• No results found

Green versus conventional product placement and disclosures : the positive effect of greenness

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Green versus conventional product placement and disclosures : the positive effect of greenness"

Copied!
53
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

Green versus Conventional Product Placement and Disclosures: The Positive Effect of Greenness

Anja Krafczyk

University of Amsterdam

Master’s Thesis

Graduate School of Communication Master’s program Communication Science

Supervisor: Dr. Marijn Meijers

Anja Krafczyk

Student no.: 10965718

(2)

Abstract

This study examined the influence of green brands compared to conventional brands in product placement on warmth, persuasion knowledge, resistance and brand attitude.

Additionally, the effect of sponsorship disclosures was studied. An experiment (N = 155) was executed and showed that a green product placement compared to conventional product placement positively affects brand attitudes, which is mediated by an increase in brand

warmth. Besides, no support was found that green product placement activates less persuasion knowledge than conventional product placement. However, it could be confirmed that

attitudinal persuasion knowledge, but not conceptual persuasion knowledge, is positively associated with resistance. Despite finding this association, less resistance did not positively affect brand attitudes. In addition, no support was found for the assumed negative effect of disclosure on brand attitude through a higher activation of persuasion knowledge and

resistance. As this study is the first that examined the effect of green product placement, it is a great contribution to product placement research and green advertising literature in general.

Keywords: product placement, green brands, disclosures, warmth, persuasion knowledge, resistance

(3)

Green versus Conventional Product Placement and Disclosures: The Positive Effect of Greenness

Imagine you are sitting in front of your television watching the television series House of Cards and you notice Zoe Barnes (one of the characters) eating Ben & Jerry’s. The just described setting is called product placement: the placement of a brand in non-commercial content such as a television series or movie (Karrh, 1998). Marketers use this technique for the purpose of advertising brands in a more natural fashion, hoping to avoid the activation of persuasion knowledge and as such influencing brand attitudes positively (Karrh, 1998; Avery & Ferraro, 2000; Russel, 2002). The point of interest in the just mentioned example lies on the brand (Ben & Jerry’s) which is a green brand. Green brands mainly differ from conventional brands such as Heineken in their connection to environmental friendliness and sustainability (Hartmann, Ibáñez & Sainz, 2006). Does it therefore work different when a green brand is used in product placement instead of a conventional brand? Until now, no previous study has investigated the effects of green product placement. Based on that, the present study tries to fill a research gap focusing on green product placement.

It is expected that the placement of green brands works better (i.e. more persuasive) than the placement of conventional brands. On one hand, because research has shown that people have a more positive attitude towards green brands than conventional brands

(Schuhwerk & Lefkoff-Hagius, 1995; Kong & Zhang, 2013). People perceive green brands as more ethical, trustworthy and warmer than conventional brands due to their association with goodwill (Chernev & Blair, 2015). Therefore, a similar positive effect for green brands compared to conventional brands is expected when using them in product placement.

On the other hand, green product placement may also work due to a different

underlying process that concerns the perception of advertising. In 2012 the company “Green Product Placement” integrated green brands into movies for the first time (Green Product Placement, 2015). Due to the novelty of green product placement, people rarely know this

(4)

type of advertising and are therefore probably less aware of its persuasive intent (Friestad & Wright, 1994; Tutaj & van Reijmersdal, 2012). As a result, it may be argued that people show less resistance towards green product placement than towards conventional product placement which benefits subsequent evaluations (Friestad & Wright, 1994; Campbell & Kirmani, 2008; Fransen, Smit & Verlegh, 2015). Taken together this may lead to more positive brand

attitudes with green product placement than with conventional product placement.

However, in the last years, programs have to disclose when using product placement (Avery & Ferrao, 2000; Cain, 2011). The inclusion of disclosures in television series or movies has shown to make the persuasive intent of the product placement more apparent (Boerman, van Reijmersdal & Neijens, 2012). If this is translated to the current situation, the positive effect of green product placement compared to conventional product placement due to a lower perception of the persuasive intent might disappear with disclosures.

Based on these processes, it is expected that green product placement works better due to brand warmth on one side and a lower activation of persuasion knowledge and resistance on the other. However, when the product placement will be disclosed, green product

placement will only profit from brand warmth and no longer from a lower activation of persuasion knowledge and resistance. Nevertheless, green product placement will still be evaluated as more positive than conventional product placement, the effect will only be less pronounced when the product placement is disclosed.

The research gap around green product placement provides relevance to address this topic in the present study. Furthermore, enhancing the awareness for green brands and

increasing more positive attitudes towards green brands underlies the societal relevance of the present study. When this can be achieved, green behavior probably increases which can help conquering global warming which is a great issue. For this purpose the following research question will be addressed:

(5)

RQ: What is the influence of green product placement versus conventional product

placement on consumer’s attitude towards the brand? How do warmth, persuasion knowledge, and resistance mediate this relationship? And how does sponsorship disclosure moderate the effect of the mediators persuasion knowledge and resistance on consumer’s attitude towards the brand?

Theoretical Background

Product Placement of Conventional versus Green Brands

Product placement is a more unobtrusive form of advertising brands and its products than traditional print and television advertising (Balasubramanian, 1994; Hackley, Tiwsakul & Preuss, 2008). It is originally and more correctly called brand placement which refers to its definition: the placement of a brand in a mass media program that can be a television series or movie (Karrh, 1998; Russel, 2002). More and more companies use the product placement technique instead of traditional forms of advertising (Hackley et al., 2008). The huge hype around product placement is based on the idea that it is perceived less as advertising, because the brand is embedded in non-advertising content (Hackley et al., 2008). With this advantage of native advertising, companies try to increase the awareness of their brands while breaking through the clutter of traditional advertising and overcoming consumers’ avoidance behavior towards advertising (Roehm, Roehm & Boone, 2004; Rotfeld, 2006; Wojdinski, 2016).

Researchers have paid attention to product placement for more than two decades and have studied its effects on factors such as memory and evaluations of the brand placed (Nebenzahl & Secunda, 1993; Karrh, 1998; Russel, 2002; van Reijmersdal, Neijens & Smit, 2007; Verhellen, Dens & de Pelsmacker, 2015). Up to date the brands that are part of these studies belong to the category conventional brands, which may be explained by the fact that

(6)

conventional brands are commonly used in product placement (Nelson & McLeod, 2005; Sutherland et al., 2010). An example for the use of conventional brands in product placement would be James Bond drinking Heineken in the movie Skyfall. Consumers are already more or less used to seeing conventional brands such as Heineken, Coca Cola, or Pepsi in movies nowadays.

Next to the category of conventional brands, another type of brands exists: green brands. Organic and biological brands such as the ice-cream brand Ben & Jerry’s, the Dutch chocolate brand Tony’s Chocolonely, or the German refreshment drink brand Bionade are green brands. Product placement of green brands, so called green product placement, first emerged in 2012 when Beth Bell, the founder of the company “Green Product Placement”, started integrating green brands into films (Green Product Placement, 2015). Due to the novelty of green product placement, there is a lack of research regarding this practice. The necessity to study the effects of green product placement besides conventional product placement is supported by van Reijmersdal et al. (2007). The authors emphasize that the results concerning product placement are always related to the specific brand used that leads to the assumption that green product placement probably shows different effects than

conventional product placement does.

