• No results found

Satire or hard news, does not make a difference : a study about the effects of political satire on attitudes towards the refugee crisis with a moderating effect of watching satire in line with one’s political preference

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Satire or hard news, does not make a difference : a study about the effects of political satire on attitudes towards the refugee crisis with a moderating effect of watching satire in line with one’s political preference"

Copied!
35
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Satire or hard news, does not make a difference

A study about the effects of political satire on attitudes towards the refugee crisis with a

moderating effect of watching satire in line with one’s political preference or not

Cilia Meinen 10986162 Master Thesis

Graduate School of Communication

Master’s programme Communication Science L. Jacobs

28-06-19

Word count: 6509

Abstract

During the recent years, satire TV shows have increased in popularity. Especially, in the United States, several late-night shows exist that resemble news shows, but actually are a satire-based program for entertainment purposes. These late-night shows look like news shows and because of that resemblance, the audience often perceives these shows as an objective news source. The question is whether these satire shows can have an effect on individual’s attitudes and whether being exposed to a message that is in line with one’s political preference or not has an influence. This study examined these effects via conducting an online experiment, with four experimental conditions. However, no such effects were found when analyzing the results. These findings do have implications for society and for future research.

(2)

2

Introduction

During the recent years, satire TV shows have increased in popularity. Especially, in the United

States, several late-night shows exist that resemble news shows, but actually are a satire-based

program for entertainment purposes. Examples include The Late Show, Jimmy Kimmel Live,

The Daily Show and Full Frontal. However, this is exactly where it gets tricky. These late-night

shows look like news shows and because of that resemblance, the audience often perceives

these shows as an objective news source (Holbert, 2013). This may be problematic due to the

distinct goal attainment of satire. Unlike traditional news, satire shows are primarily meant to

entertain the audience instead of to inform it. The entertainment aspect has been shown to be

harmful to knowledge of particular groups of people since most of the time satire is meant to entertain and only portrays facts that fit the joke (Boukes, Boomgaarden, Moorman & de

Vreese, 2015). Still, this persuasive effect has proven to be minimal, making that individuals

would not be really misled (Holbert, 2013).

The effects of political satire on attitudes has been studied before. Prior research has shown that opinionated news can have an effect on citizens’ attitudes (Boukes, Boomgaarden,

Moorman & de Vreese, 2014; Lee, 2014). However, less research has been conducted

regarding the moderating role of political preference. Nevertheless, investigating the relationship

between satire and attitudes towards the addressed issue and the influence of political

preference is relevant. Selective exposure predicts that it is likely that citizens will gear more

towards information that is already in line with their political preference, as prior research has

shown (Iyengar & Hahn, 2009; Knobloch-Westerwick & Meng, 2009). It is therefore relevant to know whether messages that are not in line with one’s political preference can still have an

effect on citizens’ attitudes or not (Boukes et al., 2014). More interestingly, what has not been

studied in depth before, are the consequences of opinion formation when citizens are exposed

(3)

3 communication perspective, it is relevant to know how selective exposure plays a role in forming

attitudes. Disentangling the effects of being exposed to a satire message that is not in line with one’s predispositions, hence, contributes to extant knowledge by shedding more light on the

consequences of satire, suggesting its impact is conditional. The issue that is going to be

studied is the refugee crisis that burst out from spring 2015 onwards (Feldman & Borum

Chattoo, 2019). This crisis refers to issues that the European Union (EU) is dealing with right

now regarding refugees from the Middle East. It is called a crisis because the flow of refugees is

large and the European countries are divided by the way they should shelter them. The choice

for attitudes regarding the refugee crisis stems from the consideration that the issue had to be

somewhat controversial because then citizens are more likely to have an outspoken opinion

about the issue (Bischof & Rupnow, 2017). Hence, the main research question in this study is

as follows:

RQ: ‘To what extent does political satire have an influence on the attitudes of Dutch citizens

about the refugee crisis and what is the influence of political preference?’.

To test these effects, an online experiment will be conducted, with four experimental conditions.

The conditions consist of four different videos which will be left-wing satire, right-wing satire,

neutral satire and hard news. The videos will be similar to existing late-night shows which will

make it possible to reflect the results on reality.

Theoretical Framework

Political satire

Political satire is a form of political entertainment where a message about a political issue is

(4)

4 actors. They include the source of satire (the satirist), the recipient (the satiree) and the target of

the satire (the satirized). The satire is not received properly when the recipient is not in

agreement with the humor source. This means that when the satiree and the satirist are not

agreeing on the satirized, then the satire message would have less impact. Via entertainment

and humor, satire attracts attention to issues that would otherwise be ignored (Feldman &

Young, 2008). Moreover, when humor is used to bring a message about a controversial topic to

the table, people are less likely to counter-argue this message (Boukes et al., 2015; Nabi,

Moyer-Gusé & Byrne, 2007; Young, 2008).

Effects of political satire on attitudes

According to the Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986), the way

individuals process information is determined by two different routes: the central route and the

peripheral route. On one hand, when individuals take the central route to process information,

they really think about the message. They consider different arguments and evaluate these in a critical way. The extent to which someone considers a message is called ‘message elaboration’,

and this can result in attitude change. On the other hand, individuals can process information by

not really thinking in depth about a message, thereby accepting or ignoring a message without

taking a critical look at it. There is no message elaboration involved as part of this route. The

ELM provides a framework for the amount of message elaboration. This theory states that, this depends on various aspects, such as individuals’ involvement in the issue or, the product or

service that is being addressed in the message. Applying this framework to political satire, it

follows that the way people process an issue is likely to have an effect on their attitude

regarding that same issue. The question remains, however, whether political satire has the

ability to influence the way individuals process the message in comparison to individuals who

were not exposed to political satire. This theory in particular is highly relevant for the purpose of

(5)

5 recent studies have tried to replicate the findings by Petty and Cacioppo (1986). Cole, Ettenson,

Reinke and Schrader (1990) have found some conflicting results. They found little support for

the theory using an experimental design. In contrast, Kitchen, Kerr, Schultz, McColl and Pals

(2014) have found that the original model was outdated, arguing that extensions are necessary

to include effects of digital media, such as the internet.

