and

bootstrapped confidence intervals were provided by PROCESS. Overall, 1000 bootstrap

samples were used for bootstrap confidence interval calculations. Indirect effects were only

proved to be significant if both lower 95% confidence intervals (LCI) and upper 95%

confidence intervals (UCI) are either above zero or below zero. Otherwise, there is no

significant indirect effect.

Results

(28)

Regarding how well the each stimuli could covey the issue of plastic pollution, there is a

significantly difference between the eight condition, F(7,220)=2.154, p<.05. Independent

sample T-test revealed that the impact-oriented video (M=5.69, SD=1.26) conveyed the issue

of plastic pollution better than impact-oriented infographic (M=4.50, SD=1.37); t(52)=-3.32,

p=.002 and the solution-oriented video (M=4.80, SD=1.50); t(49)=2.306, p=.025). And

surprisingly, the impact-oriented photograph (M=5.29, SD=1.40) was also considered to

convey the issue of plastic pollution better than impact-oriented infographic (M=4.50,

SD=1.37); t(57)=-2.19, p=.033. Regarding how complicated the different stimuli were

perceived, there is a significant difference at the p<.05 level between the eight conditions,

F(7,220)=2.435, p=.02. Independent sample t-test’s indicated that the infographic (M=5.18,

SD=1.68) was the most complicated to understand and more complicated than the video

(M=6.08, SD=1.41); t(52)=-2.119, p=.039 and photograph (M=6.32, SD=1.14); t(47)=-3.032,

p=.004 in the impact-oriented conditions. The results therefore partially prove H3a that

infographics would covey the issue of plastic pollution better than the other types of visual

representations. However, infographics are not easily understood by the public.

2. Affective responses to visuals

There was a significant influence of the eight conditions on participants’ affective reaction

to the visual materials in the different conditions, F(7,220)=13.933, p<.001. Post-hoc

(29)

a positive emotional reactions while the impact-oriented visual materials generated a rather

negative emotional reactions. To be specific, the solution-oriented infographic (M=4.76,

SD=.912) generated more positive emotional reactions than the impact-oriented infographic

(M=3.54, SD=1.55); t(43)=-3.612, p=.001. The solution-oriented video (M=4.40, SD=1.76)

led to more positive emotional responses than the impact-oriented video (M=2.15, SD=1.26);

t(43)=-5.237, p<.001. The solution-oriented photograph (M=3.13, SD=1.46), as well, was

considered to be more positive than the impact-oriented photograph (M=2.10, SD=1.08);

t(59)=-3170, p=.002. However, there was no statistically significant difference between the

solution-oriented video and the solution-oriented infographic. Overall, as expected, the results

prove H1a that the solution-oriented visual materials generate positive emotional reactions

while the impact-oriented visual materials generate rather negative emotional reactions.

In terms of the question regarding whether the image is boring or appealing, there is a

significant difference between the eight conditions, F(7,220)=2.558, p=.015. Independent

sample T-Test’s indicated that participants found the solution-oriented cartoon (M=5,

SD=1.53); t(58)=2.87, p=.006 and the solution-oriented video (M=5.16, SD=1.41);

t(53)=3.34, p=.002 more appealing than the solution-oriented photograph (M=3.97,

SD=1.25). The results are in line with H4a that cartoons are the most appealing images

compared to the other types of visual representations.