There are two main differences between conventional product placement and green product placement that could explain the hypothesized differential effects: firstly, green product placement is a more novel practice than conventional product placement and

secondly, green product placement has a greater connection to environmental friendliness and sustainability than conventional product placement. Concerning the novelty of green product placement, research has shown that novel advertising techniques are evaluated more

positively than well-known advertising techniques (Stafford & Stafford, 2002; Dahlén 2005; Dahlén & Edenius, 2007; Sheinin, Varki & Ashley, 2011). People are less likely to perceive communication as advertising when they see or hear it for the first time compared to

(7)

communication they encounter several times per day. In consequence the overall negative associations people have with advertising, such as the negative feeling towards a persuasive attempt, are less likely to get activated with novel advertising techniques (Dahlén & Edenius, 2007; Tutaj & van Reijmersdal, 2012). As green product placement is a more novel technique compared to conventional product placement, it can be expected that green product placement profits from its novelty factor and will therefore be perceived as more positive than

conventional product placement.

The second distinction that can be made between green product placement and conventional product placement refers to the brands connection with environmental friendliness and sustainability. Whereas the company’s goal of a conventional brand is to reach a high return on investment (ROI), the company’s ultimate goal of a green brand is to take responsibility for the society (Davis, 1960; Perrow, 1961; Chernev & Blair, 2015). The contribution to social and environmental well-being can be expressed in various ways: the reduction of CO2 emission, the use of recyclable packaging, the use of natural or organic

materials from local farmers, the investment in projects or initiatives that strive to stop global warming et cetera (Dennis, Neck & Goldsby, 1998; Newell & Paterson, 2010). All these actions express the so-called Corporate Social Responsibility (CSR) of a company (Dennis et al., 1998). CSR is an important part of a company that markets green brands, but not of a company that deals with conventional brands. The connection of green brands with CSR could exhibit an advantage compared to conventional brands, because most people appreciate when organizations take responsibility for society (Chernev & Blair, 2015; Juwaheer,

Pudaruth & Noyaux, 2012). Consumer’s positive attitude towards CSR was even strengthened by the issue of global warming, because CSR is one form of taking a much needed step

towards slowing down global warming (Grimmer & Woolley, 2014).

The association of green brands along with a company’s goodwill is likely to lead to a positive evaluation of the brand itself. As conventional brands lack this association, green

(8)

brands are likely to be evaluated as more positive than conventional brands. This view is supported by research that compared print advertising of green brands with print advertising of conventional brands (Schuhwerk & Lefkoff-Hagius, 1995; Kong & Zhang, 2013). Most of the time, print advertising of a green brands is evaluated as more positive than print

advertising of a conventional brands. Print advertising research showed that people have a more positive brand attitude towards green brands than towards conventional brands and that green advertising in general elicits positive effects (Schuhwerk & Lefkoff-Hagius, 1995; Kong & Zhang, 2013; Hartmann et al., 2006; Grimmer & Woolley, 2014; Huang, Yang & Wang, 2014; Nagar, 2015). Based on the preceding reasoning concerning the novelty factor of product placement using green brands as well as its association with CSR the following hypothesis will be tested:

H1: Green product placement will lead to a more positive brand attitude than

conventional product placement.

The fact that green brands are expected to be evaluated more positively than

conventional brands when used in product placement could be explained by brand warmth. As such, this might be an important mediator if the expected effect of green product placement versus conventional product placement on brand attitude can be confirmed.

Brand Warmth

Warmth is defined as a positive, mild, volatile emotion (Aaker, Stayman & Hagerty, 1986). The emotion warmth is often used to describe a person such as referring to someone as a “warm person” (Kervyn, Fiske and Malone, 2012). Besides using the term to describe a person, warmth is also used to describe entities such as brands: brand warmth. The fact that people can perceive brands as warm and that warmth is actually commonly used to express

(9)

ones feelings towards a brand is discussed by Kervyn et al. (2012). The authors developed a framework to illustrate with which attributes people express ones evaluation of a certain brand: The Brands as Intentional Agents Framework (BIAF). The BIAF is based on the assumption that people rate a brand according to its good intentions (besides rating a brand on its ability to realize its intention, i.e. competence); a brand can have a good intention (i.e. well-intentioned) such as protecting the environment or a bad intention (i.e. ill-intentioned) such as making profit. Kervyn et al. (2012) showed that well-intentioned brands are perceived as warmer than ill-intentioned brands. As a green brand is more likely to be perceived as a well-intentioned brand than a conventional brand, because of the company’s motive to do something that benefits society as a whole or at least does not damage, it can be expected that a green brand will be perceived as warmer than a conventional brand.

Besides illustrating the different relations between distinct brand types (green versus conventional) and brand warmth with the BIAF, the expectation that green brands are

perceived as warmer than conventional brands can also be explained by means of the spillover effect. The spillover or halo effect is basically a bias of evaluations (Thorndike, 1920;

Beckwith & Lehmann, 1975). For example, cookies labelled “organic” are evaluated as healthier than conventional cookies. The reason for this misperception is that people infer from the organic label to the caloric content of the cookies (Schuldt & Schwarz, 2010; Schuldt & Hannahan, 2013). In other words: the overall positive impression of an object (i.e. organic cookies) is transferred to subsequent evaluations related with the object (i.e. caloric content) (Thorndike, 1920; Schuldt & Hannahan, 2013).

Earlier in this paper it was discussed that a company promoting green brands engages in CSR, while a company promoting conventional brands lacks this engagement. A company that engages in CSR is evaluated more positively than a company that does not engage in CSR in the sense that it is rated as more trustworthy, more ethical and warmer (Chernev & Blair, 2015). If we look at the perception of warmth towards a company that engages in CSR

(10)

activities and the concept brand warmth, it seems likely that these are interconnected: the feeling of warmth towards a company (that engages in CSR) probably spills over to the company’s brands (i.e. green brands) which leads to the perception of warmth towards green brands. To summarize, it is likely that green brands will be perceived as warm, but

conventional brands will not be perceived as warm. When using green brands versus conventional brands in product placement, the following is expected:

H2: Green brands that use product placement will be perceived as warmer than

conventional brands that use product placement.

In consequence, it can be expected that the perception of brand warmth has a positive effect on brand attitudes, as brand warmth depicts a positive evaluation of a brand. Bernritter, Verlegh and Smit (2016) showed in a social media study that non-profit brands are perceived as warmer than for-profit brands. The perception of warmth subsequently influenced the consumer’s behavior which was shown in an increase in “likes” on the social media platform Facebook (Bernritter et al., 2016). A beneficial effect due to a perception of brand warmth is also expected in the present study:

H3: Brand warmth will mediate the positive effect of green product placement versus

conventional product placement on brand attitude.

After discussing the positive path from product placement via brand warmth to brand attitude, in the next section another path will be sketched that takes a critical look on the effectiveness (i.e. persuasiveness) of product placement.

(11)

Persuasion Knowledge

The persuasiveness of communication strongly depends on the extent to which the receiver perceives it to be intended to be persuasive (Friestad & Wright, 1994). Persuasion knowledge helps the receiver to recognize whether communication is meant to persuade and subsequently helps the receiver to cope with the persuasive attempt. Friestad & Wright (1994) define persuasion knowledge as an overall comprehension of persuasion and an expertise in coping with persuasive attempts. When persuasion knowledge is activated, the effect of advertising is weakened. This process is consolidated in the Persuasion Knowledge Model (Friestad & Wright, 1994).

