• No results found

Weaknesses in executive functioning predict the initiating of adolescents’ alcohol use - 502154

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Weaknesses in executive functioning predict the initiating of adolescents’ alcohol use - 502154"

Copied!
9
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

Weaknesses in executive functioning predict the initiating of adolescents’

alcohol use

Peeters, M.; Janssen, T.; Monshouwer, K.; Boendermaker, W.J.; Pronk, T.; Wiers, R.;

Vollebergh, W.

DOI

10.1016/j.dcn.2015.04.003

Publication date

2015

Document Version

Final published version

Published in

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

License

CC BY-NC-ND

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):

Peeters, M., Janssen, T., Monshouwer, K., Boendermaker, W. J., Pronk, T., Wiers, R., &

Vollebergh, W. (2015). Weaknesses in executive functioning predict the initiating of

adolescents’ alcohol use. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 139-146.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.04.003

General rights

It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s)

and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open

content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulations

If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please

let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material

inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter

to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You

will be contacted as soon as possible.

(2)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Developmental

Cognitive

Neuroscience

jou rn a l h om ep ag e : h t t p : / / w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / d c n

Weaknesses

in

executive

functioning

predict

the

initiating

of

adolescents’

alcohol

use

Margot

Peeters

a,∗

,

Tim

Janssen

b

,

Karin

Monshouwer

a,c

,

Wouter

Boendermaker

b

,

Thomas

Pronk

b

,

Reinout

Wiers

b

,

Wilma

Vollebergh

a

aUtrechtUniversity,Utrecht,TheNetherlands bUniversityofAmsterdam,TheNetherlands cTrimbosInstitute,TheNetherlands

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory:

Received25September2014 Receivedinrevisedform8April2015 Accepted9April2015

Availableonline17April2015

Keywords: Executivefunctioning Alcoholuse Bingedrinking Adolescents Survivalanalyses Cognition

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Recently,ithasbeensuggestedthatimpairmentsinexecutivefunctioningmightberiskfactorsfor theonsetofalcoholuseratherthanaresultofheavyalcoholuse.Inthepresentstudy,weexamined whethertwoaspectsofexecutivefunctioning,workingmemoryandresponseinhibition,predictedthe firstalcoholicdrinkandfirstbingedrinkingepisodeinyoungadolescentsusingdiscretesurvival anal-yses.AdolescentswereselectedfromseveralDutchsecondaryschoolsincludingbothmainstreamand specialeducation(externalizingbehavioralproblems).Participantswere534adolescentsbetween12 and14yearsatbaseline.Executivefunctioningandalcoholusewereassessedfourtimesoveraperiod oftwoyears.Workingmemoryuniquelypredictedtheonsetoffirstdrink(p=.01)andfirstbinge drink-ingepisode(p=.04)whileresponseinhibitiononlyuniquelypredictedtheinitiatingofthefirstdrink (p=.01).Theseresultssuggestthattheassociationofexecutivefunctioningandalcoholconsumption foundinformerstudiescannotsimplybeinterpretedasaneffectofalcoholconsumption,asweaknesses inexecutivefunctioning,foundinalcoholnaïveadolescents,predicttheinitiatingof(binge)drinking. Though,prolongedandheavyalcoholusemightfurtherweakenalreadyexistingdeficiencies.

©2015TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierLtd.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBY-NC-ND license(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

1. Introduction

Recentstudieshavedemonstratedassociationsbetween cog-nitivedeficitsand(heavy)drinkinginadolescents(Fernieetal., 2013;Hansonetal.,2011;Khuranaetal.,2012;Niggetal.,2006; Squegliaetal.,2009;Tapertetal.,2002).Especiallybinge drink-ingseemstobeassociatedwithcognitivedeficitsinadolescents andyoungadults(Hermensetal.,2013;Peetersetal.,2014).An importantquestionthatarisesfromthesefindingsiswhetherthese deficitsarepresentpriortoandpredicttheonsetofdrinkingor whetherthe(heavy)useofalcoholinducesthesedeficits.Although thisrelationshiphasbeeninvestigatedinseveralstudies(Fernie etal.,2013;Hansonetal.,2011;Khuranaetal.,2012;Niggetal., 2006;Squegliaetal.,2009;Tapertetal.,2002),noneofthesestudies haveincludedalcohol-naïveadolescentsatbaseline,whichclearly limitstheinterpretationoftheassociationsfound.Inthepresent studywethereforeexaminedwhethertherelativeweaknessesin

∗ Correspondingauthorat:UtrechtUniversity,Heidelberglaan1,3584CSUtrecht, TheNetherlands.Tel.:+31302533056.

E-mailaddress:m.peeters1@uu.nl(M.Peeters).

executivefunctions(i.e.,workingmemoryandresponseinhibition) predicttheinitiationofthefirstalcoholicdrinkandthefirstbinge drinkingepisodeinyoungadolescents.

1.1. Cognitivedevelopmentinadolescence

During adolescence several important cognitive functions matureanddevelop.Executivefunctionssuchasworking mem-ory,attentionandresponseinhibition,continuetomatureuntillate adolescence(BlakemoreandChoudhury,2006;Caseyetal.,2008). Thesefunctionsareimportantfortheplanning,organizationand coordinationofothercognitiveprocesses(Baddeley,1983;Miyake etal.,2000),andinvolveneuralnetworksincludingdifferentareas intheprefrontalcortex.Incontrasttootherbrainregions(e.g.,the visualcortex),theprefrontalcortexundergoeschangesuntillate adolescence(BlakemoreandChoudhury,2006).Workingmemory, inparticular,continuestomatureuntillate adolescence(18–21 years;Lunaetal.,2004).Inaddition,basiclevelsofresponse inhibi-tionarealreadypresentinearlychildhood;youngchildrenalready abletoinhibitresponses,however,improvementofthisfunction continuesintoadolescence(Lunaetal.,2004).Bothworking mem-oryandresponseinhibitionareimportantforcognitivecontrol;the

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.04.003

(3)

140 M.Peetersetal./DevelopmentalCognitiveNeuroscience16(2015)139–146

abilitytosuppressthoughtsandimpulsesinordertoachieve long-termgoals(BungeandWright,2007;Lunaetal.,2004;Steinberg, 2007).Cognitivecontrolhasoftenbeenrelated torisk behavior (Caseyetal.,2008;Steinberg,2007).Ithasbeenarguedthatthe slowermaturationoftheprefrontalcortex,andtheaccompanying gradualdevelopmentofcognitivecontrol,mightunderlietheoften observedincreasesinriskybehaviorsduringadolescence,including bingedrinking(Caseyetal.,2008;Steinberg,2007).Adifferentline ofresearchhassuggestedthattheincreaseinrisk-takingbehavior isamotivationalissue(CroneandDahl,2012).Itismore reward-ing,andthusmoremotivating,foradolescentsthanitisforadults toengageinriskybehaviorsbecauseoffactorssuchaspeer accep-tanceorpopularity,whichmightexplaintheincreasedrisk-taking behaviorduringadolescence(CroneandDahl,2012).Thedelayed developmentoftheprefrontalcortexandassociatedcognitive con-trolskillsinadolescentsmightthereforenotrepresenta“deficit” orimmaturefunctioning,butratherahighlyflexibleandadaptive development,tunedtothesocialdemandsadolescentsencounter (foradetaileddescriptionseeCroneandDahl,2012).Nevertheless, inbothlinesofresearch,thedevelopmentofcognitivecontrolin lateadolescenceplaysanimportantroleinexplainingrisk-taking behaviorsinadolescents.

