• No results found

The Breast Size Satisfaction Survey (BSSS): Breast size dissatisfaction and its antecedents and outcomes in women from 40 nations

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The Breast Size Satisfaction Survey (BSSS): Breast size dissatisfaction and its antecedents and outcomes in women from 40 nations"

Copied!
20
0
0

Bezig met laden.... (Bekijk nu de volledige tekst)

Hele tekst

(1)

University of Groningen

The Breast Size Satisfaction Survey (BSSS)

Tran, Ulrich S; Barron, David; Afhami, Reza; Aimé, Annie; Almenara, Carlos A; Alp Dal,

Nursel; Amaral, Ana Carolina Soares; Andrianto, Sonny; Anjum, Gulnaz; Argyrides, Marios

Published in:

Body Image

DOI:

10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.01.006

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

it. Please check the document version below.

Document Version

Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date:

2020

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):

Tran, U. S., Barron, D., Afhami, R., Aimé, A., Almenara, C. A., Alp Dal, N., Amaral, A. C. S., Andrianto, S.,

Anjum, G., Argyrides, M., Atari, M., Aziz, M., Banai, B., Borowiec, J., Brewis, A., Cakir Kocak, Y., Campos,

J. A. D. B., Carmona, C., Chaleeraktrakoon, T., ... Vidal-Mollón, J. (2020). The Breast Size Satisfaction

Survey (BSSS): Breast size dissatisfaction and its antecedents and outcomes in women from 40 nations.

Body Image, 32, 199-217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.01.006

Copyright

Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Take-down policy

If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

(2)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Body

Image

j o u r n al ho me p ag e :w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a te / b o d y i m a g e

The

Breast

Size

Satisfaction

Survey

(BSSS):

Breast

size

dissatisfaction

and

its

antecedents

and

outcomes

in

women

from

40

nations

Viren

Swami

a,b,∗

,

Ulrich

S.

Tran

c

,

David

Barron

b

,

Reza

Afhami

d

,

Annie

Aimé

e

,

Carlos

A.

Almenara

f

,

Nursel

Alp

Dal

g

,

Ana

Carolina

Soares

Amaral

h

,

Sonny

Andrianto

i

,

Gulnaz

Anjum

j

,

Marios

Argyrides

k

,

Mohammad

Atari

l

,

Mudassar

Aziz

m

,

Benjamin

Banai

n

,

Joanna

Borowiec

o

,

Alexandra

Brewis

p

,

Yeliz

Cakir

Kocak

q

,

Juliana

Alvares

Duarte

Bonini

Campos

r

,

Carmen

Carmona

s

,

Trawin

Chaleeraktrakoon

t

,

Hong

Chen

u

,

Phatthanakit

Chobthamkit

t,v

,

Bovornpot

Choompunuch

w

,

Togas

Constantinos

x

,

Aine

Crumlish

y

,

Julio

Eduardo

Cruz

z

,

Simon

E.

Dalley

A

,

Devi

Damayanti

B

,

Joanna

Dare

C

,

Stacey

M.

Donofrio

A

,

Anja

Draksler

D

,

Michelle

Escasa-Dorne

E

,

Elaine

Frances

Fernandez

F

,

Maria

Elisa

Caputo

Ferreira

G

,

David

A.

Frederick

C

,

Antonio

Alías

García

H

,

Shulamit

Geller

I

,

Alexias

George

x

,

Louai

Ghazieh

J

,

Cosmin

Goian

K

,

Colin

Gorman

y

,

Caterina

Grano

L

,

Jonathan

Eliahu

Handelzalts

I

,

Heather

Horsburgh

M,1

,

Todd

Jackson

N

,

Lady

Grey

Javela

Javela

Delgado

O

,

Marija

Jovi ´c

P

,

Marko

Jovi ´c

Q

,

Adam

Kantanista

o

,

Sevag

K.

Kertechian

J

,

Loes

Kessels

R

,

Magdalena

Król-Zieli ´nska

o

,

Garry

Kuan

S

,

Yee

Cheng

Kueh

T

,

Sanjay

Kumar

U

,

Ingela

Lundin

Kvalem

V

,

Caterina

Lombardo

L

,

Ernesto

Luis

López

Almada

W

,

Christophe

Maïano

e

,

Mandar

Manjary

U

,

Karlijn

Massar

R

,

Camilla

Matera

X

,

Juliana

F.

Figueiras

Mereiles

G

,

Norbert

Meskó

Y

,

Hikari

Namatame

Z

,

Amanda

Nerini

X

,

Felix

Neto

aa

,

Joana

Neto

ab

,

Angela

Nogueira

Neves

ac

,

Siu-Kuen

Ng

ad

,

Devi

R.

Nithiya

ae

,

Salma

Samir

Omar

af

,

Mika

Omori

ag

,

Maria

Serena

Panasiti

ah

,

Irena

Pavela

Banai

n

,

Eva

Pila

ai

,

Alessandra

Pokrajac-Bulian

aj

,

Vita

Postuvan

D

,

Ivanka

Prichard

ak

,

Magdalena

Razmus

al

,

Catherine

M.

Sabiston

am

,

Reza

N.

Sahlan

an

,

Jacob

Owusu

Sarfo

ao

,

Yoko

Sawamiya

Z

,

Stefan

Stieger

ap,aq

,

Cindi

SturtzSreetharan

p

,

Eugene

Tee

F

,

Gill

A.

ten

Hoor

R

,

Kulvadee

Thongpibul

ar

,

Arun

Tipandjan

as

,

Otilia

Tudorel

at

,

Tracy

Tylka

au

,

Zahir

Vally

av

,

Juan

Camilo

Vargas-Nieto

aw

,

Luis

Diego

Vega

ax

,

Jose

Vidal-Mollón

s

,

Mona

Vintila

at

,

Deborah

Williams

ay

,

Amber

Wutich

p

,

Yuko

Yamamiya

az

,

Danilo

Zambrano

aw

,

Marcelo

Callegari

Zanetti

Aa

,

Ivanka ˇZivˇci ´c-Be ´cirevi ´c

aj

,

Martin

Voracek

c

aSchoolofPsychologyandSportScience,AngliaRuskinUniversity,Cambridge,UnitedKingdom bCentreforPsychologicalMedicine,PerdanaUniversity,Serdang,Malaysia

cDepartmentofBasicPsychologicalResearchandResearchMethods,SchoolofPsychology,UniversityofVienna,Vienna,Austria dDepartmentofArtStudies,TarbiatModaresUniversity,Tehran,Iran

eDepartmentofPsychoeducationandPsychology,UniversitéduQuébecenOutaouais,Saint-Jérôme,Canada

fInstituteforResearchonChildren,Youth,andFamily,FacultyofSocialStudies,MasarykUniversity,Brno,CzechRepublic gMidwiferyDepartment,MunzurUniversity,Tunceli,Turkey

hFederalInstituteofEducation,Science,andTechnologyofSoutheastMinasGerais,SãoBarbacena,Brazil iDepartmentofPsychology,IslamicUniversityofIndonesia,Yogyakarta,Indonesia

jDepartmentofSocialSciencesandLiberalArts,InstituteofBusinessAdministration,Karachi,Pakistan

∗ Correspondingauthorat:SchoolofPsychologyandSportScience,AngliaRuskinUniversity,Cambridge,CambridgeshireCB11PT,UnitedKingdom. E-mailaddress:viren.swami@anglia.ac.uk(V.Swami).

1 HeatherHorsbughisnowattheSchoolofEducationandSocialSciences,UniversityoftheWestofScotland,Paisley,UnitedKingdom.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.01.006

(3)

SchoolofHealthSciences,NeapolisUniversityPaphos,Paphos,Cyprus

lDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofSouthernCalifornia,LosAngeles,UnitedStates

mDepartmentofSocialSciences,ShaheedZulfikarAliBhuttoInstituteofScienceandTechnology,Karachi,Pakistan nDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofZadar,Zadar,Croatia

oDepartmentofPhysicalEducationandLifelongSports,Pozna´nUniversityofPhysicalEducation,Pozna´n,Poland pSchoolofHumanEvolutionandSocialChange,ArizonaStateUniversity,Tempe,UnitedStates

qMidwiferyDepartment,EgeUniversity, ˙Izmir,Turkey

rDepartmentofFoodandNutrition,SchoolofPharmaceuticalScienceofSãoPauloStateUniversity,SãoPaulo,Brazil sDepartmentofResearchMethodsandDiagnosisinEducation,UniversityofValencia,Valencia,Spain

tDivisionofPsychology,FacultyofLiberalArts,ThammasatUniversity,Pathumthani,Thailand uSchoolofPsychology,SouthwestUniversity,Chongqing,China

vSchoolofPsychology,UniversityofKent,Canterbury,UnitedKingdom

wDepartmentofEducationalPsychologyandCounseling,FacultyofEducation,KhonKaenUniversity,KhonKaen,Thailand xDepartmentofPsychology,PanteionUniversityofAthens,Athens,Greece

ySchoolofPsychology,UlsterUniversity,Coleraine,NorthernIreland,UnitedKingdom zDepartmentofPsychology,UniversitydelosAndes,Bogotá,Colombia