When looking at research concerning product placement (of conventional brands), one can see that the native advertising technique product placement can activate persuasion knowledge (Russel, 2002; Verhellen et al., 2015; Wojdinski, 2016). For example, Russel (2002) showed that product placement is perceived as a persuasive attempt when the brand is not perfectly integrated in the movie and therefore appears unnatural. Furthermore, Verhellen et al. (2015) showed that a prominent product placement decreases its persuasiveness. The effect of green product placement on persuasion knowledge has not yet been researched. Based on research concerning conventional product placement, a possible effect of green product placement will be discussed. This will be done by comparing conventional to green product placement.

As mentioned before, the persuasion knowledge of a person is of great relevance for the effectiveness of communication, such as product placement, and it develops with experience. It can be expected that people have quite some experience with conventional product placement, because it has been practiced by marketers for approximately 35 years and the amount of brands placed since then is quite substantial (Babin, 1996). For example, just in the last year, 430 brands were placed in the 31 top movies of 2015 in the United States (Sauer, 2016). The opposite (i.e. low experience) is probably true for green product placement, as it is

(12)

a novel technique that has been practiced for no longer than 4 years and counts 170

productions and 65 brands in total (Green Product Placement, 2015). Based on these facts, it can be expected that people have more persuasion knowledge about conventional product placement than about green product placement. In summary, it can be expected that a person will less likely perceive the placement of a green brand as a persuasive attempt, than the placement of a conventional brand. Based on the previous section the following hypothesis will be tested:

H4: Green product placement will lead to a lower activation of persuasion knowledge

than conventional product placement.

As shortly mentioned, the perception of a persuasive attempt elicits a reaction (i.e. coping behavior). This reaction can be a form of resistance, which will be discussed in the following section.

Resistance

The Persuasion Knowledge Model argues that the activation of persuasion knowledge consequently exhibits a reaction towards the persuasive attempt which is in general of

negative valence (Friestad & Wright, 1994; Campbell & Kirmani, 2008). This (negative) reaction is a behavior that helps the person dealing with the persuasive attack (i.e. coping behavior) and is most commonly a type of resistance (Friestad & Wright, 1994; Campbell & Kirmani, 2008). Reactance theory argues that people respond negatively when they perceive a threat of freedom, which can be caused by a persuasive attempt. They do this to restore their natural desire for freedom (Brehm & Brehm, 1981). Furthermore, resistance can also be triggered by a concern of deception, which is likely to occur with advertising (Fransen et al., 2015). Alongside the framework of Fransen et al. (2015) these two motives for resistance (i.e.

(13)

threat of freedom and concern for deception) make people to contest communications. This negative reaction to persuasive communication (i.e. product placement) will probably lead to negative evaluations of the advertising.

Until now, the factor resistance has not been studied in product placement research and therefore adding to the field of product placement. So far, only a direct relation between persuasion knowledge and brand attitude has been investigated. Gibson, Redker and

Zimmerman (2013) showed that the activation of persuasion knowledge negatively affects brand attitudes. However, the mediating role of resistance is shown in research concerning advertising in general and native advertising specifically (Wei, 2009; van Reijmersdal et al., 2016). In addition and based on abundant research into the Persuasion Knowledge Model (Friestad & Wright, 1994; Campbell & Kirmani, 2008; Fransen et al., 2015), the following hypotheses were formulated:

H5: A high activation of persuasion knowledge will lead to a higher activation of

resistance than a low activation of persuasion knowledge.

H6: A high activation of resistance will lead to a lower evaluation (i.e. more negative)

of the brand (i.e. brand attitude) than a low activation of resistance.

The persuasive attempt of product placement might become more salient due to a sponsorship disclosure, which is becoming more and more common. Therefore, the moderating effect of disclosures will be examined.

Sponsorship Disclosures

The Federal Communications Commission (FCC) that regulates communications by television and other devices expressed their concern about product placement (Avery & Ferraro, 2000). They argue that product placement is an unethical technique, because

(14)

consumers are unaware of its persuasive attempt. Therefore, the FCC strives to inform consumers about the incorporation of brands (i.e. commercial content) in movies, television series and shows (i.e. non-commercial content) by placing a logo (mostly an illustration of the letter “P”) and/ or text (such as “This program contains advertising by […]”) in the upper right or left corner of the screen. This indication of product placement is called sponsorship

disclosure (Cain, 2011).

Based on the Persuasion Knowledge Model, the recognition of communication as a persuasive attempt activates the message receiver’s persuasion knowledge which in turn influences subsequent processes. The disclosure of product placement that is basically directly pointing to advertising will probably lead to the activation of persuasion knowledge, as it acknowledges the persuasive attempt of product placement. Research about sponsorship disclosures of (conventional) product placement confirms that indeed disclosures can lead to the activation of persuasion knowledge (Boerman et al., 2012; Boerman, van Reijmersdal & Neijens, 2015; Matthes & Naderer, 2016). For example, Boerman et al. (2012) showed that a disclosure of six seconds compared to a shorter or no disclosure leads to the activation of persuasion knowledge.

For green product placement without a sponsorship disclosure it was argued that the probability that persuasion knowledge gets activated is very low, because people do not perceive it as advertising. However, when people are made aware of the persuasive intent of green product placement due to a sponsorship disclosure, it can be expected that they will be more likely to perceive product placement as advertising. As such activation of their

persuasion knowledge will be more likely. Subsequently, people will resist the persuasive attempt more which will lead to a lower brand attitude. That process is shown in a study about online native advertising by van Reijmersdal et al. (2016). The authors demonstrated that disclosing sponsored content in a blog evokes persuasion knowledge that in turn activates

(15)

resistance and consequently degrades evaluations (i.e. brand attitude). This leads to the following hypothesis:

H7: Green product placement will lead to a lower activation of persuasion knowledge

than conventional product placement without a disclosure, but with a disclosure green product placement will activate as much persuasion knowledge as conventional product placement.

Study overview

It is assumed that the effect of green product placement versus conventional product placement on brand attitude takes place via two underlying processes, see Figure 1. On one hand, green product placement might benefit from a perception of brand warmth in

comparison to conventional product placement. On the other hand, green product placement might profit from a lower activation of persuasion knowledge, due to the novelty of this advertising technique and thus evoke less resistance. When a disclosure is incorporated in the product placement, it is expected that green product placement will activate as much

persuasion knowledge as conventional product placement. However, the perception of brand warmth will still lead to a higher attitude towards the green brand than the conventional brand.

(16)

Figure 1. Conceptual model of the hypotheses.

Method

In order to create the stimulus material for the four conditions (a green product

placement with (-out) disclosure and a conventional product placement with (-out) disclosure) two similar brands needed to be selected: a green brand and a conventional brand. These two brands had to fulfill three basic requirements to be suitable. Firstly, both brands had to be equally and highly familiar to a certain population that serves as a target group in this experiment as research showed that brand familiarity affected the results in prior product placement studies (Verhellen et al., 2015). Secondly, both brands needed to be equally and highly liked as this factor can also bias outcome measures (Dens et al., 2012). Thirdly, the green brand had to be perceived as green (i.e. environmentally friendly) and the conventional brand had to be perceived as significantly less green. To find two brands that meet these requirements, two pretests were executed.