Prior research, has also shown that when satire is too complex, individuals are less likely to

process the arguments given by the satirist in a critical way (Young, 2008). Still, Nabi et al.

(2007) have found that individuals process the arguments from soft news and political

entertainment sources not as thorough as from hard news or debates. In contrast, LaMarre and

Walther (2013) have found that individuals who watched a late-night show, reflected more about

the issue addressed on the show than individuals who watched political (hard) news on the

same issue. However, individuals who watched the late-night show may think more about the

issue compared to the individuals who watched the political news. Individuals who watched the

late-night show thought less about the underlying political message than individuals who

watched the political news. Boukes et al. (2014) have shown that news that is opinionated can affect individuals’ political attitudes, but that this a mediated effect by two other factors:

presumed media influence and hostile media responses.

The findings stated above deal with the way people process messages that are sent through

satire. As the ELM prescribes, the way individuals process a message, can affect the attitude

that person will have about the message they received. Therefore, these findings above could

have some implications on the question whether political satire can have an effect on

someone’s attitude towards the issue. Importantly, it seems that opinions are not formed in a

vacuum, but that it is relevant to take into account other factors as well. One of these relevant factors are citizens’ predispositions or their preexisting attitudes.

(6)

6 Political preference and selective exposure

As explained above, Simpson (2003) states that satire works best when the satirist and the satiree agree about the satirized. This would implicate that when individuals are exposed to

satire that is not in line with their own political preference, the satire has less effect. This implies

that when individuals are exposed to a piece of satire that is not in line with their political

preference, this would have no or less effect on their attitudes towards the issue compared to

when the satire is in line with their political preference. This can be partly explained by theories

regarding selective exposure and self-selection.

From the literature on public opinion formation, we know that political preferences are a key

predictor of various political attitudes and behaviours (LaMarre, Landreville & Beam, 2009). This

has been studied with regard to political satire as well. LaMarre, Landreville and Beam (2009)

studied the American late-night show of Stephen Colbert, The Colbert Report, and have found

that both conservatives and liberals found him equally funny. No significant difference was

present between the two different political ideologies. They argue that both groups see the

humor but perceive the intended message differently. More specifically, both groups interpret

the messages in a way that is in line with their own ideology. This implies that political preference does not have an effect on people’s attitudes concerning the message since

individuals will interpret the message in such a way that it is in line with their predispositions.

Another study that supports this implication is that of Knobloch-Westerwick (2012). She has

found that when individuals were exposed to political messages that were in line with their

preexisting attitudes, their attitudes where enhanced. However, exposure to counter attitudinal

messages had no significant effects. This again suggests that being exposed to a message that is not in line with one’s political preference has no influence on their attitudes.

(7)

7 Additionally, people have a tendency to look for information that they are already interested in and/or that is in line with their predispositions. This is called the ‘selective exposure hypothesis’.

This hypothesis suggests that if individuals are allowed to choose from a wide range of

information, they will select issues and information that are already in line with their own

interests and opinions (Dvir-Gvirsman, Garrett & Tsfati, 2018). Thus, when people have a

certain political ideology they are not likely to watch a late-night that has clearly a very different political view. This could be linked to Katz’ Uses and Gratifications Theory (Griffin, 2015). This

theory suggests that individuals use media for their own needs. If individuals only consume

satire to fulfill their own needs, the chances are high that they will not consume political satire

that is not in line with their political ideology.

Iyengar and Hahn (2009) have found that when comparing the news outlets preferred by

individuals with different political ideologies, the conservatives and republicans prefer to watch

news provided by Fox News and to avoid CNN and NPR. This is completely the other way

around for democrats and liberals: They prefer news from CNN and NPR and avoid Fox News.

This is in line with the theories presented above. Likewise, Knobloch-Westerwick and Meng

(2009) have found that individuals spend significantly more time on reading political information

in line with their already existing attitudes than on political information that was not in line with

their attitudes. It is also more common that individuals only read information that is in line with

their attitudes and information of both sides, than only reading counter attitudinal information.

These studies both implicate that people gear more towards information that is already in line

with their political preference, and that information that is in line with citizens pre-existing

attitudes will also yield a stronger effect.

(8)

8 The focus of this study is on satire and the effects it has on individuals’ attitudes towards the

issue that is discussed within the satire. The issue chosen to discuss is the refugee crisis in

Europe that burst out since 2015 (Feldman & Borum Chattoo, 2019). The refugee crisis refers to

the flow of refugees that is coming from the Middle East. The issue has become controversial

because several incidents happened, like stealing and harassing, where refugees were

involved. The decision to use this issue is based on two factors. First, it is a hotly debated and

salient issue in the Netherlands at this moment and it is a controversial and sensitive topic.

Therefore, Dutch citizens are likely to have a strong opinion about the refugee crisis where, they

either want to welcome and allow refugees or they want to close the gates. Often, there is not

really a clear in between, which makes it easier to manipulate the conditions in a right-wing and

a left-wing message. Second, it is common for satirists to use controversial topics, since it is

easier to make fun of more sensitive topics (Boukes et al., 2015; Nabi, Moyer-Gusé & Byrne,

2007; Young, 2008).

Based on the findings and theories stated above, the following hypotheses can be formulated. H1: ‘Watching a piece of satire has a stronger effect on attitudes towards the refugee crisis compared to watching a piece of hard news.’

H2: ‘The effect satire has on attitudes towards the refugee crisis will be stronger when the satire is in line with one’s political preference compared to satire that is not in line with one’s

political preference.’