AFacultyofBehaviouralandSocialSciences,UniversityofGroningen,Groningen,Netherlands BDepartmentofPsychology,AhmadDahlanUniversity,Yogyakarta,Indonesia

CDepartmentofPsychology,ChapmanUniversity,Orange,UnitedStates

DSloveneCentreforSuicideResearch,AndrejMarusicInstituteandDepartmentofPsychologyFAMNIT,UniversityofPrimorska,Koper,Slovenia EDepartmentofAnthropology,UniversityofColoradoColoradoSprings,UnitedStates

FDepartmentofPsychology,HELPUniversity,ShahAlam,Malaysia GFederalUniversityofJuizdeFora,JuizdeFora,Brazil

HDepartmentofEducation,UniversityofAlmeria,Almeria,Spain

ISchoolofBehavioralSciences,AcademicCollegeofTel-AvivYaffo,Yaffo,Israel JDepartmentofHumanResources,SaintJosephUniversity,Beirut,Lebanon KDepartmentofSocialWork,WestUniversityofTimis¸oara,Timis¸oara,Romania LDepartmentofPsychology,SapienzaUniversityofRome,Rome,Italy

MSchoolofSocialSciences,UniversityofSouthampton,Southampton,UnitedKingdom NDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofMacau,Taipa,China

ODepartmentofPsychology,UniversidaddelRosario,Bogotá,Colombia

PDepartmentofMarketingManagementandPublicRelations,UniversityofBelgrade,Belgrade,Serbia QDepartmentofPlasticandReconstructiveSurgery,SchoolofMedicine,UniversityofBelgrade,Belgrade,Serbia RDepartmentofWorkandSocialPsychology,MaastrichtUniversity,Maastricht,Netherlands

SSchoolofHealthSciences,UniversitiSainsMalaysia,KubangKerian,Kelantan,Malaysia

TUnitofBiostatisticsandResearchMethodology,SchoolofMedicalSciences,UniversitiSainsMalaysia,KubangKerian,Kelantan,Malaysia UDepartmentofPsychology,D.A.V.College,Muzaffarnagar,India

VDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofOslo,Oslo,Norway

WFacultyofHumanities,UniversidadCatólicaCampusItapúa,Encarnación,Paraguay

XDepartmentofEducation,Languages,Intercultures,Literatures,andPsychology,UniversityofFlorence,Florence,Italy YDepartmentofGeneralandEvolutionaryPsychology,InstituteofPsychology,UniversityofPécs,Pécs,Hungary ZDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofTsukuba,Tsukuba,Japan

aaDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofPorto,Porto,Portugal

abDepartmentofPsychology,CatholicUniversityofBrasília,Taguatinga,Brazil

acDivisionofResearch,PhysicalEducationCollegeoftheBrazilianArmy,RiodeJaneiro,Brazil adPhysicalEducationUnit,ChineseUniversityofHongKong,HongKong,China

aeDepartmentofPhysiology,MahatmaGandhiMedicalCollegeandResearchInstitute,SriBalajiVidyapeth,India afDepartmentofDermatology,Venereology,andAndrology,AlexandriaUniversity,Alexandria,Egypt

agDepartmentofPsychology,OchanomizuUniversity,Tokyo,Japan

ahSantaLuciaFoundation,ScientificInstituteforResearchandHealthcare,Rome,Italy aiSchoolofKinesiology,UniversityofWesternOntario,Ontario,Canada

ajDepartmentofPsychology,FacultyofHumanitiesandSocialSciences,UniversityofRijeka,Rijeka,Croatia akCollegeofNursingandHealthSciences,FlindersUniversity,Adelaide,Australia

alInstituteofPsychology,MarieCurie-SkłodowskaUniversity,Lublin,Poland

amFacultyofKinesiologyandPhysicalEducation,UniversityofToronto,Toronto,Canada anDepartmentofClinicalPsychology,IranUniversityofMedicalSciences,Tehran,Iran aoDepartmentofNursing,AllNationsUniversityCollege,Koforidua,Ghana

apDepartmentofPsychologyandPsychodynamics,KarlLandsteinerUniversityofHealthSciences,KremsanderDonau,Austria aqDepartmentofPsychology,ResearchMethods,Assessment,andiScience,UniversityofKonstanz,Konstanz,Germany arDepartmentofPsychology,FacultyofHumanities,ChiangMaiUniversity,ChiangMai,Thailand

asIndiraGandhiGovernmentGeneralHospitalandPostgraduateInstitute,Puducherry,India atDepartmentofPsychology,WestUniversityofTimis¸oara,Timis¸oara,Romania

auDepartmentofPsychology,OhioStateUniversity,Columbus,UnitedStates

avDepartmentofPsychologyandCounseling,UnitedArabEmiratesUniversity,AlAin,UnitedArabEmirates awDepartmentofPsychology,FundaciónUniversitariaKonradLonrez,Bogotá,Colombia

axDepartmentofPsychology,UniversidadLatinadeCostaRica,SanJosé,CostaRica ayCollegeofHealthSolutions,ArizonaStateUniversity,Phoenix,Arizona,UnitedStates azDepartmentofUndergraduateStudies,TempleUniversityJapan,Tokyo,Japan AaDepartmentofPhysicalEducation,SãoJudasTadeuUniversity,SãoPaulo,Brazil

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory:

Received13September2019

Receivedinrevisedform24January2020 Accepted25January2020

Availableonline4February2020

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

TheBreastSizeSatisfactionSurvey(BSSS)wasestablishedtoassesswomen’sbreastsizedissatisfaction andbreastedexperiencesfromacross-nationalperspective.Atotalof18,541womenwererecruitedfrom 61researchsitesacross40nationsandcompletedmeasuresofcurrent-idealbreastsizediscrepancy,as wellasmeasuresoftheorisedantecedents(personality,Westernandlocalmediaexposure,andproxies ofsocioeconomicstatus)andoutcomes(weightandappearancedissatisfaction,breastawareness,and

(4)

Keywords: Breastsize Breastideals Breastawareness Personality Psychologicalwell-being Cross-culturalpsychology

psychologicalwell-being).Inthetotaldataset,47.5%ofwomenwantedlargerbreaststhanthey cur-rentlyhad,23.2%wantedsmallerbreasts,and29.3%weresatisfiedwiththeircurrentbreastsize.There weresignificantcross-nationaldifferencesinmeanidealbreastsizeandabsolutebreastsize dissatisfac-tion,buteffectsizesweresmall(␩2=.02–.03).Theresultsofmultilevelmodellingshowedthatgreater Neuroticism,lowerConscientiousness,lowerWesternmediaexposure,greaterlocalmediaexposure, lowerfinancialsecurity,andyoungeragewereassociatedwithgreaterbreastsizedissatisfactionacross nations.Inaddition,greaterabsolutebreastsizedissatisfactionwasassociatedwithgreaterweightand appearancedissatisfaction,poorerbreastawareness,andpoorerpsychologicalwell-beingacrossnations. Theseresultsindicatethatbreastsizedissatisfactionisaglobalpublichealthconcernlinkedtowomen’s psychologicalandphysicalwell-being.

©2020ElsevierLtd.Allrightsreserved.

1. Introduction

Whenawoman placesherhandover herheart,it liesonand betweenherbreasts.Ifherchestisthehouseofherbeing,from whichradiatesherenergytomeettheworld,herbreastsarealso entwinedwithhersenseofself.[...]Formanywomen,ifnotall, breastsareanimportantcomponentofbodyself-image;awoman maylovethemordislikethem,butsheisrarelyneutral(Young, 1992,p.215).

Thebodyisahighlysymbolicandimportantsiteforthe cre-ation,negotiation,andperpetuationofsocialandpowerrelations

(Backett-Milburn&McKie,2001;Jeffreys,2005;Scheper-Hughes&

Lock,1987;Thompson,Heinberg,Altabe,&Tantleff-Dunn,1999).

In this view,macro-level culturalfactors – such as patriarchal structures (Bartky, 1990; Jeffreys, 2005), masculine aesthetics

(Dworkin,1974),andbeautysystems(Bordo,1993;Turner,1984)

– tie women’s moral, economic, and socio-political worth to theirphysicalappearanceandwillingnesstoenactpersonalbody projects.Concomitantly,thosestructuresorsystemsalso propa-gateprescriptiveandunrealisticbeautyideals(Swami,2015,2020), rewardingwomenwhostriveandattainthoseidealsandpunishing ordenigratingthosewhodonot(Forbes,Collinsworth,Jobe,Braun,

&Wise,2007;Kimmel,2011;Ramati-Ziber,Shnabel,&Glick,2019;

Swami,Colesetal.,2010).Totheextentthatthosebeautyideals

areinternalisedbywomen,itoftenresultsinahostof detrimen-taloutcomes(e.g.,negativebodyimage,symptomsofdisordered eating,poorer psychologicalwell-being)when women perceive themselves as being unable to embody those ideals (Rodgers,

Campagna,&Attawala,2019;Thompsonetal.,1999;Tomiyama

etal.,2018).