H6 H5 H7 H4 H3 H2 H1 Product Placement Green vs. Conventional Disclosure Yes vs. No Brand Warmth Brand Attitude Persuasion Knowledge Resistance

(17)

Pretest 1

The first pretest had a within-subject design with Brand Type: Green vs. Conventional as conditions. A sample of 17 participants (Mage = 24.65, SD = 2.09, 82.4% female, 70.6%

German, 29,4% other nationality) was randomly exposed (online) to 20 green and

conventional brands such as beverage brands, cosmetic brands, and grocery store brands. Participants were asked to answer the following three questions regarding every brand: “Do you like the brand?”, “Is the brand familiar to you?” and “Do you perceive the brand as green (environmentally friendly)?” on a 7-point Likert-scale (1 = “Strongly disagree”; 7 = “Strongly agree”). The results showed that the German refreshment drink brand Bionade (Mliking = 5.81, SD = 1.05; Mfamiliar = 5.69, SD = 1.92; Mgreen = 5.44, SD = 0.89) and the

German grocery store brand Alnatura (Mliking = 5.35, SD = 1.22; Mfamiliar = 5.88, SD = 2.03;

Mgreen = 6.06, SD = 1.09) scored high on familiarity, liking and greenness within the German

participants. However, none of the other researched brands of the categories refreshment drink and grocery store scored equally high on familiarity and liking and significantly lower on greenness (see Table 1). The brands Bionade and Alnatura fulfilled the requirements to function as a green brand in this present research. However, a second pretest needed to be done to find a matching conventional brand. It was also decided to restrict the experiment to German participants, as differences between brands and countries were too big.

Pretest 2

The second pretest also had a within-subject design with Brand Type: Green vs. Conventional as conditions. The sample consisted of 21 German participants (Mage = 33.24,

SD = 14.63, 66.7% female). Participants were exposed to a selection of 10 brands (five refreshment drink brands and five German grocery store brands) with the same procedure as

(18)

in the first pretest. The results of a paired-sample t-test indicated that the refreshment drink brand Coca Cola (M = 6.19, SD = 1.33) was as familiar as Bionade (M = 6.19, SD = 0.93). This is shown by a non-significant result, t (20) < .01, p = 1.000, 95% CI [-0.68, 0.68]. The results also showed that Coca Cola (M = 2.67, SD = 1.28) scored significantly lower on greenness than Bionade (M = 5.57, SD = 0.93). This is reflected in a significant difference with a large effect size, t (20) = 7.23, p < .001, 95% CI [2.07, 3.74], d = 2.60. Furthermore, a non-significant result showed that Coca Cola (M = 5.19, SD = 1.60) scored equally high on liking as Bionade did (M = 5.67, SD = 0.86), t (20) = 1.39, p = .180, 95% CI [-0.24, 1.19]. The brands Coca Cola and Bionade provided the best match concerning the requirements (see Table 2) and it was decided to use the brand Coca Cola for the conventional product

placement conditions and Bionade for the green product placement conditions. To increase the comparability of these two chosen brands to a maximum, liking will be measured in order to be able to control for it, if necessary.

Stimulus Development

For the purpose of developing the four product placement conditions, a short scene (2 minutes 14 seconds) of the German movie Vaterfreuden was chosen. In the scene a discussion takes place while some people are sitting around a table. One of the beer bottles standing on the table was replaced by a Bionade bottle (green conditions) or a Coca Cola bottle

(conventional conditions), see Figure 2. The editing was done by a professional. In the disclosure conditions the sponsorship disclosure (text and logo) used on German television was placed in the upper right corner of the screen for 6 seconds, see Figure 3. The

effectiveness of a sponsorship disclosure that consists out of a text and logo and fades in for 6 seconds is supported by product placement research (van Reijmersdal et al., 2012). To

(19)

manipulate one scene instead of using existing scenes with product placement. While using this method, the factors modality, plot connection and displaying stay equal in all conditions, which is of great importance as previous research has shown that these influence the effects of product placement (Russel, 2002; Verhellen et al., 2015). The brands are presented only visually (modality), were congruent with the plot (plot connection) and showed up 14 times (displaying frequency).

Previous research showed that people are not always aware of the disclosure, but being aware is necessary for the disclosure to work as intended (Boerman et al., 2012). Therefore, the participants in the disclosure conditions were, additionally to the disclosure in the scene, informed about the sponsored content in the scene with the use of a short text and were exposed to the sponsorship disclosure logo before watching the scene.

Main Experiment

Participants, Design and Procedure

The design of this research represented a 2 factor (Brand Type: Green vs.

Conventional condition) × (Disclosure: Yes vs. No) between-subject design. Participants (N = 435) were recruited via e-mail or Facebook to take part in the online experiment on the website Qualtrics. After excluding not completed responses (n = 193), the sample comprised 242 participants (Mage = 41.23, SD = 15.55, 55.8% female).

After opening the link to the experiment, participants were provided with general information about the experiment and had to give consent to be able to continue. Firstly, brand liking towards the green and conventional brand was measured to be able to control for differences. Thereupon, the participants were randomly assigned to one of the four conditions (green product placement without disclosure vs. conventional product placement without disclosure vs. green product placement with disclosure vs. conventional product placement

(20)

with disclosure) and exposed to the corresponding scene. Right after the exposure it was checked whether participants remembered the placed brand by measuring brand recall and brand recognition as a manipulation check. Thereupon, participants were asked to rate their attitude towards the brand (dependent variable). The rating of the dependent variable was followed by the scales of the expected mediating variables brand warmth, persuasion knowledge, and resistance. Subsequently, the appropriateness of the manipulation was checked and participants were asked some questions concerning the scene, so that potential confounding factors could be controlled for in later analyses if deemed necessary. Finally, participants were requested to fill in demographics and rate their environmental self-identity (control variable). Environmental self-identity was taken into account as previous research showed that this may have a large influence on the effectiveness of green communication (Nelson & McLeod, 2005). In case randomization was not successful, this could be controlled for. At the very end, participants received a debriefing in which the purpose of the study was explained.

Measurements

Brand Liking. First of all, brand liking towards the green brand Bionade and the conventional brand Coca Cola was measured to be able to control for the small difference in brand liking that was observed in Pretest 2. To prevent that this measure influenced further research, 13 filler items were used. This measure included in total 15 items such as “I like the brand Bionade.” and “I like the brand Haribo.”. Participants were asked to rate the 15 food brands on a 7-point Likert-scale (1 = “Strongly disagree; 7 = “Strongly agree”). High scores of brand liking corresponded to more brand liking, whereas low scores corresponded to less brand liking (M = 4.40; SD = 1.87).

(21)

Brand Recall. In order to check if participants saw the brand placed in the movie scene (manipulation check), first of all, brand recall was measured by asking participants to list every brand that they remembered seeing in the movie scene of Vaterfreuden (Russel, 2002; Boerman et al., 2012). The item was coded with 0 (not recalled) and 1 (correctly recalled). 81.4% of the participants recalled the brand correctly.

Brand Recognition. Secondly, brand recognition was measured to check if participants recognized the placed brand in the case they were not able to recall it

(manipulation check) (Russel, 2002). For this purpose participants were exposed to a list of 10 brands (Bionade, Coca Cola and eight filler items) and asked to check the brands they

remembered seeing in the movie scene Vaterfreuden (0 = “No”; 1 = “Yes”). 82.6% of the participants recognized the brand correctly.

Brand Attitude. The dependent variable was the participant’s attitude towards the brand (brand attitude) that was placed in the movie scene and was measured with a six 7-point differential scale: unpleasant/ pleasant, bad/ good, negative/ positive, unfavorable/ favorable, poor quality/ high quality and dislike/ like (Bruner, 2009). A principal component analysis (PCA) showed that the six items loaded on 1 factor (eigenvalue 5.07) that explained 84 per cent of the variance. The reliability of the scale was good, Cronbach’s alpha = .96. Therefore, the average score of the six items was used to measure this concept (M = 4.44; SD =

1.62).The scale showed to measure brand attitude as intended: the higher participants scored on brand attitude, the more positive was their attitude toward the brand.