Method

Strategy

The study will consist of an online experiment since a causal relationship is being measured. It

(9)

9 analysis (Gravetter & Forzano, 2012). In an experiment - the internal validity is higher because

the experiment is conducted in a controlled environment. If randomization is successful and the

sample size is sufficiently high, any changes in the outcome can only be due to the stimulus.

Still, external validity may be lower because the experiment is conducted in a controlled

environment (Gravetter & Forzano, 2012), which means that it may not be a situation that

occurs in real life making generalizability across a non-experimental setting uncertain. To

maximize external validity, however, in this study an online piece of political satire is used that is

quite similar to how people consume media in real life.

Sample

The final sample consisted of 146 participants (n = 146), 26 per cent of the participants were

male and 74 per cent were female (Mage = 25.96, SDage = 8.94). Because of ethical

considerations and the fact that citizens are only entitled to vote at or above the age of 18, all

respondents had to be (older than) 18. The respondents were recruited via social media, Whatsapp and e-mail. This type of sampling is called a ‘convenience sample’ because

respondents are recruited through own contacts on social media (Boeije, ’t Hart & Hox, 2009).

This may have a negative effect on the external validity because it is likely not to be

representative of the population, shedding doubt on the generalizability of the findings

(Gravetter & Forzano, 2012). Still, since this study does not focus on the means and is not concerned with providing population statistics, but instead focuses on the differences between

the groups following exposure to a stimulus, this is arguably less problematic.

Factorial design

In this study, a 2 (exposure to satire: satire vs. hard news) x 2 (political preference: in line with

political preference vs. not in line with political preference) within-between-subjects factorial design was used. Hence, the independent variable is ‘Exposure to satire’ and the moderator is

(10)

10 ‘In line with political preference’. The variable ‘Exposure to satire’ is operationalized as being

exposed to satire or not. In this case, not being exposed to satire means being exposed to a hard news message, which was as neutral as possible. The variable ‘In line with political

preference’ means that someone is exposed to a message that is in line with their political

preference or not, which has been measured in a pre-test. In terms of messages, a distinction

has been made between a left-wing and a right-wing message. Because there are two factors

which both consist of two conditions, the experiment exists of four conditions in total (satire

left-wing, satire right-left-wing, neutral satire and hard news). The design is a mixture of a

between-subjects and a within-between-subjects design. Every respondent was only exposed to one of the four

conditions, but refugee crisis attitudes were measured twice (before and after the stimulus) and

were then compared. The choice for a between-subjects design is because this eliminates the

chance of having to deal with carry over-effects (Gravetter & Forzano, 2012). This consideration

is relevant in this study because if a participant first watches the video that is in line with their

political preference, and after watches the video that is not in line with their political preference,

could have an effect on their attitude towards the refugee crisis. This could for example be more

negative if they first watch the video that is in line with their political preference than when they

only watch the video that is not in line with their political preference. The danger with the

within-subjects element of this study is that people might be primed because the questions will be

about the immigration crisis. This could possibly cause awareness with the participants because they could start focusing on this topic and know that that is what the research is about. This

could have an influence on the results of the study. Importantly, our mixed design approach also

means that the allocation to conditions is not fully random, as this is done on the basis of their

predispositions. Still, regarding all other characteristics, there should be no differences between

the participants in the various experimental conditions.

(11)

11 For the stimuli, four different videos were used which were edited by the author specifically for

the purpose of this study. Three of these videos consisted of a satirical message about the

refugee crisis. One was more left-wing, another one was more right wing and the third video

was neutral satire. The fourth video was a hard news story about the same issue which was as

neutral as possible. The setting of these videos was exactly the same with the same host and

background, only the spoken text was slightly different, adjusted to the condition. The hard news

video was also without jokes, since that is what makes it satire or not. The written text of the

stimuli materials is included in Appendix 1.

Pilot-study

To make sure the stimuli are correctly perceived by the participants, a pilot-study was

conducted. This was done by sending out a survey which consisted of eight videos. More

specifically, there were two different versions of stimulus material, and after every video the participants were asked to answer the same three questions. The first question was ‘where

would you place this video on the political spectrum?’, where the respondents could answer

through a slider for ‘0’ to ‘100’, where ‘0’ was the most left-wing, ‘50’ was neutral and ‘100’ was

the most right-wing. The next question was ‘What genre would you assign this video to?’, where

they could choose between ‘political entertainment’, ‘late-night talk show’, ‘political satire’, ‘hard

news (daily news, objective news)’ and ‘I don’t know’. The final question was ‘To what extent would you say that this video is political satire?’, where the respondents could answer through a

slider ranging from ‘0’ to ‘100’ where ‘0’ was ‘no satire at all’ and ‘100’ was ‘purely satire’.

Thirteen participants took part in this pilot study (69.2% female, Mage = 24.15, SDage =

9.06). All respondents watched all eight videos and answered all questions. To analyze this

data, the two corresponding videos were compared to assess which of the two versions presented the better manipulation. For the variables ‘political spectrum’ and level of satire’, a

(12)

12 were intended since both variables are interval/ratio variables. For the variable ‘genre’, a chi2

-test was done to see whether the two versions differentiated, since it is a nominal variable.

From the testing, the second version (video 2) turned out to be the best perceived as

political satire. There was a significant difference on this variable for all of the satire videos, only

the hard news video was in both versions perceived as no satire. Full results regarding the tests

are included in Table 2. When looking at where on the political spectrum individuals would place

the videos, only the right-wing videos differed significantly, t (12) = -2.88, p = 0.014, CI = [-4.93;

-2.88], d = 0.800. Comparing the means of the two versions shows that the second version was

perceived as more right-wing than the first version (M1 = 53.77, SD1 = 19.90; M2 = 73.92, SD2 =

20.16). These factors combined justify the decision to use version 2 for the actual experiment.