Muchof the academic literature hasfocused on attainment ofathinideal,butsomeworksuggeststhistheorisingmayalso applytowomen’sbreasts(Gripsrud,Ramvi,Froggett,Hellstrand,

&Manley,2018;Lee, 1997)and “breastedexperiences”(Young,

1992,p.215).Thisisunsurprisingconsideringevidencethatthe breastsplayanimportantroleinjudgementsandobjectificationof women(e.g.,Bareket,Shnabel,Abeles,Gervais,&Yuval-Greenberg,

2019;Dixson,Grimshaw,Linklater,&Dixson,2011;Swami&Tovée,

2013b),andarereportedtobeafocusofwomen’sbodyanxieties

(e.g.,Beck,Ward-Hull,&McLear,1976;Lee,1997;Millsted&Frith,

2003;Young,1992).Yet,comparativelylittleiscurrentlyknown

aboutwomen’sbreastdissatisfaction,particularlyintermsof cross-nationalvariations,aswellasantecedentsandoutcomes.Torectify thisgapintheliterature,wereporthereontheresultsoftheBreast SizeSatisfactionSurvey,across-nationalsurveyofbreastsize ide-alsandbreastsizedissatisfactioninwomenacross40nations.Not onlydoesthissurveyrepresentthelargestmulti-sitestudyever conductedontheissueofbreastsizedissatisfaction,italsoprovides importantinsightsthatwillbeofusetohealthcarepractitioners, policy-makers,andactivists.

1.1. Breastsizeanddissatisfaction

Women’sbreastsvaryalongmanydimensions,suchasshape, symmetry,andfirmness(Atiye&Chahine,2018),butmany con-temporarybeautysystemsreportedlyobjectifywomeninterms of theirbreastsize (Calogero &Thompson,2010; Mazur,1986;

Seifert,2005;Tantleff-Dunn,2001).Inparticular,largebreastsare

commodifiedandfetishisedinmanyformsofmainstreamWestern media(Einon,2012;Gerald&Potvin,2009;Ward,Merriwether,&

Caruthers,2006),particularlymediathat(re)produce

stereotypi-calgenderrolediscourse(Graff,Murnen,&Krause,2013;Yockey,

King,Vidourek,Burbage,&Merianos,2019)andheteronormative

expectations(Murnen,Poinsatte,Huntsman,Goldfarb,&Glaser,

2015;Rodriguez&Hernandez,2019;Rogers&Liebler,2017;for

a counter-point,seeWebb, Vinoski,Warren-Findlow,Burrell, &

Putz, 2017).Thatis,in contemporary culturalimagery,“female

breasts arenot celebrated orscrutinized forwhat theydo, but for how theyaresupposed to look”(Naugler,2009, p.101). In addition to de-coupling breasts fromtheir functional purposes (e.g.,child-feeding),contemporarypopularculturealsotieslarge breaststoconceptionsofwomanhood,somuchsothatlargebreasts havebecomeadefiningcharacteristicoffemininity(Dozier,2005;

Gripsrud,2008;Young,1992).

Thiscommodificationandscrutinisationofbreastscaninfluence howwomenfeelabouttheirownbodies(Bonillas,2009;Goldsmith

&Byers, 2016; Millsted &Frith, 2003; Webb, Jacox,&

Temple-Oberle,2019).StudiesofNorthAmericanandWesternEuropean

womenhavedocumentedhighlevelsofbreastsizedissatisfaction –typicallyoperationalisedasadiscrepancybetweencurrentand idealbreastsizesand,lessfrequently,asattitudesaboutbreastsize –sinceatleastthe1950s(Forbes&Frederick,2008;Forbes,Jobe,&

Revak,2006;Jacobi&Cash,1994;Jourard&Secord,1955;

Tantleff-Dunn,2002;Tantleff-Dunn&Thompson,2000).Forexample,one

largesurveyofNorthAmericanheterosexualwomen(N=26,703) reportedthatthemajority(70%)weredissatisfiedwithsomeaspect oftheirbreastsandthat,oftheseparticipants,28%wantedlarger breasts(Frederick,Peplau,&Lever,2008).InWesternEurope, stud-ieshavereportedthatamajorityofsampledwomenintheUnited Kingdom(Swami,Cavelti,Taylor,&Tovée,2015,Study3=54.3%, Study4=53.3%) or closeto amajority in theUnitedKingdom

(Swami,Caveltietal.,2015,Study2=49.7%;Swami&Furnham,

2018:44.2%)andItaly(Lombardo,Panasiti,Vacca,Grano,&Swami, 2019:44.4%)wantedlargerbreaststhantheycurrentlyhad.

Muchlessresearchhasexaminedratesofbreastsize dissatis-factionoutsideNorthAmericaandWesternEurope,aneglectthat isimportantbecausetheethnographicrecordsuggestssome vari-ationinidealbreastsize,atleasthistorically(Ford&Beach,1951). Inparticular,someculturesmayessentialisetheperformativityof womanhood andgender identitythroughsymbolicassociations with largebreasts. For example, somescholars have discussed theheightenedculturalpressureexperiencedbyLatinAmerican

(5)

womentodemonstratefemininityand sexualitythroughbreast fullness(Correa&Shohamy,2018;Guimarãesetal.,2015). Con-sistentwiththisview,Junqueiraetal.(2019)reportedthat65.5% ofBrazilianwomenintheirstudydesiredlargerbreaststhanthey currentlyhad,althoughitshouldbenotedthesampleconsisted primarilyofyoungwomen.Incontrast,EastandSoutheastAsian womentypicallyhavesmallerbreaststhanwomeninotherparts oftheworld(Limetal.,2018),andsomeliteraturehasproposed thatbreastshavehistoricallyplayedasubsidiaryroleintermsof theculturalobjectificationofwomeninthisregion(Miller,2003;

seealsoMenon,2019)andareemphasisedlessoftenin

percep-tionsoffemininity(B ˛ak-Sosnowska,Pawlicka,&Warchał,2016;

seealsoChing&Xu,2019).Likewise,somenon-Western

commu-nitiesmayconstruelargebreastsasproblematic,eitherbecause largebreastsareperceivedasacauseofdisease(e.g.,breastcancer;

Naanyuetal.,2015)orbecausebeinglarge-breastedis

stereotypi-callyassociatedwithhypersexuality(Liebelt,2019)andnarcissism

(seeSmith,2017).

Nevertheless, it is also possible that breast size ideals are becomingincreasinglyhomogenisedacrosstheglobe.For exam-ple,in relation to thethin ideal, scholars have discussed how theinter-relatedforces ofglobalisation(particularlyinterms of theproliferationofWesternmassmediaandWesternisedbeauty ideals)andmodernisation(e.g.,changingnutritionalandlifestyle patterns, disruptions to gender inequality and traditional gen-derroles)haveservedtoenforceahomogenisedpreference for thinnessacrossmuchoftheglobe(Anderson-Fye,2018;Brewis,

Wutich,Falletta-Cowden,&Rodriguez-Soto,2011;Swami,2015).

Thisissupportedbyempiricaldatashowingarelatively standard-isedpreferenceforfemalethinnessandsimilarratesofactual-ideal weightdiscrepancyacrossnations (e.g.,Swami,Fredericketal., 2010).Similarprocesseshavenotbeenexaminedvis-à-visbreast size,althoughsomescholarshavesuggestedtheincreasing com-modificationandobjectificationoflargebreastsinsomepartsof theworld(e.g.,inEastandSoutheastAsia;Rongmuangetal.,2011;

Wong,2019),aswellasamarkedshiftawayfromafocusonthe

functionsofthebreaststowardtheirtreatmentinpurelyaesthetic terms(Miller,2006).Intheabsenceofempiricaldata,however,it isdifficulttoknowtowhatextentahomogenisingpatterncould beobservedglobally,andsoafirstaimofthepresentstudywasto examinebreastsizeidealsandbreastdissatisfactionacrossdiverse nationalsites.