(30)

conditions on self-efficacy and issue importance by conducting one-way ANOVA’s. First

looking at efficacy, there is a large significant difference across conditions regarding

self-efficacy at the p<.05 level, F(7, 220)=2.62, p=.013. The following Post Hoc Comparison

using the LSD test indicated that the mean score for the impact-oriented infographic (M=4.18,

SD=1.66) was significantly different from the solution-oriented infographic (M=5.28,

SD=1.33). The impact-oriented video (M=3.77, SD=1.925), as well, was significantly

different from the solution-oriented video (M=5.20, SD=1.080). Also, it is worth mentioning

that the post hoc test comparing the solution-oriented infographic (M=5.28, SD=1.33) and the

solution-oriented photograph (M=4.43, SD=1.547) approached significance (p=.054). To

further examine the between-groups differences, independent sample T-Test’s were conducted

and further revealed that the solution-oriented image was associated with a greater sense of

self-efficacy than the impact-oriented image no matter the form of visuals. To be specific, the

solution-oriented infographic (M=5.28, SD=1.33) led to higher perceptions of self-efficacy

than the impact-oriented infographic (M=4.18, SD=1.66); t(55)=-2.76, p=.008. The

solution-oriented video (M=5.20, SD=1.08) also led to significantly higher perceptions of self-efficacy

than the impact-oriented video (M=3.77, SD=1.93); t(40)=-3.29, p=.002 (See figure 1 for a

visual representation of the differences). In terms of the difference between different forms of

visuals, independent sample T-Test’s indicated that there is actually a significant difference

(31)

solution-oriented infographic (M=5.28, SD=1.33) led to significantly higher perceptions of

self-efficacy than the solution-oriented photograph (M=4.43, SD=1.55); t(57)=2.24, p=.029. To

sum up, the results prove H2a and H3b, suggesting that the solution-oriented video and the

solution-oriented infographic depicting plastic pollution lead to higher perceptions of

self-efficacy (see figure 1 below for a visual representation of the differences).

Figure 1. Mean levels of perceived self-efficacy for different visuals on plastic pollution solution

Looking at issue importance, there was a significant effect of the eight visual conditions

on issue importance, F(7, 220)=2.398, p=.022. Post-hoc tests using the LSD test indicated that

the mean differences between the impact-oriented video and the impact- oriented infographic

were statistically significant (p=.003), and the solution-oriented video and the solution-

oriented infographic were significantly different (p=.042). Independent sample T-Test’s were

further conducted and showed that the impact-oriented video (M=6.08, SD=1.23) generated

greater perceptions of issue importance than the impact-oriented infographic (M=4.96,

4 4.2 4.4 4.6 4.8 5 5.2 5.4

Infographic Cartoon Video Photograph

Pe rv ie d s elf -e ff ic acy Solution-related visuals

(32)

SD=1.64); t(50)=-2.83, p=.007. And the solution-oriented video (M=5.68, SD=1.145), as

well, led to an increased sense of issue importance compared to the solution-oriented

infographic (M=4.93, SD=1.307); t(52)=-2.22, p=.031 (see figure 2 and figure 3 below for a

visual representation of the differences). The results partially confirm the expectations. Only

the impact-oriented video depicting plastic pollution were found to significantly generate a

greater sense of issue importance (H2b). And surprisingly, even the solution-oriented video

depicting plastic pollution led to higher perceptions of issue importance.

Figure 2. Mean levels of perceived issue importance for different visuals on plastic pollution impact

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Infographic Cartoon Video Photograph

P erc ived iss ue im porta nc e impact-related visuals

(33)

Figure 3. Mean levels of perceived issue importance for different visuals on plastic pollution solution

Last but the not least, the current study examined relationship between different visuals

depicting plastic pollution solution and visuals depicting plastic pollution impact. According

to independent sample T-Test’s, overall, images depicting plastic pollution solution (M=4.84,

SD=1.48) were associated with greater perception of self-efficacy than images depicting

plastic pollution impact (M=4.13, SD=1.85); t(215)=-3.202, p=.002, whereas there was no

any significant difference regarding issue importance. Looking at affective reactions, as

expected, images depicting plastic pollution solution (M=3.85, SD=1.609) generated strong

positive emotional responses compared to images depicting plastic pollution impact (M=1.39,

SD=1.39); t(221)=-.6.219, p<.001. H1a are therefore proven.