Brand Warmth. The mediator brand warmth is defined as a positive, warm feeling towards a brand (Aaker et al., 1986) and was measured on a 7-point Likert-scale (1 =

“Strongly disagree”; 7 = “Strongly agree”). The scale which was adapted from Aaker, Vohs and Mogilner (2010) consisted out of the three following items: “I find that the brand Coca Cola/ Bionade is warm/ generous/ kind”. A PCA showed that the three items form a

(22)

uni-dimensional scale, because they all load on 1 factor (eigenvalue 2.43; explained variance = 81.08%). Furthermore, the scale proved to be reliable, Cronbach’s alpha = .88. The average of the three items formed the index measure for brand warmth (M = 3.47, SD = 1.37): a high score reflected more brand warmth than a low score.

Persuasion Knowledge. Persuasion knowledge was measured as a mediator. The concept persuasion knowledge consists of two dimensions: conceptual persuasion knowledge and attitudinal persuasion knowledge (Rozendaal et al., 2011). The former one is defined as people’s perception of communication (i.e. product placement) as advertising and can be measured with a single item: “To what extend was the item about Coca Cola/ Bionade in the scene of the movie Vaterfreuden advertising?” on a 7-point Likert-scale (1 = “Strongly disagree”; 7 = “Strongly agree”) (Boerman et al., 2012). A high score on conceptual persuasion knowledge depicted a greater perception of communication as advertising (M = 5.61; SD = 1.61).

The latter dimension, attitudinal persuasion knowledge, is defined as people’s critical feelings toward communication (Rozendaal et al., 2011). The following three items formed the measure for attitudinal persuasion knowledge in this study: “I think the item about Coca Cola/ Bionade in the scene of the movie Vaterfreuden is honest (reversed)/ trustworthy (reversed)/ convincing (reversed) and were rated on a 7-point Liker-scale (1 = “Strongly disagree”; 7 = “Strongly agree) (Boerman et al., 2012). The original scale to measure this dimension which consisted out of five items was reduced to these three items based on the results of a PCA: two components were extracted and only by leaving out two items a reliable scale could be composed The composed scale loaded on 1 factor (eigenvalue 2.46; explained variance = 81.83%) with a high reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = .89). Participants who reported a high score on attitudinal persuasion knowledge felt highly critical towards the product placement (M = 4.55; SD = 1.27).

(23)

Resistance. The mediator resistance consists of two dimensions: cognitive resistance, which can be defined as negative cognitions and affective resistance which can be described as negative feelings (Zuwernik & Cameron, 2003; Knowles & Linn, 2004). Both types of resistance were measured on a 7-point Likert-scale (1 = “Strongly disagree”; 7 = “Strongly agree”). The former, cognitive resistance, included the following four items: “While viewing, I contested/ refuted/ doubted/ countered the information in the scene of the movie

Vaterfreuden.” (Zuwernik & Cameron, 2003). Based on a PCA, it could be concluded that the scale was unidimensional (eigenvalue 3.43; explained variance = 85.75). Besides that, it was reliable, Cronbach’s alpha = .94. Participants who scored high showed more cognitive resistance towards the product placement than participants that scored low (M = 3.21, SD = 1.48).

The latter, affective resistance, was measured as well with four items: “While watching the scene of the movie Vaterfreuden I felt angry/ enraged/ irritated/ annoyed.” (Zuwernik & Cameron, 2003). By executing a PCA, one component was extracted (eigenvalue 3.41) that explained 85 per cent of the variance. Reliability of this scale was also good, Cronbach’s alpha = .94. Participants who reported a high affective resistance were reflected by a higher score on this variable (M = 2.62, SD = 1.50). Because of a strong positive association between these two components (r = 0.52, p < .001) the average score of cognitive resistance and affective resistance was used to measure the concept resistance (M = 2.89, SD = 1.30).

Greenness of the Brand. It was measured whether participants who were in the green product placement conditions perceived the green brand (Bionade) as green and whether participants who were in the conventional product placement conditions perceived the

conventional brand (Coca Cola) as less green: on a 7-point Likert-scale (1 = “Not green”; 7 = “Green”). The perceived greenness of the brands was measured to check for the manipulation of the factor brand type. A high score reflected the perception of a brand as green (M= 3.72, SD = 1.89).

(24)

Disclosure. Furthermore, the manipulation of the factor disclosure was checked. This was done to ensure that participants who were in the disclosure conditions also recognized seeing the disclosure. For this purpose participants were asked if they saw the particular sponsorship disclosure in the movie scene Vaterfreuden (1 = “Yes”, 2 = “No” and 3 = “No idea”).

Movie Liking. Because prior research showed that context variables such as how much one likes the movie scene can bias participants’ responses (van Reijmersdal, Smit & Neijens, 2010), movie liking was measured with the following scale (out of seven items): “I enjoyed watching the scene of the movie Vaterfreuden”, “I don’t regret watching [… ]”, “I’m glad I saw […]”, “I like the story of […]”, “The acting in […] is good”, “[…] is a good movie scene” (Dens et al., 2012). Participants were asked to rate these items on a 7-point Likert-scale (1 = “Strongly disagree”; 7 = “Strongly agree”). The seven items formed a reliable Likert-scale (eigenvalue 4.89; extracted variance = 69.83%; Cronbach’s alpha = .93). A high average score on the scale represented a great liking of the movie scene of Vaterfreuden (M = 4.23; SD = 1.34).

Movie Seen Before. Also a prior exposure to the movie and thereby to the movie scene could influence the participant’s responses (Russel, 2002). Therefore, participants were asked if they had seen the movie before (1 = “Yes”, 2 = “No” and 3 = “No idea”).

Movie Mood. A third factor concerning the movie scene itself that could affect other measures is the genre of the movie (Van Reijmersdal et al., 2010). For this reason participants were asked to rate how they perceived the mood of the movie scene (1 = “Sad”; 5 = “Funny”). A low rating reflected the perception of a tragedy and a high rating reflected the perception of a comedy (M = 2.92; SD = 1.05).

(25)

Demographics. The standard demographic variables gender, age, nationality and education were recorded as well.

Environmental Self-Identity. By reason of prior research about green brands that showed a possible influence of how environmentally conscious a person is, the concept environmental self-identity was included (do Paço & Reis, 2012; Grimmer & Woolley, 2014). This last scale of this experiment consisted of the three following items: “Acting

environmentally friendly is an important part of who I am.”, “I am the type of person who acts environmentally friendly.”, “I see myself as an environmentally friendly person.” on a 7-point Likert-scale (1 = “Strongly disagree”; 7 = “Strongly agree”). This scale was used in research by van der Werff, Steg and Keizer (2013) and showed to be a reliable measure of the concept environmental self-identity (eigenvalue 2.35; explained variance = 78.23; Cronbach’s alpha = .84). The scores on the three items were thus taken together: the higher the score, the higher the participants’ environmental self-identity (M = 5.09, SD = 0.88).