1.2. Antecedentsofbreastsizedissatisfaction

Here,wealsoconsideredantecedentsofbreastsizeidealsand breastsizedissatisfaction,whichhavenotbeeninvestigatedinany depthinpreviousresearch(Lombardoetal.,2019).Onesuch poten-tialantecedentissocioeconomicstatus:intermsofthethinideal, forexample,cross-sectional (e.g.,Mintem, Horta,Domingues,&

Gigante,2015;Swami,Fredericketal.,2010;Swami,Jones,Einon,

&Furnham,2009;Swami&Tovée,2005b,2005b),experimental

(Swami&Tovée,2006),andethnographic(Anderson-Fye&Brewis,

2017)researchhasdocumentedaninverserelationshipbetween socioeconomicstatusandidealisationofathinideal.Inexplanation, ithasbeensuggestedthatindividualsfromsitesofrelativelylow socioeconomicstatusmayidealiselargerbodysizesbecausebody fatissymbolicofresourcesecurity(e.g.,accesstofoodorwealth), whereasthinnesscomestobeassociatedwithwealthandstatusin sitesofrelativelyhighersocioeconomicstatus(forreviews,seeFox,

Feng,&Asal,2019;Swami,2015,2020).Asimilarpatternhasbeen

proposedforbreast sizeideals, withcross-sectionaland experi-mentalresearchsuggestingapreferenceforlargerbreastsizeswith decreasingsocioeconomicstatus(Dixson,Vaseyetal.,2011;Swami

&Tovée,2013a;butseeGray&Frederick,2012).Assuch,itmight

beexpectedthattherewouldbevariationinwhatisperceivedto

betheidealbreastsizeasafunctionofinter-individualdifferences insocioeconomicstatus.

Ontheotherhand,ratesofbreastsizedissatisfaction(i.e.,a dis-crepancybetweencurrentandidealbreastsizes)mightbeexpected tobelower among participantsof lower socioeconomicstatus. For example,based ondata fromresearchsites in two nations (MalaysiaandSouthAfrica)thatdifferedinsocioeconomicstatus,

Swami,Fredericketal.(2010)reportedsignificantlylower

actual-idealweightdiscrepancy inparticipantsfromsites ofrelatively lowcomparedtohighsocioeconomicstatus.Similarfindingshave alsobeenreportedinBrazil(Laus,Miranda,Almedia,BragaCosta,

&Ferreira,2012),MalaysianBorneo(Swami,Kannan,&Furnham,

2012),andMexico(Penelo,Negrete,Portell,&Raich,2013),with participantsfromsitesofhighersocioeconomicstatusreporting morenegativebodyimagecomparedtothosefromsitesoflower socioeconomicstatus.Thissuggeststhatparticipantsofrelatively lowsocioeconomicstatuswouldreportlowerratesofbreastsize dissatisfaction.Thereareanumberofreasonsforexpectingthis patternofresults(forreviews,seeAnderson-Fye&Brewis,2017;

Swami,2015), includinglowerpressurefrombeautysystemsto

attainbeautyidealsinsitesofrelativelylowsocioeconomicstatus andlowerexposuretoWesternmassmediathatconstructlarge breastsasideal.

In terms of the latter, previous cross-national research has shown that exposureto Western mediais significantly associ-atedwithboththeidealisationofthinfemalebodiesandwomen’s bodydissatisfaction(Swami,Fredericketal.,2010).Someresearch hasexaminedtheimpactof exposuretomassmediaonbreast sizeideals and breastsize dissatisfaction.For example, qualita-tiveresearchwithwomenfromtheUnitedStateshasimplicated exposuretolargebreastsdepictedinmassmediaasanimportant influenceonbreastsizedissatisfaction,despiteparticipants’stated dislikeof those mediated images (Goodman & Walsh-Childers, 2004).Similarly,exposuretoidealisedbodiesinmassmediahas beenassociatedwithapreferenceforlargerbreastsincollege-aged NorthAmericanwomen(Harrison,2003)andhasalsobeen associ-atedwithnegativefeelingsaboutwomen’sownbreasts(Frederick,

Daniels,Bates,&Tylka,2017).OnestudywithruralNicaraguans

foundthatexposuretoWesterntelevisionwasassociatedwitha preferenceforlargerbreasts(Thornborrow,Jucker,Boothroyd,&

Tovée,2018),althoughthestudyonlyconsideredmen’sratingsof

women.

Alternatively,somescholarshavesuggestedthatmediaeffects havebeenexaggerated(e.g.,Ferguson,2018;Swami,2020).For example,Ferguson’s(2013)meta-analysis,which included stud-iesthatweremainlyconductedinadvancedeconomies,concluded therewasnooveralleffectofmediaexposureonwomen’sbody dissatisfaction.Instead,theyconcludedthatmediaeffectsonbody dissatisfactionwerelimitedtothosewithpre-existingbody dissat-isfactionorpossiblythosewithvulnerablepersonalitytraits,such ashighNeuroticism.Infact,asidefromthispotentialmoderating role,Neuroticismhasalsobeenfoundtobedirectlyassociatedwith morenegativebodyimage(Frederick, Sandhu,Morse,&Swami,

2016;Sutin&Terracciano,2016;Swami,Taylor,&Carvalho,2011,

2013).Indeed,onerecentsystematic reviewreportedthat Neu-roticismwaspositivelyassociated withnegativebody imagein women(Allen&Walter,2016),possiblybecauseindividualshighin Neuroticismaremoreself-consciousandvulnerabletobodyimage threats.Relationshipsbetweenotherpersonalitytraitsandbody imagearemoreequivocal(Allen&Celestino,2018;Allen&Walter,

2016;Swami&Furnham,2016),thoughitshouldbenotedthatno

studyhasexaminedassociationswithbreastsizedissatisfaction specifically.

(6)

1.3. Outcomesofbreastsizedissatisfaction

Intermsofthebodyimageliterature,breastsizedissatisfaction istypicallyconceptualisedasafacetofnegativebodyimagemore generally(Ålgarsetal.,2011;Swami,Caveltietal.,2015).Thus, wewouldexpectsignificantassociationsbetweenbreastsize dis-satisfactionandotherindicesofnegativebodyimage.Indeed,the availableevidencesupportssuchassociations:greaterbreastsize dissatisfactionhasbeenfoundtobesignificantlyassociatedwith greaterbodydissatisfaction(Forbes&Frederick,2008;Frederick

etal.,2008;Swami,Caveltietal.,2015;Swami&Furnham,2018),

greater actual-idealweight discrepancy (Junqueira et al., 2019;

Swami,Caveltietal.,2015), lowerbodyappreciation(Junqueira

etal.,2019;Swami,Caveltietal.,2015),greaterappearance

pre-occupation(Koff&Benavage,1998),andgreaterdriveforthinness

(Swami,Caveltietal.,2015).Thestrengthofassociationshas

gen-erally been weak-to-moderate (e.g., Swami & Furnham, 2018), althoughitshouldbenotedthatatleastonestudyfoundno sig-nificantassociationwithbodyimagedisturbance(Tantleff-Dunn

&Thompson,2000).Beyondindicesof bodyimage,twostudies

have reported that greater breast size dissatisfaction is signifi-cantly,albeitweakly, associatedwithlowerself-esteem(Koff &

Benavage,1998;Swami,Caveltietal.,2015;butforanulleffect,

seeTantleff-Dunn&Thompson,2000).

Anotherimportantoutcomeofbreastsizedissatisfactionmaybe decreasedbreastawareness,whichinvolvesfamiliaritywithone’s breasts(i.e.,howtheynormallylookandfeel,andhowtheychange throughouta woman’slife),as wellas theconfidence tonotice anddetectanychange(e.g.,throughbreastself-examinationfor breastcancersymptoms)andtoseekimmediatemedicalhelpwhen achangeis detected(McCready,Littlewood,&Jenkinson,2005;

Thornton&Pillarisetti,2008).Althoughhealthcarepoliciesinmany

countriescontinuetoadvocate breast self-examinationalone, a growingbody ofevidencesuggeststhat,whendoneonitsown andwithoutpropertraining,breastself-examinationmaynot nec-essarilyimprovebreastcancerdetectionandmortalityreduction (forareview,seeKösters&Gøtzsche,2003).Instead,amore holis-ticapproachbasedonbreastawarenessappearstoofferimproved efficacyintermsofbreastcancerdetection(Harmer,2011;Mant, 1991).Importantly,inacohortofBritishwomen,greaterbreastsize dissatisfaction–butnotbodydissatisfaction–wassignificantly associatedwithlowerbreastawareness(i.e.,lessfrequentbreast self-examination, lower confidence in detecting breast change, andgreaterdelayinseekingprofessionalhelpfollowingabreast change) (Swami &Furnham,2018).In explanation,theauthors suggestedthat theseassociationsmaystem fromwomen seek-ingtoavoidfocusingonthesiteoftheirdissatisfactionornegative emotions(e.g.,shameandembarrassment)thatresultfromvisual inspectionofone’sbreasts.