After the main effect tests, the next step was to conduct an analysis with PROCESS to

investigate whether self-efficacy and issue importance mediate the effects of the different

visuals on individual behavior and political behavior (see figure 4 below for a conceptual map

4.4 4.6 4.8 5 5.2 5.4 5.6 5.8

Infographic Cartoon Video Photograph

pe rc eived iss ue im porta nc e solution-related visuals

(34)

of the indirect effects model tested in this study).

Figure 4. Conceptual map of the indirect effects

Looking at self-efficacy, consistent with the ANOVA results reported above, regression

analysis using PROCESS indicated that the solution-oriented infographic and the solution

oriented video were significantly associated with greater perceptions of self-efficacy

compared to the oriented infographic (B=1.10, SE=.3977, p=.0079) and the

impact-oriented video (B=1.43, SE=.4395, p=.001). In addition, consistent with the ANOVA results

reported above, the solution-oriented infographic was associated with greater perceptions of

self-efficacy compared to the solution-oriented photograph (B=-.843, SE=.3765, p=.002).

Looking at issue importance next, consistent with the ANOVA results reported above, the

impact-oriented video generated stronger perceptions of issue importance compared to the

impact-oriented infographic (B=1.1126, SE=.3976, p=.007). The solution-oriented video, as

well, led to greater perceptions of issue importance (B=.7490, SE=.3370, p=.031) compared

(35)

Looking at the relationship between the mediators and the dependent variables, perceived

self-efficacy had a significant positive association with individual behavior (B=.3193,

SE=.1174, p=.008) and political behavior (B=.3900, SE=.1119, p=.001). Surprisingly, while

testing for the effect of the solution-oriented video compared to the impact-oriented video,

there is no significant effect between self-efficacy and individual behavior and political

behavior here (only including the respondents in these two conditions in this analysis). On the

other hand, perceived issue importance also had a significant positive association with

individual behavior (B=.7172, SE=.1106, p<.001) and political behavior (B=.3108, SE=.1279,

p=.019).

Looking at the indirect effect, the solution-oriented infographic had a significant indirect

effect on individual behavior (B=.2031, LCI=.0764, UCI=.3621) and political behavior

(B=.1958, LCI=,0971, UCI=.8804), through the mediator of self-efficacy compared to the

impact-oriented infographic. The impact-oriented video and the solution-oriented video had

significantly positive indirect effect on individual behavior (B=.3122, LCI=.2068,

UCI=1.4564) and political behavior (B=.2012, LLCI=.0545, UCI=.8922) compared to the

impact-oriented infographic and the solution-oriented infographic, through the mediator of

issue importance. As a result, the H5a and H5b partially hold true as solution-oriented

infographic does have a significant indirect effect through the mediator of perceived

(36)

mediator of issue importance.

Looking at the overall solution-oriented visuals and the impact-oriented visuals combined,

the solution-oriented visuals had a significantly positive effect on self-efficacy (B=.7105,

SE=.2219, P=.002) and self-efficacy was significantly associated with individual behavior

(B=.3591, SE=.0564, p<.001) and political behavior (B=.1720, SE=.0507, p<.001). These

results indicated that the indirect effect was significant. The solution-related visuals were

associated with greater individual behavior (b=.0923, LCI=.0952, UCI=.4673), and political

behavior (B=.0525, LCI=.034, UCI=.2436) and mediated by self-efficacy. However, no

independent variables had an indirect effect through perceived issue importance on individual

and political behavior. The results prove H5a but partially rejected H5b that not all

impacted-oriented visuals had an indirect effect through perceived issue importance on individual and

political behavior.