Variable Video 1 Left Video 1 Neutral Video 1 Right Video 1 Hard News Video 2 Left Video 2 Neutral Video 2 Right Video 2 Hard News Political Spectrum 40.23(21.00) 45.77(15.92) 53.77(19.90) 43.62(12.59) 29.15(20.01) 46.54(12.10) 73.92(20.16) 47.54(8.65) Level of satire 40.92(31.39) 42.00(27.59) 39.23(29.77) 13.00(14.73) 77.92(16.14) 71.08(20.72) 78.08(20.29) 8.92(14.04) Table 1

(13)

13 Dependent variable

In this study, ‘attitude towards the refugee crisis’ was the dependent variable. This variable was

measured using a preexisting scale to secure the internal validity, since these scales are already validated. ‘Attitude towards the refugee crisis’ was measured by using a scale

consisting of fifteen items (Stephan, Ybarra & Bachman, 1999). This was measured using a

7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). One of the items was: ‘Refugees should learn to conform to the rules and norms of the Dutch society as soon as

possible after they arrive’. The items were changed accordingly to fit the specific issue of the

refugee crisis. The full list of items can be found in Appendix 2. Items were reversed scored

when necessary to create a scale where higher scores meant a more negative attitude towards

refugees. Since a pre- and post-test was done, a difference score was calculated by extracting

the score of the pre-test from the score of the post-test. A negative score means that the

attitude towards refugees has become more negative compared to the pre-test. A more positive

score means that the attitude towards refugees has become more positive. A score of zero

indicates that the attitude has remained the same. Variable

Video 1&2 Left Video 1&2 Neutral Video 1&2 Right Video 1&2 Hard News Political spectrum 0.077 0.879 0.014 0.156 Genre 0.411 0.049 0.458 0.002 Level of satire 0.005 0.015 0.003 0.138 Table 2

(14)

14 A factor analysis and a reliability analysis were conducted to evaluate whether the scale

is valid and reliable. The factor analysis with Varimax rotation showed that two components

could be compiled since there were two main components with an Eigenvalue higher than one

(Eigenvalue = 4.74; Eigenvalue= 2.31). These components explain the variance for 19.85 per

cent and 19.72 per cent. There are also two smaller components with an Eigenvalue higher than

one (Eigenvalue = 1.35; Eigenvalue = 1.11). Since these two components were just barely

higher than one, and only three items loaded on those two components in total, those three

items were excluded. From the two components, seven items loaded on one component

(Highest = .77; Lowest = .56) and the other eight items loaded on the other component (Highest = .80; Lowest = .47). Next, the reliability analysis showed that the Cronbach’s Alpha was above

.80 on both components (α = .81; α = .80), making both scales reliable. Although there were two

scales according to the factor analysis, for this study it was relevant to detect differences

between the pre- and post-test of attitude, rather than the structure of the attitude towards refugees. Therefore, both scales have been taken together. When doing that, Cronbach’s Alpha

is above .80 (α = .82), thus reliable.

Randomization and covariates

Gender, age and level of education were taken up as covariates to check whether the results

are not caused or influenced by one of these factors. Comparisons were done to check if the distribution of these factors was equal between the different experimental conditions. Gender was measured through a multiple choice question where ‘male’ was coded as ‘0’, ‘female’ was

coded as ‘1’ and ‘other’ was coded as ‘2’. Age was measured as an open question and level of

education was measured as a multiple choice question with eight options (see Appendix 2 for

the exact operationalization). The test confirmed that there were no differences on the

covariates throughout the conditions for specific sociodemographic variables. For the satire

(15)

15 age (t (58) = 0.45, p = .653, CI [-3.65; 5.78]) and level of education (chi2 (3) = 3.40, p = .333). For the political preference conditions, no significant differences were found as well in terms of

gender (chi2 (1) = 0.00, p = .964), age (t (41) = 0.03, p = .978, CI [-4.89; 5.03]), level of education (chi2 (3) = 1.41, p = .702) and political ideology (t (41) = -0.53, p = .600, CI [-16.27; 9.52]). The randomization was successful.

Procedure

The experiment was conducted online. First, participants were presented with the inform consent which describes what will be done with the participant’s data and what their rights are.

After having read this, the participant could click on the ‘I agree’ button to proceed with the

experiment. If they opted for the ‘I do not agree’ button, the participant would be led towards the

end of the survey. After agreeing to participate, the participant was led to questions about their

demographic information and background variables, which included gender, age, level of

education, internet usage, social media usage and political interest. Next, the participant had to

answer questions as part of the pre-test of their attitude toward refugees. After that, a briefing

for the video appeared where they were instructed to turn on their volume since they were about

to watch a video. The participants were told that they could go to the next page after watching

the video. After watching the video, they received the questions as part of the post-test of their

attitude toward refugees. After these questions, the manipulation checks were asked. Finally, the participant was debriefed and thanked for their participation. The full survey is taken up in

Appendix 1 to 3.

Analytical strategy

In the following section, first the results of the manipulation checks will be reported and below

that the results of the analyzed data will be displayed. Hypothesis H1 expected that watching a

(16)

16 compared to watching a piece of hard news. To test this effect, a two-way ANOVA was done. In this analysis, ‘attitude towards refugees’ was the dependent variable and ‘satire condition’ was

the independent variable.

Hypothesis H2 expected that the effect satire has on attitudes towards the refugee crisis would be stronger when the satire is in line with one’s political preference compared to satire

that is not in line with one’s political preference. To test this effect, a two-way ANOVA will be

done. In this analysis, ‘attitude towards refugees’ is the dependent variable and ‘satire condition’

will be the independent variable, ‘political preference’ will be the moderator.