Whilethesestudiesareimportantintheirownright,itis impor-tanttonotethat theyaregenerallylimited topopulationsin a smallhandfulofworldregions(i.e.,NorthAmericaandWestern Europe)anditisuncleartowhatextentsimilarassociations(i.e., withnegativebodyimage, psychologicalwell-being, and breast awareness)mightbeexpectedinsamplesfromagreaterdiversity of populations.The associationbetweenbreast size dissatisfac-tionandbreastawarenessinparticulardeservesgreaterattention acrossnations. For example, ifscholars areto design interven-tionstopromoteimprovedbreastawarenessbasedonreducing breastsizedissatisfaction,itwillfirstbeimportanttoshowthat theassociationsbetweenthesevariablesarerobustacrossnational groups.However,cross-nationaldifferencesinculturalbeliefsand attitudes(e.g.,bodyorbreast embarrassment,lackof a preven-tivehealthorientation,andfatalism;forareview,seeLee,2015) mayattenuateanyassociationbetweenbreastsizedissatisfaction andbreastawarenessinsomenationalgroups.Forthesereasons,

weexaminedassociationsbetweenbreastsizedissatisfactionand keyoutcomevariables inthepresentstudy,namelyweightand appearancedissatisfaction, psychologicalwell-being, and breast awareness.

1.4. TheBreastSizeSatisfactionSurvey

Whilethenumberofstudiesfocusedonbreastsize dissatisfac-tionhasgrown,animportantlimitationofthisresearchisthatithas primarilyconsideredtheexperiencesofwomeninNorthAmerica andWesternEurope,totheexclusionofwomeninotherpartsof theworld.Thisisnotablebecauseitshouldnotbeassumedthatall populationswillsharesimilarexperiencesoftheirbreastsorthat findingsfromWestern,educated,industrialised,rich,and demo-cratic(WEIRD)societieswillgeneralisetoothersettings(Henrich,

Heine,&Norenzayan,2010).Moreover,as discussedby Swami,

Cavelti et al.(2015), there areadditional limitations that

ham-per our understanding of women’s breasted experiences, even in WEIRD populations, including wide variation in the manner inwhichbreastsizedissatisfactionisoperationalised,theuseof measuresofbreastsizedissatisfactionwithunknownoruntested psychometricproperties,andarelianceonsamplesofcollege-aged women.Inshort,thereisasyetnosystematicinvestigationofbreast sizeidealsandbreastsizedissatisfactionacrossalargeanddiverse setofnations,andscholarshavecalledforfurtherworkthatfocuses onwomen’sbreastedexperiences(Gillen&Markey,2019;Prieler

&Choi,2014).

Toaddresstheseissues,wereportonthefindingsoftheBreast SizeSatisfactionSurvey(BSSS),whichinvolved18,541womenin 61researchsitesacross40nations completingavalidated mea-sure of perceptual breast size dissatisfaction (i.e.,current-ideal breastsizediscrepancy;Swami,Caveltietal.,2015).Thisallowed us to examine the extent to which there exist cross-national differencesinratingsofidealbreastsizeand breastsize dissat-isfaction.Basedonpreviouscross-nationalresearchonbodyimage (e.g.,Swami,Fredericketal.,2010),weexpectedtheretobe sig-nificant cross-national differences across both scores, but also expectedthose differencestobenegligible-to-small instrength (Hypothesis1).Thisalsomirrorstheargumentspresentedabove, which suggestthatbreast sizeidealsare becomingincreasingly homogenisedoruniformacrosstheglobeasaresultofthe pro-cessesofglobalisationandmodernisation.Inadditiontotestingfor thesecross-nationaldifferences,theBSSSdatasetalsoallowedusto examineantecedentsandoutcomesofbreastsizedissatisfaction.

In terms of antecedents, we includedvalidated measuresof theBigFivepersonalityfacetsandnovelmeasuresofexposureto Westernandlocalmedia,whichweexaminedforcross-national measurement invariance here. In addition, we also included measures of financial security and urbanicity as proxies for socioeconomicstatus and, for exploratorypurposes,participant age. In terms of outcomes,we includedvalidated measures of appearanceandweightdissatisfaction,breastawareness(breast self-examinationfrequency,confidenceindetectingbreastchange, anddelayinseekingprofessionalhelpfollowingbreastchange), and psychological well-being (subjective happiness and self-esteem).Multilevelmodellingwasusedtoexaminetherobustness of these antecedents and outcomes of breast size dissatisfac-tion across nations. In terms of antecedents, we hypothesised thatpersonality(specifically,higherNeuroticism),greater expo-suretoWesternand localmedia,greaterfinancialsecurity,and higherurbanicitywouldbesignificantlyassociatedwithgreater breast sizedissatisfaction (Hypothesis2).Interms ofoutcomes, wehypothesisedthatgreaterbreastsizedissatisfactionwouldbe significantly associated with greater appearancedissatisfaction, greaterweightdissatisfaction,lowerbreastawareness(i.e.,lower breastself-examinationfrequency,lowerconfidenceindetecting

(7)

Fig.1. SchematicRepresentationoftheHypothesisedAssociationsbetweenAntecedentsandOutcomesofBreastSizeDissatisfaction.Note:Personality=Opennessto Experience,Conscientiousness,Agreeableness,Extraversion,andNeuroticism;BSE=BreastSelf-Examination;Confidence=ConfidenceinNoticingBreastChange.

breastchange,andgreaterdelayinseekingprofessionalhelp fol-lowing breast change), lowerhappiness, and lower self-esteem (Hypothesis3).Fig.1presentsaschematicrepresentationofthese hypothesisedassociations,whichweexpectedtobestableacross allnationssurveyedintheBSSS.

2. Method

2.1. Breastsizesatisfactionsurveyoverview

ThedatareportedherewerecollectedaspartoftheBSSS,a col-laborativeresearchprojectinvolving104scientistsworkingacross 61researchsitesin40nations.Acallforcollaboratorswasplaced onasocialnetworkingsiteforscientistsinSeptember2017,sent outtopotentialcollaboratorsinternationallybythefirstauthor, anddisseminatedopportunisticallyanddirectlytointerested par-tiesuntilDecember2018.TheBSSSdataset,therefore,represents sitesthatwereselectedonaconveniencebasis.Thisisreflected inthefactthat,ofthe61researchsites,mostwereinAsia(23) andEurope(22).Incontrast,therewerefewerresearchsitesin SouthAmerica(8),NorthAmerica(5),Africa(2),andAustralia(1). OnceacollaboratoragreedtobeinvolvedintheBSSS,theywere expectedtoapplyforlocalethicsapprovalwhererequired, trans-late(wherenecessary)andadapt theBSSSsurveyfor localuse, recruitaminimumof200women(relaxedinfourcases:Koforidua, Osaka,Rijeka,andTelAviv)fromthecommunity(relaxedinfour cases–Germany,Hungary,theNetherlands,andPeru–where par-ticipantswererecruitedonline)tocompletetheBSSSsurvey,and returnthedatatothefirstauthorbyApril2019.

2.2. Participants

The BSSS dataset included a total of 18,541 women from 40 nations, ranging in age from 19 to 94 years (M=34.19, SD=13.71)andinself-reportedbodymassindex(BMI)from12.04 to49.78kg/m2(M=23.57,SD=4.54).Intermsofethnic/racial

affil-iation,78.9%self-reportedasbeingmembersoftheethnic/racial majorityoftheirrespectivenationsand9.6%asmembersofan ethnic/racialminority,whereas 11.5%wereunsure.Interms of educationalattainment,0.7%hadnoformaleducational qualifi-cation,4.0%hadcompletedprimaryeducation,21.8%secondary education,21.6%werestillinfull-timeeducation,30.8%hadan undergraduatedegree,16.6%apostgraduatedegree,and4.5%had someotherqualification.Interms oftheircurrentplaceof resi-dence,31.0%ofparticipantslivedinacapitalcity,14.3%incapital citysuburbs,26.1%inaprovincialcity(morethan100,000

resi-dents),16.7%inaprovincialtown(morethan10,000residents), and11.8%inruralareas.Withregardstotheirfinancialsecurity, 22.5%ofparticipantsreportedthattheyfeltlesssecurerelativeto othersoftheirownageintheircountryofresidence,57.8%equally secure,and19.8%moresecure.

Thelistofresearchsitesandnations,includingnation-specific sample characteristics are reported in Table 1 (for individual research sites and associated sample sizes, seeSupplementary Materials). Nations differed with a large effect size in mean age, F(39, 17441)=73.92, p< .001, ␩2=.14 (lowest: Malaysia,

highest: Norway), with a medium effect size in mean BMI, F(38, 18088)=46.49, p< .001, ␩2=.09 (lowest: China, highest:

Egypt), and with a small-to-medium effect size in mean self-rated financial security, F(39, 18422)=21.28, p< .001, ␩2=.04

(lowest:Brazil,highest:UnitedArabEmirates).Therewerealso significantbetween-nationdifferencesinthedistributionof par-ticipants by ethnicity status, ␹2(78, 18367)=2875.01, p< .001,

W=.40(lowestaffiliationtomajority:CostaRicaandIndia, high-est:ThailandandCroatia),education(secondary/tertiaryvs.other), ␹2(39,18432)=1891.20,p<.001,W=.32(lowest:China,highest:

Austria),andurbanicity(urbanvs.rural),␹2(39,18468)=1871.53,

p<.001,W=.32(lowest:Ireland,highest:Colombia,Pakistan,and Peru).