General Discussion

This is the first study that experimentally tests the impact of a range of different types of

visual representations on perceived self-efficacy and issue importance regarding plastic

pollution. The current study also builds on previous studies regarding the indirect effects of

visuals on individual behavioral change and political behavior change through the mediators

of perceived self-efficacy and issue importance. Together the results offer support for the

(37)

In accord with the expectations based on the previous studies (Hart & Feldman, 2016;

O'Neill et al., 2013; O'Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009; Chapman et al., 2016), a greater positive affective responses in participants’ attitude are observed in the solution-oriented visuals, whereas the impact-oriented visuals are found to generate negative affective

responses. And among four types of visual representations, cartoons and videos are found

particularly more captivating for the public in comparison with infographics and photographs.

Surprisingly, the result on how complicated the each stimuli were perceived runs counter to

the expectation. Participants generally reported that they found the impact-oriented

infographic complicated to understand and didn’t think it conveyed the issue of plastic

pollution better than the impact-oriented photograph. Even though infographics were

expected to make complex issues more comprehensible, the integration of visualization, data

and text messages may however run the risk of rendering the images more complex and

confusing for audiences to process (de Haan, et al., 2017).

Looking at self-efficacy, the results confirm the hypotheses and replicate the results from

previous studies using the same experiment approach and a Q-sort approach (Hart &

Feldman, 2016; O'Neill et al., 2013; O'Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009) that the

solution-oriented visuals depicting plastic pollution have the potential to evoke people’s feeling to do

something about plastic pollution. The current study particularly finds that solution-oriented

(38)

self-efficacy compared to impact-oriented infographics and impact-oriented videos, while

solution-oriented cartoons and solution-oriented photographs don’t really show the significant

results. Additionally, regarding the visual representations used, among four types of visuals

representations, the solution-oriented infographics performs better than the solution-oriented

photographs in increasing perceptions of self-efficacy. It is highly possible that infographics

using graphs and charts to explain possible plastic pollution solutions are perceived to be

more engaging and motivating than photographs.

Looking at issue importance, the results runs counter to the previous experiment study

(Hart & Feldman, 2016) suggesting the impact-oriented visuals have no significant impact on

the perceptions of issue importance. The results proved H2b that videos depicting plastic

pollution impact perform better than other three types of visuals in promoting a sense of

importance about plastic pollution. Additionally, the current study also finds that even videos

depicting plastic pollution solution have significant impact on the perceptions of issue

importance. This engaging nature of the videos holds true as studies suggested that videos

have the potential to influence awareness (Acierno, Rheingold, Resnick & Stark-Riemer,

2004; Occa & Suggs, 2016). Besides, the fact that videos could lead to higher perceptions of

issue importance may be due to the core characteristics of video which tend to be more vivid

and contain more movement and noise, and thus allow people to easily resonate with the issue

(39)

Looking at indirect effects on behavioral change, the current study is also in line with the

communication guide and previous study findings (O'Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009; O'Neill

et al., 2013; Metag et al., 2016; Chapman et al., 2016; Hart & Feldman, 2016) suggesting that

the solution-oriented visuals coupling with information highlighting possible behavioral

actions have potential for motivating audiences’ behavioral change. The current study finds

that infographics that integrate graphs and visual information as well as concrete possible

solutions have the greater potential to motivate individual behavioral change as well as

increase the willingness to participate in political actions through the mediator of perceived

self-efficacy. And overall, as predicted, the four types of visuals depicting plastic pollution

solution are found to be the most motivating for individual to change individual behavior and

join political actions through the mediation of self-efficacy. The findings strengthen the

importance of self-efficacy as the mediation role in enhancing environmental behavior and

political behavior since previous study (Chapman et al, 2016) without mediation of

self-efficacy test suggested that images of solutions were the least motivating for participants. On

the other hand, through the mediator of issue-importance, videos depicting both plastic

pollution impact and plastic pollution solution scores higher on influencing individual

behavior and political behavior compared to infographics. The chosen video showing real-life

disturbing footage of plastics floating on the oceans as well as inspiring beach cleanup

(40)

importance and intentions for behavioral change. The results aligned with the literatures

which suggested videos have the potential to immediate influence issue awareness and further

provoke behavioral change.