1.2. Executivefunctioningandalcoholuse

Working memory (i.e., keeping information active) and responseinhibitionaretwoimportantexecutivefunctionsoften examinedin relationtoalcoholuse(Khuranaetal.,2012; Nigg etal.,2006;Verdejo-Garciaetal.,2008;Wiersetal.,2007).Deficits inresponse inhibitionand workingmemoryappear toincrease automaticandimpulsiveresponseinsuchawaythatbehavioral responsesinrelationtoalcoholstimuliaremorelikelydirectedby immediatesatisfactionofneedsthanpursuanceoflong-termgoals (Grenardetal.,2008;Thushetal.,2008;Peetersetal.,2012,2013; Wiersetal.,2007).Inotherwords,deficitsinexecutivefunctioning limitindividualstorespondinacontrolledandplannedmannerto alcoholstimuli,leadingtomoreimpulsiveandautomaticresponses whichhavebeenassociatedwithincreasedalcoholuse(Grenard etal.,2008;Thushetal.,2008;Peetersetal.,2012).

Severallongitudinalstudies haveexamined thedirect effect ofrelativelypoorexecutivefunctioningondrinkingbehavior in adolescence.Niggandcolleagues(2006),forinstance,foundthat poorinhibitionskillsinearlyadolescents(12–14years,N=498) predictedlater (15–17years)problematicalcohol use.Likewise, Khuranaandcolleagues(2012)foundthatrelativelypoor work-ing memorypredicted growth in the frequency of alcohol use ina community sample ofadolescents (N=358).Moreover,the authorsfoundthattheeffectofworkingmemoryonalcoholusewas mediatedbyimpulsivity suggestingthat poorworkingmemory functioningmightincreaseimpulsivebehaviorwhichsubsequently increasesalcoholuseamongadolescents.Inaddition,Fernieand colleagues(2013)foundthatseveralmeasuresofimpulsivity(i.e., delaydiscounting,risktakingandresponseinhibition)predicted increasein alcoholuseamong youngadolescents(12–13years, N=287).Thequestionremains,however,whetherthesedeficits precedeandpredicttheonsetofadolescents’drinking,sincethese studies have examined executive functioning after adolescents alreadyconsumedtheirfirstdrink,thusleavingopenthe possibil-itythattheimpairedexecutivefunctioningwasalreadytheresult ofearlierdrinking.

1.3. Currentstudy

In the present study we examined the predictive effect of workingmemoryandresponseinhibition,twoexecutivefunctions undertheheadingofcognitivecontrol,andfrequentlyassociated

withriskbehavior,ontheinitiatingofthefirstalcoholicdrinkina sampleofyoungadolescents(12–15years,beforethelegalalcohol buyingageof16–intheNetherlandsatthetimeofthestudy). Althoughcognitioninvolvesmany moreaspects thanexecutive functions,wedecidedtofocusonthesetwocognitions,since partic-ularlythesefunctionsareassumedtoplayavitalroleinadolescent alcoholuseastheycontinuetomatureinadolescence( Verdejo-Garciaetal.,2008).Bingedrinkingisacommondrinkingpattern amongyoungadolescents,andithasbeenlinkedtoseveral(health) riskbehaviors(Milleretal.,2007).Wethereforealsoexaminedthe predictiveeffectofexecutivefunctioningontheinitiatingofthe firstbingedrinkingepisode(i.e.,fiveormoreglassesonaday). Bothadolescentsform mainstreamas wellasadolescentsfrom specialeducation(SE)wereincludedinthestudytoascertain vari-ationindrinkingbehavior.Adolescentsin SEschoolshavebeen foundtodrinkmoreheavily(Kepperetal.,2011)comparedto ado-lescentsfrommainstreameducation.AdolescentsattendtheseSE schoolswhentheyhavebehavioralproblems(e.g.,conduct prob-lems,hyperactivity,attentionproblems),althoughtheymaynot necessarilybediagnosedwithabehavioraldisorder.These exter-nalizingbehavioralproblemsareassociatedwithrelativelyweak WMfunctioning(Barkley,1997;Pihletal.,1990),andmightplace adolescentsatriskforproblematicalcoholuseandlateralcohol dependence.

We hypothesizedthat relativelyweakworkingmemoryand responseinhibitionwouldpredicttheinitiatingofthefirstalcoholic drinkandthefirstbingedrinkingepisodeinthissample. Discrete-timesurvivalanalyseswasusedtodeterminetheinitiatingofthe firstdrinkandbingedrinkepisodeduringthetwo-yearfollow-up ofthestudyandtoevaluatethepredictiveeffectofexecutive func-tioning.Moreover,bothfunctionshavepreviouslybeenassociated withadolescentalcoholuseandproblemdrinking(Khuranaetal., 2012;Niggetal.,2006),althoughtheiruniquecontributiontothe initiatingofdrinkinghasnotyetbeenexaminedsimultaneously.

2. Materialsandmethod

2.1. Participants

Thisstudywaspartofalargerstudyinwhichweassessed self-reportedandbehavioralcognitions,andpersonalitystylesrelated toalcoholuseandotherriskbehaviors.Participantswererecruited fromseveralmainstreamand 17SEschoolsintheNetherlands. Maleswereslightlyoverrepresented(69%boysversus31%girls) becausemoreboysthangirlsattendSEschools(Oswaldetal.,2003). Forbothsamples,informedconsentofthechildaswellas pas-siveparentalconsentwasrequested.Inthemainstreamsample, 37parentsdeclinedparticipationoftheirchild.FortheSEsample, 15parentsand7studentsdeclinedparticipationinthisstudy.The mainstreamsampleincluded250adolescents(69%girls),selected fromanationalsurveystudy,whichwaspartoftheInternational Health Behavior of School-aged Children-survey (HBSC, Zanotti etal.,2012).AdolescentswhoparticipatedintheDutchnational studywerecontactedforadditionalassessmentsafterthe comple-tionofthemainsurvey.TheSEsampleincluded374adolescents (12%girls).Forthepurposeofthisstudy,adolescentsbetween12 and15yearswereselected,resultinginasampleof534(371boys, 163girls)adolescents.