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Breastsizedissatisfaction

AllparticipantswereaskedtocompletetheBreastSizeRating Scale(BSRS;Swami,Caveltietal.,2015),afiguralratingscale con-sistingof14computer-generatedimagesofwomenwithincreasing breastsize.Imageswerepresentedingreyscaleandwithoutthe appearanceoffacialfeaturessoastominimisetheimpactofthese featuresonratings.Participantswereaskedtoratetheimagethat mostcloselymatchedtheircurrentbreastsizeandtheimagethey wouldmostliketopossess,withresponsesmadeona14-point scale(1representingthefigurewiththesmallestbreastsize,14 rep-resentingthefigurewiththelargestbreastsize).Anindexofbreast sizedissatisfactioncanbecomputedasboththeabsolutedifference betweenidealandcurrentbreastsizeratings(sothathigherscores reflectgreaterbreastsizedissatisfactionregardlessofthedirection ofthisdissatisfaction)orassignedscores(sothatnegativevalues representadesireforsmallerbreastsandpositivevaluesrepresent adesireforlargerbreasts).Bothscoreswereusedinour analy-ses,assignpostedbelow.ScoresderivedfromtheBSRShavebeen showntohavegoodconstructvalidity,acceptabletest-retest

(8)

relia-Table1

SampleDescriptionsofDatafromtheBreastSizeSatisfactionSurvey.

Nation n Meanage Meanbodymass

index %Ethnic/racial majority %Secondary/ tertiary education %Urban residence Meanfinancial security

%Preferenceforlarger breasts/smaller breasts/satisfiedwith currentbreastsize

Australia 201 36.30(13.95) 24.54(4.87) 86 90 97 2.17(0.57) 43/28/28 Austria 229 27.20(11.28) 22.56(3.57) 83 91 87 2.13(0.69) 45/24/31 Brazil 822 31.74(10.85) 24.36(4.12) 53 70 95 1.68(0.64) 51/21/28 Canada 463 38.19(15.46) 25.19(5.30) 79 75 90 2.21(0.64) 41/30/29 China 897 24.16(9.19) 20.45(2.73) 91 27 98 1.92(0.56) 73/14/13 Colombia 483 37.72(13.83) 24.62(3.68) 77 64 100 1.91(0.68) 33/11/57 CostaRica 225 34.05(13.16) 24.95(4.65) 44 81 96 2.09(0.65) 41/23/36 Croatia 414 33.62(13.77) 22.81(3.28) 94 69 89 2.16(0.59) 55/17/28 Cyprus 284 34.26(12.21) 23.31(4.67) 90 78 83 1.89(0.64) 41/22/37 Egypt 200 34.27(9.43) 29.07(6.17) 87 77 99 2.13(0.70) 52/36/13 Germany 387 27.21(13.40) 21.97(3.06) 87 83 74 2.19(0.65) 54/20/26 Ghana 174 28.18(9.37) 24.68(6.87) 43 79 94 2.11(0.73) 33/16/52 Greece 1888 34.74(13.15) 23.41(4.05) 81 64 92 1.93(0.62) 47/23/30 Hungary 831 28.50(11.60) 23.52(4.84) 88 54 82 2.02(0.64) 51/19/30 India 441 38.54(9.19) 24.63(4.26) 43 72 75 1.98(0.73) 42/40/18 Indonesia 266 28.77(8.86) 22.66(3.94) 89 72 85 2.04(0.59) 51/19/30 Iran 946 33.92(8.86) 24.71(4.55) 94 89 98 2.02(0.63) 34/33/33 Ireland 219 34.11(16.51) 24.19(4.59) 71 66 36 1.90(0.62) 41/34/26 Israel 187 36.65(13.85) 23.03(3.43) 82 80 83 2.19(0.63) 37/27/36 Italy 747 39.34(13.76) 22.88(4.24) 87 75 89 1.94(0.58) 49/23/29 Japan 423 39.86(17.88) 20.98(2.77) 82 53 94 2.07(0.53) 70/20/10 Lebanon 406 34.87(10.84) 24.45(4.01) 48 90 98 1.79(0.62) 56/20/24 Malaysia 720 22.50(4.81) 22.25(4.67) 71 79 82 1.75(0.66) 55/23/22 Netherlands 512 43.34(15.21) 25.68(5.12) 83 65 69 2.00(0.64) 34/26/40 Norway 254 49.94(15.05) 25.26(4.28) 88 82 71 2.27(0.61) 28/28/44 Pakistan 419 37.68(14.18) 23.55(4.60) 79 85 100 2.20(0.76) 30/49/21 Paraguay 202 37.45(14.32) 26.15(4.91) 80 44 83 2.10(0.70) 31/19/51 Peru 232 31.89(11.65) 24.00(3.56) 47 54 100 2.03(0.69) 51/16/32 Philippines 200 42.29(21.04) N/A 77 85 94 1.93(0.80) 45/22/34 Poland 999 33.33(13.61) 23.21(4.00) 90 71 76 1.92(0.53) 50/22/28 Portugal 203 41.99(15.23) 24.26(3.99) 78 63 96 1.93(0.62) 33/21/45 Romania 428 30.49(13.64) 22.56(5.07) 62 39 79 1.95(0.66) 54/17/29 Serbia 211 34.28(12.90) 22.55(3.86) 83 73 95 2.07(0.70) 50/15/36 Slovenia 343 35.93(13.23) 23.36(3.83) 82 85 72 1.94(0.47) 50/20/31 Spain 712 37.94(14.12) 23.49(4.44) 83 70 72 1.98(0.64) 36/19/45 Thailand 644 29.59(9.24) 22.56(4.89) 95 70 90 1.87(0.59) 60/17/23 Turkey 211 34.13(11.38) 23.65(4.54) 71 64 99 1.69(0.62) 46/26/28 UAE 205 26.71(9.78) 24.23(4.84) 75 64 89 2.30(0.68) 52/28/20 UK 204 36.70(14.62) 24.01(2.75) 90 77 94 1.86(0.73) 56/32/25 USA 699 38.51(14.85) 24.88(5.39) 69 82 91 1.96(0.77) 43/32/25 Total 18541 34.11(13.39) 23.58(13.69) 79 69 88 1.97(0.65) 48/23/30

Note.Numbersaremeansandstandarddeviations(inparentheses),andpercentages(roundedtothenearestinteger)whereindicatedotherwise.Numberswerebasedon allavailabledata.UAE=UnitedArabEmirates,UK=UnitedKingdom,USA=UnitedStatesofAmerica;%Preferenceforlargerbreasts/smallerbreasts/satisfiedwithbreast size=percentagesofparticipantswithsignedbreastsizedissatisfactionscores>0/<0/=0.

bilityupto3months,andadequatepatternsofconvergentvalidity

inwomen(Junqueiraetal.,2019;Swami,Caveltietal.,2015).

2.3.2. Appearanceandweightdissatisfaction

FollowingFredericket al.(2016),participantswereaskedto respondtotwoitemsaskingabouttheirsatisfactionwith physi-calappearanceandweight,respectively(1=Extremelydissatisfied, 7 =Extremelysatisfied). Althoughsingle-item measuresof body imageareunlikelytocapturethecomplexityofbodyimage con-structs, theirscores neverthelesspresent adequate evidence of convergentvalidity(Sandhu&Frederick,2015).Foranalytic pur-poses,scores onboth itemswere reverse-codedsothat higher scoresreflectedgreaterdissatisfactionwithone’sappearanceand weight.

2.3.3. Personality

ParticipantswereaskedtocompletetheFive-ItemPersonality Inventory(FIPI;Gosling,Rentfrow,&Swann,2003),which mea-surestheBigFivepersonalityfacetsofOpennesstoExperience(“I seemyselfasopentonewexperiences”),Conscientiousness(“...as dependableandself-disciplined”),Extraversion(“...asextroverted andenthusiastic”),Agreeableness(“...aswarmandsympathetic

toothers”),andNeuroticism(“...anxiousandeasilyupset”)with oneitemforeach facet.Allitemswereratedona 5-pointscale rangingfrom1(stronglydisagree)to5(stronglyagree).Although therearelimitationstomeasuringtheBigFive facetsusing sin-gleitems,Gosling etal.(2003)reportedthat scoresontheFIPI haveadequatelevelsofconvergentvalidity(e.g.,significant correla-tionswithothermeasuresoftheBigFive)andadequatetest-retest reliabilityuptotwoweeks.