Results

Manipulation checks

To make sure that respondents perceived the videos correctly, a manipulation check was

conducted. There were three questions in the survey to check this. The first question was: ‘Were you able to watch and hear the video correctly?’. This was measured by a multiple choice

question with the options ‘yes’ and ‘no’. The participants who answered ‘no’ (5 participants)

where taken out of the study since they were not exposed to the stimulus material properly. The next question was: ‘To what extent would you assign the video as political satire?’. This

question was measured with a slider ranging from ‘0’ to ‘100’, where ‘0’ was ‘no satire at all’ and

‘100’ was ‘pure satire’. The last question was: ‘Where on the political spectrum would you place

the video?’. This question was also measured with a slider ranging from ‘0’ to ‘100’, where ‘0’

was ‘left-wing’, ‘50’ was ‘neutral’ and ‘100’ was ‘right-wing’.

To test this, a t-test was conducted since both questions - ‘to what extent do you think is

was satire’ and ‘where on the political spectrum would you place this video’ - were measured on

interval/ratio level.

From Levene’s test, it was shown that the groups are equal, F (57, 56.57) = 0.62, p =

(17)

[-17 50.38; -29.87]. The satire videos were perceived as more satire-like (M = 65.72, SD = 18.41)

than the non-satire or hard news condition (M = 25.60, SD = 20.79). This means that the

manipulation has worked.

Next, from Levene’s test, it was shown that the groups are equal, F (53, 51.50) = 0.70, p

= .408. A significant difference between the two groups was found, t (53) = -2.05, p = .045, CI

[-21.14; -0.24]. The left-wing video was perceived as more left-wing (M = 47.61, SD = 21.18) and

the right-wing video was perceived as more right-wing (M = 58.30, SD = 17.16). This means that

this manipulation worked as well.

Satire

Hypothesis H1 expected that watching a piece of satire has a stronger effect on attitudes towards the refugee crisis compared to watching a piece of hard news. Levene’s test is not

significant, F (1, 110) = 0.83, p = .482. This means that the variances between the groups are

not equal. No significant effect has been found between watching satire or not and change in attitude (n = 114), F (1, 110) = 0.00, p = .960, ƞ2 = .01. This means that individuals’ attitudes do

not change more or less when watching satire (M = 0.40, SD = 3.70) or when watching hard

news (M = 0.43, SD = 2.70). This means that the hypothesis cannot be supported.

In line with political preference

Hypothesis H2 expected that the effect of satire on attitudes towards the refugee crisis will be stronger when the satire is in line with one’s political preference compared to satire that is not in line with one’s political preference. Levene’s test is not significant, F (1, 110) = 0.83, p = .482.

This means that the variances between the groups are not equal. No significant effect has been

found between watching satire or not and change in attitude and the influence of watching that is in line with one’s political preference or not (n = 114), F (1, 110) = 0.00, p = .615, ƞ2 = .01.

(18)

18 in line with one’s political preference (M = 0.57, SD = 3.72) does not have an influence on the

effect of satire on one’s attitude. This means that the hypothesis cannot be supported.

Discussion

Conclusion

The goal of this study was to investigate whether watching political satire has an effect on individuals’ attitudes regarding the refugee crisis and if watching satire that is in line with one’s

political preference or not has an influence on these effects.

The results show that whether someone is watching satire or hard news does not change the attitude towards the refugee crisis of these individuals. This finding is in contrast

with prior research (Boukes et al., 2014; LaMarre & Walther, 2013; Nabi et al., 2007; Young,

2008). As discussed earlier, the Elaboration Likelihood Model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986) expects

that when watching satire, individuals would use the peripheral route to process the message

that is sent through the satire instead of the central route. This would mean that attitudes would

be formed based on emotions and without critical consideration, because satire plays in on the

emotions of its audience. The results of this study may implicate that satire does not evoke a

different way of message elaboration compared to hard news. On the other hand could the

results of this study depend on some limitations, which will be discussed below. Especially since

the results do not correspond with the prior studies it is necessary to consider these limitations. Variable

In line with pol pref

Not in line with pol pref

Satire No satire

Attitudes towards

refugee crisis 0.17(3.70) 0.57(3.72) 0.40(3.70) 0.43(2.70) Table 3

(19)

19 Moreover, there can be concluded that watching satire that is in line with one’s political

preference, or not, does not have an influence on the effects of satire on attitude. This means

that individuals who watch satire that is in line with their political preference do not show a

bigger or smaller change in attitude than individuals who watch satire that is not in line with their

political preference. As discussed before, this finding is in line with the study of LaMarre,

Landreville and Beam (2009). Their study showed that participants with different ideologies did

not differ in attitude when watching the same show. Their explanation was that individuals with

different political ideologies found the same content equally as funny, meaning that they would

process the information fitting with their preexisting attitudes. This could be an explanation for

the findings of this study as well, but there was no testing done in that area. It would be a

suggestion for future research to look into these effects more. This is partly in line with what

Knobloch-Westerwick (2012) found as well. She found no changes in attitudes when individuals

were exposed to content that was not in line with their preexisting attitudes. Looking back to the ‘selective exposure hypothesis’, it was discussed that the theory explains that when individuals

get to choose from a wide range of information, they will select topics or issues that are already

in line with their own interests and opinions (Dvir-Gvirsman, Garrett & Tsfati, 2018). Thus, when

people have a certain political ideology they are not likely to watch a late-night show that has a different political view. This could also be linked to Katz’ Uses and Gratifications Theory(Griffin,

2015). This theory also suggests that people use media for their own needs. When individuals only consume satire to fulfill their own needs, the chances will be that they will not consume

political satire that is not in line with their political ideology (Griffin, 2015). This would imply that

maybe these conditions created do not really occur in real life, which makes the generalizability

lower.