Taken together, the results from the current study demonstrate that visual communication

do matter while communicating pro-environmental issues. The findings particularly suggest

that videos and infographics are particularly the most effective visual communication tools

compared to photographs and cartoons to increase public perceptions of self-efficacy and

issue importance, and further prompt people to take individual actions and participate in

political actions. Cartoons and photographs, on the other hand, are considered to be more

captivating for audience and could easily convey the issues. Furthermore, as all the previous

studies have suggested, visuals depicting solution are no doubt the best to address

environmental issues to increase public perceptions of self-efficacy, and also important for

motivating actions no matter in individual sphere or political sphere. The solution-oriented

images are particularly effective if the visuals couple with the action messages. At the same

time, the findings of this study question about the reliability of previous studies that imagery

has no significant role in influencing perceived issue importance (Hart & Feldman, 2016). Also, the current study doesn’t replicate the previous research that the images of plastic pollution impact could possibly cause boomerang effect on efficacy (O'Neill &

(41)

Limitations

As with other studies, the current study has three drawbacks. Firstly, as the current study

examines the effects of different types of visual representations, each particular visual

material was chosen to represent each visual category. Even though the current study has tried to select the visuals that are as comparable as possible, it’s still possible that the unique

characteristic of each visual material would contribute to different results. Moreover, departed

from previous studies that are mainly focus on climate change, the current student focuses on

different environmental issue and as a result, it is more likely to result in different outcomes.

Further studies are needed to continue to investigate the effects on perception and behavioral

change by using a wider scope of visuals. Secondly, the current study only examines the

intention to behavioral change instead of investigating actual behavioral change. The current

study is therefore unable to prove whether the actual behaviors are actually taken by

individuals. Lastly, the current study doesn’t test the moderated mediation effects of

environmental identity and environmental behavior variable before exposing participants to

each visual materials. Depending on different levels of people’s perceived environmental

identity, the effects of each visual representation on behavioral change via mediator of

self-efficacy and issue importance might differ. In addition, the current study is the first one to use

experimental method to test the effects of different visual representations. It’s also importance

(42)

In summary, the results would be of interest to governments and pro-environmental

organizations as the study offers insights into the effective usage of visual communication

regarding environmental issues on public perception and behavior change. These insights

could also further provide further guidelines for pro-environmental campaigners to effectively

and strategically redesign their campaign messages and broader development with greater

(43)

Reference

Acierno, R., Rheingold, A. A., Resnick, H.S., & Stark-Riemer, W. (2004). Preliminary

evaluation of a video-based intervention for older audlt victims of violence. Journal of

traumatic stress, 17(6), 535-541.

Ashfaq, A. (2012). A Study of International Issues Through Cartoon Communiaction: The

Cases of Pakistan and Norwegian Newspapers from September 2008 to Febrauray 2009..

Malaysian Journal of Communication, 28(1), 55-76.

Bandura, A. (2009). Social Cognitive Theory of Mass Communication. Media Psychology,

(3)3, 265-299.

Briggs, H. (2018). Record concentration of microplastics found in Arctic. BBC NEWS.

Retrieved from http://www.bbc.com/news/science-environment-43879389

Carrington, D. (2018). Microplastic pollution in oceans is far worse than feared, say scientists.

The Guardian. Retreived from

https://www.theguardian.com/environment/2018/mar/12/microplastic-pollution-in-oceans-is-far-greater-than-thought-say-scientists

Changelolgy An all-purpose theroy of behavior change. (.n.d.). Retrieved from

https://www.enablingchange.com.au/enabling_change_theory.pdf

Changeology An all-purpose theory of behaviour change. (n.d.). Retrieved from

(44)

Chapman, D. A., Corner, A., Webster, R., & Markowitz, E.M. (2016). Climate visuals: A

mixed methods investigation of public percpetions of climate images in three countries.