2.3.4. Mediaexposure

To measureexposureto Westernand localmass media,we adaptedthemediaexposurescaledevelopedandusedbySwami,

Fredericketal.(2010).Initsoriginalversion,thescaleincluded8

itemsthataskedaboutfrequencyofexposuretoWestern(4items) andlocal(4items)televisionshows,movies,magazines,andmusic, butwereplacedthetwomusicitemswithtwoitemsabout expo-suretoInternetsitesin thepresentstudy.Allitemswererated ona5-pointscale(1=Lessthanonceamonth,2=Onceortwicea month,3=Onceaweek,4=Severaltimesaweek,5=Everyday).The dimensionality,internalconsistency,andmeasurementinvariance ofscoresonthismeasurearereportedintheResults.

(9)

2.3.5. Breastawareness

Toprovideanindexofbreastawareness,wefollowedSwami

andFurnham(2018)inusingthreeitemsfromtheBreast

Mod-uleoftheCancerAwarenessMeasure(BCAM;Linselletal.,2010). TheBCAMisaself-reportedmeasureofmultipledomainsofbreast cancerawarenesswithadequateconstructvalidityandtest-retest reliabilityupto2weeks.Thefirstitem askedaboutbreast self-examinationfrequency(“Howoftendoyoucheckyourbreast?”) withresponsesmadeona4-pointscale(1=Rarelyornever,2=At leastonceeverysixmonths,3=Atleastonceamonth,4=Atleast onceaweek).Linselletal.(2010)suggestedthatwomenweremore likelytobebreastawareiftheyengagedinbreastself-examination atleastonceaweekoronceamonth.Theseconditemaskedabout participants’confidenceinnoticingachangeintheirbreasts(“Are youconfidentyouwouldnoticeachangeinyourbreasts?),with responsesmadeona 4-pointscale (1=Notatall confident,2= Slightlyconfident,3=Fairlyconfident,4=Veryconfident).Thefinal itemaskedparticipants,usinganopen-endedformat,howsoon theywouldcontactahealthprofessionaliftheynoticedachange intheirbreasts(“Ifyoufoundachangeinyourbreasts,howsoon wouldyoucontactyourdoctor?).Responsestothequestionwere scoredbasedona7-pointscaledevelopedbySwamiandFurnham

(2018):1=Immediatelyorassoonaspossible,2=Withinafewdays,

3=Withinaweek,4=Withinamonth,5=Withinthreemonths,6 =Delayaslongaspossible,7=Wouldnotseedoctor.Researchers ineachsitescoredparticipants’responsesascloselyaspossible totheseresponseoptionsandinsomecasesdiscussed categori-sationswithotherscientistsunaffiliatedwiththeprojectand/or withthefirstauthor. For comparativepurposes, womenin the UnitedKingdomareencouragedtocontacttheirdoctorora health-careprofessionalassoonaspossibleupondiscoveringachangein theirbreasts,asearlydetectionincreasesthelikelihoodofpositive outcomes(NationalHealthService,2015).

2.3.6. Psychologicalwell-being

Twosingle-itemmeasureswereusedtoassessdistinctaspects ofpsychologicalwell-being.First,weaskedparticipantsto com-pletetheGlobalHappinessItem(Bradburn,1969),whichprovides an index of subjectively-assessed happiness(“Taking all things together,howwouldyousaythingsarethesedays?”).Theitem wasratedona3-pointscale(1=Nottoohappy,2=Prettyhappy,3= Veryhappy)andscoreshavebeenshowntohaveadequate conver-gentvalidity(e.g.,Lyubomirsky&Lepper,1999;Swami,2008).The seconditemwastheSingle-ItemSelf-EsteemScale(SISE;Robins,

Hendin,&Trzesniewski,2001),inwhichparticipantsareaskedto

ratethestatement“Ihavehighself-esteem”ona7-pointscale(1 =Notverytrueofme,7=Verytrueofme).SISEscoreshavebeen showntohaveadequateconstructvalidity(Robinsetal.,2001).

2.3.7. Socioeconomicstatus

Becauseunderstandingsofsocioeconomicstatusarelikelyto varybothwithinandacrossnations,andduetodifficultiescreating acommonmetricofsocioeconomicstatusacrossnations,weused twoproxiesforsocioeconomicstatus inthepresentwork.First, followingSwami,Kannanetal.(2012),weaskedparticipantsto self-reporthowfinanciallysecuretheyfeltrelativetoothersoftheir ownageintheircountryofresidence(1=Lesssecure,2=Same,3 =Moresecure).Second,weaskedparticipantsabouttheircurrent placeofresidence(i.e.,urbanicity),withresponseoptionsadapted

fromPedersenandMortensen(2001)asfollows:Capitalcity,Capital

citysuburbs,Provincialcity(morethan100,000residents),Provincial town(morethan10,000residents),andRuralareas.Responseoptions werecollapsedintourbanversusruralfordescriptivepurposesat thenationallevelandwereassignedvalues1–5(intheaboveorder) forfurtherstatisticalanalysis.

2.3.8. Demographics

Participantswereaskedtoprovidetheirdemographicdata con-sisting of age, height,and weight using open-ended questions. Heightandweightwererecodedintokilogramsandmetres,and wereusedtocomputeself-reportedBMIaskg/m2.Self-reported

height and weight data are strongly correlated with measured

data(Spencer,Appleby,Davey,&Key,2002)andwereincludedin

thepresentstudyfordescriptivepurposes.2ImprobableBMI

val-ues(<12or>50kg/m2;Swami,Weis,Barron,&Furnham,2018)

werediscarded(0.1%ofthetotaldataset).Participantsalso indi-catedtheirhighesteducationalqualificationbyselectingoneof several options presented (No formal education, Primary educa-tion,Secondaryeducation,Stillinfull-timeeducation,Undergraduate degree, Postgraduate degree, Other). Response options were col-lapsedintosecondary/tertiary(Secondaryeducation,Undergraduate degree,Postgraduatedegree)versusother(remainingcategories)for descriptivepurposesatthenationallevel.Finally,toensure com-parability of ethnicor racial backgrounds acrossresearchsites, participantswereaskedtoself-describetheirethnic/racial affilia-tionbyselectingoneofthreepresentedoptionsrelativetotheir nationofresidence(Ethnic/racialmajority,Ethnic/racial minority, Notsure).

2.4. Testadaptation

WhereEnglishwasnottheprimarylanguageorlinguafrancain anation,themeasuresincludedintheBSSSsurveyweretranslated intotheappropriatelocallanguage(seeSupplementary Materi-alsforthelistoflanguages).ThiswasdoneusingBrislin’s(1970)

back-translationtechnique,whichusesaniterativeprocessof inde-pendentforward-andback-translationbyindependentbilingual translators.Specifically,abilingual translatorfirstblindly trans-latedthe measuresincludedin theBSSS questionnaire – along withinstructions,responsecategories,aninformationsheet,and aninformedconsentform–fromEnglishtothelocallanguage.A secondbilingualtranslatorthenback-translatedthematerialfrom thelocallanguagetoEnglish.Thetwoversionsofthematerials werecompared for equivalence and, where issues were raised, thesewerediscussedwiththefirstauthorandresolvedthrough consensus.In practice,therewere veryfew translationalissues encountered.Alltranslationsareavailablefromthefirstauthor. 2.5. Procedures

Ethicsapprovalfortheoverallprojectanddatamanagement wasobtainedfromthedepartmentalethicscommitteeatAnglia Ruskin University (approval number: ESH17-006). In addition, mostBSSScollaboratorsobtainedethicsapprovalfromlocalethics committeesorInstitutionalReviewBoards.Threeexceptionsto thisoccurredinAustria,CostaRica,andGermany,wherenational lawsdidnotrequireethicsapproval.Intheseand allcases,the projectwasconducted inaccordance withtheprinciplesofthe Declaration of Helsinki (6th revision, 2008) and following local

institutionalguidelines.OncetheBSSSinstrumentshadbeen trans-latedand/oradaptedforlocaluse,collaboratorsrecruitedsamples ofwomenfromthecommunityusingdirectapproachesinareas ofcongregateactivityineachresearchsite.Attemptsweremade toreduceselectionbiasbysamplingatdifferenttimesoftheday andfromdifferentsitesofcongregateactivity,andinsomecases this method wassupplemented withadvertisements to poten-tial participantsplaced in local media. Four exceptions to this generalrecruitmentmethodoccurredinGermany,Hungary,the

2Thereweredifficultiesinunderstandingandcompletingheightandweightdata

(10)

Netherlands,andPeru,wheredatawerecollectedonline follow-ingbest-practiceguidelines(e.g.,Meade&Craig,2012).Inclusion criteriaforallresearchsitesincludedbeingfluentinthelocal lan-guageandbeing18yearsofageorolder.Potentialparticipantswere givenaninformationsheet,whichprovidedbriefdetailsaboutthe studyandanestimatedsurveycompletiontime(10min). Partici-pantswhoagreedtotakepartprovidedwritteninformedconsent or digital informed consent for online studies. Thesurvey was anonymous and participantstook partvoluntarily and without remuneration.Allparticipantsreceivedwrittendebriefing infor-mationupon returnof completedquestionnaires.Alldatawere collectedin2018-2019.