(20)

20 From the results, the conclusion is drawn that there may be no effect of political satire on

attitudes towards the refugee crisis and whether the message is in line with one’s political

preference may not have an influence on this. This has some implications for society as for

science. First of all, the results of this study implicate that watching satire or watching hard news

does not make a difference when looking at attitudes regarding the refugee crisis. So, when

satirists are looking to persuade their audiences, the findings suggest that this may not work

entirely the way the satirists plan it to work. In the case that the satirist wants to change the

attitudes of their audiences about the refugee crisis, they could be disappointed by the results.

On the other hand, for society as a whole, it may not be that bad that attitudes of citizens are not

that easily changeable, at least when talking about the refugee crisis. Would it be the case that

individuals were persuaded that easily, it would suggest that people do not really take a critical

look at a sensitive issue like the refugee crisis. As discussed above, the findings are not entirely

in line with the findings of prior research, which would suggest that there is a need to look into

this topic even further. Possibilities are that the results on both ends are a little bit more

nuanced.

Second, the results of this study do also implicate that watching satire that is in line with one’s political preference, or not, may not change citizens’ attitudes towards the refugee crisis.

This means that whether individuals get exposed to satire, the chances are low that whether

they agree with the satirist or not, does not make a difference in their attitudes, regarding

refugees at least. This means that when satirists want to enhance the attitudes towards the

refugee crisis of their audiences, for example, then they might get disappointed by the results as

well. This could be disadvantageous for the satirists, since satire is often used to, through

comedy and humor persuade people (Boukes et al., 2015; Nabi, Moyer-Gusé & Byrne, 2007;

Young, 2008). Since there is not much research done on these effects, it would be suggested

(21)

21 Limitations

The refugee crisis is a hotly debated and salient topic in the Netherlands. Therefore, citizens will

be more likely to have knowledge about the issue and there does not need to be too much

information in the survey to the point where they might get primed. This would also implicate

that most citizens will be on the same level of knowledge on this specific issue. On the other

hand, a possible limitation may be that people leave the experiment because of the

controversial topic, especially since there are going to be questions about people’s opinions

about this controversial topic (Gravetter & Forzano, 2012). Politics is a sensitive topic in general,

so this would be the case for most political topics. It is necessary to keep that in mind when

doing further research.

It should also be considered that this study did not take changeability of attitudes into

account. This could be an explanation why the attitudes of the respondents did not change

significantly. For further research it would be suggested to study the duration of media effects

with a small amount of stimuli.

The size of the sample used in this study could have had an influence on the results as well. A small sample size can lead to no generalizability to the entire population, which results in

lower external validity.

Moreover, due to the convenience-sample, highly-educated participants were over

represented. These citizens are known to be less susceptible to media effects, and could be

more involved with and better informed about politics.

Lastly, it is hard to make an accurate duplicate of a late-night talk show, especially since the materials were not developed in collaboration with real satirists. This means that it is

possible that the materials did not resemble a real late-night talk show. It would be suggested

for future research to take this into account as well.

(22)

22 Bischof, G., & Rupnow, D. (2017). Migration in Austria. Retrieved from

http://www.oapen.org/

Boeije, H., Hart, ‘t, H., & Hox, J. (2009). Onderzoeksmethoden. Den Haag, NL: Boom

Lemma uitgevers.

Boukes, M., Boomgaarden, H. G., Moorman, M., & De Vreese, C. H. (2014). News with an

attitude: Assessing the mechanisms underlying the effects of opinionated news. Mass Communication and Society, 17(3), 354–378. DOI: 10.1080/15205436.2014.891136

Boukes, M., Boomgaarden, H. G., Moorman, M., & de Vreese, C. H. (2015). At odds:

laughing and thinking? The appreciation, processing, and persuasiveness of political satire.

Journal of Communication, 65(5), 721-744. DOI: 10.1111/jcom.12173

Cole, C., Ettenson, R., Reinke, S., & Schrader, T. (1990). The Elaboration Liklihood

Model (ELM): Replications, extensions and some conflicting findings. Advances in Consumer Research, 17, 231–236. Retrieved from www. https://web-b-ebscohost-com

Dvir-Gvirsman, S., Garrett, R. K., & Tsfati, Y. (2018). Why do partisan audiences

participate? Perceived public opinion as the mediating mechanism. Communication Research, 45(1), 112–136. DOI: 10.1177/0093650215593145

Feldman, L., & Borum Chattoo, C. (2019). Comedy as a route to social change: The effects of satire and news on persuasion about syrian refugees. Mass Communication and Society, 22(3), 277–300. DOI: 10.1080/15205436.2018.1545035

Feldman, Lauren, & Young, D. G. (2008). Late-night comedy as a gateway to traditional

news: An analysis of time trends in news attention among late-night comedy viewers during the 2004 presidential primaries. Political Communication, 25(4), 401–422. DOI:

10.1080/10584600802427013

Gravetter, F.J., & Forzano, L.B. (2012). Research methods for the behavioral sciences.

(23)

23 Griffin, E. (2015). Uses and Gratifications of Elihu Katz. In J. W. J. Beentjes, & R. J. W. van der Wurff (Eds.), Inleiding Communicatiewetenschap (pp. 87–96). New York, USA:

McGraw-Hill.

Holbert, R. L. (2013). Developing a normative approach to political satire: An empirical perspective. International journal of communication (Online), 7, 305–323. Retrieved from

http://go.galegroup.com

Iyengar, S., & Hahn, K. S. (2009). Red media, blue media: Evidence of ideological selectivity in media use. Journal of Communication, 59(1), 19–39. DOI:

10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.01402.x

J. Kitchen, P., Kerr, G., E. Schultz, D., McColl, R., & Pals, H. (2014). The Elaboration

Likelihood Model: Review, critique and research agenda. European Journal of Marketing, 48(11/12), 2033–2050. DOI: 10.1108/EJM-12-2011-0776

Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2012). Selective exposure and reinforcement of attitudes and partisanship before a presidential election. Journal of Communication, 62(4), 628–642. DOI:

10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01651.x

Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Meng, J. (2009). Looking the other way. Communication Research, 36(3), 426–448. DOI: 10.1177/0093650209333030

LaMarre, H. L., Landreville, K. D., & Beam, M. A. (2009). The irony of satire: Political

ideology and the motivation to see what you want to see in The Colbert Report. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 14(2), 212–231. DOI: 10.1177/1940161208330904

LaMarre, H. L., & Walther, W. (2013). Ability matters: Testing the differential effects of

political news and late-night political comedy on cognitive responses and the role of ability in micro-level opinion formation. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 25(3), 303–322.