Data were collected at intervals of 6–8 months acrossfour waves.Atbaseline,525adolescentsparticipatedinthestudy(98% of534,notethatsomeadolescentswereabsentduringthefirst wavebutparticipatedinthefollowingwaves).Overall,415(78%) adolescentsparticipatedinwave2,399(75%)adolescents partici-patedinwave3,and425(80%)adolescentsparticipatedinwave4. Missingcaseswereimputedusingamultipleimputationprogram

(4)

(i.e.,multipleimputationbychainedequations(MICE),whichis explainedbelow).Thedataoverfourwaveswerecollectedeither viatheInternet (inthecase of mainstreameducation) orvia a trainedresearchassistant(inthecaseofSEeducation).Adolescents fromSEcompletedthequestionnairewithpenandpaperfollowed byacomputersessiontoassessthecomputertasks.FiveoftheSE studentscompletedonlythecomputertasksatbaselineanddidnot completethequestionnaire.Sinceweusedmultipleimputationto handlemissingcases,weincludedtheseadolescentsinthestudy forfurtheranalysis.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Demographicalmeasures

Sex,ageandexternalizingbehavioralproblemswereassessed usingaquestionnaire.TheStrengthandDifficultiesQuestionnaire (Goodman,1997)wasusedtoassessexternalizingbehavior prob-lems(hyperactivityandconductproblems).Thescaleincludes25 items,withthreeansweringcategories(i.e.,nottrue,somewhat trueorcertainlytrue).Forthepurposeofthisstudyweusedthe externalizingsubscaleofthisquestionnaire,whichincludesitems suchas“Iamrestless,overactiveandcannotstaystillforlong”or “Ioftenloosetemper”.

2.2.2. Alcoholuse

Todeterminetheinitiatingofdrinkingandbingedrinking(five ormoreglassesonaday),weaskedparticipantstoindicate,for weekendand weekdays separately,the numberof glasses they consumedonanaveragesingleday(e.g.,“Whenyouconsume alco-holonaweek/weekenddayhowmanyglassesdoyoudrink).For thepurposeofthesurvivalanalysis,thiscontinuousmeasurewas recodedasdichotomousvariable.Theinitiatingoffirstalcoholic drinkwasdefinedasdrinkingatleastonestandardglassof alco-hol(containing12.5mlofpurealcohol)onaweekorweekendday. Participantsreceivedeithera“one”ora“zero”onbothvariables, whichservedasanindicatoroftheoccurrenceoftheevent(i.e., ini-tiatingofdrinkingorbingedrinking;“one”indicatedthattheevent occurred).

2.2.3. Workingmemory

Working memory performance was assessed with the Self OrderedPointingTask(SOPT;PetridesandMilner,1982). Partic-ipantscompleteda concreteversion oftheSOPT.Pictureswere simultaneouslyplaced at different positions onscreen, starting withapracticetrialof4picturesfollowedbyfourtrialswith6, 8,10, and12 pictures, respectively.Thepositionof pictures on screenwasrandomlyshuffledinanumberofvariationsequaltothe amountofpictures.Participantswereaskedtoselecteachpicture oncethroughoutthevariations,butnottoselectthesame posi-tiontwiceinarow.Participantswereencouragedtocompletethe taskwithoutanyerrors.Irrespectiveoftheerrorsmade,all par-ticipantsreceivedthefeedback“welldone”aftereachtrial.With respecttothesecondinstruction,thetaskwasprogrammednotto allowselectingthesamelocationtwiceinarow.Foreachtesttrial, aproportionalerrorscorewascomputedbydividingthenumberof correctresponsesbythenumberoftotalpictureswithineachtrial toaccountfortaskdifficulty(CraggandNation,2007).Thetotal SOPTscorewascalculatedbytakingthemeanoftheproportion ofcorrectscoresofthefourtrials.Higherscoresindicatedbetter workingmemoryfunctioning.

2.2.4. Responseinhibition

ResponseinhibitionwasassessedusingtheStrooptask(Stroop, 1935).TheStrooptask,asameasureofresponseinhibition,has beenusedsuccessfullyasamoderatorinpreviousstudies(Houben andWiers,2009;Peetersetal.,2012).Participantswereinstructed

toindicatethecoloroftheword(i.e.,red,green,blue,oryellow) thatappearedonthescreenbypressingthecorrespondingkeyon thekeyboardwhileignoringthemeaningoftheword.Participants startedwithapracticeblock,whichconsistedof40trialswith sym-bols(e.g.,@@@@or&&&&).Thepracticeblockwasfollowedbya testblockwith28trials.Trialscouldbecongruent(i.e.,meaningof thewordmatchesthecolor),neutral(i.e.,coloredsymbolsinstead ofwords),orincongruent(i.e.,meaningoftheworddiffersfromthe color),andtheywerepresentedinrandomorderbetween partici-pants.Eachtrialwasrepeateduntilacorrectanswerwasgiven.An errormessage,includingadescriptionofthekeysusedandtheir correspondingcolor,followedanincorrectresponse.Aresponse inhibitionscorewascalculatedbysubtractingtheRTsonneutral trialsfromtheRTsonincongruenttrials,withhigherRTs indicat-ingmoreproblemswithcontrol.Foranalyticalpurposes,RTswere dividedby1000.

2.3. Missingdata

Becausewe wanted todeterminewhethera participant ini-tiateddrinkingand/orbingedrinkingduringstudyfollow-up,it wasnecessarytohaveadatasetthatwasascompleteas possi-ble.Missingdata,especiallywhenitinvolvesariskfactororevent, isparticularlyproblematicinsurvivalanalyses(vanBuurenetal., 1999).Multipleimputation(MI)isa commonlyusedmethodto handlemissingdata.PreviousresultsindicatedthatMIisareliable andvalidmethod(Sterneetal.,2009).However,tocheckwhether thiswasalsothecaseforthepresentsample,asimulationstudy wasperformed(performedonthehigh-risksample,whichhadthe largestnumberofmissingdata).Todeterminethebestmethodto handlethemissingdata,severaloptionswerecompared,including listwisedeletion,manualfillinmethods(e.g.,lookingatpreviousor subsequentwave/lookingatmajority/fillinginzerosateach miss-ing),andMI.Wecomparedthefivemethodsontwocriteriathat wererelevantforourstudy:(1)percentageofonsettersthatwould beidentifiedbyeachmethodand(2)thepredictiveeffectoftwo variablescommonlyassociatedwithalcoholuse,namelysensation seekingandimpulsivity.Theresultsrevealedthatmultiple impu-tationwastheoptimalmethodtohandlethemissingdatainthe currentstudy(papersubmittedforpublication,detailed descrip-tionofthesimulationstudycanberequestedfromthefirstauthor). We usedtheprogram MICE(R-packageinR)tocreate five dif-ferentimputationfiles,whichweretransportedtoMplusversion 7(MuthénandMuthén,1998–2010)forsurvivalanalyses.MICE replaces themissing information withplausible values byboth minimizingtheuncertaintyandthebiasofthemodel(vanBuuren etal.,1999).Plausiblevaluesareimputedbasedoncarefully cho-senconfoundingvariablesthatserveaspredictorsofthemissing values.Forthepresentstudy,wecheckedcorrelationmatricesto determinethevariablesthatshouldbeusedtopredictmissingdata. Bothcovariatesandtheoutcomemeasurewereincludedin the imputationmodel,asrecommendedbythedevelopers(vanBuuren andOudshoorn,2000).Forthebinaryvariables,wechosethe logis-ticregressionmethod.Forallothercontinuescovariates,weused thepredictivemeanmatchingmethod.Lifetimealcoholuse,daily smoking,druguse(cannabis,ecstasy,cocaine,hallucinogens), gen-der,age,andpersonality(impulsivity,sensationseeking,anxiety, andhopelessness)wereselectedaspredictorsofmissingness.