2.6. Analyticstrategy

Allwithin-nationdatawerepooledsothatanalysesproceeded atthelevel ofthe nation,ratherthan individualresearchsites. AnalysisoftheBSSSdatathenproceededinfourstages.First,we investigatedcross-nationaldifferencesincurrentandidealbreast size,andinabsolutemeanbreastsizedissatisfaction,using analy-sesofvariance,andreportonthepercentagesofparticipantswith apreferenceforlargerbreasts,smallerbreasts,andnodiscrepancy betweenidealandcurrent breastsizes(Hypothesis1).Wethen checkedwhetheridealbreastsizeratingsweresignificantly asso-ciatedwithsocioeconomicstatus(urbanicityandfinancialsecurity) acrossnationsusingamultilevelmodel(Davidovetal.,2018;van

derVijveretal.,2008)withthetwovariablesaslevel-1predictors

andnationasalevel-2predictor.Interceptswereallowedtovary betweennations.Alllevel-1predictorsweregrandmean-centred andMplus8.2wasutilisedforanalyses,usingMonteCarlo integra-tiontoevaluatethelikelihoodfunction(Heck&Thomas,2015).We reportontheoutcomevarianceexplainedbythemodelonlevels-1 and-2(R12andR22)usingapproximateformulaeprovidedbyBryk

andRaudenbush(1992),whichcomparesthemodelofinterestto

abaselinemodelandincludesonlyrandomintercepts.

Second,todeterminewhetherthemediaexposureitemsformed one-dimensionalscales(separatelyforWesternandlocalmedia), multi-groupmodelswerefittedontheitemsassessingmedia expo-sure,testing for measurement invarianceof theseitems (Chen, 2008).Forthisanalysis,datafromAustriaandGermany,Croatia andSlovenia,theUnitedKingdomandIreland,andJapanandChina, respectively,hadtobemergedinordertomakecomputations fea-sible(thatis,wemergeddataacrossnationswithsimilarcultural backgroundsandthatweregeographicneighbours).Wefirst exam-inedconfiguralinvariance(i.e.,whetherscoresonthe4itemsof eachmeasureformedone-dimensionalscoresineverynation)and thenfullmeasurementinvariance(i.e.,whetheritemparameters –seebelow–wereidenticalacrossnations).Iffullmeasurement invariancewasnotobserved,itemparameterswererelaxedin indi-vidualnationstotestforpartialmeasurementinvariance(which meansthattheparametersofsome,butnotall,itemswereequal acrossnations).Items weretreated asordered-categorical vari-ables,usingthematrixofpolychoriccorrelationsandtheWLSMV estimator(weightedleastsquareestimatorusingadiagonalweight matrixwithstandarderrorsandmean-andvariance-adjusted chi-squareteststatisticthatuseafullweightmatrix)inMplus8.2. Missingdata(0.4%)weretreatedusingfullinformationmaximum likelihood(FIML)estimation,whichissuperiortoothermethods fordealingwithmissingdata(Enders&Bandalos,2001).Eachitem wasmodelledwithtwo sets of parameters:one discrimination parameter(itemloading)andm–1(m=numberofitemresponse categories)thresholdparameters,whichdescribetheprobability foreachresponseoption.Setsofitemparameterswerefreedin tandemforpartialmeasurementanalyses(Sass,2011).Wereport ␻totalasmeasureofreliability(Dunn,Baguley,&Brunsden,2014) forthemanifestscoresofthetwomediaexposurescales.However,

standardisedfactorscoresofWesternandlocalmediaexposure wereusedforfurtheranalyses.

Fortheevaluationofmodelfit,thecomparativefitindex(CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and the standardised root mean squareresidual(SRMR)wereused,utilisingguidelinesprovidedby

HuandBentler(1999)andSchermelleh-Engel,Moosbrugger,and

Müller(2003))(CFI,TLI:>.90acceptablefit,>.95goodfit;SRMR:

<.10acceptablefit,<.05goodfit).WLSMVestimatesthemodel degreesoffreedom(df)fromthedata(Muthén,duToit,&Spisic, 1997);thedfarenotderivedbycomparingthenumberofavailable andestimatedparameters,asinmaximumlikelihood(ML) estima-tion.CFIandTLIusethedftopenaliseformodelcomplexity(the TLImorestronglythantheCFI).UnderMLestimation,thisentails TLI<CFI.However,inthemulti-groupcontextofthepresentstudy, estimationofdfbyWLSMVcouldeitherexcessivelydisadvantage TLItoCFIvalues (especiallyinmodelswitha largernumberof estimatedparameters;i.e.,configuralinvariancemodels)or exces-sivelydisadvantageCFItoTLIvalues (especiallyinmodelswith fewestimatedparameters,suchasfullandpartialmeasurements models,leadingtoCFI<TLI,whichisnotpossibleunderML estima-tion).Thus,modelfitwasconsideredacceptableiftheSRMRand eitherCFIorTLIvaluesindicatedanacceptablefit.Forthe vari-ousmeasurementinvarianceanalyses,modelfitwasinterpreted independently,astheWLSMVestimatordoesnotallowfordirect comparisonsbetweenmodels basedonindices likeCFI (Sass, 2011);becauseofthelargesamplesizesinvolved,wealsodidnot relyon␹2tests,asthesetendtobecometooliberal(rejectingthe

nullhypothesistoooften)withincreasingN(Cheung&Rensvold,

1999).

In the third stage of analysis, using multilevel modelling, we examinedassociations of potential antecedents withbreast sizedissatisfaction,namelypersonality(OpennesstoExperience, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Neuroticism), Westernandlocalmediaexposure(factorscoresfromthe afore-mentionedanalyses),socioeconomicstatus(urbanicity,financial security), andage(level-1predictors), usingnationasa level-2 predictorandallowingforrandomnessin theinterceptand the slopes of Western and local media exposure (i.e.,allowing for differencesbetweennationsintheseparameters;slopesand inter-ceptswereallowedtocorrelate)(Hypothesis2).Fig.1 presents a schematic representation of this model. Multilevel modelling wasutilisedbecauseofthenestednatureofthedata(participants withinnations)(Davidovetal.,2018;Hox,1998).Furthermore,it allowedtestingofwhetherassociationsofmediaexposurewith breastsizedissatisfactionvariedacrosscountries.Guidedbythe resultsofthisinitialmodel,level-1predictorswerethenremoved iftheywerenotsignificantlyassociatedwithbreastsize dissat-isfaction,andslopesofWesternandlocalmediaexposurewere estimatedasfixedeffectsiftheseparametersdidnotexhibit sig-nificantamountsofrandomnessaccordingtolikelihoodratio(LR) tests(Hox,1998).Theremainingsetoflevel-1predictorsservedin theanalysesofthefourthstage.

Inthefourthstageofanalysis,thefinalmodelofthethirdstage incorporatedappearanceandweightdissatisfaction,breast aware-ness,andpsychologicalwell-beingpotentialoutcomesofbreast sizedissatisfaction(Hypothesis3)(seeFig.1).Conceptually,we testedinthismultilevelmodelwhetherbreastsizedissatisfaction mediatedtheeffectsoftheantecedentsontheseoutcomes. Differ-encesbetweennationsintheinterceptsofoutcomeswereallowed forinthismodel.Fortheanalysesinthethirdandfourthstages, alllevel-1predictorsweregrandmean-centred(Hofmann&Gavin, 1998)3 .Mplus8.2wasutilised,usingMonteCarlointegrationto

3Grandmean-centeringsubtractsthegrandmeanofapredictorusingthemean

Referenties

GERELATEERDE DOCUMENTEN

Simulation results revealed that given one gallery (Training) face image and four different pose images as a probe (Testing), PCA based system is more accurate in recognizing

METHODS In order to obtain a well-founded explication of key values and behaviors of highly effective leaders in sustaining Lean, five subsequent research phases were followed, drawn

Across sports, selection decisions are mostly based on overall impressions of scouts and trainers (clinical judgment), whereas using explicit decision rules (actuarial judgment)

Furthermore, interviews with civil servants from the national government will be used to analyse how the integration of local knowledge and participation in

It is the collection of these ideas, possession, rule of reason and federation, that are central to Proudhon’s idea about mutualism.. 120 Men will not be in a state of

The( results( show( a( positive( relationship( between( leader( age( and( leader( legitimacy( and( a( positive( relationship( between( leader( legitimacy( and(

participants felt responsible for engaging in good health care practices and for monitoring their own health. So, long before a woman was diagnosed with breast cancer and

The human resources allocation will be used for providing additional and higher value honours bursaries (R42 million), postdoctoral fellowships (R11 million), the