(24)

24 Lee, F. L. F. (2014). The impact of online user-generated satire on young people’s political

attitudes: Testing the moderating role of knowledge and discussion. Telematics and Informatics, 31(3), 397–409. DOI: 10.1016/j.tele.2013.08.002

McCombs, M., & Reynolds, A. (2009). How the news shapes our civic agenda. In J. Bryant, & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media Effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 1–16). New York,

United States: Routledge.

Molz, G., & Stiller, M. (2019). Attitudes and opinions about refugees in Germany –

correlates with conspiracy and political mindsets. Current Psychology, , 1–10. DOI:

10.1007/s12144-018-0108-0

Nabi, R. L., Moyer-Gusé, E., & Byrne, S. (2007). All joking aside: A serious investigation

into the persuasive effect of funny social issue messages. Communication Monographs, 74(1), 29–54. DOI: 10.1080/03637750701196896

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and

peripheral routes to attitude change. New York, Verenigde Staten: Springer-Verlag.

Simpson, P. (2003). On the discourse of satire. Philadelphia, United States: John

Benjamins Publishing Company.

Stephan, W. G., Ybarra, O., & Bachman, G. (1999). Prejudice toward immigrants. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29(11), 2221–2237. Retrieved from www.researchgate.net

Young, D. G. (2008). The privileged role of the late-night joke: Exploring humor's role in

disrupting argument scrutiny. Media Psychology, 11(1), 119-142. DOI:

(25)

25

Appendix 1

Texts of stimulus materials (translation from Dutch to English)

1. Satire left-wing

Intro: And then this, refugee shelters are empty.

Video 1: NOS Journaal video is shown, talks about vacant refugee shelters in The Netherlands.

Host: So, this is what we’re dealing with. A lot of people we scared that there would be no room

to shelter refugees, but it turns out that some of the shelters are empty. Question is: what to do

with this?

Video 2: people in the streets are asked their opinions about the refugee crisis, one guy is

vague. This gives the host room to make a joke.

Host: *Sarcastic tone of voice* Exactly! What are the media even talking about, there are only about ten refugees. Last week I heard there was a girl missing but I didn’t see her either.

But anyway, how is it possible that these shelters are empty?

Video 3: Video explains that in essence The Netherlands (and other European countries) didn’t

(26)

26 Host: Yes, The Netherlands promised to adopt 6.000 refugees, but they only took 2.300. This is

like you are opening a hotel which fits 1.000 guests to only rent out ten rooms. *host acts

jokingly*

But, this is a serious issue. *clip of Dutch politician who says “I can’t believe it, this is serious’ is

inserted* *reacts to clip* Yes, calm down. The Netherlands does make room for 6.000 refugees

but only adopts one third. So I say: Mark Rutte, keep your promises!

2. Neutral satire

Intro: And then this, refugee shelters are empty.

Video 1: NOS Journaal video is shown, talks about vacant refugee shelters in The Netherlands.

Host: So, this is what we’re dealing with. A lot of people we scared that there would be no room

to shelter refugees, but it turns out that some of the shelters are empty. Question is: what to do

with this?

Video 2: people in the streets are asked their opinions about the refugee crisis, one guy is vague. This gives the host room to make a joke.

Host: *Sarcastic tone of voice* Exactly! What are the media even talking about, there are only about ten refugees. Last week I heard there was a girl missing but I didn’t see her either.

(27)

27 Video 3: Video explains that in essence The Netherlands (and other European countries) didn’t

adopt as many refugees as agreed.

Host: Yes, The Netherlands promised to adopt 6.000 refugees, but they only took 2.300. That means a lot of money is wasted on empty shelters. *Joking tone of voice* That’s like paying for

a school trip knowing you are not even going, because you’re not even going to school there!

But, this is a serious issue. *clip of Dutch politician who says “I can’t believe it, this is serious’ is

inserted* *reacts to clip* Yes, calm down. The Netherlands needs to sort out their stuff, fulfill your agreements, or don’t agree to them in the first place.

3. Satire right-wing

Intro: And then this, refugee shelters are empty.

Video 1: NOS Journaal video is shown, talks about vacant refugee shelters in The Netherlands.

Host: So, this is what we’re dealing with. A lot of people we scared that there would be no room

to shelter refugees, but it turns out that some of the shelters are empty. Question is: what to do with this?

Video 2: people in the streets are asked their opinions about the refugee crisis, one guy is

vague. This gives the host room to make a joke.

Host: *Sarcastic tone of voice* Exactly! What are the media even talking about, there are only about ten refugees. Last week I heard there was a girl missing but I didn’t see her either.

(28)

28 But anyway, how is it possible that these shelters are empty?

Video 3: Video explains that in essence The Netherlands (and other European countries) didn’t

adopt as many refugees as agreed.

Host: Yes, The Netherlands promised to adopt 6.000 refugees, but they only took 2.300. *Sarcastic tone of voice* You aren’t going to open a hotel in the far corner of Friesland that fits

1.000 people. *Host makes joke*

But, this is a serious issue. *clip of Dutch politician who says “I can’t believe it, this is serious’ is

inserted* *reacts to clip* Yes, calm down. So, The Netherlands agrees on things they don’t want

to agree on, and the taxpayer has to pay for it. And that, has to stop!