2.4. Analyticalprocedure

First,descriptivesfordemographicalinformation,alcoholuse andexecutivefunctioningwerepresentedforthetwosamples sep-arately(mainstreamversusSE,Table1)andfortheonsetandbinge sampleseparately (Table2).Second,thefiveimputeddatafiles werepreparedfor discrete-timesurvival analysis.Sincetheaim

(5)

142 M.Peetersetal./DevelopmentalCognitiveNeuroscience16(2015)139–146

Fig.1. Structuralsurvivalmodelwithageindicators(12–17years)andpredictors(i.e.,responseinhibitionandworkingmemory)oftheinitiatingof(binge)drinking.

Table1

Descriptivesforthetwosamples(mainstreamversusspecialeducation)separately.

Mainstream education Special education t-Test Age 13.84(.82) 13.70(.73) t(532)=−1.93 Sex(%boys) 39 88 Hyperactivityproblems(SDQ) 1.62(.46) 1.93(.48) t(501)=10.94* Conductproblems(SDQ) 1.26(.23) 1.60(.38) t(501)=6.72*

Alcoholquantitybaseline .28(1.52) 3.13(7.66) t(503)=4.81*

Workingmemory .80(.09) .73(.10) t(493)=−7.51*

Responseinhibition .07(.22) .21(.35) t(492)=4.89*

SDQ=StrengthandDifficultiesQuestionnaire.

* p<.05.

Table2

Descriptivestatisticsforthetotalsample,theonsetsampleandthebingedrinking sample.

Total(N=534) Onset(N=336) Binge(N=458)

M SD M SD M SD

Agebaseline 13.75 .76 13.69 .77 13.73 .78

Alcoholuse 2.19 6.45 0 0 .35 .83

Responseinhibition .16 .32 .13 .30 .14 .30

Workingmemory .75 .11 .76 .10 .76 .11

Note:Thetotalsamplecontainsallparticipantsfrombothsampleorigins (main-streamandspecialeducation).Theonsetsamplecontainsonlythepartofthetotal samplethatdidnotdrinkatbaseline.Thebingesamplecontainsonlythepartofthe totalsamplethatdidnotbingeatbaseline.

ofthestudywastoexamineexecutivefunctioningasriskfactorfor

initiatingafirstalcoholicdrink,weexcludedalladolescentswho

alreadyconsumedalcoholatbaseline(fortheonsetsample)andall

adolescentswhoalreadyengagedinbingedrinking(drinkingfive

ormoreglasses)atbaseline(forthebingesample).Foreach

ado-lescent,theageoftheonsetandtheageofthefirstbingedrinking

episodewereassessedforeveryhalfayear(e.g.,12,12.5,13,...,17),

resultingin11possibletimepointsatwhichtheeventcouldhave

occurred(seeFig.1).UsingMplusforthediscrete-survivalanalysis,

alatentfactoroftheprobabilitythatanindividualwould experi-encetheeventatacertaintimepoint(i.e.,age)wasestimated.

Survival analyses were conducted using Mplus version 7 (MuthénandMuthén,1998–2010).Thefactorloadingsassessing theprobabilityoftheeventoccurrence(seeFig.1onset/binge)were fixedatone(seeFig.1ageindicators)toascertainthattheeffect ofexecutivefunctioningontheonsetofalcoholuse/bingedrinking wasthesameforeachage.Residualvarianceswerefixedatzero. Wetestedforpossiblesexdifferencesusingmultigroupanalyses. WeusedtheclusteringoptioninMplustoaccountfortheeffectof setting(homeversusschoolassessment).

3. Results

3.1. Descriptivesforseparategroups(specialeducationversus mainstream)

Table1representsthedescriptivestatisticsforadolescentsfrom mainstreamandspecialeducationseparately.Ascanbeseenin Table 1, students in SE education significantly differed onsex, hyperactivity and conduct problems,executive functioning and alcoholusefrommainstreamstudents.Effectsizesfordifference inworkingmemoryandresponseinhibitionwereofmediumsize (Cohen’sdworkingmemory=.53,responseinhibition=.46).

3.2. Descriptivestotalgroup

Table 1 includes descriptive statistics of age and executive functioning at baseline for the total sample and the two ana-lyticalsamples(i.e.,onset andbingedrinking)separately.Small differencesforsumscoresonthetwoexecutivefunctioningtask emerged. We used t-tests to examine whetherpossible differ-ences in executive functioning were found between thegroup whoalreadyengagedindrinkingatbaseline(excludedfromthe study)comparedtoalcoholnaiveadolescentsatbaseline(included inthestudy).Significantdifferencesonexecutivefunctioningat baselineemergedbetweenthetwogroupswithlowerscoresfor workingmemory(M=.72fordrinkersandM=.76foralcoholnaïve adolescents)andhigherscoresforresponseinhibition(indicating relativelyweakerexecutivefunctioningforthedrinkers(working memory:t=3.26,p=.01;responseinhibition:t=−2.36,p=.02).

Figs.2and3representthecummulativeincidenceratesofthe initiating offirst drink(Fig.2)and first bingedrinkingepisode (Fig.3)forthetotalgroupandthenon-(binge)drinkinggroupat baseline.

3.3. Predictionoffirstdrinkandfirstbingedrinkingepisode

Table3showstheresultsofthesurvivalanalysiswithworking memoryandresponse inhibition aspredictors ofthe first alco-holicdrinkandbingedrinkingepisode.Multigroupanalysisfavored themodelin whichwe constrainedsex differencestobeequal

Table3

Oddsratio’sandstandardizedregressioncoefficientsforresponseinhibitionand workingmemorypredictingonsetofdrinkingandbingedrinking.