Global Enviromental Change, 41(2016), 172-182.

Chappatte, P. (2010). Patrick Chappatte: The power of cartoons [Video file]. Retrieved from

https://www.ted.com/talks/patrick_chappatte_the_power_of_cartoons/transcript

Evans, A. (2017). If you drop plastic in the ocean, where does it end up? The Guardian.

Retrieved from

https://www.theguardian.com/environment/2017/jun/29/if-you-drop-plastic-in-the-ocean-where-does-it-end-up

Epstein, L. & Jeffrey A. (2000). Measuring issue salience. American Journal of Political

Science, 44, 66-83.

Fernandez, M., Piccolo, L. S. G., Maynard, D., Wippoo, M., Meili, C., & Alani, H. (2016).

Talking climate change via social media: communication, engagement and behaviour.

Proceedings of the 8th ACM Conference on Web Science, 85–94.

Ferguson, L. A. (2012). Implementing a video education program to improve health literacy.

Journal for Nurse Practitioners, 8, 17–22

Fox, D. L., & Schofiled, J.W. (1989). Issue Salience, Perceived Efficacy and Perceived Risk:

A Study of the Origins of Anti-Nuclear War Activity. Journal of Applied Social

Psychology, 19 (10).

(45)

Illustrations to Improve Comprehension of Instructions. Academic Emergency

Medicine, 3(3)

Doyle, J. (2011). Picturing the clima© tic: greepeance and the representational politics of

climate change communication. Science as Culture, 16, 223-243.

Giarelli, E., & Tulman L. (2003). Methodological Issues in the Use of Published Cartoons as

Data. Qualitative Health Research, 13(7), 945-956.

Green, M. J., & Myers, K. R. (2010). Graphic medicine: use of comics in medical education

and patient care. BMJ: British Medical Journal (Online), 340.

Green, S. (2008). No words necessary: The cartoonists tackle climate change. Independent.

Retrieved from

https://www.independent.co.uk/environment/green-living/no-words-necessary-the-cartoonists-tackle-climate-change-859017.html

Griffin, M. (2008). Visual competence and media literacy: Can one exist without the other?

Visual Studies, 23, 113-129.

Hart, P. S., & Feldman, L. (2016). The Impact of Climate Change-Related Imagery and Text

on Public Opinion and Behavior Change. Science Communication, 38(4), 415-441.

de Haan, Y., Kruikemeier, S., Lecheler, S, Smit, G., & van der Nat, R.(2017). When Does an

Infographic Say More Than a Thousand Words? Audience evaluations of news

visualizations. Journalism Studies, 1-20.

(46)

Facebook. Social Media Today. Retrieved from

https://www.socialmediatoday.com/social-business/new-research-shows-video-content-outperforming-all-other-types-facebook

Jin, L., Chen, Y., Wang, T., Hui, P., & Vasilakos, A.V. (2013). Understanding User Behavior in

Online Social Networks: A Survey. IEEE Communications Magazine, 51(9), 144-150.

Kline, K.N. (2007). Cultural sensitivity and health promotion: Assessing breast cancer

education pamphlets designed for African American women. Health Communication,

21, 85-96.

Kollmus, A., & Agyeman, J. (2010). Mind the Gap: Why do people act environmentally and

what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education

Research, 8(3), 239-260.

Lauren, N., Smith, L.D.G., Louis, W.R., & Dean, A.J. (2017). Promoting Spillover: How Past

Behaviors Increase Environmental Intentions by Cueing Self-Perceptions.

Environmental and Behavior, 1-24.

Lazard, A., & Atkinson, L. (2015). Putting environmental infographics center stage: The role of visuals at the elaboration likelihood model’s critical point of persuasion. Science Communication, 37(1), 6-33.