4. Hard news

Intro: Refugee shelters are empty.

Video 1: NOS Journaal video is shown, talks about vacant refugee shelters in The Netherlands.

Host: So, this is what we’re dealing with. A lot of people we scared that there would be no room

to shelter refugees, but it turns out that some of the shelters are empty. Question is: how is this

possible?

Video 3: Video explains that in essence The Netherlands (and other European countries) didn’t

(29)

29 Host: The Netherlands promised to adopt 6.000 refugees, but they only took 2.300. The press

(30)

30

Appendix 2

Measurements

Demographics

1. What is your gender?

a. Female

b. Male

c. Other

2. What is your age?

[measured in years]

3. What is your highest level of education (Dutch system)?

a. Basisonderwijs (Primary school)

b. Lager beroeps onderwijs (lbo) (Lower practical eductation)

c. Lager voortgezet onderwijs (vmbo, mavo) (Highschool, lower level)

d. Middelbaar beroepsonderwijs (mbo) (College, lower level)

e. Hoger voortgezet onderwijs (havo, vwo) (Highschool, higher level)

f. Hoger beroepsonderwijs (hbo) (College)

g. Wetenschappelijk onderwijs (wo) (University)

h. No education

Filler questions

1. To what extend are you interested in politics?

0 --- 100

Not interested at all Very interested

(31)

31 a. Never

b. Daily

c. Weekly

d. Monthly

3. How often do you use social media?

a. Never/I do not have social media accounts

b. Daily

c. Weekly

d. Monthly

Self-reported political orientation

Where on the political spectrum would you place yourself?

0 --- 100

Left Right

Attitudes pre- and posttest

Answers were recorded using a 7-point Likertscale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7

(strongly agree).

1. Refugees should learn to conform to the rules and norms of the Dutch society as soon

as possible after they arrive.

2. The refugee crisis is undermining the Dutch culture.

3. The values and believes of refugees regarding work are basically quite similar to those

of most Dutchmen.

4. The values and beliefs of refugees regarding moral and religious issues are not

(32)

32 5. The values and believes of refugees regarding family issues and socializing children are

basically quite similar to those of most Dutchmen.

6. The values and beliefs of refugees regarding social relations are not compatible with the

beliefs and values of most Dutchmen.

7. Refugees should not have to accept Dutch ways.

8. Refugees get more from this country than they contribute.

9. The children of refugees should have the same right to attend public schools in The

Netherlands as the Dutch do.

10. The refugee crisis has increased the tax burden on Dutchmen.

11. Refugees are not displacing Dutch workers from their jobs.

12. Refugees should be eligible for the same health-care benefits received by Dutchmen.

13. Social services have become less available to Dutchmen because of the refugee crisis.

14. The quality of social services available to Dutchmen has remained the same, despite the

refugee crisis.

15. Refugees are entitled to subsidized housing or subsidized utilities (water, sewage,

electricity) as poor Dutchmen are.

Exposure checks

1. Where you able to see and hear the video correctly? a. Yes

b. No

Manipulation checks

2. To what extent would you assign the video as political satire?

0 --- 100

(33)

33 3. Where on the political spectrum would you place the video?

0 ---50--- 100

(34)

34

Appendix 3

Debriefing per condition

Condition: Satire left-wing

Debriefing

You almost reached the end of the survey. It is important to mention that the video you just

watched is not based on real facts. The video was developed specially for this experiment and

does not contain real facts or opinions. This purpose of this study was to measure the influence of political satire on one’s attitude. The video you just watched was meant to imitate a satire

piece similar to satire shows as Zondag met Lubach, with a left-wing angle.

Condition: Neutral satire

Debriefing

You almost reached the end of the survey. It is important to mention that the video you just

watched is not based on real facts. The video was developed specially for this experiment and

does not contain real facts or opinions. This purpose of this study was to measure the influence of political satire on one’s attitude. The video you just watched was meant to imitate a satire

piece similar to satire shows as Zondag met Lubach, with a neutral angle.

Condition: Satire right-wing

Debriefing

You almost reached the end of the survey. It is important to mention that the video you just

watched is not based on real facts. The video was developed specially for this experiment and

does not contain real facts or opinions. This purpose of this study was to measure the influence of political satire on one’s attitude. The video you just watched was meant to imitate a satire

(35)

35

Condition: Hard News

Debriefing

You almost reached the end of the survey. It is important to mention that the video you just

watched is not based on real facts. The video was developed specially for this experiment and

does not contain real facts or opinions. This purpose of this study was to measure the influence of political satire on one’s attitude. The video you just watched was meant to imitate a news item

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

experiencing a rollercoaster, regardless of whether this was in VR 6DoF or VR 3DoF, did not cause a change in participants’ score for attraction. Thus, both Hypothesis 1a and 1b

(…) Because what this course is giving you, is about the normal life. What is happening in the life somehow. So if you are already in the society, like for me, I guess, better than

Moreover, this work examines the moderating effects of technological cooperation and home country governmental subsidies on the underlying relationship between

Specifically, it considers (1) a pegged exchange rate regime; (2) central bank’s flow-of-fund constraint to reflect the role of government intervention in the

It has been reported that an artificial 2D dispersive electronic band structure can be formed on a Cu(111) surface after the formation of a nanoporous molecular network,

The aims of this study were to assess what improvement in travel time could be made by Genetic Algorithms (GA) compared with random delivery route solutions, and to assess how

The results of the first stage of the demonstrator process, of both a metamodel and a finite element model, are propagated to the second stage finite element model for a new set

Het doel van hoofdstuk 7 en 8 was om het entraineren in natuurlijke omstandigheden beter te begrijpen door vast te stellen wat het effect van seizoen (fotoperiode) en week structuur