Onset(N=336) Binge(N=458)

Odds ˇ p Odds ˇ p

Responseinhibition 2.20 .80 .01 1.45 .37 .57

(6)

Fig.2. Cumulativeincidenceratesoftheageofonsetforthetotalsampleandtheonsetsampleseparately.

(BICconstrained onset=1338, BIC unconstrained onset=1347;

BICconstrained binge=1645, BICunconstrained binge=1696),

suggesting no significantdifferences in thepredictive effect of

working memory and response inhibition on the initiating of

(binge)drinking.Bothexecutivefunctionswereanalyzedina

mul-tivariatemodeltoexaminetheuniqueinfluenceofeachfunction.

Bothworkingmemoryandresponseinhibitionpredictedthe

initi-atingofthefirstalcoholicdrink.Weakerworkingmemory(ˇ=−.51,

OR=.15,p=.01)andpoorerresponseinhibition(ˇ=.80,OR=3.12,

p=.01,higherscoresindicatepoorerresponseinhibition)increased

thelikelihoodthatadolescentswouldinitiatedrinkingduringstudy

follow-up,indicating anearlier initiating offirstdrinkand first

bingedrinkingepisodeforthoseadolescentswithrelativelyweaker

executivefunctions.

Workingmemory,butnotresponseinhibitionpredictedthe

ini-tiatingofafirstbingedrinkingepisode.Lowerscoresonworking

memory(ˇ=−.08,OR=.33,p=.04)increasedthelikelihoodthat

adolescentswouldinitiatebingedrinkingduringstudyfollow-up.

4. Discussion

Thepresentstudywasoneofthefirsttoexaminethepredictive

effectofexecutivefunctioningontheinitiatingofthefirstalcoholic

drinkandbingedrinkingepisodeamongyoungadolescents,and

thefirsttoexamineadolescentswhowerealcohol-naïveat

base-line.Theresultsindicatedthatrelativelyweakworkingmemory

predictedboththeinitiatingofthefirstalcoholicdrinkandthefirst

bingedrinkingepisode,beyondtheeffectofresponseinhibition.In

addition,relativelypoorresponseinhibitionpredictedthe

initiat-ingofdrinkingbeyondtheeffectofworkingmemory,however,no

effectwasfoundforthepredictionoffirstbingedrinkingepisode.

ParticularlyadolescentsfromSEeducationrevealedrelativelypoor

workingmemoryfunctioningandresponse inhibition,a finding

supportedbypreviousstudies (Barkley, 1997;Pihlet al.,1990;

Niggetal.,2006).Itseemslikelythattheapoorerlevelof exec-utivefunctioning,observedintheseadolescents,placethematrisk foranearlyinitiatingof(binge)drinkingwhicheventuallymight increasetheriskforproblematicalcoholuse,laterinadolescence (KingandChassin,2007).

The findings of this study are in agreement with those of Khuranaand colleagues(2012)whofoundthat executive func-tioning,andmorespecificallyworkingmemory,predictedincrease in frequency of drinkingamong young adolescents.Fernie and colleagues(2013)foundsimilarresults, revealing thatresponse inhibition(andtwoothermeasuresofimpulsivity)predicted alco-holusesixmonthslater.Niggand colleagues(2006)foundthat executivefunctioningpredictedproblemdrinkingbehaviorinhigh risk adolescents (adolescents from alcoholic families). In their study,poorresponseinhibitionatage12–14predictedtheonsetof alcoholrelatedproblemsatage15–17.However,unlikeNiggetal. (2006),wedidnotfindapredictiveeffectofresponseinhibition onbingedrinking.Adifferentsample,adifferenttask,adifferent outcomevariableanddifferentanalyzingtechniqueswereusedin thecurrentstudy,whichmightexplainthedivergentresults.Itis possiblethattheStrooptask,usedinthisstudytoassessresponse inhibition, wasless sensitiveindetectingdifferences compared

(7)

144 M.Peetersetal./DevelopmentalCognitiveNeuroscience16(2015)139–146

totheworkingmemorytask,perhapsexplainingwhywedidnot findauniqueeffectofresponseinhibitionontheinitiatingofthe firstbingedrinkingepisode.Inaddition,resultsindicatethat deci-sionmakingisimpairedafteralcoholconsumption.Goodworking memoryfunctioning allowsbetterdecisionmaking.It therefore mightbepossiblethatpoorerworkingmemoryfunctioningimpairs decisionmakingresultinginmoredrinking/bingedrinking(George etal.,2005).

Khuranaand colleagues (2012) found that impulsivity fully mediatedtheeffectofworkingmemoryonthedrinkingbehaviorof adolescents,suggestingthatweaknessesinworkingmemoryaffect behaviorthroughdisinhibitionandimpulsivity.Contrarytowhat Khuranaandcolleagues(2012)found,theresultsofthepresent studyindicatedthatatleasttwoexecutivefunctionsuniquely con-tributedtothepredictionoftheinitiatingofdrinkingbehavior.In addition,inthepresentstudy,workingmemorywasaunique pre-dictorofbingedrinkinginadolescentswhiledisinhibitionwasnot. Thelatterfindingisinagreementwithseveralstudiesthathave foundassociationsbetweenbingedrinkingandworkingmemory functioning(StephensandDuka,2008a;Cregoetal.,2009;Squeglia etal.,2011).However,thesecross-sectionalstudies(Cregoetal., 2009;Squegliaetal.,2011)donotshedlightonthedirectionofthe relation.Althoughtheresultsofthepresentstudydonotruleout areverseeffectofalcoholuseoncognitivefunctioning(e.g., bidi-rectionaleffectcf.Peetersetal.,2014),theresultsdorevealthat someweaknessesinexecutivefunctioningprecedetheinitiating ofdrinkingbehavior.Thecontinueduseofalcoholmayaggravate thepre-existingdeficits(Khuranaetal.,2012;Peetersetal.,2014), causingdelayindevelopmentorevendeclineintheperformance onexecutivefunctioningtasks.