Manjoo, F. (2013). Videos Are Better Than Photographs. Slate. Retrieved from

(47)

_photographs_the_rise_of_short_form_video_services.html

Metag, J., Schäfer, M. S., Füchslin, T., Barsuhn, T., & Königslöw, K. K. (2016). Perception of

climate change imagery evoked salience and self-efficacy in Germany, Switzerland,

and Austria. Science Communication, 38, 197-227.

Manzo, K. (2010). Beyond polar bears? Re-envisioning climate change. Royal Meteorological

Society, 17, 196-208.

Messaris, P. (1997). Visual persuasion: The role of images in advertising. Sage.

Neuendorf, K. A. (2002). The Content Analysis Guidebook. Retrieved from

https://books.google.nl/books?id=huPVtmu4sigC&pg=PR3&dq=the+systematic,+obje

ctive,+quantitative+analysis+of+message+characteristic&source=gbs_selected_pages

&cad=2#v=onepage&q=the%20systematic%2C%20objective%2C%20quantitative%2

0analysis%20of%20message%20characteristic&f=false

Nocke, T., Sterzel, T., Bottinger, M., & Wrobel, M. (2008). Visualization of Climate and

Climate Change Data: An Overview. Digital Earth Summit on Geoinformatics 2008:

Tools for Global Change Research.226-232.

Occa, A., & Suggs, L. S. (2016). Communicating Breast Cancer Screening With Young

Women: An Experimental Test of Didactic and Narrative Messages Using Video and

Infographics. Journal of Health Communication, 21, 1-11.

(48)

climate change engagement. Global Environmental Change, 23, 413-421.

O'Neill, S. J., & Smith, N. (2014). Climate change and visual imagery. Wiley Interdisciplinary

Reviews: Climate Change, 5(1), 73-87.

O'Neill, S.J., & Nicholson-Cole, S. (2009). "Fear Won't Do it" Promoting positive

engagement with climate change through visual and iconic representations. Science

Communication, 30, 355-379.

Rimal, R. N. (2000). Closing the Knowledge-Behavior Gap in Health Promotion: The

Mediating Role of Self-Efficacy. Health Communication, 12(3), 219-237.

Rosenstone, S.J., & Hansen, J.M. (2003). Mobilization, participation, and democracy in

America (1st ed). New York, NY: Perrson.

Roughneen, S. (2018). British diver exposes sea of plastic rubbish off Bali coast. The

Telegraph. Retrieved from

https://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/2018/03/06/british-diver-exposes-sea-plastic-rubbish-bali-coast/

Russell, R. (2017). UN resolves to end ocean plastic waste. Deutsche Welle. Retrieved from

http://www.dw.com/en/un-resolves-to-end-ocean-plastic-waste/a-41690999.

Segerberg, A. (2017). Online and Social Media Campaigns for Climate Change Engagement.

Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Climate Science.

Severtson, D.J., & Henriques, J.B. (2009). The effect of graphics on environmental health risk

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Evaluating the risk of ovarian cancer before surgery using the ADNEX model to differentiate between benign, borderline, early and advanced stage invasive, and secondary

• The family was traditionally viewed as the basic social unit of society. • The family was traditionally defined as consisting in a heterosexual marriage, oc- curring once in

In its Judgment, the Supreme Administrative Court linked these constitutional limitations to the original entitlement of the people (p. In the absence of such entitle-

› The visuals helped to make an abstract model more lively and engaging. › They invited the auditee to think &amp; respond fast

Note that when we totally forget about intra- and inter-sensor constraints, there is no need for MAUT or any other procedure for determining the optimal sensor usage, since we

En cas de non-tenue de l'élection présidentielle le 23 décembre 2OL8, ou de changement politique majeur affectant le processus électoral, la conférence des leaders

These insights were gathered through an analysis of discourse and practice regarding Syrian refugees in Egypt and a study into UNHCR’s advocacy effort towards

MLCAPE = Mean Level Convective Available Potential Energy lowest 100mb MLLCL = Mean Layer of Lifted Condensation Level Height. Baker