Results

Manipulation Checks

Brand memory. Seventeen percent of the participants (n = 42) who were exposed to one of the four conditions did not recognized the brand placed in the movie scene. As brand recognition and recall were significantly and very strongly correlated (r = 0.90, p < .001), it was decided that the results of brand recognition will function as an indicator for brand memory in this study (Russel, 2002). Based on the aim of the present study to compare different brands (green versus conventional) in product placement, it was decided to exclude all participants that did not recognized the brand correctly (following Boerman et al., 2012). Therefore, a sample of N = 200 participants was included.

(26)

Greenness of the Brand. A one-way analysis of variance was carried out to see if the brand type was perceived as intended. A large significant effect was found, F (1, 194) = 142.46, p < .001, η² = .423 indicating that participants in the green product placement conditions were more likely to perceive the brand as green (M = 4.97, SD = 1.42) than

participants in the conventional product placement conditions (M = 2.52, SD = 1.45). Thus the manipulation of the brand type was successful.

Disclosure. Despite the implementation of an explicit disclosure, 38,5% of the participants (n = 45) who were assigned to one of the two disclosure conditions stated not to have seen a disclosure. As the recognition of the disclosure is critical for the effects, it was decided to exclude these participants in the analyses which left a sample of N = 155 participants (following Boerman et al., 2012).

Confounding Variable Analyses

To check for confounding variables the measured control variables were correlated with the mediators and dependent variable, see Table 3. Brand liking was positively correlated with brand warmth (r = 0.52, p < .001), negatively correlated with attitudinal persuasion knowledge (r = -0.24, p = .003), and positively correlated with brand attitude (r = 0.79, p < .001). Next, results showed that movie liking was negatively correlated with attitudinal persuasion knowledge (r = -0.29, p < .001) and with resistance (r = -0.45, p < .001). Whether people have seen the movie before was negatively correlate with brand warmth (r = -0.18, p = .029). Furthermore, movie mood showed to be negatively correlated with attitudinal

persuasion knowledge (r = -0.19, p = .025), and positively correlated with brand warmth (r = 0.17, p = .041). The results also showed that the older the participants were, the lower the participants scored on brand warmth (r = -0.20, p = .014) and brand attitude (r = -0.17, p =

(27)

.035). Lastly, a negative correlation between environmental self-identity and brand attitude was found (r = -0.16, p = .049).

Randomization Check

For the control variables that correlated significantly with the dependent variable or mediators, a randomization check was executed to test if they need to be controlled for in the main analyses. A two-way analysis of variance was conducted to check if the brands were equally liked in all conditions. The analysis showed that the brands were not equally liked in the two product placement conditions (green versus conventional), indicated by a small significant effect F (1, 149) = 6.99, p = .009, η2 = .05. No significant difference was found for the disclosure conditions, F (1, 149) = 0.82, p = .367 and the interaction was not significant either, F (1, 149) = 0.07, p = .798. Participants who were exposed to the green product placement were already more likely to indicate that they like the brand (Bionade) upfront more (M = 4.88, SD = 1.43) than participants who were exposed to the conventional product placement (Coca Cola) (M = 4.08, SD = 2.10). Whether participants were exposed to a

product placement with disclosure (M = 4.65, SD = 1.87) or without disclosure (M = 4.32, SD = 1.81) was not affected by brand liking. In the following analyses brand liking will be used as a covariate to control for the difference between conditions concerning brand type.

A two-way analysis of variance for movie liking showed no significant effects

between conditions. There was no significant effect of brand type, F (1, 149) = 0.42, p = .520, nor of disclosure, F (1, 149) = 0.19, p = .664. The interaction between brand type and

disclosure was marginally significant, F (2, 149) = 3.41, p = .067, η2 = .02. The effect was small. The participants liked the movie equally in all conditions (green product placement (M = 4.08, SD = 1.24), conventional product placement (M = 4.21, SD = 1.41), with disclosure

(28)

(M = 4.09, SD = 1.43), without disclosure (M = 4.20, SD = 1.32)), thus there was no need to control for movie liking.

To check if there was need to control for whether participants had seen the movie before or not, a chi-square test was executed. The results showed that out of 74 participants in the green product placement condition, 10 (11.8%) had seen the movie before and out of 76 participants in the conventional product placement condition 18 (13.5%) had seen the movie before which reflects no significant difference between the brand type conditions, χ² (1) = 0.67, p = .412. A second chi-square test for the disclosure conditions showed that out of 70 participants in the disclosure condition 11 (11.2%) had seen the movie before and out of 80 participants in the non-disclosure condition 13 (12,8%) had seen the movie before. This was also reflected in a non-significant result, χ² (1) = 0.01, p = .929. As these two analyses showed, there was no need to control for a possible prior exposure to the movie.

A second variable that needed to be checked for randomization regarding the movie was movie mood. Therefore, a two-way analysis of variance for movie mood with brand type and disclosure as independent variables was executed. The results indicated no significant effects for brand type, F (1, 145) = 0.07, p = .791, nor for disclosure, F (1, 145) = 1.44, p = .232, nor for the interaction, F (2, 145) = 0.42, p = .518. Thus, in both brand type conditions (green (M = 2.97, SD = 1.05) versus conventional (M = 2.93, SD = 1.10)) and in both disclosure conditions (yes (M = 2.84, SD = 1.09) versus no (M = 3.05, SD = 1.06)) the

perception of movie mood did not differ significantly, which indicated that there was no need to control for movie mood.

Furthermore, the results of a two-way analysis of variance for age revealed no significant effects for brand type, F (1, 146) = 0.08, p = .773, and disclosure, F (1, 146) = 2.21, p = .140, nor for the interaction, F (2, 146) = 1.33, p = .250. As for the green product placement conditions (M = 39.27, SD = 15.15), conventional product placement conditions (M = 40.05, SD = 14.38), disclosure conditions (M = 37.75, SD = 14.29), and no disclosure

(29)

conditions (M = 41.38, SD = 14.72) was the average age the same, there was no need to control for the variable age.

The last randomization check was aimed to check for the variable environmental self-identity. The results of a two-way analysis of variance showed that brand type had a

significant effect (small) on environmental self-identity, F (1, 149) = 5.04, p = .026, η2 = .03. The effect of disclosure was not significant, F (1, 149) = 0.33, p = .566, neither the interaction effect, F (2, 149) = 0.19, p = .663. By evaluating the mean scores, one could see that

participants who were exposed to a green product placement had a higher rating of

environmental self-identity (M = 5.19, SD = 0.75) compared to participants who were exposed to a conventional product placement (M = 4.90, SD = 0.90). There was no difference between in scores, depending on the disclosure conditions (yes (M = 5.01, SD = 0.88) versus no (M = 5.07, SD = 0.80)). The former difference could be due to a priming effect: participants saw themselves as more green, because they had seen a green brand (Bionade). In addition to this alternative explanation for the effect of brand type on environmental self-identity, a former test showed that environmental self-identity is only correlated with the dependent variable brand attitude, but not with the assumed mediators. These aspects led to the decision, that there is no need to control for environmental self-identity.

To sum up, the randomization check showed that there is a need to control for brand liking, but none of the other variables. While controlling for brand liking the upfront more positive evaluation of a green brand in comparison to a conventional brand will not explain the results of the main analyses. Therefore, the results can be ascribed to the effect of using different brand types (green versus conventional) in product placement.