4.1. Limitations

Besidesthestrengthsofthestudy,suchasthelargesample sizeofyoungadolescentsandtheinclusionof ahigh-risk sam-ple,somelimitations shouldbeconsidered.Althoughtheexact samemeasureswiththesameinstructionswereusedinthetwo differentsamples(mainstreamandSEeducation),theassessment procedurewasslightlydifferent.Adolescentsinthehigh-risk sam-plecompletedtasksunder theguidanceof a researchassistant whileadolescentsin themainstreamsample completed assess-mentathome(i.e.,onlineassessment).Forthatreasonweadjusted for clustering effects due to differences in assessment setting. Furthermore,itis possiblethattheexclusion ofdrinkersatthe studybaselinelimitstherepresentativenessofthecurrent sam-plecomparedtonationalaverages.Though,adolescentswhowere excluded fromthe analysis revealed significantworse working memoryfunctioningandpoorerresponseinhibition,whichisin linewithourfindings(anearlyinitiatingofalcoholuseispredicted byrelativelyweakexecutivefunctioning).Thegeneralizabilityof theseresultsis restricteddue tothespecificcharacteristicsthe specialeducationsample(e.g.,externalizingbehavioralproblems). Unfortunately,we couldnot controlfor behavioral disorders in ouranalysisbecauseinformationaboutDSMIVbehavioral disor-derswasnotcollected(onlygeneralandlimitedinformationon externalizingbehaviorwasavailablebymeansoftheSDQ). Nev-ertheless,intheDutcheducationalsetting,approximately5%of thestudentsin secondaryeducationattenda schoolfor special education.Therefore,thesestudentsreflectanimportantpartof theDutchstudentpopulation.Furthermore,multipleimputation hasbeencriticizedasmethodofhandlingmissingdatabecauseit couldaddnoisetothedata(Rubin,1996).Thiscriticismappears toloseitsvaluebecauserecentsophisticatedimputationmethods areabletohandlemissingdatabettercomparedtoolder meth-ods,suchaslistwisedeletion(Rubin,1996;Sterneetal.,2009).In addition,thesimulationstudyinthepresentstudysupportedthis

critique.Moreover,inthepresentstudybingedrinkingwasdefined asdrinkingfiveormoreglassesonaday.Generally,bingedrinking referstodrinkingfiveormoreglassesatoneoccasion(Wechsler andNelson,2001).However,itseemsunlikelythatadolescentsof thisage(12–16years)regularlyhavetheopportunitytodrinkin theafternoonaswellasintheeveningonweekenddays. More-over,bingedrinkersconsumedonaveragealmostnineglasseson abingedrinkingday,whichcanbeinterpretedasheavydrinking forsuchyoungadolescents.Nevertheless,itispossiblethatsome oftheadolescentbingedrinkersinthisstudydidnotconsumeall fiveglassesinaroworatoneoccasion.Arelatedpossiblelimitation ofthestudyisthatalcoholusewasassessedbasedonthe partici-pants’ownindication,withnoexternalcorroborationfromparents orfriends.Thispotentiallyallowedparticipantstooverreportor underreporttheiralcoholuse.Nevertheless,astudybyKoningand colleagues(2010)revealedthatself-reportofalcoholuseisa reli-ablemethodtoassessalcoholconsumptionofadolescents.Lastly, recentstudiessuggestthatrepeatedcyclesofbingedrinkingare associatedwithexecutivedysfunction,andpoorworkingmemory performanceinparticular(Dukaetal.,2004;StephensandDuka, 2008b).Sinceweonlyexaminedthefirstepisodeofbinge drink-ingwewerenotabletolookatthisrelation.Futureresearchcould includenumberofbingeepisodesasimportantcovariateinrelation toexecutivefunctioning.

4.2. Implicationsandconclusion

Thefindingsofthecurrentstudyhaveimplicationsfortheory andforprevention.First,tothebestofourknowledgethisstudyis oneofthefirsttodemonstratethatdeficitsinexecutive function-ingprecedetheinitiatingofdrinkingamongadolescents.Previous studieshave foundstrongindications for executivefunctioning beingariskfactor foralcoholuseamongadolescents,however, thedesignofthesestudiespreventedtoderivestronginferences aboutdeficitsinexecutivefunctioningprecedingtheactualonsetof drinkingbehavior(seePeetersetal.,2014,foranoverview).Second, severalclinicalstudieshavefoundcognitivedeficitsafterheavyand prolongedalcoholuse(Hansonetal.,2011;Tapertetal.,2002).It ispossiblethatsomeofthesefounddeficitswerealreadypresent beforedrinkingbehaviorwasinitiated,andamagnifiedandbiased effectofalcoholonadolescents’cognitivefunctioningmayarise. Itshouldbenoted,however,thatthesestudiesexcluded adoles-centswithanexternalizingbehavioraldisorder(conductdisorder, ADHD),whilethecurrentstudyincludedadolescentswith exter-nalizingbehavioralproblems.Inaddition,deficitshavebeenfound forawiderangeofcognitivefunctions(e.g.,attention,visuospatial functioning),andalcoholmightaffectspecificpartsofthe adoles-centbraindifferently.Furtherresearchisneededtospecifywhich cognitivefunctionsaresusceptibletoheavydrinkingandwhich functionsaremainlyofinterestinpredictingthedrinkingonsetof adolescents.

Withrespecttoprevention,thefindingsofthepresentstudy confirmtheimportanceofearlyinterventions.Ithasbeen demon-strated thatexecutive functionsmoderate theeffectof implicit cognitivemotivationalprocessesonalcoholandsubstanceusein adolescents(Grenardetal.,2008;Peetersetal.,2012;Thushetal., 2008).Astheseprocesses getstronger withincreaseddrinking, delayoftheonsetofdrinkingappearstobeagoodgeneralstrategy topreventproblematicalcoholuseinadolescents(Koningetal., 2009,2011).Assomeadolescentsappeartobeatagreaterriskfor anearlyonsetofdrinking,additionaltargetedpreventionmight bewarrantedas well(cf.Conrodet al.,2008).Recently several promisinginterventionshavebeenintroducedtoincreaseresponse inhibition(Houbenetal.,2011)andworkingmemory(Klingberg etal.,2005;Dovisetal.,2012)inadolescentsandyoungadults.

(8)

Thesetrainingmethodscontributetoanincreaseinexecutive con-trol,andtheymighteventuallypostponetheageoftheonset.

Thefindingsofthisstudydemonstratethatpre-existingdeficits inexecutivefunctioningpredicttheinitiatingofthefirstalcoholic drinkandfirstbingedrinkingepisodeamongyoungadolescents. The results suggest that relatively weakworking memory and disinhibitionarerisk factorsfor theearlyinitiating of drinking. Moreover,weakworkingmemoryfunctioningisariskfactorfor bingedrinking.Acontinuedheavydrinkingpatternmightfurther worsenexecutivefunctioning(Squegliaetal.,2009;Tapertetal., 2002), beyondthese pre-existingeffects. Futureresearch could benefitfromdisentanglingcognitivedeficitsinducedbyalcoholuse fromcognitivedeficitsthatpredictalcoholuse,withthecurrent findingsprovidingafirstmodeststep.

Conflictofinterest

Theauthorsdeclarenoconflictofinterest.

Acknowledgements

ThisstudywassupportedbytheDutchHealthCareResearch Organization(ZON-Mw;60-600-97-172)andaVicigrant 453-08-001fromtheDutchNationalScienceFoundation(N.W.O.)awarded toprof.Dr.ReinoutW.Wiers.

References

Baddeley,A.,1983.Workingmemory.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.B302,311–324.

Barkley,R.A.,1997.Behavioralinhibition,sustainedattention,andexecutive func-tions:constructingaunifyingtheoryofADHD.Psychol.Bull.121,65–94.

Blakemore,S.J.,Choudhury,S.,2006.Developmentoftheadolescentbrain: implica-tionsforexecutivefunctionandsocialcognition.J.ChildPsychol.Psychiatry47, 296–312.

Bunge,S.A.,Wright,S.B.,2007.Neurodevelopmentalchangesinworkingmemory andcognitivecontrol.Curr.Opin.Neurobiol.17,243–250.

Casey,B.,Jones,R.M.,Hare,T.A.,2008.Theadolescentbrain.Ann.N.Y.Acad.Sci. 1124,111–126.

Conrod,P.J.,Castellanos,N.,Mackie,C.,2008.Personality-targetedinterventions delaythegrowthofadolescentdrinkingandbingedrinking.J.ChildPsychol. Psychiatry49,181–190.

Cragg,L.,Nation,K.,2007.Self-orderedpointingasatestofworkingmemoryin typicallydevelopingchildren.Memory15,526–535.

Crego,A.,Holguín,S.R.,Parada,M.,Mota,N.,Corral,M.,Cadaveira,F.,2009.Binge drinkingaffectsattentionalandvisualworkingmemoryprocessinginyoung universitystudents.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.33,1870–1879.

Crone, E.A., Dahl, R.E., 2012. Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 636–650.

Duka,T.,Gentry,J.,Malcolm,R.,2004.Consequencesofmultiplewithdrawalsfrom alcohol.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.280,233–246.

Dovis, S., Van der Oord, S., Wiers, R.W., Prins, P.J., 2012. Can motivation normalizeworkingmemoryandtaskpersistenceinchildrenwith attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder?Theeffectsofmoneyandcomputer-gaming.J. Abnorm.ChildPsychol.40,669–681.

Fernie,G.,Peeters,M.,Gullo,M.J.,Christiansen,P.,Cole,J.C.,Sumnall,H.,Field, M.,2013.Multiplebehaviouralimpulsivitytaskspredictprospectivealcohol involvementinadolescents.Addiction108,1916–1923.

George,S.,Rogers,R.D.,Duka,T.,2005.Theacuteeffectofalcoholondecisionmaking insocialdrinkers.Psychopharmacology(Berl)182,160–169.

Goodman,R.,1997.TheStrengthsandDifficultiesQuestionnaire:aresearchnote.J. ChildPsychol.Psychiatry38,581–586.

Grenard,J.L.,Ames,S.L.,Wiers,R.W.,Thush,C.,Sussman,S.,Stacy,A.W.,2008.

Workingmemorycapacitymoderatesthepredictiveeffectsofdrug-related associationsonsubstanceuse.Psychol.Addict.Behav.22,426–432.

Hanson,K.L.,Medina,K.L.,Padula,C.B.,Tapert,S.F.,Brown,S.A.,2011.Impactof adolescentalcoholanddruguseonneuropsychologicalfunctioninginyoung adulthood:10-yearoutcomes.J.ChildAdolesc.Subst.Abuse20,135–154.

Hermens,D.F.,Lagopoulos,J.,Tobias-Webb,J.,DeRegt,T.,Dore,G.,Juckes,L.,Latt, N.,Hickie,I.B.,2013.Pathwaystoalcohol-inducedbrainimpairmentinyoung people:areview.Cortex49,3–17.

Houben,K.,Wiers,R.W.,Jansen,A.,2011.GettingaGriponDrinkingBehavior: trainingworkingmemorytoreducealcoholabuse.Psychol.Sci.22,968–975.

Houben,K.,Wiers,R.W.,2009.Responseinhibitionmoderatestherelationship betweenimplicitassociationsanddrinkingbehavior.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res. 33,626–633.

Kepper,A.,Monshouwer,K.,VanDorsselaer,S.,Vollebergh,W.,2011.Substanceuse byadolescentsinspecialeducationandresidentialyouthcareinstitutions.Eur. Child.Adoles.Psych.20,311–319.

Khurana,A.,Romer,D.,Betancourt,L.M.,Brodsky,N.L.,Giannetta,J.M.,Hurt,H.,2012.

Workingmemoryabilitypredictstrajectoriesofearlyalcoholuseinadolescents: themediationalroleofimpulsivity.Addiction108,506–515.

King,K.M.,Chassin,L.,2007.Aprospectivestudyoftheeffectsofageofinitiation ofalcoholanddruguseonyoungadultsubstancedependence.J.Stud.Alcohol Drugs68(2),256–265.

Klingberg,T.,Fernell,E.,Olesen,P.J.,Johnson,M.,Gustafsson,P.,Dahlström,K., Gill-berg,C.G.,Forssberg,H.,Westerberg,H.,2005.Computerizedtrainingofworking memoryinchildrenwithADHD–arandomized,controlledtrial.J.Am.Acad. ChildPsychiatry44,177–186.

Koning,I.M.,Harakeh,Z.,Engels,R.C.M.E.,Vollebergh,W.A.M.,2010.Acomparisonof self-reportedalcoholusemeasuresbyearlyadolescents:questionnairesversus diary.J.Subst.Use15(3),166–173.

Koning,I.M.,VandenEijnden,R.J.,Verdurmen,J.E.,Engels,R.C.,Vollebergh,W.A., 2011.Long-termeffectsofaparentandstudentinterventiononalcoholusein adolescents:aclusterrandomizedcontrolledtrial.Am.J.Prev.Med.40,541–547.

Koning,I.M.,Vollebergh,W.A.M.,Smit,F.,Verdurmen,J.E.E.,VanDenEijnden,R.J.J.M., TerBogt,T.F.M.,Stattin,H.,Engels,R.C.M.E.,2009.Preventingheavyalcoholuse inadolescents(PAS):clusterrandomizedtrialofaparentandstudent interven-tionofferedseparatelyandsimultaneously.Addiction104,1669–1678.

Luna, B.,Garver, K.E.,Urban,T.A., Lazar,N.A.,Sweeney, J.A.,2004. Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Dev. 75, 1357–1372.

Miller,J.W.,Naimi,T.S.,Brewer,R.D.,Jones,S.E.,2007.Bingedrinkingandassociated healthriskbehaviorsamonghighschoolstudents.Pediatrics119,76–85.

Miyake,A.,Friedman,N.P.,Emerson,M.J.,Witzki,A.H.,Howerter,A.,Wager,T.D., 2000.Theunityanddiversityofexecutivefunctionsandtheircontributions tocomplex“frontallobe”tasks:alatentvariableanalysis.Cogn.Psychol.41, 49–100.

Muthén,L.K.,Muthén,B.O.,1998–2010.MplusUser’sGuide,sixthed.Muthén& Muthén,LosAngeles,CA.