(30)

Main Analyses

Brand Type on Brand Attitude. In order to test Hypothesis 1 which stated the hypothesized main effect of brand type in product placement on brand attitude, a one-way analysis of covariance was conducted. The results indicated that brand type has a medium strong significant effect on brand attitude, F (1, 149) = 27.98, p <.001, ηp2 = .16. The

covariate brand liking also significantly influenced participants’ brand attitude, F (1, 149) = 240.55, p < .001, ηp2 = .62. This effect was large. Due to the results, Hypothesis 1 was

supported: the placement of a green brand (Bionade) (M = 4.85, SD = 0.11) led to a higher (i.e. more positive) brand attitude than the placement of a conventional brand (Coca Cola) (M = 4.06, SD = 0.10).

Brand Type on Brand Warmth. Furthermore, a one-way analysis of covariance was executed to test whether distinct brand types in product placement lead to distinct perceptions of brand warmth (Hypothesis 2). The results showed a significant (small) effect on brand warmth, F (1,150) = 6.93, p = .009, ηp2 = .04, and a significant (medium) effect on the

covariate brand liking, F (1, 150) = 46.03, p < .001, ηp2 = .24. As expected, green brands in

product placement are perceived as warmer (M = 3.76, SD = 0.13) than conventional brands in product placement (M = 3.28, SD = 0.13).

Brand Warmth on Brand Attitude. It was further hypothesized that the perception of brand warmth directly influences the attitude towards the brand (Hypothesis 3). To assess if this is indeed the case, a multiple linear regression analysis was computed with brand warmth and brand liking as predictors and brand attitude as dependent measure. Indicated by

significant results, F (2, 149) = 168.75, p < .001, 69 per cent of the variation in brand attitude (R² = 0.69) could be predicted by brand warmth (β = 0.31, t = 5.82, p < .001, 95% CI [0.24, 0.50]) and brand liking (β = 0.63, t = 11.88, p < .001, 95% CI [0.45, 0.63]). Thus, Hypothesis 3 was supported: a higher brand warmth is associated with a more positive brand attitude.

(31)

Brand Warmth Mediation. Hypothesis 3 also entailed that the influence of distinct brand types in product placement on brand attitude is mediated by brand warmth. Therefore, a mediation analysis was performed and computed three regression equations. A bootstrapping analyses with 1000 samples (Preacher & Hayes, 2004) confirmed mediation through brand warmth (indirect effect = -0.15, SE = 0.07, 95% confidence interval [-0.3355, -0.0401]. Based on the PROCESS analysis that extracted a significant indirect effect of brand warmth, it could be concluded that brand warmth mediates the influence of brand type on brand attitude.

Brand Type and Disclosure on Persuasion Knowledge. By the execution of a two-way analysis of covariance with brand type and disclosure as independent variables,

persuasion knowledge as dependent variable and brand liking as covariate, Hypotheses 4 and 7 were tested. As the concept persuasion knowledge was measured with two distinct scales to extract attitudinal as well as cognitive persuasion knowledge that were not correlated with one another (r = 0.13, p = .123), the analysis concerning persuasion knowledge had to be

computed twice.

The first analysis with attitudinal persuasion knowledge as dependent measure revealed no significant effects of brand type, F (1, 147) = 1.58, p = .211, nor of disclosure, F (1, 147) = 0.75, p = .387, nor of the interaction between brand type and disclosure, F (2, 147) = 0.01, p = .907. Solely the covariate brand liking showed to have a significant (small) effect, F (1, 147) = 6.45, p = .012, ηp2 = .04. Based on these results, Hypotheses 4 and 7 could not be

confirmed with attitudinal persuasion knowledge as dependent measure. Green product placement did not lead to a lower activation of attitudinal persuasion knowledge (M = 4.46, SD = 0.15), than conventional product placement (M = 4.72; SD = 0.15) (Hypothesis 4). Furthermore, the activation of attitudinal persuasion knowledge was not higher when the product placement was disclosed (M = 4.50; SD = 0.15), than when the product placement was not disclosed (M = 4.68; SD = 0.14) (Hypothesis 7).

(32)

The second analysis with conceptual persuasion knowledge as dependent measure instead of attitudinal persuasion knowledge showed similar results. There was no significant effect of brand type, F (1, 148) = 0.99, p = .322, nor of disclosure, F (1, 148) = 1.19, p = .278. Also brand liking did not influence conceptual persuasion knowledge, F (1, 148) = 0.78, p = .379. However, there was a marginally significant (small) interaction effect, F (2, 1478) = 3.46, p = .065, ηp2 = .02. There were no differences in the activation of conceptual persuasion

knowledge between the four conditions (green product placement (M = 5.67, SD = 0.18), conventional product placement (M = 5.92; SD = 0.18), disclosure (M = 5.66; SD = 0.18), and no disclosure (M = 5.93; SD = 0.17).

In sum, the results of these two analysis concerning persuasion knowledge did not confirm the hypothesized effect that green product placement will lead to a lower activation of persuasion knowledge than conventional product placement (Hypothesis 4). In addition, the hypothesized effect that due to a disclosure green product placement will activate a similar amount of persuasion knowledge than conventional product placement could also not be confirmed (Hypothesis 7).

Persuasion Knowledge and Resistance. It was predicted that the activation of persuasion knowledge subsequently leads to resistance (Hypothesis 5). This prediction was tested with a multiple linear regression analysis. Firstly, attitudinal persuasion knowledge and brand liking were used as predictors for resistance. The model was significant, F (2,149) = 4.10, p = .018, but explained only 5 per cent of the variance (R² = 0.05). Attitudinal

persuasion knowledge predicted resistance, β = 0.24, t = 2.86, p = .005, 95% CI [0.07, 0.39], but brand liking did not, β = 0.04, t = 0.53, p = .597. Thus, Hypothesis 5 can be supported (for attitudinal persuasion knowledge): a higher activation of attitudinal persuasion knowledge leads to more resistance among participants than a lower activation of attitudinal persuasion knowledge.

(33)

Secondly, the same analysis was performed, but instead of using attitudinal persuasion knowledge as a predictor, conceptual persuasion knowledge was used as a predictor for resistance. The model was not significant, F (2,150) = 0.46, p = .630, so resistance could not be predicted by conceptual persuasion knowledge, β = 0.08, t = 0.95, p = .346, and also not by brand liking, β = -0.02, t = -0.22, p = .828.

To sum up, attitudinal persuasion knowledge led to resistance, but conceptual persuasion knowledge did not. Hypothesis 5 could only be partly confirmed.

Resistance and Brand Attitude. As a consequence of a higher degree of resistance, it was hypothesized that the evaluation of brand attitudes would decrease (Hypothesis 6). To test Hypothesis 6, a multiple linear regression analysis was executed. The results of the analysis showed that the model was significant, F (2, 149) = 125.57, p < .001 and explained 63 per cent of the variance (R² = 0.63). Surprisingly, the variation could not be explained by resistance, β = -0.06, t = -1.17, p = .245, but by brand liking, β = 0.79, t = 15.79, p < .001, 95% CI [0.60, 0.77]. Based on these results, Hypothesis 6 was rejected: more resistance did not lead to a lower brand attitude than less resistance.

Conclusion and Discussion

The present study aimed answering the following research question: What is the influence of green product placement versus conventional product placement on consumer’s attitude towards the brand? How do warmth, persuasion knowledge, and resistance mediate this relationship? And how does sponsorship disclosure moderate the effect of the mediators persuasion knowledge and resistance on consumer’s attitude towards the brand? In order to answer these questions seven hypotheses were formulated and tested in an experiment. Firstly, the results of the main effect and the first path of the model concerning the mediation

(34)

of brand warmth will be discussed. After that the second path of the model, which predicted a mediation by persuasion knowledge and resistance and a moderation by disclosure, will be discussed.