Nigg,J.T.,Wong,M.M.,Martel,M.M.,Jester,J.M.,Puttler,L.I.,Glass,J.M.,Adams, K.M.,Fitzgerald,H.E.,Zucker,R.A.,2006.Poorresponseinhibitionasa predic-torofproblemdrinkingandillicitdruguseinadolescentsatriskforalcoholism andothersubstanceusedisorders.J.Am.Acad.ChildAdolesc.Psychiatry45, 468–475.

Oswald,D.P.,Best,A.M.,Coutinho,M.J.,Nagle,H.A.L.,2003.Trendsinthespecial educationidentificationratesofboysandgirls:acallforresearchandchange. Exceptionality11,223–237.

Pihl,R.O.,Peterson,J.,Finn,P.R.,1990.Inheritedpredispositiontoalcoholism: char-acteristicsofsonsofmalealcoholics.J.Abnorm.Psychol.99,291–301.

Peeters,M.,Wiers,R.,Monshouwer,K.,vandeSchoot,R.A.J.G.,Janssen,T.,Vollebergh, W.,2012. Automaticprocessesinat-riskadolescents: theroleof alcohol-approachtendenciesandresponseinhibitionindrinkingbehavior.Addiction 107,1339–1346.

Peeters,M.,Monshouwer,K.,Schoot,R.A.,Janssen,T.,Vollebergh,W.A.M.,Wiers, R.W.,2013.Automaticprocessesandthedrinkingbehaviorinearlyadolescence: aprospectivestudy.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.37,1737–1744.

Peeters,M.,Vollebergh,W.A.M.,Wiers,R.W.,Field,M.,2014.Psychologicalchanges andcognitiveimpairmentsinadolescentheavydrinkers.AlcoholAlcohol.49, 182–186.

Petrides,M.,Milner,B.,1982.Deficitsonsubject-orderedtasksafterfrontal-and temporal-lobelesionsinman.Neuropsychologia20,249–262.

Rubin,D.B.,1996.Multipleimputationafter18years.JAmStatAssoc91,473–489.

Stephens,D.N.,Duka,T.,2008a.Cognitiveandemotionalconsequencesofbinge drinking:roleofamygdalaandprefrontalcortex.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.363, 3169–3179.

Squeglia, L.M., Spadoni, A.D., Infante, M.A., Myers, M.G., Tapert, S.F., 2009.

Initiatingmoderatetoheavyalcoholusepredictschangesin neuropsychol-ogicalfunctioningforadolescentgirlsandboys.PsycholAddict.Behav.23, 715–722.

Squeglia,L.M.,Schweinsburg,A.D.,Pulido,C.,Tapert,S.F.,2011.Adolescentbinge drinkinglinkedtoabnormalspatialworkingmemorybrainactivation: differen-tialgendereffects.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.35,1831–1841.

Steinberg,L.,2007.Risktakinginadolescencenewperspectivesfrombrainand behavioralscience.Curr.DirectPsychol.Sci.16,55–59.

Stephens,D.N.,Duka,T.,2008b.Cognitiveandemotionalconsequencesofbinge drinking:roleofamygdalaandprefrontalcortex.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.B363, 3169–3179.

Sterne,J.A.,White,I.R.,Carlin,J.B.,Spratt,M.,Royston,P.,Kenward,M.G.,Wood,A.M., Carpenter,J.R.,2009.Multipleimputationformissingdatainepidemiological andclinicalresearch:potentialandpitfalls.Br.Med.J.338,b2393.

Stroop,J.R.,1935.Studiesofinterferenceinverbalreactions.J.Exp.Psychol.18, 643–662.

Tapert, S.F., Granholm, E., Leedy, N.G., Brown,S.A., 2002. Substance useand withdrawal:neuropsychologicalfunctioningover8yearsinyouth.J.Int. Neu-ropsychol.Soc.8,873–883.

Thush,C.,Wiers,R.W.,Ames,S.L.,Grenard,J.L.,Sussman,S.,Stacy,A.W.,2008. Inter-actionsbetweenimplicitandexplicitcognitionandworkingmemorycapacity inthepredictionofalcoholuseinat-riskadolescents.DrugAlcoholDepend.94, 116–124.

vanBuuren,S.,Boshuizen,H.C.,Knook,D.L.,1999.Multipleimputationofmissing bloodpressurecovariatesinsurvivalanalysis.Stat.Med.18,681–694.

(9)

146 M.Peetersetal./DevelopmentalCognitiveNeuroscience16(2015)139–146

vanBuuren,S.,Oudshoorn,C.,2000.MultivariateImputationbyChainedEquations. MICEV1.0User’sManual.TNOPreventieenGezondheid,Leiden.

Verdejo-García,A.,Lawrence,A.J.,Clark,L.,2008.Impulsivityasavulnerability markerforsubstance-usedisorders:reviewoffindingsfromhigh-riskresearch, problemgamblersandgeneticassociationstudies.Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev.32, 777–810.

Wechsler,H.,Nelson,T.F.,2001.BingedrinkingandtheAmericancollegestudents: What’sfivedrinks?Psychol.Addict.Beh.15,287–291.

Wiers,R.W.,Bartholow,B.D.,vandenWildenberg,E.,Thush,C.,Engels,R.C.,Sher,K.J., Grenard,J.,Ames,S.L.,Stacy,A.W.,2007.Automaticandcontrolledprocessesand thedevelopmentofaddictivebehaviorsinadolescents:areviewandamodel. Pharmacol.Biochem.Behav.86,263–283.

Zanotti,C.,Morgan,A.,Currie,D.,Looze,M.D.,Roberts,C.,Samdal,O.,Smith,O.R.F., Barnekow,V.,2012.SocialDeterminantsofHealthandWell-BeingAmongYoung People.WorldHealthOrganizationRegionalOfficeforEurope.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

transparent benchmarks. From here, I consider three important channels in order to be effective. First, the provision of independent analysis on fiscal policy issues. Second,

Hiphop en Globalisering hebben we gezien dat – ondanks de globale commercia- lisering van de mainstream hiphop – artiesten wereldwijd de Amerikaanse hiphop niet louter trachten

For the environmental condition munificence I find that underperforming firms who operate in munificent environments increase their risk taking when they rely on

The critical view towards the securitization process of the hands-off approach at WCIT-12 also echoed in the press, a New York Times editor critically analysed: “The

Het faunabeheer is op 3 niveaus geregeld: per provincie door de FaunaBeheerEenheid (FBE), per streek door de WildBeheerEenheden (WBE‘s) en landelijk door het Faunafonds voor

De voorbehandeling door middel van de draaien- Bij 6 keer voorbehandelen is een klein verschil te de borstel wordt blijkbaar door de koeien als sti- zien tussen de voor- en

If the publication is distributed under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license above, please follow below link for the End

Four main categories of executive functioning (inhibition, verbal working memory, visuospatial working memory, and shifting) are examined as mediators, to examine whether