The first hypothesized path of the model was fully supported by the results of the current study. This regards Hypotheses 1 to 3. The results showed that green product placement indeed leads to a higher brand attitude than conventional product placement (Hypothesis 1). The green brand Bionade was better liked than the conventional brand Coca Cola after being exposed to these brands in a product placement. This is in line with green advertising research that showed a positive brand attitude towards green brands that use advertising (Schuhwerk & Lefkoff-Hagius, 1995; Kong & Zhang, 2013; Hartmann et al., 2006; Grimmer & Woolley, 2014; Huang et al., 2014; Nagar, 2015). The present study reveals that the positive effect of green brands versus conventional brands that is found in traditional advertising can be extended to the native advertising type of product placement.

Besides the fact that the placement of green brands led to a more positive brand attitude, the study also showed that green brands are perceived to be warmer than

conventional brands when used in product placement (Hypothesis 2). As hypothesized, the perception of warmth leads to an increase in brand attitude which confirms Hypothesis 3 that predicted brand warmth would mediate the effect of brand type on brand attitude. Related to theory and prior research, the perception of a company as warm due to its engagement in CSR indeed spills over to the brand and leads to a higher brand warmth of green brands than

conventional brands which is in line with research by Chernev and Blair (2015). The authors showed that the perception of a company’s goodwill influences consumer’s product

evaluations. Consumers evaluated products of companies that engage in CSR more positively (i.e. better performing) than products of companies that do not engage in CSR (Chernev & Blair, 2015). Also Bernritter et al. (2016) showed a similar effect of brand warmth which indicated to mediate the effect of brand type on behavior. In a social media study they showed

(35)

that non-profit brands are perceived as warmer than for-profit brands and therefore are more often “liked” on the social media platform Facebook (Bernritter et al., 2016). To summarize, the results of the present study regarding the effect of brand type on brand attitude mediated by brand warmth are in line with prior research and add the insight that this effect also occurs in the context of product placement.

Concerning the second (lower) path of the assumed model, the hypotheses could be only partly confirmed by the results. To start, Hypothesis 4 assumed that the use of green brands in product placement leads to a lower activation of persuasion knowledge than the use of conventional brands in product placement. The present study showed no support for this hypothesis. Based on the results, this hypothesized effect was rejected. An explanation for this contradicting effect might be that the placement of the brand was perceived as very obvious and therefore also in the green product placement condition persuasion knowledge was activated. Verhellen et al. (2015) defined a visual placement that is frequently repeated as a prominent placement that leads to the activation of persuasion knowledge. So, even within the green product placement conditions, participants did not perceive the product placement as trustworthy, honest, and convincing and noticed the persuasive attempt. The factor novelty of the green product placement was thus of less relevance. However, this might be more relevant when using a placement that is less frequently repeated and therefore unobtrusive.

Subsequently, it was tested if persuasion knowledge and resistance have a positive relationship and this was indeed the case which supported Hypothesis 5. A higher activation of attitudinal persuasion knowledge led to more resistance than a lower activation of

attitudinal persuasion knowledge. This result matches with the Persuasion Knowledge Model that assumes that due to an activation of persuasion knowledge coping behaviors such as resistance are activated (Friestad & Wright, 1994; Campbell & Kirmani, 2008).

Interestingly, conceptual persuasion knowledge was not related to resistance, nor to attitudinal persuasion knowledge - which was tested in an additional correlation analysis. The

(36)

lacking relation of conceptual persuasion knowledge with resistance and attitudinal

persuasion knowledge could be due to how people define advertisement. In the experiment participants were asked to what extend they perceive the item about the brand (Coca Cola or Bionade) as advertising. As product placement is native advertising and not a common type of advertising such as traditional print advertising, participants might not have defined product placement as adverting as they rather associate traditional types of advertising with the word advertising. Dahlén and Edenius (2007) support this reasoning by a study that compared traditional advertising with non-traditional types of advertising such as advertorials. The enhanced persuasiveness of traditional advertising was explained by the fact that non-traditional types of advertising is less perceived as advertising compared to non-traditional types of advertising because the boundaries with the surrounding content are less clear (Dahlén & Edenius, 2007).

Hypothesis 6 was rejected based on the results: more resistance did not lead to a more negative brand attitude than less resistance. An explanation for this effect could be the

influence of brand warmth on brand attitude. The upper path of the model, as well as the lower path of the model, influence both attitudes towards the brand. Due to a green product placement and therefore the perception of brand warmth, the green brand was evaluated positively. The positive brand attitude due to the process of the upper path might have extinguished the expected negative effect of resistance on brand attitude (lower path).

Lastly, the hypothesized effect concerning a moderation of disclosure was not confirmed (Hypothesis 7). It was assumed that green product placement leads to a lower activation of persuasion knowledge than conventional product placement, when there is no implementation of disclosures. However, the integration of disclosures will offset the benefit of green product placement compared to conventional product placement, due to its novelty. And this will lead to a similar activation of persuasion knowledge not matter whether the brand used in product placement is a green brand or a conventional brand. This expected

(37)

moderation effect of disclosure was not found. There was no difference in persuasion knowledge when the product placement was disclosed or not. Here, the same explanation as for the lacking effect of an influence of brand type on persuasion knowledge could serve: due to the frequency of displaying the brand, the persuasive attempt was highly obvious to the participants. Therefore, participants also perceived the scene as highly persuasive when it was not disclosed. This result is not in line with the research by van Reijmersdal et al. (2016) where an effect of disclosure on persuasion knowledge in online native advertising was found. These contradicting effects of the prior study and the study by van Reijmersdal et al. (2016) could be explained by the difference in advertising types. Product placement, as well as online advertising in a blog belongs to the category native advertising. Nevertheless, this does not mean that these two types of advertising are the same in all aspects. In the online native advertising, the brand was only mentioned once in the blog post, while in the product

placement of the present study the brand was shown 14 times. The difference in prominence, with online native advertising as less obtrusive than product placement, could serve as an explanation for the different results.

Managerial Implications

The present study provides some managerial implications. To start, the results of this study can add some value to the discussion about sponsorship disclosures. The FCC strives for disclosing product placement to protect the consumer against subtle persuasion attempts. As the effects did not vary based on a disclosure in the sense that product placement activated the same amount of persuasion knowledge regardless of disclosures, it can be assumed that a consumer is already aware of the persuasive attempt. Thus, when a brand is frequently placed there is no need for a disclosure. The concern about disclosing product placement might be more relevant for subtle forms of product placement.

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Om de verliesnorm voor fosfaat te halen, gebruiken bedrijven met 16.000 kg melk per ha zónder een beheersovereenkomst in 2008 al geen kunstmestfosfaat meer en is het fosforge- halte

This thesis is the result of a research into the attitude of members of the Dutch Communist Party, CPN and labour union, EVC, towards cooperation with social democrats

All studied alcohols show similar vibrational lifetimes of the OH stretching mode and similar HB dynamics, which is described by the fast (~200 fs) and slow components (~4 ps).

Table 3: Top URLs and Hashtags in User Groups By URL Bias Liberal URL Users Conservative URL Users Neutral URL Users.. Top

Tabel 6.14 rapporteer die getal studente wat onderskeidelik kontakklasse, en vakansieskole bygewoon het per GOS-program en modules, en wat gedurende Oktober 2009

Of course thinking (“reason”) and believing (“faith”) are concrete acts of human beings which, like every concrete (natural and social) structure or event, in principle